The coexistence of food insecurity and obesity has been identified in diverse ethnic minorities and low-income settings( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 – Reference Kac, Velasquez-Melendez and Schluessel 6 ). African-American populations living in urban areas in the USA have high rates of obesity (45·0 %) and related chronic diseases( Reference Wang and Beydoun 7 ). Compared with the general population, African Americans also have a higher prevalence of food insecurity( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Coleman-Jensen, Nord and Singh 8 , Reference Bonanno and Li 9 ), with 24·6 % of African-American households experiencing food insecurity in 2012 compared with 14·5 % of US households overall( Reference Coleman-Jensen, Nord and Singh 8 ). Poverty exacerbates these associations( Reference Coleman-Jensen, Nord and Singh 8 ).
A possible explanation for the association between food insecurity and obesity is that families in limited-resource conditions may compromise dietary quality in exchange for energy( Reference Crawford and Webb 2 , Reference Franklin, Jones and Love 10 – Reference Leung, Epel and Ritchie 12 ). Budgetary constraints may result in increased purchasing of low-cost, non-nutrient-rich and energy-dense foods, and decreased consumption of healthy foods such as fruit and vegetables( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 – Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 , Reference Zachary, Palmer and Beckham 11 , Reference Leung, Epel and Ritchie 12 ). Likewise, federal food assistance programmes that target hunger and food insecurity seem to play a key role in this phenomenon( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Larson and Story 3 , Reference Bonanno and Li 9 , Reference Kharmats, Jones-Smith and Cheah 13 , Reference Mayer, Hillier and Bachhuber 14 ). Food insecurity is defined as a household-level economic and social condition of limited or uncertain access to adequate food, whereas hunger is an individual-level physiological condition caused by a lack of food( 15 ). Participation in the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP) has been directly associated with obesity in women( Reference Larson and Story 3 , Reference Meyerhoefer and Yang 16 ), and with unhealthier food patterns for African-American populations, especially at the beginning and end of the monthly benefit cycle( Reference Leung, Epel and Ritchie 12 , Reference Kharmats, Jones-Smith and Cheah 13 , Reference Leung and Villamor 17 ).
Differential food access is another possible explanation for the link between food insecurity and less-healthy food choices among ethnic minority populations( Reference Bonanno and Li 9 , Reference Zachary, Palmer and Beckham 11 , Reference Mayer, Hillier and Bachhuber 14 , Reference Powell, Slater and Mirtcheva 18 – Reference Carter, Dubois and Tremblay 20 ). Urban neighbourhoods that are both low-income and predominantly African-American tend to have limited access to healthy food sources (e.g. supermarkets)( Reference Powell, Slater and Mirtcheva 18 , Reference D’Angelo, Suratkar and Song 21 ), as well as greater access to corner stores and other types of convenience store that are associated with higher obesity and chronic disease rates( Reference Gittelsohn, Steeves and Mui 19 , Reference D’Angelo, Suratkar and Song 21 , Reference Lee, Rowan and Powell 22 ).
The majority of studies on the relationship between food security and obesity have focused on socio-economic factors( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Larson and Story 3 , Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 , Reference Franklin, Jones and Love 10 , Reference Leung and Villamor 17 , Reference Leung, Williams and Villamor 23 , Reference Ramsey, Giskes and Turrell 24 ) and energy and nutrient intakes( Reference Leung, Epel and Ritchie 12 , Reference Kharmats, Jones-Smith and Cheah 13 , Reference Ramsey, Giskes and Turrell 24 , Reference Drewnowski and Specter 25 ), and little is known about other factors influencing food decisions made by minority populations( Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 , Reference Suratkar, Gittelsohn and Song 26 ). In Baltimore City, African-American food-insecure households have shown greater healthy eating self-efficacy and healthier food acquisition compared with food-secure households, even though they were less likely to adopt healthy cooking methods( Reference Suratkar, Gittelsohn and Song 26 ). The literature is conflicting regarding the relationship between food-related psychological factors( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Suratkar, Gittelsohn and Song 26 , Reference Surkan, Coutinho and Christiansen 27 ), access to food in the context of food insecurity( Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 , Reference Zachary, Palmer and Beckham 11 , Reference Carter, Dubois and Tremblay 20 ) and food-related behaviours, such as the allocation of economic resources( Reference Crawford and Webb 2 , Reference Dennisuk, Coutinho and Suratkar 28 ). Additionally, to date few studies have investigated how beliefs and attitudes towards health and body weight influence food security and nutritional status( Reference Crawford and Webb 2 , Reference Larson and Story 3 ).
