Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-44mx8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-16T07:58:11.956Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations of polygenic risk score, environmental factors, and their interactions with the risk of schizophrenia spectrum disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2025

Fatima Zahra Rami
Affiliation:
Research Institute of Clinical Medicine of Jeonbuk National University and Biomedical Research Institute of Jeonbuk National University Hospital, Jeonju, South Korea Department of Psychiatry, Jeonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, South Korea
Hyungwoo Seo
Affiliation:
Department of Statistics and Actuarial Science, Soongsil University, Seoul, South Korea
Chaeyeong Kang
Affiliation:
Research Institute of Clinical Medicine of Jeonbuk National University and Biomedical Research Institute of Jeonbuk National University Hospital, Jeonju, South Korea
Seunghwan Park
Affiliation:
Department of Statistics and Actuarial Science, Soongsil University, Seoul, South Korea
Ling Li
Affiliation:
Research Institute of Clinical Medicine of Jeonbuk National University and Biomedical Research Institute of Jeonbuk National University Hospital, Jeonju, South Korea Department of Psychiatry, Jeonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, South Korea
Thi-Hung Le
Affiliation:
Research Institute of Clinical Medicine of Jeonbuk National University and Biomedical Research Institute of Jeonbuk National University Hospital, Jeonju, South Korea Department of Psychiatry, Jeonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, South Korea
Sung-Wan Kim
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Chonnam National University Medical School, Gwangju, South Korea
Seung-Hee Won
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Kyungpook National University, Daegu, South Korea
Wonil Chung*
Affiliation:
Department of Statistics and Actuarial Science, Soongsil University, Seoul, South Korea Program in Genetic Epidemiology and Statistical Genetics, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
Young-Chul Chung*
Affiliation:
Research Institute of Clinical Medicine of Jeonbuk National University and Biomedical Research Institute of Jeonbuk National University Hospital, Jeonju, South Korea Department of Psychiatry, Jeonbuk National University Medical School, Jeonju, South Korea
*
Corresponding authors: Wonil Chung and Young-Chul Chung; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Wonil Chung and Young-Chul Chung; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Emerging evidence indicates that gene–environment interactions (GEIs) are important underlying mechanisms for the development of schizophrenia (SZ). We investigated the associations of polygenic risk score for SZ (PRS-SZ), environmental measures, and their interactions with case–control status and clinical phenotypes among patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSDs).

Methods

The PRS-SZ for 717 SSD patients and 356 healthy controls (HCs) were calculated using the LDpred model. The Korea-Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I (K-PERS-I) and Early Trauma Inventory-Self Report (ETI-SR) were utilized as environmental measures. Logistic and linear regression analyses were performed to identify the associations of PRS-SZ and two environmental measures with case–control status and clinical phenotypes.

Results

The PRS-SZ explained 8.7% of SZ risk. We found greater associations of PRS-SZ and total scores of the K-PERS-I with case–control status compared to the ETI-SR total score. A significant additive interaction was found between PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I. With the subdomains of the K-PERS-I and ETI-SR, we identified significant multiplicative or additive interactions of PRS-SZ and parental socioeconomic status (pSES), childhood adversity, and recent life events in association with case–control status. For clinical phenotypes, significant interactions were observed between PRS-SZ and the ETI-SR total score for negative-self and between PRS-SZ and obstetric complications within the K-PERS-I for negative-others.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that the use of aggregate scores for genetic and environmental measures, PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I, can more accurately predict case–control status, and specific environmental measures may be more suitable for the exploration of GEIs.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Schizophrenia (SZ) is a complex disorder influenced by various factors, including genetic and environmental factors. Genetic factors explain a substantial portion of the risk, as indicated by twin studies demonstrating >80% heritability (Sullivan, Kendler, & Neale, Reference Sullivan, Kendler and Neale2003). However, environmental factors, particularly a history of significant childhood adversity (CA), also play key roles in the development and course of the disease (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Mondelli, Stilo, Trotta, Sideli and Ajnakina2020). The relationships among these factors have been extensively discussed, and hypotheses of gene–environment interactions (GEIs) for SZ have been proposed (Murray, Reveley, & McGuffin, Reference Murray, Reveley and McGuffin1986; Schulsinger, Parnas, Mednick, Teasdale, & Schulsinger, Reference Schulsinger, Parnas, Mednick, Teasdale and Schulsinger1987; Strahilevitz, Reference Strahilevitz1974). The identification of GEI effects in psychiatric disorders offers three key benefits: it allows new genetic and environmental main effects to be discovered (Kraft, Yen, Stram, Morrison, & Gauderman, Reference Kraft, Yen, Stram, Morrison and Gauderman2007), it enhances the understanding of the underlying biological pathways (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2006), and it offers clinical insights for personalized medicine and lifestyle recommendations (Dempfle et al., Reference Dempfle, Scherag, Hein, Beckmann, Chang-Claude and Schäfer2008; Uher & McGuffin, Reference Uher and McGuffin2008). Although GEIs are important, their identification requires sample sizes ranging from thousands (for candidate genes) to tens of thousands (for genome-wide association studies); thus, the statistical power can be limited (Thomas, Reference Thomas2010).

When used as a single metric of molecular genetic risk, the polygenic risk score (PRS) has greatly enhanced the capacity for identifying associations with phenotypes and GEIs (Choi, Mak, & O’Reilly, Reference Choi, Mak and O’Reilly2020; Lin, Huang, Liu, Tsai, & Kuo, Reference Lin, Huang, Liu, Tsai and Kuo2019). There is evidence that substantially greater predictive power can be achieved using the PRS rather than a small number of genome-wide significant single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) (Domingue, Trejo, Armstrong-Carter, & Tucker-Drob, Reference Domingue, Trejo, Armstrong-Carter and Tucker-Drob2020; Dudbridge, Reference Dudbridge2013). Many recent psychiatry studies have utilized PRS approaches to investigate GEIs (Soga, Teo, & Parhar, Reference Soga, Teo and Parhar2021; Woolway et al., Reference Woolway, Smart, Lynham, Lloyd, Owen and Jones2022; Yao, van der Veen, Thygesen, Bass, & McQuillin, Reference Yao, van der Veen, Thygesen, Bass and McQuillin2023). In SZ, prior studies have reported positive interactions between the PRS for SZ (PRS-SZ) and factors such as cannabis use (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Jones, Burgess, Bowden, Davey Smith, Zammit and Munafò2017; Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019; Pasman et al., Reference Pasman, Verweij, Gerring, Stringer, Sanchez-Roige and Treur2018), urbanicity (Colodro-Conde et al., Reference Colodro-Conde, Couvy-Duchesne, Whitfield, Streit, Gordon and Kemper2018; Maxwell, Coleman, Breen, & Vassos, Reference Maxwell, Coleman, Breen and Vassos2021; Paksarian et al., Reference Paksarian, Trabjerg, Merikangas, Mors, Børglum and Hougaard2018), and CA (Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019; Mas-Bermejo et al., Reference Mas-Bermejo, Papiol, Via, Rovira, Torrecilla and Kwapil2023; Pignon et al., Reference Pignon, Peyre, Ayrolles, Kirkbride, Jamain and Ferchiou2022; Pries et al., Reference Pries, Klingenberg, Menne-Lothmann, Decoster, van Winkel and Collip2020a; Pries et al., Reference Pries, Klingenberg, Menne-Lothmann, Decoster, van Winkel and Collip2020b; Saarinen et al., Reference Saarinen, Hietala, Lyytikäinen, Hamal Mishra, Sormunen and Lavonius2024; Sallis et al., Reference Sallis, Croft, Havdahl, Jones, Dunn and Davey Smith2021; Smigielski et al., Reference Smigielski, Papiol, Theodoridou, Heekeren, Gerstenberg and Wotruba2021; Trotta et al., Reference Trotta, Iyegbe, Di Forti, Sham, Campbell and Cherny2016). However, contrasting findings also exist; some authors have reported negative associations between PRS-SZ and cannabis use (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Colbert, Jeffries, Tillman, Bigdeli and Karcher2023) and CA (Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019; Mas-Bermejo et al., Reference Mas-Bermejo, Papiol, Via, Rovira, Torrecilla and Kwapil2023; Pignon et al., Reference Pignon, Peyre, Ayrolles, Kirkbride, Jamain and Ferchiou2022; Pries, Klingenberg, et al., Reference Pries, Klingenberg, Menne-Lothmann, Decoster, van Winkel and Collip2020a; Pries, Klingenberg, et al., Reference Pries, Klingenberg, Menne-Lothmann, Decoster, van Winkel and Collip2020b; Saarinen et al., Reference Saarinen, Hietala, Lyytikäinen, Hamal Mishra, Sormunen and Lavonius2024; Sallis et al., Reference Sallis, Croft, Havdahl, Jones, Dunn and Davey Smith2021; Smigielski et al., Reference Smigielski, Papiol, Theodoridou, Heekeren, Gerstenberg and Wotruba2021; Trotta et al., Reference Trotta, Iyegbe, Di Forti, Sham, Campbell and Cherny2016).

