Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:33:47.089Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shared polygenic contribution between childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Marian L. Hamshere*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Evangelia Stergiakouli
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Kate Langley
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Joanna Martin
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Peter Holmans
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Lindsey Kent
Affiliation:
Bute Medical School, University of St Andrews, Fife, UK
Michael J. Owen
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Michael Gill
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Trinity Centre for Health Sciences, Dublin, Ireland
Anita Thapar
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Mick O'Donovan
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
Nick Craddock*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
*
Professor N. Craddock, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Heath Park, Cardiff CF14 4XN, UK. Email: [email protected].
Professor N. Craddock, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Heath Park, Cardiff CF14 4XN, UK. Email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Background

There is recent evidence of some degree of shared genetic susceptibility between adult schizophrenia and childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) for rare chromosomal variants.

Aims

To determine whether there is overlap between common alleles conferring risk of schizophrenia in adults with those that do so for ADHD in children.

Method

We used recently published Psychiatric Genome-wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium (PGC) adult schizophrenia data to define alleles over-represented in people with schizophrenia and tested whether those alleles were more common in 727 children with ADHD than in 2067 controls.

Results

Schizophrenia risk alleles discriminated ADHD cases from controls (P = 1.04 × 104, R2 = 0.45%); stronger discrimination was given by alleles that were risk alleles for both adult schizophrenia and adult bipolar disorder (also derived from a PGC data-set) (P = 9.98 ×10−6, R2 × 0.59%).

Conclusions

This increasing evidence for a small, but significant, shared genetic susceptibility between adult schizophrenia and childhood ADHD highlights the importance of research work across traditional diagnostic boundaries.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

Large-scale genetic association studies have identified replicable susceptibility alleles for some psychiatric phenotypes. Reference Owen, Craddock and O'Donovan1 This includes both common and rare variants. A striking finding has been that susceptibility variants identified to date often confer susceptibility to more than one of the traditional clinical phenotypes, a finding that has the potential to increase understanding of the biological relationship between these traditional diagnostic categories. Reference Owen, Craddock and O'Donovan1-Reference Craddock, Kendler, Neale, Nurnberger, Purcell and Rietschel3 The focus of this study is childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and its potential genetic relationship to adult schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. There is evidence of some degree of shared genetic susceptibility between schizophrenia and childhood ADHD for rare chromosomal variants. Reference Williams, Zaharieva, Martin, Langley, Mantripragada and Fossdal4 In the present study, we have considered another possible source of genetic overlap, namely common single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and we have extended the analysis to consider bipolar disorder as well as schizophrenia. The only prior study that has started to examine this, selected six bipolar genetic risk variants and identified no strong association with ADHD or with scores on the Mood Disorder Questionnaire. Reference Landaas, Johansson, Halmoy, Oedegaard, Fasmer and Haavik5 Here, we have used a more powerful, genome-wide approach and examined whether, when they are considered en masse using polygenic score analysis, Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 multiple schizophrenia Reference Ripke, Sanders, Kendler, Levinson, Sklar and Holmans7 and bipolar disorder Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon and Craddock8 common risk alleles contribute to ADHD risk.

Method

In general, we followed the polygenic score analysis approach described by the International Schizophrenia Consortium (ISC). Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 The basic principle of that analysis was that a set of many alleles that discriminated case status in a ‘discovery’ schizophrenia case-control sample, also significantly discriminated case status in an independent ‘target’ schizophrenia case-control sample. We used the published Psychiatric Genome-wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium (PGC) data-sets (schizophrenia and bipolar disorder) as two separate discovery sets and our ADHD data as the target set.

Sample

Discovery data

We made use of the PGC schizophrenia Reference Ripke, Sanders, Kendler, Levinson, Sklar and Holmans7 and bipolar disorder Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon and Craddock8 case-control data-sets. The schizophrenia sample comprised 9394 cases and 12 462 controls analysed at 1 252 901 SNPs. The bipolar disorder sample comprised 7481 cases and 9250 controls analysed at 2 427 089 SNPs. These studies contain individuals of European origin collected across Europe, the USA, and Australia. We note that there are a number of control individuals that contribute to both the PGC schizophrenia and bipolar disorder samples, including the Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium (WTCCC) - Phase 1 control sample. 9