In the current study, we conjectured that adult caregivers who reported higher levels of food insecurity will show poorer household food acquisition patterns, be less likely to buy foods at grocery stores and fresh produce markets, be more likely to visit local convenience stores, be less likely to show positive psychosocial factors (such as higher knowledge, intentions and self-efficacy) for making healthier food choices and performing healthful cooking practices, and show more reserved attitudes towards healthier eating and body-weight satisfaction for themselves and their children. We address the following questions:
-
1. How do child and adult overweight and obesity differ by food security status among our sample?
-
2. What are the relationships of adult food-related attitudes, psychosocial factors and behaviours with household food insecurity and child and adult excess body weight?
Methods
Study setting and design
A cross-sectional survey was administered to participants in the baseline phase of B’More Healthy Communities for Kids (BHCK), an ongoing 5-year obesity prevention trial that aims to increase the demand for and access to healthy and affordable foods through integrated interventions at the individual, family, youth leader, recreation centre, food store, carry-out restaurant, wholesaler and policy levels( Reference Gittelsohn, Steeves and Mui 19 ).
The study sample came from fourteen low-income and predominantly African-American neighbourhoods in Baltimore City that were considered food deserts( Reference Haering and Franco 29 ), i.e. a geographic area where the distance to a supermarket is more than 0·40 km (0·25 mile); the median household income is at or below 185 % of the federal poverty level; over 40 % of households have no vehicle available; and the average Healthy Food Availability Index score for supermarkets, convenience and corner stores is lower than that of the food sources located in areas where the majority of the population is white. Fourteen recreation centres were selected as central points from which to define the geographic zones.
Sampling
Households were recruited from within low-income African-American neighbourhoods as part of baseline data collection for the parent study. To be eligible the family needed to: (i) reside within a radius of 2·4 km (1·5 miles) from a recreation centre; (ii) have at least one child aged 10–14 years old; (iii) have no intention of moving within 2 years; and (iv) have a caregiver who was the primary food shopper for the household willing to participate. Among households recruited and screened as eligible, twenty-four were selected to be interviewed in each recreation zone (a description of the randomization method can be found elsewhere( Reference Gittelsohn, Steeves and Mui 19 )). If a selected household was unable to complete the interview, then the next eligible one was chosen from a recruitment list of 2250 eligible households.
We obtained an average of twenty-one household interviews per recreation zone (n 298). Thirteen per cent of households approached agreed to be part of the study.
The study was approved by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health Institutional Review Board. Families signed the written informed consent and received $US 50 (total) in gift cards for participation. Parents provided consent for their child to participate and children provided written assent.
Measures
The 176-item Adult Impact Questionnaire (AIQ) included items to measure demographics and household socio-economic information (sex, education, marital status, employment status, household income and housing arrangement), food source purchasing, acquisition of non-prepared foods for the household, food preparation methods, psychosocial factors, household participation in food assistance programmes, anthropometry and food security.
The data were collected between June 2013 and June 2014, through interviews with the adult caregiver primarily responsible for household food purchasing and preparation. Interviews lasted on average 90 min. Data collectors were public health students and staff who underwent extensive training and received certification on the study instruments. Data were checked for errors and missing information by the interviewer and a second party following each interview, after which they were entered and cleaned by a third party.
Food source destinations
We assessed the most common food sources used in the past 30 d (‘How many times did you purchase or get foods from the following locations?’). Food sources were categorized as: farmers’ market in the city; local or urban farm stand; mobile produce cart; street food vendor; public market; the Virtual Supermarket program; local corner store; supermarket or grocery store; wholesale food store; local carry-out; fast-food chain restaurant; specialty store (i.e. bakery, African store, coffee shop); sit-down restaurant, bar or pub; convenience store; food pantry; church or community centre; family and/or friends.
Food acquisition patterns
Food acquisition, preparation, health beliefs and attitudes, and psychosocial factor scales were developed and assessed for internal consistency reliability using Cronbach’s α.