Although most studies have primarily focused on GEIs using single environmental factors, each factor represents a small portion of the dense network of possible environmental exposures. The introduction of a cumulative environmental score would provide a single measure, thereby enhancing risk prediction and advancing research to more fully comprehend the collective impact of the environment and its interactions with genetics in the context of psychosis. Several tools have been developed to measure cumulative environmental load in the form of a single aggregate score, similar to the PRS used in genetics. Examples include the exposome score for SZ (ES-SZ) (Cuesta et al., Reference Cuesta, Papiol, Ibañez, García de Jalón, Sánchez-Torres and Gil-Berrozpe2023; Pries et al., Reference Pries, Dal Ferro, van Os, Delespaul, Kenis and Lin2020), the Maudsley Environmental Risk Score (ERS) (Vassos et al., Reference Vassos, Sham, Kempton, Trotta, Stilo and Gayer-Anderson2020), the Polyenviromic Risk Score (PERS) (Padmanabhan, Shah, Tandon, & Keshavan, Reference Padmanabhan, Shah, Tandon and Keshavan2017), and the Psychosis Polyrisk Score (PPS) (Oliver et al., Reference Oliver, Spada, Englund, Chesney, Radua and Reichenberg2020; Oliver, Radua, Reichenberg, Uher, & Fusar-Poli, Reference Oliver, Radua, Reichenberg, Uher and Fusar-Poli2019). While these tools incorporate similar risk factors, they differ in how the aggregate score is calculated. ERS and PPS scores are estimated by scaling odds ratios (ORs) or relative risks (RRs) with population prevalence for each risk factor, and the PERS score is obtained by simply summing the ORs of the risk factors. ES-SZ employs a more advanced approach using weighted coefficients derived from a single predictive model, which accounts for the interdependency of exposures (Pries et al., Reference Pries, Lage-Castellanos, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019). In contrast, the K-PERS-I score is calculated using ORs and RRs from Western studies, adjusted for the population proportions of risk factors identified from multiple resources of the Korean data. However, only a limited number of studies have investigated GEIs using these tools in SZ, and they have yielded mixed results (Cuesta et al., Reference Cuesta, Papiol, Ibañez, García de Jalón, Sánchez-Torres and Gil-Berrozpe2023; Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019; Mas et al., Reference Mas, Boloc, Rodríguez, Mezquida, Amoretti and Cuesta2020; Pries, Dal Ferro, et al., Reference Pries, Dal Ferro, van Os, Delespaul, Kenis and Lin2020). Notably, there has been a lack of studies examining GEIs in Asian SZ patients using these comprehensive measures of polyenvironmental factors.

We utilized the Korea-Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I (K-PERS-I) (Jeon et al., Reference Jeon, Kang, Piao, Kim, Kim and Lee2022), a comprehensive measure of multiple environmental factors associated with SZ, developed based on the proportions of risk factors (exposure) in the Korean population. Moreover, considering suggestions that GEIs can only involve monogenic factors (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington2005; Stefanis et al., Reference Stefanis, Henquet, Avramopoulos, Smyrnis, Evdokimidis and Myin-germeys2007) and comprehensive measures may not be appropriate tools for studying GEIs (Assary, Vincent, Keers, & Pluess, Reference Assary, Vincent, Keers and Pluess2018), we also adopted three specific environmental measures: subdomains of the K-PERS-I, as well as the total and subdomains of the Early Trauma Inventory-Self Report (ETI-SR) (Bremner, Bolus, & Mayer, Reference Bremner, Bolus and Mayer2007), which evaluates CA.

In this study, we aimed to calculate PRS-SZ using the Korean Genomics Center (KOGIC) dataset and East Asia (EAS) summary statistics from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC). Subsequently, using the two aggregate scores (PRS-SZ and total K-PERS-I), we investigated the associations of two main factors and their interactions with case–control status, as well as with clinical phenotypes assessed using the Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS) and the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSDs). Additionally, we explored these associations using three specific environmental measures. The subsequent results were compared in order to see the possible advantages of using the comprehensive measure, K-PERS-I, over three specific environmental measures.

Methods

Participants

Patients (n = 818) were recruited from outpatients and inpatients treated at four hospitals (Jeonbuk [JB], Chonnam, Kyungpook, and Haeundae Paik National University Hospitals [NUH]) from December 2014 to February 2021. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) SSDs including SZ, schizoaffective disorder, schizophreniform disorder, and psychotic disorder not otherwise specified; (b) and age between 18 and 59 years. Diagnoses were established using the DSM-IV-TR criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and the Korean version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Yoo et al., Reference Yoo, Kim, Noh, Oh, Kim and Namkoong2006). The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) intelligence quotient (IQ) ≤ 70; (b) acute, unstable, or severe medical/neurological conditions; or (c) pregnancy or lactation. Healthy controls (HCs) were recruited through advertisements at JBNUH (n = 356) and underwent psychiatric interviews using the screening module of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams2002). HCs were excluded if they had a current or previous diagnosis of mental disorders, a clinically significant medical condition, or first-degree relatives with psychosis (to minimize the effects of genetic loading). The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work complied with the ethical standards of relevant national and institutional Human Experimental Commissions and the 1975 Declaration of Helsinki, as amended in 2008. All procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Ethics Committee of JBNUH (approval number CUH 2014-11-002).

Assessments

Psychopathology was evaluated using PANSS (Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1987; Yi et al., Reference Yi, Ahn, Shin, An, Joo and Kim2001). To maximize inter-rater reliability across sites, psychiatrists with more than 3 years of experience in this field participated in the ratings process, and several workshops were held during the recruitment period (Cohen’s kappa ≥0.8). Self-rating scales, such as BCSS (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Freeman, Smith, Kuipers, Bebbington and Bashforth2006) and ETI-SR, were administered. For simplicity, we used only two subdomains of the ETI-SR, negative-self and negative-others, by reversing the scores of positive-self and positive-others. The reliability and validity of the Korean version of the ETI-SR were confirmed in Korean patients with depression (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87) (Jeon et al., Reference Jeon, Lee, Lee, Jeong, Kim, Lee and Jeon2012). Environmental risk factors associated with SZ were measured using the K-PERS-I (Jeon et al., Reference Jeon, Kang, Piao, Kim, Kim and Lee2022), which consisted of six domains: paternal age at birth, parental socioeconomic status (pSES), obstetric complications, urbanicity, CA, and recent life events. For domain and scoring system definitions, see Supplementary Tables: Material 1.