Target data

We interrogated genotype data from a UK/Irish ADHD GWAS. Reference Stergiakouli, Hamshere, Holmans, Langley, Zaharieva and Hawi10 The sample comprises 727 children aged 4-18 years, recruited from UK community child psychiatry and paediatric clinics and from Dublin, Ireland. All children were White, of European origin and met research diagnostic criteria for DSM-III-R 11 /DSM-IV 12 or ICD-10 13 for ADHD or hyperkinetic disorder. Case exclusion criteria included bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, autism, epilepsy, intellectual disability (IQ <70) or any known major neurological or medical disorders. After complete description of the study to the participants, written informed consent was obtained. The ADHD GWAS sample also included 5081 UK controls from WTCCC2. Reference Evans, Spencer, Pointon, Su, Harvey and Kochan14 Genotyping for the ADHD cases was performed on the Illumina (San Diego, California, USA, www.illumina.com) Human 660W-Quad BeadChip, and genotyping for the controls was performed using the Illumina Human 1.2M BeadChip. The samples, quality control assessment and results of the primary ADHD GWAS are described in detail in a previous paper. Reference Stergiakouli, Hamshere, Holmans, Langley, Zaharieva and Hawi10

Statistical analysis

Approximately half of the WTCCC2 control sample had been used in the primary bipolar disorder GWAS reported by WTCCC1 9 and a schizophrenia GWAS reported by our own group, Reference O'Donovan, Craddock, Norton, Williams, Peirce and Moskvina15 both of which contributed to the PGC studies of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia respectively. It was, therefore, necessary to remove those and any overlapping case individuals from our ADHD GWAS. First, we identified individuals that were included in the earlier studies (genotyped on Affymetrix GeneChip 500K Mapping Array; Santa Clara, California, USA, www.affymetrix.com) that were also present in the primary ADHD GWAS. To do this we used 71 565 SNPs that were common to the studies that had also passed our quality control procedures. These SNPs were used to estimate identity-by-descent statistics for all pair-wise combinations of individuals. Across the two data-sets, we identified 2474 related individuals in the control sample, of which 2452 were duplicates, and no related case individuals. The related controls comprised 13 parent-offspring pairs, 7 sibling pairs, and 2 half-sibling or avuncular pairs. The duplicates and close relatives were removed from the control set giving a sample of 2067 control and 727 case individuals for our analysis of ADHD cases v. controls. Similar quality control approaches have been applied to our ADHD, Reference Stergiakouli, Hamshere, Holmans, Langley, Zaharieva and Hawi10 bipolar disorder 9 and schizophrenia Reference O'Donovan, Craddock, Norton, Williams, Peirce and Moskvina15 case and control samples to exclude overlapping and related individuals. Given that we have previously provided the results of the full ADHD GWAS elsewhere, Reference Stergiakouli, Hamshere, Holmans, Langley, Zaharieva and Hawi10 we do not provide individual-level SNP results here.

We used PLINK (1.07) for manipulation of the genetic data and to produce polygenic scores. All statistical analyses were performed with R (2.11.1). Both PLINK and R were run on Linux (Fedora release 13, x86_64).

Polygenic analysis

We made comparisons between ADHD cases and controls to determine whether the schizophrenia and/or bipolar derived polygenic scores were significantly different. We first selected a set of SNPs in relative linkage equilibrium in our ADHD and control sample using a sliding window of 200 SNPs, moving it along the genome in steps of 5 SNPs and dropping a SNP when the pair-wise estimate of linkage disequilibrium (r 2) was greater than 0.2 (PLINK command: -indep-pairwise 200 5 0.2). Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira and Bender16 From the results of the GWAS for each PGC sample, we identified for the selected SNPs their corresponding P-values, associated alleles, and the odds ratios (OR) for those alleles. Based on the result of both the ISC Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 and PGC studies, our primary analyses are based on an ‘association’ threshold P T<0.5. Alleles that are more common in the cases for which the two-tailed P-values met the condition P T<0.5 are termed the ‘score’ alleles. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 For each individual in the target data, we obtained the polygenic score for that individual which corresponds to the mean per-SNP product of the number of score alleles and the loge odds ratios. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 We employed logistic regression analysis to compare the polygenic scores in the target set (ADHD v. controls). As with our ADHD GWAS, Reference Stergiakouli, Hamshere, Holmans, Langley, Zaharieva and Hawi10 to allow for population stratification within the sample, the regression analysis is performed conditional on two covariates (the first two principal components estimated from our GWAS data using EIGENSTRAT Reference Patterson, Price and Reich17,Reference Price, Patterson, Plenge, Weinblatt, Shadick and Reich18 software designed for this purpose). For each analysis we report the P-value (two-tailed) and pseudo-R 2, the latter being a measure of the estimated variability in case-control status explained. Analyses were performed separately for score alleles derived from schizophrenia and bipolar disorder data-sets.