Household food acquisition patterns were determined based on how often the household acquired selected foods over the past 30 d (‘How many times did you get these foods?’). It is important to note that food acquisition encompasses food that was purchased outright, purchased with food safety-net programme benefits (SNAP and Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC)) and food that was obtained for free. A food list with fifty-four items was designed on the basis of: (i) previous research done in this community( Reference Suratkar, Gittelsohn and Song 26 , Reference Surkan, Coutinho and Christiansen 27 ); (ii) formative research with the population of interest( Reference Gittelsohn, Steeves and Mui 19 ); and (iii) to reflect items that would be promoted during the BHCK intervention. The healthier food acquisition score was the sum of the frequency of obtaining twenty-seven healthy foods for each respondent (n 296). Additive scores ranged from 2 to 315 with a mean of 39·31 (sd=30·54, α=0·79). Similarly, the less-healthy food acquisition score was a sum of the frequency of acquiring twenty-seven less-healthy foods during the same recall period (n 294). Scores ranged from 6 to 311 with a mean of 54·47 (sd=39·77, α=0·78). High scores indicate that foods were obtained more frequently. The healthier and less-healthy food items are presented in the online supplementary material (Supplemental Box 1).
Healthy food preparation
We asked participants to rank the top three most common cooking methods used when they prepared meats (e.g. fish, chicken, turkey, pork, beef, including bacon and ground meat), eggs, greens (except lettuce) and potatoes. Cooking methods were assigned scores as follows: deep fry, or pan fry with butter or margarine (−2); pan fry with oil, or not prepared at home (−1); pan fry, drained (0); broiled, boiled, grilled, steamed, microwaved, baked without added fat, pan fry without oil, pan fry with water, raw, use of cooking spray, pan fry, drained and rinsed with hot water (+2). The healthy food preparation score was calculated by the weighted mean score for each food, taking into account the following proportion: 60 % (first method), 30 % (second method) and 10 % (third method). Finally, the scores for all eight foods were calculated and totalled to obtain an overall score for each household (n 290). Total healthiness of food preparation scores ranged from −2·67 to +4·93 with a mean of 1·38 (sd=1·45, α=0·54).
Healthy food beliefs and attitudes
Using a 5-item Likert scale, we developed four subscales to represent different dimensions about beliefs and attitudes towards healthy food: Affordability (three items); Convenience (three items); Importance (three items); and Taste (one item). We also developed a Body Image and Health subscale (three items). The subscale items are presented in the online supplementary material (Supplemental Box 2). The level of agreement or disagreement with each of the six positive statements was coded as: strongly disagree (1), disagree (2), undecided (3), agree (4) or strongly agree (5); and inversely coded for the remaining seven negative statements. Total scores ranged from 3 to 15 with a mean of 9·67 (sd=2·92, α=0·76) for Affordability; from 3 to 15 with a mean of 10·08 (sd=2·00, α=0·42) for Convenience; from 4 to 15 with a mean of 12·44 (sd=1·88, α=0·59) for Importance; and from 1 to 5 with a mean of 3·93 (sd=0·94) for Taste. Higher beliefs and attitudes scores on healthy foods reflect that the respondent showed a positive predisposition to behave consistently favourably towards healthy food choices because he/she believes that they are affordable, convenient, important to family health and palatable. Total scores ranged from 3 to 15 with a mean of 8·87 (sd=2·78, α=0·61) for the Body Image and Health subscale. Higher belief and attitude scores on body image reflected caregiver body-weight satisfaction for themselves and their children.
Food-related psychosocial factors
Psychosocial constructs were based on Social Cognitive Theory (online supplementary material, Supplemental Box 3)( Reference McAlister, Perr and Parcel 30 ). Eleven questions assessed the level of caregiver food and nutrition knowledge related to food purchasing, food preparation and understanding of information from nutrition facts labels. Food knowledge scores were calculated for each household by summing the number of correct responses to questions (n 297). The range for these scores was 2 to 11 with a mean of 7·08 (sd=1·76, α=0·42).
Ten questions about intentions for healthy eating focused on how respondents intend to purchase and prepare food for themselves and their children. The responses were scored by assigning 2 points to the healthiest option, 1 point to a less healthy choice, and 0 to the least healthy choice. Scores ranged from 2 to 20 with a mean of 11·31 (sd=4·16, α=0·72; n 293). Higher healthy eating intention scores indicated that the respondent had a positive inclination towards healthy eating.
Ten self-efficacy questions elicited the respondent’s level of confidence in performing daily food-related activities, such as making healthier food choices, using nutrition facts labels and performing healthful cooking practices. The responses were based on the options: would be very easy, somewhat difficult, very difficult or impossible to do regularly, and responses were scored from 3 to 0, respectively. Total scores ranged from 11 to 30 with a mean of 24·82 (sd=3·82, α=0·68; n 298).