Calculation of the PRS-SZ

The JBNUH genotype dataset (for details on the quality control of genetic data, see Supplementary Tables: Material 2 and Supplementary Figures 1 and 2), was converted from UCSC hg19 genomic coordinates to hg38 using CrossMap (v0.6.4). The KOGIC dataset of the Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology consists of 28,692,913 SNPs in 1,047 Korean individuals, primarily from the Ulsan metropolitan region in the southern part of the Korean peninsula (Jeon et al., Reference Jeon, Bhak, Choi, Jeon, Kim and Jang2020). After the conversion, the dataset retained 437,188 SNPs. KOGIC genotype data were used as a reference panel for genotype imputation in the JBNUH dataset, conducted in two stages. First, pre-phasing of each chromosome was performed using Eagle (v2.4.1). Subsequently, Minimac3 (v2.0.1) was used to impute phased genotypes for approximately 28.7 million markers in the KOGIC genotype data, retaining variants with an imputation INFO score > 0.8. A global PRS-SZ was then generated using East Asian (EAS) summary statistics (Lam et al., Reference Lam, Chen, Li, Martin, Bryois and Ma2019) and KOGIC data as the linkage disequilibrium (LD) reference panel. For the PRS-SZ calculation, we utilized 2,525,966 SNPs that overlapped with the SNPs in the EAS summary statistics. Instead of limiting the analysis to significant SNPs, we included all available SNPs. The effect sizes were reweighted using the LDpred-inf algorithm to account for causal variants and non-infinitesimal genetic architecture. This approach incorporates a point-normal prior for posterior mean effect size estimation, implemented via the Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) method, which assumes a Gaussian mixture prior: $ {\beta}_i\sim N\left(0,\frac{h_g^2}{M}\times p\right) $ with probability p, and  $ {\beta}_i\sim 0 $ with probability 1 − p, where p is the proportion of causal SNPs. The method is optimized by considering different values of p (10−3, 3 × 10−3, 0.01, 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1) (Privé, Arbel, & Vilhjálmsson, Reference Privé, Arbel and Vilhjálmsson2021; Vilhjálmsson et al., Reference Vilhjálmsson, Yang, Finucane, Gusev, Lindström and Ripke2015). The study population n = 1,073 participants (717 with SZ) was tested for the predictive performance of the global PRS-SZ. Personal scores were computed by coding each variant, weighting by its relative effect size on SZ, along with standardization. The PRS-SZ with the highest discriminative capability was chosen based on the maximum area under the curve (AUC). The estimated AUCs for different proportions of causal variants (10−3, 3 × 10−3, 0.01, 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1) were 0.529, 0.670, 0.695, 0.700, 0.699, 0.698, and 0.699, respectively. The LDpred model with a 3% proportion of causal variants demonstrated the highest predictive accuracy for SZ. Furthermore, age and sex adjustments were applied to the PRS-SZ. We calculated the heritability $ \left({h}^2\right) $ for SZ with the Korean-specific LD scores and EAS summary statistics using LD score regression (Bulik-Sullivan et al., Reference Bulik-Sullivan, Loh, Finucane, Ripke, Yang and Patterson2015). The prediction accuracy for the PRS-SZ was assessed using AUC, which was subsequently converted to liability R 2, considering a population lifetime risk of SZ at 1% (Lee, Wray, Goddard, & Visscher, Reference Lee, Wray, Goddard and Visscher2011).

Statistical analyses

Gene–environment correlation between PRS-SZ and the subdomains of K-PERS-I in SSDs patients was assessed using a logistic regression model, adjusted for age, sex, and education. To examine the associations of PRS-SZ and/or total scores of K-PERS-I/ETI-SR with case–control status, we conducted four types of logistic regression models: (i) a genetic model with PRS-SZ only, (ii) an environmental model including either K-PERS-I or ETI-SR, (iii) an independent model with PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR without an interaction term, and (iv) an interaction model with PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR with an interaction term. For the total score of the K-PERS-I/ETI-SR, we performed analyses using a complete dataset and an imputed dataset. Interaction was assessed in two ways: first, through logistic regression to evaluate interaction as a departure from multiplicativity, and second, through the Relative Excess Risk due to Interaction (RERI) to assess interaction as a departure from additivity (Knol & VanderWeele, Reference Knol and VanderWeele2012). An RERI >0 indicated a positive deviation from additivity and was considered statistically significant if the 95% confidence interval (CI) did not contain zero. The RERIs were calculated using the delta method, which imposes an unnatural symmetry on the confidence limits of the underlying effect measure (Hosmer & Lemeshow, Reference Hosmer and Lemeshow1992), using the ORs derived from each model via the epiR package in R. In all analyses, we adjusted for covariates such as age, sex, and education. Multiple testing for subdomains of the K-PERS-I was corrected with false discovery rate (FDR). Nagelkerke’s R 2 value for each model was calculated with the DescTools package in R (version 0.99.52).

Additionally, logistic regression analyses were conducted with dichotomized PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR scores, using a threshold of 75% of the HC group. This threshold was selected to more effectively capture the impact of high genetic risk while minimizing the potential masking of effects by intermediate or low scores (Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019; Mas et al., Reference Mas, Boloc, Rodríguez, Mezquida, Amoretti and Cuesta2020; Pries, Dal Ferro, et al., Reference Pries, Dal Ferro, van Os, Delespaul, Kenis and Lin2020; Segura et al., Reference Segura, Mezquida, Martínez-Pinteño, Gassó, Rodriguez and Moreno-Izco2023). The subgroup with low K-PERS-I/ETI-SR and low PRS-SZ with scores less than 75% was regarded as the reference group. Sensitivity analyses were performed using alternative thresholds of 50% and 25%. We also conducted the same analysis separately for males and females. Moreover, considering that simply adjusting for these covariates could be insufficient to eliminate potential bias in the Gene × Environment interaction term (Keller, Reference Keller2014), we performed further analyses with these covariates included as interaction terms and assessed the improvement of model fit with the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC).

To investigate the associations of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR (total and subdomains) and their interactions with clinical phenotypes, PANSS, and BCSS, linear regression with the interaction model was utilized. Multiple testing correction was applied to all analyses using the FDR method, with a significance threshold of FDR < 0.05. As we had smaller sample sizes for the total K-PERS-I/ETI-SR because of different missing data in the subdomains of the K-PERS-I/ETI-SR, we conducted the main analyses again after the imputation using the mice package in R (version 3.16.0). In all analyses, we used R software (ver. 4.1.1) and adjusted for age, sex, and education as covariates.

For power calculation, we performed post hoc power calculation (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Alameda, Aas, Gayer-Anderson, Trotta and Spinazzola2024) for the PRS-SZ by K-PERS-I interaction using a simulation method in R using the standardized coefficients for PRS-SZ, K-PERS-I, and PRS-SZ * K-PERS-I, from the comparison of case–control (For details on the code used, see Supplementary Tables: Materials 3 and 4).

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants

The participants were divided into three groups: SSDs with K-PERS-I (n = 295–398), SSDs with ETI-SR (n = 474–476), and HCs (n = 320–356). Compared with HCs, the SSDs with K-PERS-I group were younger (p < 0.001); it had higher negative-self and negative-others scores (p < 0.001), and higher total and subdomains K-PERS-I scores (p < 0.001), with the exception of pSES. Similarly, compared with the HCs, the SSDs with ETI-SR group were younger (p < 0.001); it also had higher negative-self and negative-others scores (p < 0.001) and lower total and subdomain ETI-SR scores. Comparison of the SSDs with K-PERS-I and the SSDs with ETI-SR groups revealed no significant differences (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants

a SSDs with K-PERS-I (n = 280 ~ 398) versus HCs (n = 320 ~ 356); bSSDs with ETI-SR (n = 432 ~ 476) versus HCs (n = 320 ~ 356); cSSDs with K-PERS-I (n = 280 ~ 398) versus SZ with ETI-SR (n = 432 ~ 476); 269 SSDs patients with K-PERS-I group (n = 295) are overlapping with SSDs patients with ETI group (n = 474).