Table 1 Summary of results in the target sample comparing ADHD cases v. controlsFootnote a

ADHD v. controls
Discovery sample SNP selection criteria SNPs, n z-statistic R 2,% P
Bipolar disorder Bipolar disorder P<0.5 50 512 1.94 0.11 0.0519
Schizophrenia Schizophrenia P<0.5 48 494 3.88 0.45 1.04×10–4
Schizophrenia Schizophrenia P<0.5 and bipolar disorder P⩾0.5 22 595 1.63 0.080 0.102
Schizophrenia Schizophrenia P<0.5 and bipolar disorder P<0.5 25 137 4.40 0.58 1.09×10–5
Schizophrenia Same direction of effect in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder 17 303 4.42 0.59 9.98×10–6
Schizophrenia Different direction of effect in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder 7834 1.21 0.044 0.225

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism.

a. In all analyses, the ADHD cases had more risk alleles than the controls. All z-statistics are distributed with one degree of freedom and all P-values are two-tailed.

We interrogated the data further by creating polygenic scores based on simultaneous patterns of association in the PGC schizophrenia and bipolar disorder data-sets. Specifically, we were interested in testing the hypothesis that the set of alleles that showed evidence for association in both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder was the same set of alleles that was enriched for shared effects in ADHD risk. Alternative (non-null) hypotheses are that alleles that were relatively specific for one or the other disorder were enriched for those that confer risk of ADHD.

To examine these possibilities, we identified six sets of alleles. We first took the set of alleles at P T<0.5 in schizophrenia and divided them into those that did and those that did not also surpass P T<0.5 in bipolar disorder. Single nucleotide polymorphisms that surpassed P T<0.5 in both data-sets were further divided into two groups dependent on whether the allele that was over-represented in the bipolar disorder cases (compared with controls) was the same as the allele over-represented in the schizophrenia cases. These groups were then analysed separately. We note that, as expected given shared controls in the PGC schizophrenia and bipolar disorder GWAS data-sets, and also given the known overlap in polygenic risk between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 if an SNP surpassed P T<0.5 in schizophrenia, it was slightly more likely also to do so for bipolar disorder than chance (Table 1). Moreover, for the same reasons, but most particularly because of the shared controls, where SNPs surpassed P T<0.5 for both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, for most SNPs, the same allele was over-represented in both sets of cases compared with the controls. To help show the relationships between the six sets of SNPs, a graphical representation of the SNP selection is provided in the online supplement (online Fig. DS1).

Results

The polygenic score representing schizophrenia risk was significantly different between ADHD cases and controls, with higher scores observed in the ADHD cases (Table 1). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder cases also had a higher polygenic score representing bipolar disorder risk but the effect was weaker than that for the polygenic score representing schizophrenia as estimated by R 2, and just failed to attain a conventional level of statistical significance. For both analyses, the magnitudes of these effects (as defined by R 2) were much smaller than the 6% or 3% observed when the discovery and target sets both comprise schizophrenia Reference Ripke, Sanders, Kendler, Levinson, Sklar and Holmans7 or both bipolar disorder Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon and Craddock8 data-sets respectively.

In the analyses based on the patterns of association in schizophrenia conditional on that in bipolar disorder, only the set of SNPs that surpassed P T<0.5 in both samples produced a significant score difference between ADHD cases and controls (P = 1.09×10–5). This effect appeared to be largely driven by the set of alleles with the same direction of effect in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (P = 9.98×10–6). The scores derived from the set in which the alleles had opposite effects in schizophrenia than in bipolar disorder were not significantly different between ADHD cases and controls. Findings that were significant in Table 1 (plus the bipolar disorder analysis) are presented in online Fig. DS2 for a range of different significance thresholds. Consistent with the findings of the ISC, P<0.5 gives most evidence for discrimination of ADHD cases from controls. Our significant results are maintained across the full range of thresholds.