We had missing data values for some of the scales (healthier food acquisition, n 2; less-healthy acquisition, n 4; food preparing, n 8; knowledge, n 1; intention, n 5). We handled the missing data by dropping those participants from the analyses.
Anthropometric measures
We directly measured weights and heights of 298 adults/caregivers and 283 eligible children in the same household, using a Seca 213 Portable Measuring Rod stadiometer and a Tanita BF697W Duo Scale. Respondents were weighed and measured barefoot, in their clothes, removing only heavy outerwear. BMI was obtained by calculating weight/height2 (kg/m2). For adults, overweight was defined as BMI between 25·00 and 29·99 kg/m2 and obesity as BMI greater than or equal to 30·00 kg/m2, according to the WHO recommendations( 31 ). BMI for children up to 14 years old was assessed using BMI-for-age, according to the WHO Child Growth Standards( 32 ). Overweight was defined as a BMI-for-age Z-score ≥1 and <2 and obesity as a BMI-for-age Z-score ≥2 for children of the same age and sex.
Household food security status
Food security was assessed using the US Department of Agriculture’s eighteen-item Household Food Security Scale for households with children under 18 years old( Reference Coleman-Jensen, Nord and Singh 8 , Reference Bickel, Nord and Price 33 ). Household food security was determined by the number of food-insecure conditions and reported behaviours reported in the past 12 months. The questions measured a cluster of situations related to the experience of food availability and the physiological state of hunger; perceptions that the food eaten by adults or children was inadequate in quality; instances of reduced food intake or consequences of reduced intake; and instances of reduced food intake or its consequences for children. Responses to each question were coded as either affirmative (yes, sometimes true, often true, almost every month, some months but not every month) or negative (no, never true, only one or two months). We coded food security according to the methods of Bickel et al.( Reference Bickel, Nord and Price 33 ). Food security status was coded using recommended scale values( Reference Bickel, Nord and Price 33 ). For the analysis, food-insecure households with moderate and severe hunger were combined.
Data analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for demographic characteristics, attitudinal, psychosocial and behavioural variables, food security and excess body weight status. One-way ANOVA, χ 2 tests (two-sided) and Fisher’s exact tests were used to compare the groups by food security status. For all analyses P≤0·05 was the level of significance.
We performed a principal component analysis to derive patterns of food source use by adults, reducing the list of seventeen types of food source destination to a few key patterns. For better interpretation of the factors obtained (patterns), these were submitted to orthogonal rotation using the Varimax method. We used eigenvalues >1·0 to establish the number of factors to be retained and assessments of proportion of variance accounted for by these factors. In interpreting the rotated factor pattern, a selected food source was considered to load on a given factor if the absolute factor loading was >0·6( Reference Hair, Black and Babin 34 ). The retained set of factor scores were saved as continuous variables, and mean scores were calculated by food security status.
We used logistic regression analysis to assess the association between attitudinal, psychosocial and behavioural variables (which were each dichotomized with a median-split to compare higher v. lower levels) with two dichotomous outcomes at the household level: (i) food insecurity (food insecure without hunger, food insecure with moderate hunger and food insecure with severe hunger combined, compared with those who were food secure); and (ii) excess body weight (adult and child overweight and/or obese in the same household). On the basis of our conceptual framework (Fig. 1) and hypothesis, we first examined the unadjusted models for the following independent variables: patterns of food source destinations (rotated factors generated by principal component analysis); psychosocial scores (knowledge, intentions and self-efficacy); attitudinal scores (affordability, convenience, importance, taste and body image); healthier and less-healthy food acquisition scores; and food preparation score. Finally, we examined all models adjusted for the following sociodemographic covariates: caregiver age (continuous); sex; education (high school completed or high school not completed); marital status (indicator variables: never married; divorced/separated or widowed; married); employment status (indicator variables: unemployed or looking for work; student or seasonal work; employed full- or part-time); housing arrangement (indicator variables: transitional housing/shelter/group house/other; live with family who own/rent the property; rent or own property); number of people and children living in the household; and food assistance benefits received in the past 12 months (SNAP and WIC). Alpha was set at P<0·05. All analyses were performed using the statistical software package SPSS version 16·0.