Note: BCSS, Brief Core Schema Scales; DI, duration of illness; DUP, duration of untreated psychosis; ETI-SR, Early Trauma Inventory Self Report; HCs, healthy controls; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; NUH, National University Hospital; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; PNOS, psychotic disorder not otherwise specified; SES, socioeconomic Status; SSDs, schizophrenia spectrum disorders; SSDs, schizophrenia spectrum disorders; All values were mean ± standard deviation (SD) unless otherwise specified.

AUC and liability R2 for PRS-SZ versus PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR

The PRS-SZ alone showed an AUC of 0.724 and a liability R 2 of 8.7% (Supplementary Figure 3). However, when incorporating both PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I, we observed an AUC of 0.832 and a liability R 2 of 22.6%. Combining PRS-SZ and ETI-SR resulted in moderate values, with an AUC of 0.766 and a liability R 2 of 12.8% (Table 2). The SNP heritability for SZ calculated from the Korean-specific LD scores and EAS summary statistics was 0.20 ± 0.01.

Table 2. AUC and liability R 2 of PRS-SZ versus PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on case–control status

All variables were adjusted for age, sex, and education.

Note: AUC, area under the receiver operating curve; ETI-SR, Early Trauma Inventory Self Report; HCs, healthy controls; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; PRS-SZ, Polygenic Risk Score-Schizophrenia; SSDs, schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

Gene–environment correlation

We tested for correlations between PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I subdomains. No significant correlations were found in the patient group (Supplementary Table 1).

Main and interaction effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on case–control status

When using the K-PERS-I as an environmental measure, the main effects in all four models were significantly associated with case–control status (PRS-SZ: adjusted OR = 2.20, 95% CI = 1.74–2.80, p = 1.68 × 10−11, K-PERS-I: adjusted OR = 3.26, 95% CI = 2.66–4.05, p = 7.41 × 10−28, PRS-SZ + K-PERS-I: adjusted OR = 3.39, 95% CI = 2.73–4.25, p = 7.01 × 10−27, PRS-SZ + K-PERS-I + PRS-SZ × K-PERS-I: adjusted OR = 3.37, 95% CI = 2.71–4.25, p = 3.74 × 10−26). No significant multiplicative interaction was observed, whereas a significant additive interaction was identified with a RERI of 3.20 (95% CI: 0.29–6.12, p = 0.015) (Figure 1a). Nagelkerke’s R 2 values were highest for the independent and interaction models. Simulation using the standardized regression coefficients for the PRS-SZ by K-PERS-I multiplicative interaction yielded a power estimate of 5.6%. It was estimated that a minimum sample size of 180,000 individuals would be required to achieve 80% power. In contrast, for the additive interaction term, the power was approximately 88%, and the minimum sample size required to achieve 80% power was 520 individuals. When using ETI-SR as the environmental measure, all four models showed significant results, but no significant interaction effects were identified. Notably, the ORs for ETI-SR associated with SZ risk were lower than those observed for K-PERS-I (Table 3).

Figure 1. (a) Additive effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I, (b) additive effects of PRS-SZ and pSES, (c) additive effects of PRS-SZ and CA, and (d) additive effects of PRS-SZ and Recent life events. Note: CA, childhood adversity; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; PRS-SZ, Polygenic Risk Score-Schizophrenia; pSES, parental socioeconomic status; RERI, relative excess risk due to interaction.

Table 3. Main and interaction effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on case–control status

All variables were adjusted for age, sex, and education.

Note: ETI-SR, Early Trauma Inventory Self Report; HCs, healthy controls; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; OR, odd ratio; PRS-SZ, polygenic risk score-schizophrenia; RERI, relative excess risk due to interaction; SSDs, schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

In additional analyses using dichotomized PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR with thresholds set at 75% of the HC group, the subgroup with high K-PERS-I and high PRS-SZ showed the highest OR (10.95, 95% CI = 5.89–20.35, p < 0.001) compared with the other three subgroups. For the subgroup with high ETI-SR and high PRS-SZ, the pattern was generally similar, but the OR was lower compared with the subgroup with a high K-PERS-I and high PRS-SZ (Figure 2). Sensitivity analyses using thresholds of 50% and 25% of the HC group showed similar results (Supplementary Figure 4). We found no different results between genders (Supplementary Table 2). Further analyses including three covariates (age, sex, and education) *GEss showed no significant results in all four models (Supplementary Table 3). AIC and BIC values of the new models assessing model fit were increased compared to the original models (Supplementary Table 4).

Figure 2. Regression results of subgroups of the K-PERS-I /ETI-SR and PRS-SZ were divided with a 75% cutoff of the control group. Note: ETI-SR, Early Trauma Inventory Self Report; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; N, number; OR, odd ratio; PRS-SZ, Polygenic Risk Score-Schizophrenia.

When using the K-PERS-I subdomains in the interaction models, both PRS-SZ and each subdomain were significantly associated with SZ risk. However, a significant multiplicative interaction with PRS-SZ was observed for pSES only (adjusted OR = 1.39, 95% CI =1.17–1.66, p = 2.70 × 10−4, FDR = 0.002). Notably, its Nagelkerke’s R 2 value was 19.2%. Regarding additive interactions, PRS-SZ showed significant associations with pSES (RERI = 1.27, 95% CI = 0.32–2.23, p = 0.005, FDR = 0.013), childhood adversity (RERI = 3.69, 95% CI = 1.08–6.30, p = 0.003, FDR = 0.013), and recent life events (RERI = 1.50, 95% CI = 0.13–2.88, p = 0.016, FDR = 0.027) (Figure 1ad, Supplementary Table 5). The additive interaction between PRS-SZ and pSES showed a power of 97.3%, with a minimum sample size of 450 individuals required to achieve 80% power. When using the ETI-SR subdomains, similar main effects were observed; however, no significant interactions remained after FDR correction (Supplementary Table 6).

Main and interaction effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on the PANSS and BCSS in patients with SSDs

For the PANSS, an interaction model with the K-PERS-I showed neither main effects nor additive interaction. However, in the model with the ETI-SR, PRS-SZ was significantly associated with the total score (Beta = 2.66, SD = 1.26, p = 0.036, FDR = 0.036). In contrast, the total ETI-SR score was only associated with positive symptom score (Beta = 0.93, standard deviation (SD) = 0.28, p = 0.001, FDR = 0.003). No significant interaction was evident. For the BCSS, the interaction model with the K-PERS-I showed neither main effects nor interaction. However, in the model with the ETI-SR, the total ETI-SR score was significantly associated with negative-self (Beta = 0.99, SD = 0.19, p = 4.14 × 10−7, FDR = 4.14 × 10−7) and negative-others (Beta = 1.38, SD = 0.18, p = 1.34 × 10−13, FDR = 2.68 × 10−13) (Table 4).

Table 4. Main and interaction effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on the PANSS or BCSS in patients with SSDs

All variables were adjusted for age, sex, and education.