For polygenic score analysis, the number of alleles used in the analysis can have a large effect on significance and the effect size estimated from R 2. The number of alleles differed substantially between those that were in the same direction in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (n = 17 303) and those that were in opposite directions (n = 7834). We, therefore, undertook a simulation study to assess the impact of this variable on our evaluation of the contribution from alleles at SNPs surpassing P T<0.5 but for which the direction of association differed in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. From the 25 137 SNPs surpassing P T<0.5 in both disorders (i.e. schizophrenia and bipolar disorder), we selected at random a subset of 7834 SNPs (i.e. the number that had been included in the analysis of SNPs showing different directions of effect), and the remaining subset of 17 303 SNPs (i.e. the number that had been included in the analysis of SNPs showing the same direction of effect), and performed polygenic score analysis on both sets of SNPs. The difference in R 2 was estimated from the two analyses. Repeating the procedure 10 000 times gave a distribution of difference in R 2 against which we could empirically evaluate whether our observation of a weaker result for the SNPs with opposite directions of effect simply reflects a smaller set of SNPs being used. In the simulated data, the probability of observing a difference in R 2 at least as large as that observed in the real data (i.e. observed difference in R 2 = 0.55) was 0.09 (i.e. in 900 out of 10 000 random SNP draws was a larger difference in R 2 detected than in the observed data). This does not allow us to reject the null hypothesis that there is no difference in the SNPs associated with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder at P<0.5 with respect to the direction of effect. This in turn amounts to suggestive evidence (P = 0.09) that alleles that are over-represented in the cases in both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder GWAS data-sets proportionately capture more polygenic signal than those alleles that are more common in schizophrenia, but less common in bipolar disorder, when compared with controls.

Discussion

Aetiological overlaps between childhood-onset and major adult psychiatric disorders have, until relatively recently, been ignored in molecular genetic studies. Here, using a sample of 727 children and adolescents affected with ADHD and 2607 controls, we have demonstrated significant evidence for an overlap in polygenic susceptibility to schizophrenia and ADHD. No significant evidence was observed for a similar overlap between polygenic susceptibility to bipolar disorder and ADHD, although the level of significance was suggestive, so a potential relationship may be detectable in future studies with larger samples. This is the first study of its kind to identify a SNP-based relationship between ADHD risk and that for either schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. In this study, the overlap between ADHD and schizophrenia appears stronger than it is between ADHD and bipolar disorder. However, this could, in part, be related to somewhat lower power (i.e. smaller sample size) of the PGC bipolar disorder sample than the PGC schizophrenia samples used in the discovery part of the analysis, although the differences in sample size are not marked. The estimated effect sizes of the schizophrenia and bipolar disorder polygenic risk scores we report here in our ADHD sample are smaller than those observed by the ISC when taking schizophrenia-defined score alleles and investigating these in individuals with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder compared with controls (2-3% and 1-2% respectively). Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 The estimated effect sizes for bipolar disorder-defined score alleles in our ADHD v. control analysis are smaller than that observed when we reanalyse our data using the ISC schizophrenia data-set as discovery data (see the online supplement). As the ISC sample size (about 7000 individuals in total) is smaller than that of either the PGC schizophrenia or bipolar disorder sample, the discovery sample size cannot be a major factor driving the lower effect sizes seen here.

An observation that may help to provide insight into the genetic architecture of the relationship between the various disorders is the larger effect, as measured by R 2 for sets of SNPs that show similar directions of effect in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. This observation needs to be treated with some caution because of the occurrence of some shared controls within the PGC schizophrenia and bipolar disorder data-sets that biases towards finding more shared susceptibility alleles than would be expected under the null hypothesis. Nonetheless, if this observation receives support in independent studies, it would suggest that not all schizophrenia susceptibility alleles are equivalent. They may reflect different phenotypic risk effects. Alternatively, given the overlap in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, they may reflect the more ‘replicable’ or true alleles.