Our primary models for excess body weight were defined by both the child and the adult caregiver in the same household being overweight or obese. In sensitivity analyses, we assessed whether results would be substantively different had we analysed adult and child overweight/obesity in separate models.
Results
Description of the study sample
We found that 41·6 % of households had some level of food insecurity, with 12·4 % reporting having experienced moderate or severe hunger in the previous year (Table 1). The majority of adult respondents were female who were never married and had completed at least 12 years of formal education.
BHCK, B’More Healthy Communities for Kids; SNAP, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program; WIC, Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children.
a,b,c Values within a row with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P<0·05).
* One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test to compare the groups.
† Pearson χ 2 test to compare the groups (two-sided).
‡ Fisher’s exact test to compare the groups (two-sided).
§ From US Census Bureau( 40 ).
Sociodemographic associations with food security
Food-insecure households with hunger were significantly less likely to have full-time or part-time employment (35·1 %) compared with food-insecure households without hunger (53·3 %) and food-secure households (56·3 %). Seasonal jobs and student status were more prevalent among both food-secure (10·9 %) and food-insecure households with hunger (16·2 %) compared with those that were food secure without hunger. We also observed a significantly higher number of family members (mean=5·1, sd=1·9) and children (mean=3·2, sd=1·6) among food-insecure households without hunger compared with food secure (mean=4·5, sd=1·5; mean=2·6, sd=1·4, respectively) and food insecure with hunger (mean=4·2, sd=1·6; mean=2·4, sd=1·5, respectively). Most of the food-insecure households with hunger had annual household income less than $US 20 000 (62·9 %), and this proportion was significantly lower for food-secure households (44·2 %). A high proportion of food-secure (57·1 %) and food-insecure households (without hunger, 67·5 %; with hunger, 60·0 %) showed an income-to-poverty ratio under 1·0, indicating that their average income for respective family size is below the official definition of poverty.
Excess body weight and food security status
We found marginally higher rates of adult and child overweight in our sample among low-income food-secure African-American households, but higher proportions of adult and child obesity for those with food insecurity with hunger, even though differences were not statistically significant across the groups (Table 2). The proportion of overweight and obesity among individuals living in food-insecure households with hunger was 89·2 % for adults and 40·5 % for children.
* No significant differences were found between groups (P<0·05).
† Pearson χ2 test to compare the groups (two-sided),
‡ BMI=25·00–29·99 kg/m2 (WHO( 31 )).
§ BMI≥30·00 kg/m2 (WHO( 31 )).
|| BMI-for-age Z-score ≥1 and <2 for children of the same age and sex (WHO Child Growth Standards( 32 )).
¶ BMI-for-age Z-score ≥2 for children of the same age and sex (WHO Child Growth Standards( 32 )).
** Adult and child overweight and/or obese in the same household.
Food-related behaviours and psychosocial factors associated with food security status
Table 3 shows the seven patterns of food source destinations used to acquire food in the past month. Factor 1 was labelled ‘Convenience store shopping’ and reflects a purchasing pattern based on the habit of getting foods at corner stores and prepared foods from restaurants, such as carry-outs and fast-food restaurants. Factor 2, ‘Grocery store shopping’, indicates acquiring foods at supermarkets, grocery and wholesale food stores. The use of street food vendors and the support from family members and friends to acquire foods for the household are exhibited by Factor 3, named ‘Social support and local shopping’. Factor 4, ‘Eating out and online grocery shopping’, corresponds to the use of sit-down restaurants and the Virtual Supermarket program (a city-run free grocery-delivery programme for food desserts) to buy food. Acquiring food at public markets and specialty stores comprises Factor 5, entitled ‘Covered market shopping’. The habit of buying fresh produce at a local or urban farm stand contributes inversely (factor loading=−0·74) to the Factor 6 (‘Farmers’ market’ shopping’), but this healthy habit also shows a positive relationship to Factor 7 (‘Mobile produce market shopping’) reflecting frequent use of arabber or mobile produce carts (factor loading=0·63). No significant differences of means between the food security groups were found by food source use pattern.
* Factors were extracted by principal component analysis adopting the eigenvalues >1·0 criterion. Patterns extracted were subjected to Varimax rotation with Kaiser normalization. Food sources with absolute factor loadings >0·6 (given in parentheses) were maintained in the pattern.
† Percentage of total variance explained by each factor retained. Cumulative explained variance of the seven obtained factors: 58·61 %.