Note: BCSS, Brief Core Schema Scales; ETI-SR, Early Trauma Inventory Self Report; FDR, false discovery rate; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; N, number of patients with SSDs; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; PRS-SZ, polygenic risk score-schizophrenia; SSDs, schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

Additional analyses of the PANSS using K-PERS-I subdomains revealed no significant main effect of PRS-SZ, and no interaction effects remained significant after FDR correction. For the BCSS, no main effect of the PRS-SZ was evident, whereas CA was significantly associated with both negative-self (Beta = 0.72, SD = 0.27, p = 0.008, FDR = 0.008) and negative-others (Beta = 1.04, SD = 0.25, p < 0.001, FDR < 0.001). Intriguingly, a significant positive interaction between PRS-SZ and obstetric complications was identified in association with negative-others (Beta = 0.55, SD = 0.19, p = 0.004, FDR = 0.008) (Supplementary Table 7). In the model with ETI-SR subdomains, PRS-SZ was significantly associated with total, negative symptom, and general psychopathology scores; several subdomains of the ETI-SR were significantly associated with total, positive symptom, and general psychopathology scores. No significant interaction was noted. For the analyses with the BCSS, significant associations with negative-self and negative-others were only noted for the ETI-SR subdomains (Supplementary Table 8). After imputation, the results for Tables 3 and 4 were the same (Supplementary Tables 911).

Discussion

Studies of GEIs may partly explain why SZ only develops in some people who experience relevant environmental exposures. Here, we investigated associations of PRS-SZ and total K-PERS-I and their interactions with case–control status and clinical phenotypes. Additionally, we investigated these associations using the K-PERS-I subdomains, as well as the total and subdomains of the ETI-SR.

In the present study, the PRS-SZ explained approximately 9% of SZ risk, which is similar to the reported 7% of the variance on the liability scale (Ripke et al., Reference Ripke, Neale, Corvin, Walters, Farh and Holmans2014) but higher than other reported rates of ~3% (Lam et al., Reference Lam, Chen, Li, Martin, Bryois and Ma2019) or 3.4% (Agerbo et al., Reference Agerbo, Sullivan, Vilhjálmsson, Pedersen, Mors and Børglum2015). When the K-PERS-I and interaction term (PRS-SZ × K-PERS-I) were combined, the proportion of explained risk increased to 22.6%. This finding suggests that approaches considering both genetic and environmental factors and their interactions could enhance the accuracy of SZ risk prediction. Regarding the ORs for PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I in Table 3, the values of the K-PERS-I from the environmental model and two combined models (PRS-SZ + K-PERS-I and PRS-SZ + K-PERS-I + PRS-SZ × K-PERS-I) were greater than the values of PRS-SZ from the genetic model. These findings indicate that the contributions of environmental factors and the combined effects of genetic and environmental factors in association with SSD risk are greater than the contributions of genetic effects alone. However, this pattern was not observed when the ETI-SR was regarded as an environmental factor. The ORs of the ETI-SR from the environmental model and two combined models were smaller than the ORs of PRS-SZ from the genetic model. Moreover, the ORs of the ETI-SR were smaller than the ORs of the K-PERS-I. These findings suggest that using the K-PERS-I, a more comprehensive tool that measures multiple environmental factors, may be more effective than using the ETI-SR for predicting SSD risk. However, this interpretation should be approached with caution, as the sample sizes between the two datasets differ.

For the interaction with the K-PERS-I, we observed contrasting results between multiplicative versus additive models. Considering that an additive model can provide a superior representation of biological synergy and inform public health decisions within the sufficient cause framework (Kendler & Gardner Reference Kendler and Gardner2010; Rothman, Reference Rothman1976), the result of the additive model needs to be more highlighted. It suggests that the combined effect of genetic and environmental risk factors is greater than the sum of their individual effects, which is in align with the findings of GEI studies using the ES-SZ (Pries, Dal Ferro, et al., Reference Pries, Dal Ferro, van Os, Delespaul, Kenis and Lin2020) and ERS (Mas et al., Reference Mas, Boloc, Rodríguez, Mezquida, Amoretti and Cuesta2020). The implication of this synergistic interaction should be incorporated into psychoeducation and public health policy. The lack of significant interactions with the ETI-SR in both models may be due to different characteristics of the two measures: several subdomains of the K-PERS-I capture pre/perinatal or chronic exposures that are likely to affect or interact with genetic risk, while ETI-SR focuses on acute events, which may act independently of genetic predisposition. This emphasizes the need to consider the duration and nature of environmental exposures in GEI studies. One meta-analysis reported that the current four studies on the interaction between PRS and childhood adversity yielded inconsistent findings (Woolway et al., Reference Woolway, Smart, Lynham, Lloyd, Owen and Jones2022). Interestingly, when PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR were dichotomized using a threshold of 75% of the control group value, the subgroups with high K-PERS-I/ETI-SR and high PRS-SZ showed the highest ORs, approximately 9–10, compared with the other three subgroups. If replicated in population-based cohort studies, these results could have important implications for the early identification of individuals at risk of developing SSDs and the provision of more intensive psychosocial interventions that target modifiable environmental factors in patients with SSDs.

When using the subdomains of the K-PERS-I, we found a significant interaction, departing from both multiplicativity and additivity, between PRS-SZ and pSES in association with SSD risk, indicating a synergistic effect. This suggests that the impact of pSES on SSD risk is moderated by PRS-SZ. To our knowledge, this is the first report demonstrating a significant interaction between pre-existing genetic liability and pSES in association with SSD risk, although a negative finding has been documented (Agerbo et al., Reference Agerbo, Sullivan, Vilhjálmsson, Pedersen, Mors and Børglum2015). Notably, Hatzimanolis et al., Reference Hatzimanolis, Stefanatou, Kattoulas, Ralli, Dimitrakopoulos and Foteli2020 found a significant interaction between familial risk of psychosis and pSES in terms of influencing social premorbid adjustment in childhood among patients with psychosis. There could be several explanations for this synergistic effect; poor pSES may lead to lower education, neighborhood deprivation, and health inequity relative to an affected individual’s siblings, all of which are factors related to mental health (González et al., Reference González, Estarlich, Murcia, Barreto-Zarza, Santa-Marina and Simó2023; Luo, van Grieken, Yang-Huang, van den Toren, & Raat, Reference Luo, van Grieken, Yang-Huang, van den Toren and Raat2022). Moreover, the cumulative effects of these interconnected factors may contribute to a synergistic effect, or the pSES may have exerted detrimental effects on SSD-associated mutants to produce more dysfunctional changes through epigenetic regulation. We identified significant additive interactions between PRS-SZ and both childhood adversity and recent life events. A synergistic interaction result between PRS-SZ and childhood adversity is in line with previous studies (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Alameda, Di Forti, Quattrone and Dazzan2023; Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin2019). Furthermore, given the interaction between PRS-major depressive disorder and childhood trauma (Peyrot et al., Reference Peyrot, Milaneschi, Abdellaoui, Sullivan, Hottenga and Peyrot2014), it reinforces the idea that genetic risk and early-life stressors can act together to increase susceptibility to mental health disorders. Similarly, recent life events, like stress or loss, can trigger or exacerbate genetic susceptibility to SZ. However, a study found that ES-SZ moderated the link between stressful life events and mental health, while PRS-SZ did not (Pries et al., Reference Pries, van Os, Ten Have, de Graaf, van Dorsselaer and Bak2020), highlighting the complexity of GESs. For the contribution of the K-PERS-I subdomains to SZ risk, all six factors were significant. Given that high paternal age is a risk factor for SZ and the average marriage age is increasing these days, this should be considered in psychoeducation and public health policy. Unexpectedly, urbanicity, which is strongly associated with SZ risk in other populations (Plana-Ripoll, Pedersen, & McGrath, Reference Plana-Ripoll, Pedersen and McGrath2018), was found to be a protective factor. This may be explained by easy access to mental healthcare services (Park, Park, Kwon, Kang, & Noh, Reference Park, Park, Kwon, Kang and Noh2016) and lower rates of drug abuse (Jang et al., Reference Jang, Lee, Park, Kang, Lee and Kim2023) in Korea compared to Western countries.