It is important to acknowledge that our findings do not distinguish between susceptibility alleles that may be (a) highly non-specific and span across multiple psychiatric conditions (not just schizophrenia and ADHD) and those that may be (b) relatively specific to schizophrenia and ADHD. Distinguishing between these possibilities will require studies that include a wider range of psychiatric phenotypes.

The links between ADHD, other early neurodevelopmental disorders (notably autism) and later-onset major psychiatric disorders are of growing interest. A subgroup of children with ADHD, but not a majority, develops later mood and bipolar affective disorder. The relationship with schizophrenia has been less well researched, although it is well recognised that a substantial proportion of those with schizophrenia and their offspring show early developmental problems that include attentional difficulties. Studies of ADHD in families in which a parent is affected with schizophrenia show that, in comparison with the offspring without ADHD, the offspring affected with ADHD experienced more neurological dysfunction, Reference Rieder and Nichols19,Reference Oner and Munir20 functional impairment, Reference Oner and Munir20 psychotic-like symptoms Reference Keshavan, Sujata, Mehra, Montrose and Sweeney21 and an increased risk of developing schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Reference Rieder and Nichols19,Reference Keshavan, Montrose, Rajarethinam, Diwadkar, Prasad and Sweeney22 A history of DSM-IV childhood ADHD is also found to be more frequent in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and in individuals at risk of developing these disorders than in the general population, although the reported prevalence rate of ADHD varies widely, a likely consequence of sample characteristics. Reference Peralta, de Jalon, Campos, Zandio, Sanchez-Torres and Cuesta23 Our results suggest that these observed links may in part follow from a shared genetic liability to ADHD and schizophrenia and that further investigation of whether they index specific neurodevelopmental impairments is needed.

Our observation of an overlap in genetic susceptibility for common variants in the current analysis is consistent with an observation of an overlap in genetic susceptibility between ADHD and schizophrenia for rare copy number variants (CNVs). Reference Williams, Zaharieva, Martin, Langley, Mantripragada and Fossdal4 In that study, based on a smaller sample of 366 ADHD case individuals (a subset of the sample presented here), we found that large (>500 kb), rare CNVs were significantly more common in cases than controls, Reference Williams, Zaharieva, Martin, Langley, Mantripragada and Fossdal4 and that CNVs identified in the ADHD cases were significantly enriched for loci previously reported in both autism (P = 0.0095) and schizophrenia (P = 0.010). Reference Williams, Zaharieva, Martin, Langley, Mantripragada and Fossdal4 We note that when excluding ADHD samples known to have large CNVs, our results are unchanged. Taken together with the current results, this is supportive evidence for an overlap in common, as well as rare, risk variants for ADHD and schizophrenia. We propose that in conjunction with the CNV data, this is suggestive evidence for the genetic architecture of ADHD being closer to schizophrenia than bipolar disorder.

As mentioned in the introduction, a striking finding from GWAS of psychiatric disorders has been the overlap in genetic association signals across different psychiatric disorders. An important goal of PGC, the international consortium for analysing psychiatric GWAS data, is to undertake systematic cross-disorder analyses using data-sets for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, recurrent depression, autism and ADHD. Reference Craddock, Kendler, Neale, Nurnberger, Purcell and Rietschel3 The vast majority of the ADHD data that we report here are not included within the Cross Disorders Group (CDG) analyses (see online supplement for details). When the PGC CDG analyses are available it will be important to explore the similarities and differences between findings in the two data-sets.