‡ No significant differences were found between groups (P<0·05).
§ One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test to compare the groups.
No associations between participation in food assistance programmes and food-related psychosocial factors and behaviours were found (results not shown).
In unadjusted and adjusted models, no significant differences were observed in food source usage pattern, frequency of food acquisition and healthiness of food preparation method by food security groups (Tables 3 and 5).
Regarding psychosocial factors, we did not find significant differences between food-secure and food-insecure respondents in unadjusted or adjusted models (Tables 4 and 5).
a,b,c Mean values within a row with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·05).
* One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test to compare the food security groups.
SNAP, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program; WIC, Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children.
* Significant OR and 95 % CI (P<0·05).
† Three categories of food insecurity: without hunger; with moderate hunger; and with severe hunger.
‡ Adult and child overweight and/or obese in the same household.
§ Adjusted for caregiver age, caregiver education, caregiver sex, marital status, employment status, participation in food assistance programs (SNAP and WIC), housing arrangement, number of people, number of children and income-to-poverty ratio.
|| Higher score: >50th percentile (v. ≤50th percentile).
Health beliefs and attitudes associated with food security status
In unadjusted models, we found that food-secure respondents showed a higher level of agreement with healthy food as being affordable, compared with both food insecurity groups. Food-insecure respondents without hunger tended to consider healthy foods less accessible and less convenient to buy in the neighbourhood and to prepare at home (Table 4). Each of these results remained significant in the models adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics (Table 5), with more food-secure households perceiving healthy food as being affordable and convenient, associated with 0·18 (95 % CI 0·09, 0·39) and 0·49 (95 % CI 0·24, 0·95) odds of household food insecurity, respectively.
Caregiver body-weight satisfaction score was lower in food insecurity without hunger, compared with food security (Table 4). However, upon adjustment for covariates, this relationship was no longer statistically significant.
Factors associated with excess body weight
In the adjusted models (Table 5), a higher perception of healthy food as being convenient was associated with a 57 % decrease in odds of caregiver and child excess body weight. We also found that higher satisfaction scores on beliefs about body image were associated with a 47 % decrease in odds of caregiver overweight or obesity and child overweight or obesity (adjusted model). We did not find significant associations between food-related behaviours, psychosocial factors (as exposures) and household food insecurity and excess body weight (as outcomes).
The sensitivity models that examined child and adult overweight/obesity in separate models were substantively similar with the exceptions that in models examining only adult overweight/obesity as the outcome, we find that the factor variable characterized by shopping frequently at a covered market was associated with higher odds of overweight/obesity (OR=2·51; 95 % CI 1·07, 5·91) and higher nutrition knowledge was marginally statistically significantly associated with lower odds of overweight/obesity (P=0·097; results not shown).
Discussion
The present study is the first to examine the associations between food security status, excess body weight and consumer food-related behaviours and perceptions among low-income African-American families with children living in Baltimore City. We found very high rates of adult and child overweight and/or obesity, as well as a high prevalence of families that had recently experienced food insecurity. Living in a food-secure household was associated with the perception that healthy foods are affordable and convenient. Not being an overweight/obese caregiver and not having an overweight/obese child was associated with caregiver body satisfaction and the perception that healthy foods are convenient. Nevertheless, we did not find significant differences regarding food-related behaviours and psychosocial factors across levels of food security.
Prevalence of overweight and obesity in our sample of food-insecure households with hunger was 89·2 % for adults and 45·9 % for children. There is a growing body of research corroborating the relationship between obesity and food insecurity( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Crawford and Webb 2 , Reference Larson and Story 3 , Reference Leung, Epel and Ritchie 12 , Reference Leung, Williams and Villamor 23 ), with clearer associations for women( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Larson and Story 3 , Reference Hanson, Sobal and Frongillo 35 ) but still mixed results for children( Reference Eisenmann, Gundersen and Lohman 36 ). Our findings were consistent in direction, but there was not a statistically significant relationship between food insecurity and overweight or obesity among women or children. This may be attributable either to limited variation in the socio-economic and community demographics of our sample, or potentially to the extremely high prevalence of overweight and obesity. Additionally, perceptions and attitudes towards body size may vary by social and cultural group( Reference James, Pobee and Oxidine 37 ).