In the model with the K-PERS-I, neither PRS-SZ nor K-PERS-I were associated with the PANSS or BCSS, but in the model with the ETI-SR, PRS-SZ and ETI-SR were associated with several items. These findings suggest that the K-PERS-I is not an appropriate tool for investigating its association with clinical phenotypes. Concerning the results with the model that included the ETI-SR, PRS-SZ was significantly associated with the total score of the PANSS. Studies using PRS-SZ have yielded conflicting findings regarding associations with the symptom dimensions of SZ. Evidence supporting an association between PRS-SZ and positive symptoms is scarce, whereas studies of negative symptoms have led to mixed results (Mistry, Harrison, Smith, Escott-Price, & Zammit, Reference Mistry, Harrison, Smith, Escott-Price and Zammit2018; Ronald & Pain, Reference Ronald and Pain2018). Considering these inconclusive findings and the low prediction accuracy (~6%) of PRS-SZ for genetic variation in psychiatric phenotypes (Mistry et al., Reference Mistry, Harrison, Smith, Escott-Price and Zammit2018), the utility of PRS-SZ in clinical settings appears limited. In contrast, ETI-SR was associated with positive symptoms, negative-self, and negative-others. Because negative evaluations about the self and others or negative self-concepts are associated with the formation of delusions and hallucinations (Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman, & Bebbington, Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Fowler, Freeman, Bebbington, Bashforth and Garety2006), this finding may provide insights about how CA is involved in developing positive symptoms. Additionally, albeit at an uncorrected level, a synergistic effect between PRS-SZ and ETI-SR in predicting negative-self was observed. Taken together with the case–control status results, the model with the K-PERS-I may be better in terms of discriminating case–control status, whereas the model with the ETI-SR may be more suitable for predicting symptomatology.

In the subdomain models, CA of the K-PERS-I was associated with negative-self and negative-others. The positive interaction of PRS-SZ and obstetric complications in predicting negative-others is intriguing in that the association with negative-others only manifested when both genetic and environmental factors played a role; it was absent when each set of factors was present in isolation. For the synergistic effect, it is possible that individuals with genetic liability and obstetric complications have a higher likelihood of exposure to negative life events, which may lead to the formation of negative-others. Regarding the results with the model that included subdomains of the ETI-SR, the patterns were similar to those in the model with total ETI-SR.

Although this was the first study of GEIs using the K-PERS-I, several limitations should be considered. First, careful interpretation of the results is required, given that the sample size may have been insufficient for detecting multiplicative interaction potentially leading to type II errors. However, it is of note that in terms of additive interaction, the sample size was adequate. In addition, although we addressed missing data in the total K-PERS-I or ETI-SR by imputation, caution is needed when interpreting comparisons between total scores and subdomains due to differing sample sizes. Second, the study’s cross-sectional design did not allow for an investigation of the dynamic nature of the GEIs over time, hindering causal inference. Future research should focus more extensively on prospective cohort studies, including samples at different time points. Third, as we excluded foreigners to participate, the ethnic homogeneity of the sample, comprising original residents of South Korea, limits the generalizability of the findings to other populations. Fourth, although widely used, the RERI has limitations. Since it is based on ratios, it cannot be directly interpreted as the combined effects of exposures (Zhao & Thompson, Reference Zhao and Thompson2023). Fifth, K-PERS-I does not account for the interdependency of exposures like ES-SZ. Additionally, it excludes cannabis use and immigration, which may limit its ability to fully capture environmental influences on SZ risk and its applicability across diverse populations. Finally, the retrospective assessment of the K-PERS-I and ETI-SR may be associated with recall bias. Despite these limitations, this study has notable strengths, including being the first GEI study conducted in Korea and Asian countries and uniquely investigating and comparing the utilities of two environmental measures in the models.

In conclusion, using the aggregate scores of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I, we did not observe any interactions in association with case–control status. However, when using the subdomains of the K-PERS-I or total and subdomains of the ETI-SR, several interactions were identified in association with case–control status and clinical phenotypes. These findings suggest that the aggregate scores of genetic and environmental measures, PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I, are unsuitable for investigating GEIs, which were instead detected with specific environmental measures. However, the contribution of the total K-PERS-I to case–control status was greater than the contribution of the total ETI-SR. There is a need for additional research concerning GEIs and modifiable environmental factors.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000753.

Acknowledgments

The corresponding and first authors would like to thank all participants in the study and family for their guidance and support (SDG). This study was supported by a grant from the Korea Health Technology R&D Project through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute (KHIDI), funded by the Ministry of Health & Welfare, Republic of Korea (HR18C0016) and by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIT) (RS-2024-00335201). Additionally, this work was supported by the National Research Foundation (NRF) grant, funded by the Korea government (2020R1C1C1A01012657 and 2021R1A6A1A10044154).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests exist.

Footnotes

F.Z.R. and H.S. have contributed equally to this work.