Strengths and limitations

There are several strengths of this study including the systematic genome-wide approach, the large size of the training samples and the novelty of the analysis applied to ADHD and schizophrenia. It is, however, important to consider potential limitations. As with all large-scale genotyping approaches, there is the possibility that systematic bias may influence the results. Population stratification is a confounder that can cause bias in case-control studies. We do not believe that population stratification has resulted in the findings presented here. First, we take into account principal components of our genotype data to adjust for hidden stratification. Second, the ISC excluded population stratification as an explanation for the broad effects seen between cases and controls. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 Finally, it is highly unlikely that exactly the same stratification differences would occur between the case and comparison data-sets in both discovery and target samples. Reference Hamshere, O'Donovan, Jones, Jones, Kirov and Green24 We cannot exclude the possibility that genotyping platform has influenced our results. However, extensive quality control was performed for the PGC schizophrenia and bipolar disorder samples, Reference Ripke, Sanders, Kendler, Levinson, Sklar and Holmans7,Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon and Craddock8 and the target ADHD GWAS sample Reference Stergiakouli, Hamshere, Holmans, Langley, Zaharieva and Hawi10 to ensure the genotypes for the case and control samples were comparable. A further potential source of bias is the theoretical possibility that by chance, or otherwise, our sample could have unusual phenotypic characteristics that increased the likelihood of a genetic similarity between ADHD and schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. Such an eventuality could, in principle, influence the statistical comparisons and/or limit the generalisability of findings to other samples. However, although noting that systematic phenotypic differences could occur, we are not aware of such selection effects in our sample and have no reason to believe this affects our results or conclusions.

It is important to stress that the statistical significance seen in the comparisons we report are driven by large sample sizes, not large effect sizes. As in previous studies that use the polygenic score approach, Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6-Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon and Craddock8,Reference Hamshere, O'Donovan, Jones, Jones, Kirov and Green24-Reference Ikeda, Aleksic, Kinoshita, Okochi, Kawashima and Kushima26 the proportion of variance explained is negligible (<1% of the variance in the present study). However, we should note that the underlying variance being captured indirectly may be considerably greater. Indeed, in the ISC study, Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6 although R 2 was about 3%, the true total underlying variation in liability estimated by the polygenic model was approximately 33%, a conclusion broadly supported by similar analyses of the PGC data, Reference Ripke, Sanders, Kendler, Levinson, Sklar and Holmans7,Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon and Craddock8 and by an analysis of the PGC data using an entirely different approach. Reference Lee, Decandia, Ripke, Yang, Sullivan and Goddard27 As an increasing proportion of the common genetic variation is accurately captured through increased sample sizes, it can be expected that much more of the variance attributable to the polygenic contributions, unique and shared across disorders, will be explained. This approach may therefore become an increasingly useful research tool for probing the relationships between disorders, and even potentially a useful clinical tool. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sullivan6

In summary, we provide evidence that common genetic variation contributes to risk for ADHD. ‘Risk’ alleles from schizophrenia in particular, and also bipolar disorder to some extent, are found to differentiate ADHD cases from control individuals. Our results indicate the need for further studies of the genetic relationship between ADHD and adult mood and psychotic disorders.

Funding

The ADHD research project was funded by the Wellcome Trust. We thank the Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2 for making available the control data project funded by the Wellcome Trust (grant 083948/Z/07/Z). We acknowledge use of the British 1958 Birth Cohort DNA collection, funded by the Medical Research Council grant G0000934 and the Wellcome Trust grant 068545/Z/0, as well as the UK Blood Services collection of Common Controls (UKBS-CC collection) funded by the Wellcome Trust grant 076113/C/04/Z and by NIHR programme grant to NHSBT (RP-PG-0310-1002).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the families and clinicians who participated in the ADHD research project. We also thank all those who participated in, or helped with, the bipolar disorder sample collection, including members of the Bipolar Disorder Research Network and Bipolar UK, and are indebted to the Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium (WTCCC1) for genotyping the bipolar disorder sample. We are grateful to our many colleagues within the Psychiatric GWAS Consortium (PGC) Schizophrenia Workgroup and Bipolar Disorder Workgroup for their collaborative analyses and publications that informed our current polygenic analysis.