Our data indicate that in our Baltimore sample, the food insecurity rate (41·6 %) is 1·7 times that of households headed by individuals who are non-Hispanic black nationwide and approximately three times the levels in the general US population( Reference Coleman-Jensen, Nord and Singh 8 ). Since all the households included were drawn from low-income neighbourhoods with high poverty rates( Reference Gittelsohn, Steeves and Mui 19 ), higher rates of food insecurity were expected. Also, this result is consistent with other studies that have found significant disparities in food insecurity among ethnic minorities( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 , Reference Mayer, Hillier and Bachhuber 14 , Reference Ramsey, Giskes and Turrell 24 ). A similar prevalence of household food insecurity has been found among American-Indian families (45 %)( Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 ).
In terms of sociodemographic factors associated with food insecurity, many of our results are in the expected direction and consistent with other studies. We found that caregivers from food-insecure households with hunger were significantly more likely to be unemployed and have lower incomes compared with those who are food secure. Also, there was a higher prevalence of food-insecure families without hunger (67·5 %) who had income below the poverty threshold, although the differences between the groups were not significant. Similarly, in a low-income African-American sample in Baltimore City, Suratkar et al. also observed significantly lower employment among food-insecure households( Reference Suratkar, Gittelsohn and Song 26 ). Unemployment or other events that stress household budgets may influence food security as income is clearly a critical economic determinant of food insecurity and hunger( Reference Dinour, Bergen and Yeh 1 , Reference Mullany, Neault and Tsingine 4 , Reference Leung and Villamor 17 , Reference Ramsey, Giskes and Turrell 24 , Reference Drewnowski and Specter 25 ).
Our study adds to the literature by examining consumer food decisions made by low-income caregivers who are at high risk for being food insecure and overweight or obese. In this context, we found that food source destination, food acquisition behaviours and food-related psychosocial factors were not associated with food security status. On the other hand, certain health beliefs and attitudes were associated with food security status and overweight/obesity status. Specifically, believing that healthy foods are affordable and convenient as well as having a positive body image were related to lower odds of food insecurity. In line with that, food-secure respondents showed higher food and nutrition knowledge and intentions for healthy eating compared with food-insecure households, although this trend was not statistically significant. Having a positive body image and believing that healthy foods are convenient to buy and prepare were also related to lower odds of overweight or obesity.
Our findings around perceived affordability are consistent with previous work that has found that perceiving healthy foods as unaffordable and being less satisfying (v. energy-dense foods) was negatively associated with the consumption of fruits and vegetables by food-insecure populations( Reference Larson and Story 3 , Reference Drewnowski and Specter 25 ). Investigating different theoretical constructs as predictors of food-related behaviours, such as those based on the Health Belief Model and Social Cognitive Theory, is useful to develop appropriate strategies focusing on consumer food choices and dietary practices to foster low-income families’ health promotion and obesity prevention, especially food programme recipients( Reference Franklin, Jones and Love 10 , Reference McAlister, Perr and Parcel 30 , Reference James, Pobee and Oxidine 37 ). Yet to our knowledge, no studies investigating associations between food assistance programme use, perceptions of food access and food-related psychosocial factors exist. Therefore, the BHCK programme has focused on working with wholesalers, local corner stores and carry-out restaurants in Baltimore City to increase access to healthy foods and promotes these foods through point-of-purchase activities such as cooking demonstrations, taste tests and educational materials aiming to improve food-related perceptions and promote food selection and preparation behaviours among low-income African-American youths and their caregivers( Reference Gittelsohn, Steeves and Mui 19 ).
Although we found extremely high excess body weight in the entire sample, we also observed higher levels of body-weight and body-image satisfaction among food-secure caregivers (for themselves and their children), compared with food insecure. It is worthwhile to note that ethnicity and culture influence body-size preference( Reference James, Pobee and Oxidine 37 , Reference Chen and Wang 38 ). Having a positive body image was associated with 45 % lower odds of being food insecure. Body-image satisfaction may mediate the relationship between food security and food-related psychosocial factors, such as intentions and self-efficacy to eat healthier foods( Reference Chen and Wang 38 ). We are unaware of any other studies that have examined body image in relation to food insecurity. Being overweight or obese was also associated with having a more negative body image in our sample. James et al. identified that African-American women are aware of the health risks of obesity and believe that culture and genetics made them more susceptible to obesity( Reference James, Pobee and Oxidine 37 ). Motivators to lose weight include having a health problem, improving one’s physical appearance and saving money on clothes( Reference James, Pobee and Oxidine 37 ).