References

Aas, M., Alameda, L., Di Forti, M., Quattrone, D., Dazzan, P., et al. (2023). Synergistic effects of childhood adversity and polygenic risk in first-episode psychosis: the EU-GEI study. Psychological medicine, 53(5), 19701978. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003664CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agerbo, E., Sullivan, P. F., Vilhjálmsson, B. J., Pedersen, C. B., Mors, O., Børglum, A. D., et al. (2015). Polygenic risk score, parental socioeconomic Status, family history of psychiatric disorders, and the risk for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 635. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-IV. APA.Google Scholar
Assary, E., Vincent, J. P., Keers, R., & Pluess, M. (2018). Gene-environment interaction and psychiatric disorders: Review and future directions. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology, 77, 133143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.semcdb.2017.10.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Bolus, R., & Mayer, E. A. (2007). Psychometric properties of the early trauma inventory–Self report. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 195(3), 211218. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.nmd.0000243824.84651.6cCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bulik-Sullivan, B. K., Loh, P.-R., Finucane, H. K., Ripke, S., Yang, J., Patterson, N., et al. (2015). LD score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nature Genetics, 47(3), 291295. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Gene–environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(7), 583590. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Cannon, M., McClay, J., Murray, R., Harrington, H., et al. (2005). Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: Longitudinal evidence of a gene X environment Interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 57(10), 11171127. https://doi.org/10.1016/.biopsych.2005.01.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S.-H., & O’Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: A guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nature Protocols, 15(9), 27592772. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-020-0353-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colodro-Conde, L., Couvy-Duchesne, B., Whitfield, J. B., Streit, F., Gordon, S., Kemper, K. E., et al. (2018). Association between population density and genetic risk for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(9), 901. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1581CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuesta, M. J., Papiol, S., Ibañez, B., García de Jalón, E., Sánchez-Torres, A. M., Gil-Berrozpe, G. J., et al. (2023). Effect of polygenic risk score, family load of schizophrenia and exposome risk score, and their interactions, on the long-term outcome of first-episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 53(14), 68386847. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dempfle, A., Scherag, A., Hein, R., Beckmann, L., Chang-Claude, J., & Schäfer, H. (2008). Gene–environment interactions for complex traits: Definitions, methodological requirements and challenges. European Journal of Human Genetics, 16(10), 11641172. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2008.106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domingue, B., Trejo, S., Armstrong-Carter, E., & Tucker-Drob, E. (2020). Interactions between polygenic scores and environments: Methodological and conceptual challenges. Sociological Science, 7, 365386. https://doi.org/10.15195/v7.a19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudbridge, F. (2013). Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genetics, 9(3), e1003348. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. (2002). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders, research version. New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Fowler, D., Freeman, D., Smith, B., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., Bashforth, H., et al. (2006). The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS): Psychometric properties and associations with paranoia and grandiosity in non-clinical and psychosis samples. Psychological Medicine, 36(6), 749759. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706007355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, S. H., Jones, H. J., Burgess, S., Bowden, J., Davey Smith, G., Zammit, S., & Munafò, M. R. (2017). Assessing causality in associations between cannabis use and schizophrenia risk: A two-sample Mendelian randomization study. Psychological Medicine, 47(5), 971980. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., & Bebbington, P. E. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31(2), 189195. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003312CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
González, L., Estarlich, M., Murcia, M., Barreto-Zarza, F., Santa-Marina, L., Simó, S., et al. (2023). Poverty, social exclusion, and mental health: The role of the family context in children aged 7–11 years INMA mother-and-child cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(2), 235248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01848-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guloksuz, S., Pries, L., Delespaul, P., Kenis, G., Luykx, J. J., Lin, B. D., et al. (2019). Examining the independent and joint effects of molecular genetic liability and environmental exposures in schizophrenia: Results from the EUGEI study. World Psychiatry, 18(2), 173182. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatzimanolis, A., Stefanatou, P., Kattoulas, E., Ralli, I., Dimitrakopoulos, S., Foteli, S., et al. (2020). Familial and socioeconomic contributions to premorbid functioning in psychosis: Impact on age at onset and treatment response. European Psychiatry, 63(1), e44. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1992). Confidence interval estimation of interaction. Epidemiology (Cambridge Mass), 3(5), 452456. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001648-199209000-00012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jang, K. W., Lee, H. K., Park, B. J., Kang, H. C., Lee, S. K., Kim, C. H., et al. (2023). Social stigma and discrimination toward people with drug addiction: A National survey in Korea. Psychiatry Investigation, 20(7), 671680. https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2023.0065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeon, E.-J., Kang, S.-H., Piao, Y.-H., Kim, S.-W., Kim, J.-J., Lee, , et al. (2022). Development of the Korea-Polyenvironmental risk score for psychosis. Psychiatry Investigation, 19(3), 197206. https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2021.0328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeon, J.-R., Lee, E.-H., Lee, S.-W., Jeong, E., Kim, J.-H., Lee, D., & Jeon, H. J. (2012). The early trauma inventory self report-short form: Psychometric properties of the Korean version. Psychiatry Investigation, 9(3), 229. https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2012.9.3.229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeon, S., Bhak, Y., Choi, Y., Jeon, Y., Kim, S., Jang, J., et al. (2020). Korean Genome Project: 1094 Korean personal genomes with clinical information. Science Advances, 6(22). https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aaz7835CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. C., Colbert, S. M. C., Jeffries, P. W., Tillman, R., Bigdeli, T. B., Karcher, N. R., et al. (2023). Associations between cannabis use, polygenic liability for schizophrenia, and cannabis-related experiences in a sample of cannabis users. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(3), 778787. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2), 261276. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/13.2.261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2010). Interpretation of interactions: guide for the perplexed. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science, 197(3), 170171. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.110.081331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. C. (2014). Gene × environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: The problem and the (simple) solution. Biological Psychiatry, 75(1), 1824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knol, M. J., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2012). Recommendations for presenting analyses of effect modification and interaction. International Journal of Epidemiology, 41(2), 514520. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyr218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraft, P., Yen, Y.-C., Stram, D. O., Morrison, J., & Gauderman, W. J. (2007). Exploiting gene-environment interaction to detect genetic associations. Human Heredity, 63(2), 111119. https://doi.org/10.1159/000099183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, M., Chen, C.-Y., Li, Z., Martin, A. R., Bryois, J., Ma, X., et al. (2019). Comparative genetic architectures of schizophrenia in East Asian and European populations. Nature Genetics, 51(12), 16701678. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0512-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. H., Wray, N. R., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2011). Estimating missing heritability for disease from genome-wide association studies. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 88(3), 294305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, W.-Y., Huang, C.-C., Liu, Y.-L., Tsai, S.-J., & Kuo, P.-H. (2019). Polygenic approaches to detect gene–environment interactions when external information is unavailable. Briefings in Bioinformatics, 20(6), 22362252. https://doi.org/10.1093/bib/bby086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, J., van Grieken, A., Yang-Huang, J., van den Toren, S. J., & Raat, H. (2022). Psychosocial health disparities in early childhood: Socioeconomic status and parent migration background. SSM – Population Health, 19, 101137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2022.101137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mas-Bermejo, P., Papiol, S., Via, M., Rovira, P., Torrecilla, P., Kwapil, T. R., et al. (2023). Schizophrenia polygenic risk score in psychosis proneness. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 273(8), 16651675. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-023-01633-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mas, S., Boloc, D., Rodríguez, N., Mezquida, G., Amoretti, S., Cuesta, M. J., et al. (2020). Examining gene–environment interactions using aggregate scores in a first-episode psychosis cohort. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(4), 10191025. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maxwell, J. M., Coleman, J. R. I., Breen, G., & Vassos, E. (2021). Association between genetic risk for psychiatric disorders and the probability of living in urban settings. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(12), 1355. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.2983CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mistry, S., Harrison, J. R., Smith, D. J., Escott-Price, V., & Zammit, S. (2018). The use of polygenic risk scores to identify phenotypes associated with genetic risk of schizophrenia: Systematic review. Schizophrenia Research, 197, 28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.10.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., Mondelli, V., Stilo, S. A., Trotta, A., Sideli, L., Ajnakina, O., et al. (2020). The influence of risk factors on the onset and outcome of psychosis: What we learned from the GAP study. Schizophrenia Research, 225, 6368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., Reveley, A. M., & McGuffin, P. (1986). Genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9(1), 316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliver, D., Radua, J., Reichenberg, A., Uher, R., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2019). Psychosis polyrisk score (PPS) for the detection of individuals at-risk and the prediction of their outcomes. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10(174), 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliver, D., Spada, G., Englund, A., Chesney, E., Radua, J., Reichenberg, A., et al. (2020). Real-world digital implementation of the psychosis polyrisk score (PPS): A pilot feasibility study. Schizophrenia Research, 226, 176183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.04.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padmanabhan, J. L., Shah, J. L., Tandon, N., & Keshavan, M. S. (2017). The “polyenviromic risk score”: Aggregating environmental risk factors predicts conversion to psychosis in familial high-risk subjects. Schizophrenia Research, 181, 1722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.10.