Footnotes

See editorial, pp. 81–83, this issue.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Owen, MJ, Craddock, N, O'Donovan, MC. Suggestion of roles for both common and rare risk variants in genome-wide studies of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 667–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Craddock, N, Owen, MJ. The Kraepelinian dichotomy – going, going … but still not gone. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 196: 92–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3 Craddock, N, Kendler, K, Neale, M, Nurnberger, J, Purcell, S, Rietschel, M, et al Dissecting the phenotype in genome-wide association studies of psychiatric illness. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 97–9.Google ScholarPubMed
4 Williams, NM, Zaharieva, I, Martin, A, Langley, K, Mantripragada, K, Fossdal, R, et al Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 2010; 376: 1401–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Landaas, ET, Johansson, S, Halmoy, A, Oedegaard, KJ, Fasmer, OB, Haavik, J. Bipolar disorder risk alleles in adult ADHD patients. Genes Brain Behav 2011; 10: 418–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Purcell, SM, Wray, NR, Stone, JL, Visscher, PM, O'Donovan, MC, Sullivan, PF, et al Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 2009; 460: 748–52.Google ScholarPubMed
7 Ripke, S, Sanders, AR, Kendler, KS, Levinson, DF, Sklar, P, Holmans, PA, et al Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci. Nat Genet 2011; 43: 969–76.Google Scholar
8 Sklar, P, Ripke, S, Scott, LJ, Andreassen, OA, Cichon, S, Craddock, N, et al Large-scale genome-wide association analysis of bipolar disorder identifies a new susceptibility locus near ODZ4. Nat Genet 2011; 43: 977–83.Google Scholar
9 Genome-wide association study of 14,000 cases of seven common diseases and 3,000 shared controls. Nature 2007; 447: 661–78.Google Scholar
10 Stergiakouli, E, Hamshere, M, Holmans, P, Langley, K, Zaharieva, I, Hawi, Z, et al Investigating the contribution of common genetic variants to the risk and pathogenesis of ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169: 186–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM-III-R). APA, 1987.Google Scholar
12 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). APA, 1994.Google Scholar
13 World Health Organization. The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
14 Evans, DM, Spencer, CC, Pointon, JJ, Su, Z, Harvey, D, Kochan, G, et al Interaction between ERAP1 and HLA-B27 in ankylosing spondylitis implicates peptide handling in the mechanism for HLA-B27 in disease susceptibility. Nat Genet 2011; 43: 761–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 O'Donovan, MC, Craddock, N, Norton, N, Williams, H, Peirce, T, Moskvina, V, et al Identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up. Nat Genet 2008; 40: 1053–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Purcell, S, Neale, B, Todd-Brown, K, Thomas, L, Ferreira, MA, Bender, D, et al PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 2007; 81: 559–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17 Patterson, N, Price, AL, Reich, D. Population structure and eigenanalysis. PLoS Genet 2006; 2: e190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18 Price, AL, Patterson, NJ, Plenge, RM, Weinblatt, ME, Shadick, NA, Reich, D. Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet 2006; 38: 904–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Rieder, RO, Nichols, PL. Offspring of schizophrenics. III. Hyperactivity and neurological soft signs. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36: 665–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Oner, O, Munir, K. Attentional and neurocognitive characteristics of high-risk offspring of parents with schizophrenia compared with DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Schizophr Res 2005; 76: 293–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21 Keshavan, MS, Sujata, M, Mehra, A, Montrose, DM, Sweeney, JA. Psychosis proneness and ADHD in young relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res 2003; 59: 8592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 Keshavan, M, Montrose, DM, Rajarethinam, R, Diwadkar, V, Prasad, K, Sweeney, JA. Psychopathology among offspring of parents with schizophrenia: relationship to premorbid impairments. Schizophr Res 2008; 103: 114–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23 Peralta, V, de Jalon, EG, Campos, MS, Zandio, M, Sanchez-Torres, A, Cuesta, MJ. The meaning of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms in patients with a first-episode of schizophrenia-spectrum psychosis. Schizophr Res 2011; 126: 2835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 Hamshere, ML, O'Donovan, MC, Jones, IR, Jones, L, Kirov, G, Green, EK, et al Polygenic dissection of the bipolar phenotype. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 198: 284–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25 Ruderfer, DM, Kirov, G, Chambert, K, Moran, JL, Owen, MJ, O'Donovan, MC, et al A family-based study of common polygenic variation and risk of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16: 887–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26 Ikeda, M, Aleksic, B, Kinoshita, Y, Okochi, T, Kawashima, K, Kushima, I, et al Genome-wide association study of schizophrenia in a Japanese population. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69: 472–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27 Lee, SH, Decandia, TR, Ripke, S, Yang, J, Sullivan, PF, Goddard, ME, et al Estimating the proportion of variation in susceptibility to schizophrenia captured by common SNPs. Nat Genet 2012; 44: 247–50.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Summary of results in the target sample comparing ADHD cases v. controlsa

Supplementary material: PDF

Hamshere et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Hamshere et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 116.9 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.