We were expecting to find associations between food security status and food source destinations, but the lack of significant differences for preferred food stores and food acquisition patterns between the groups may be due to the similarities in the local food environment and access to food sources among low-income African Americans living in urban areas of Baltimore City( Reference D’Angelo, Suratkar and Song 21 , Reference Haering and Franco 29 , Reference Franco, Roux and Glass 39 ). Factors including fourteen types of food source destination accounted for the most variance explained, which allows us to infer that the three food source destinations not retained (farmers’ markets, convenience stores, specialty stores) were not frequently used by our sample. In this setting, the ‘Convenience store shopping’ (Factor 1) reveals that local carry-outs, chain fast-food restaurants and local corner stores are important food venues for low-income African-American adults. Corner stores are typically small stores with limited space that stock primarily high-fat, high-sugar and high-salt foods, and have been documented as the most frequently used food source by African-American youth( Reference Dennisuk, Coutinho and Suratkar 28 ) and the second most by African-American adults living in low-income neighbourhoods( Reference D’Angelo, Suratkar and Song 21 ).
The current study has some limitations. First, it uses a cross-sectional design, making it impossible to determine how food insecurity may be associated with increased weight gain over time. Second, given there are multiple scales to evaluate household food insecurity that rely on different assumptions and metrics, comparability with other studies is difficult. Third, our survey was administered to self-identified caregivers under the assumption that they purchase most of the food and cook for other family members in the household, although the extent of this practice could vary among low-income African-American families. Fourth, given we investigated food-related psychosocial factors and behaviours, performing a complementary qualitative approach could have helped to identify and contextualize these constructs. Future studies should include methods to better understand which factors influence decision making regarding household food acquisition and eating patterns (e.g. exploring affordability, accessibility and other food access dimensions) and their relationship with food insecurity and obesity. Further, food acquisition scores were based only on frequency of items acquired, but did not take into account their quantity and size. The food acquisition score also did not identify the participant’s primary food shopping/acquiring source (i.e. a participant might go to a corner store every day for small items – for example, a can of soda – but still do his/her primary shopping at a supermarket) and our measure would not be able to differentiate these behaviours. In order to establish more direct associations between household food acquisition dynamics, food security and obesity, researchers should evaluate portion size, total quantity consumed or other factors that reflect the overall dietary quality of each family member.
Conclusions
In summary, our findings suggest that obesity and food insecurity are major problems among adults living in food deserts in Baltimore City. In this context, food insecurity was associated with employment status, family size and income. Beliefs and attitudes towards healthy food as being affordable and convenient were negatively associated with household food insecurity. Overweight and obesity in the same family was associated with the caregivers’ perception that healthy foods are less convenient and with a negative body image. Given that the households interviewed exhibited a similar pattern of food source destination, food acquisition, preparation and psychosocial factors regardless of their food security status, further qualitative research should be done to contextualize behaviours, beliefs and experiences of food insecurity. Food-related consumer behaviours are key to empowering families with children to make healthier decisions and overcome constraints in the food environment regarding access to healthy foods. The BHCK, as a sequence to these baseline data, will provide an opportunity to identify strategies to increase nutrition knowledge, self-efficacy, intentions and skills for behaviour change that have the potential to address obesity and food insecurity. Additionally, the study’s findings could inform policy decisions and food-assistance programme initiatives focused on food insecurity, given that low-income and food-insecure minorities are especially vulnerable to obesity.
Acknowledgements
Acknowledgements: The author are thankful to and acknowledge the students, staff and volunteers of the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health who contributed with data collection and the BHCK families who contributed to this research. Financial support: Research reported in this publication was supported by the Global Obesity Prevention Center (GOPC) at Johns Hopkins, and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) and the Office of the Director, National Institutes of Health (OD) under award number U54HD070725. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: G.M.V. was involved in formulating the research questions, designing the study, analysing and interpreting the data. P.J.S. was involved in interpreting the data and the article revision. J.J.-S. was involved in interpreting the data and the article revision. E.A.S. was involved in interpreting the data and the article revision. E.H. was involved in interpreting the data and the article revision. A.C.B.T. was involved in interpreting the data and the article revision. A.Y.K. was involved in interpreting the data and the article revision. J.G. is the Principal Investigator and was involved in formulating the research questions, designing the study, interpreting the data and article revision. All authors were involved in writing this article. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects/patients.
Supplementary Material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980015002888