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paksarian, D., Trabjerg, B. B., Merikangas, K. R., Mors, O., Børglum, A. D., Hougaard, D. M., et al. (2018). The role of genetic liability in the association of urbanicity at birth and during upbringing with schizophrenia in Denmark. Psychological Medicine, 48(2), 305314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001696CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, K., Park, J., Kwon, Y. D., Kang, Y., & Noh, J. W. (2016). Public satisfaction with the healthcare system performance in South Korea: Universal healthcare system. Health Policy (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 120(6), 621629. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthpol.2016.01.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasman, J. A., Verweij, K. J. H., Gerring, Z., Stringer, S., Sanchez-Roige, S., Treur, J. L., et al. (2018). GWAS of lifetime cannabis use reveals new risk loci, genetic overlap with psychiatric traits, and a causal effect of schizophrenia liability. Nature Neuroscience, 21(9), 11611170. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0206-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peyrot, W. J., Milaneschi, Y., Abdellaoui, A., Sullivan, P. F., Hottenga, J. J., Peyrot, W. J., et al. (2014). Effect of polygenic risk scores on depression in childhood trauma. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 205(2), 113119. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.143081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pignon, B., Peyre, H., Ayrolles, A., Kirkbride, J. B., Jamain, S., Ferchiou, A., et al. (2022). Genetic and psychosocial stressors have independent effects on the level of subclinical psychosis: Findings from the multinational EU-GEI study. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 31, e68. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796022000464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plana-Ripoll, O., Pedersen, C. B., & McGrath, J. J. (2018). Urbanicity and risk of schizophrenia-new studies and old hypotheses. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(7), 687688. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0551CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pries, L. K., Lage-Castellanos, A., Delespaul, P., Kenis, G., Luykx, J. J., Lin, B. D., et al. (2019). Estimating exposome score for schizophrenia using predictive modeling approach in two independent samples: The results from the EUGEI study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(5), 960965. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbz054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pries, L.-K., Klingenberg, B., Menne-Lothmann, C., Decoster, J., van Winkel, R., Collip, D., et al. (2020a). Interaction between polygenic liability for schizophrenia and childhood adversity influences daily-life emotional dysregulation and psychosis proneness. Biological Psychiatry, 87(9), S1S2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.02.033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pries, L.-K., Klingenberg, B., Menne-Lothmann, C., Decoster, J., van Winkel, R., Collip, D., et al. (2020b). Polygenic liability for schizophrenia and childhood adversity influences daily-life emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 141(5), 465475. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pries, L.-K., Dal Ferro, G. A., van Os, J., Delespaul, P., Kenis, G., Lin, B. D., et al. (2020). Examining the independent and joint effects of genomic and exposomic liabilities for schizophrenia across the psychosis spectrum. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, e182. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796020000943CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pries, L. K., van Os, J., Ten Have, M., de Graaf, R., van Dorsselaer, S., Bak, M., et al. (2020). Association of recent stressful life events with mental and physical health in the context of genomic and exposomic liability for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(12), 12961304. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Privé, F., Arbel, J., & Vilhjálmsson, B. J. (2021). LDpred2: Better, faster, stronger. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England), 36(22–23), 54245431. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btaa1029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ripke, S., Neale, B. M., Corvin, A., Walters, J. T. R., Farh, K. H., Holmans, , et al. (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature, 511(7510), 421427. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13595.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, V., Alameda, L., Aas, M., Gayer-Anderson, C., Trotta, G., Spinazzola, E., et al. (2024). Polygenic and polyenvironment interplay in schizophrenia-spectrum disorder and affective psychosis; the EUGEI First Episode Study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, sbae207. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbae207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A., & Pain, O. (2018). A systematic review of genome-wide research on psychotic experiences and negative symptom traits: New revelations and implications for psychiatry. Human Molecular Genetics, 27(R2), R136R152. https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddy157Google ScholarPubMed
Rothman, K. J. (1976). The estimation of synergy or antagonism. American Journal of Epidemiology, 103(5), 506511. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saarinen, A., Hietala, J., Lyytikäinen, L.-P., Hamal Mishra, B., Sormunen, E., Lavonius, V., et al. (2024). Polygenic risk for schizophrenia predicting social trajectories in a general population sample. Psychological Medicine, 54(8), 15891597. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172300346XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sallis, H. M., Croft, J., Havdahl, A., Jones, H. J., Dunn, E. C., Davey Smith, G., et al. (2021). Genetic liability to schizophrenia is associated with exposure to traumatic events in childhood. Psychological Medicine, 51(11), 18141821. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulsinger, F., Parnas, J., Mednick, S., Teasdale, T. W., & Schulsinger, H. (1987). Heredity-environment interaction and schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 21(4), 431436. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(87)90090-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segura, A. G., Mezquida, G., Martínez-Pinteño, A., Gassó, P., Rodriguez, N., Moreno-Izco, L., et al. (2023). Link between cognitive polygenic risk scores and clinical progression after a first-psychotic episode. Psychological Medicine, 53(10), 46344647. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722001544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smigielski, L., Papiol, S., Theodoridou, A., Heekeren, K., Gerstenberg, M., Wotruba, D., et al. (2021). Polygenic risk scores across the extended psychosis spectrum. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 600. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01720-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, B., Fowler, D. G., Freeman, D., Bebbington, P., Bashforth, H., Garety, P., et al. (2006). Emotion and psychosis: Links between depression, self-esteem, negative schematic beliefs and delusions and hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research, 86(1–3), 181188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soga, T., Teo, C. H., & Parhar, I. (2021). Genetic and epigenetic consequence of early-life social stress on depression: Role of serotonin-associated genes. Frontiers in Genetics, 11, 601868, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fgene.2020.601868CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefanis, N. C., Henquet, C., Avramopoulos, D., Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Myin-germeys, I., et al. (2007). COMT Val 158 met moderation of stress-induced psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 37(11), 16511656. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707001080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strahilevitz, M. (1974). Possible interaction of environmental and biological factors in the etiology of schizophrenia: Review and integration. The Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal/La Revue de l’Association Des Psychiatres Du Canada, 19(2), 207217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, P. F., Kendler, K. S., & Neale, M. C. (2003). Schizophrenia as a complex trait. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(12), 1187. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, D. (2010). Gene–environment-wide association studies: Emerging approaches. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11(4), 259272. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trotta, A., Iyegbe, C., Di Forti, M., Sham, P. C., Campbell, D. D., Cherny, S. S., et al. (2016). Interplay between schizophrenia polygenic risk score and childhood adversity in first-presentation psychotic disorder: A pilot study. PLOS ONE, 11(9), e0163319. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, R., & McGuffin, P. (2008). The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: Review and methodological analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(2), 131146. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4002067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vassos, E., Sham, P., Kempton, M., Trotta, A., Stilo, S. A., Gayer-Anderson, C.,et al. (2020). The Maudsley environmental risk score for psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 50(13), 22132220. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vilhjálmsson, B. J., Yang, J., Finucane, H. K., Gusev, A., Lindström, S., Ripke, S., et al. (2015). Modeling linkage disequilibrium increases accuracy of polygenic risk scores. American Journal of Human Genetics, 97(4), 576592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2015.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolway, G. E., Smart, S. E., Lynham, A. J., Lloyd, J. L., Owen, M. J., Jones, I. R., et al. (2022). Schizophrenia polygenic risk and experiences of childhood adversity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(5), 967980. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yao, K., van der Veen, T., Thygesen, J., Bass, N., & McQuillin, A. (2023). Multiple psychiatric polygenic risk scores predict associations between childhood adversity and bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 341, 137146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.08.116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yi, J. S., Ahn, Y. M., Shin, H. K., An, S. K., Joo, Y. H., Kim, S. H., et al. (2001). Reliability and VALIDITY of the Korean version of the positive and negative syndrome scale. Journal of Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 40(6), 10901105.Google Scholar
Yoo, W. S., Kim, S. Y., Noh, S. J., Oh, S. K., Kim, H. C., Namkoong, K., et al. (2006). Validity of Korean version of the mini-international neuropsychiatric interview. Anxiety and Mood, 2(1), 5055.Google Scholar
Zhao, D., & Thompson, D. M. (2023). Choosing statistical models to assess Biological interaction as a departure from additivity of effects. Epidemiology, Biostatistics, and Public Health, 18(1), 9198. https://doi.org/10.54103/2282-0930/20180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants

Figure 1

Table 2. AUC and liability R2 of PRS-SZ versus PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on case–control status

Figure 2

Figure 1. (a) Additive effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I, (b) additive effects of PRS-SZ and pSES, (c) additive effects of PRS-SZ and CA, and (d) additive effects of PRS-SZ and Recent life events. Note: CA, childhood adversity; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; PRS-SZ, Polygenic Risk Score-Schizophrenia; pSES, parental socioeconomic status; RERI, relative excess risk due to interaction.

Figure 3

Table 3. Main and interaction effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on case–control status

Figure 4

Figure 2. Regression results of subgroups of the K-PERS-I /ETI-SR and PRS-SZ were divided with a 75% cutoff of the control group. Note: ETI-SR, Early Trauma Inventory Self Report; K-PERS-I, Korea Polyenvironmental Risk Score-I; N, number; OR, odd ratio; PRS-SZ, Polygenic Risk Score-Schizophrenia.

Figure 5

Table 4. Main and interaction effects of PRS-SZ and K-PERS-I/ETI-SR on the PANSS or BCSS in patients with SSDs

Supplementary material: File

Rami et al. supplementary material

Rami et al. supplementary material
Download Rami et al. supplementary material(File)
File 848.4 KB