Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-fr72s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-17T10:30:10.211Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The long reach of puberty: mechanisms underlying sex-dependent links between pubertal timing and adult internalizing symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 April 2025

Holly T. Pham*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
Sheri A. Berenbaum
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA Department of Pediatrics, The Pennsylvania State University, Hershey, PA, USA
Lisabeth F. DiLalla
Affiliation:
Department of Family and Community Medicine, Southern Illinois University School of Medicine, Carbondale, IL, USA
Robin P. Corley
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA
Sally J. Wadsworth
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA
Chandra Reynolds
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA
Adriene M. Beltz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
*
Corresponding author: Holly T. Pham; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Pubertal development variations have consequences for adolescent internalizing problems, which likely continue into adulthood. Key questions concern the extent of these links between pubertal timing and adult symptoms, as well as the underlying mechanisms.

Methods

Longitudinal data were available for 475 female and 404 male participants. Pubertal timing was indicated by age at mid-puberty for both groups and age at menarche for female participants (both assessed continuously). Adult self-reported outcomes of recent and lifetime depression and anxiety were predicted from pubertal timing, also controlling for adolescent (then childhood) internalizing problems. Emerging adulthood self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, education level, and age at sexual initiation were examined as mediators of the pubertal timing-adult internalizing link. Multilevel models tested hypotheses.

Results

Pubertal timing had persisting and sex-dependent psychological associations. Specifically, in female, but not male, adults, early puberty was associated with all adult internalizing outcomes, and for past year and lifetime depression symptoms, even after controlling for adolescent internalizing problems. Pubertal timing links with past-year depression symptoms were mediated by age at sexual initiation, while all other persisting pubertal timing links with adult symptoms were mediated by body dissatisfaction. Most findings concerning depression held when childhood internalizing problems were also a covariate.

Conclusions

Leveraging data spanning four developmental periods, findings highlight the associations between pubertal variations and adult internalizing symptoms by revealing underlying sex-dependent behavioral pathways. Only for female participants did pubertal timing affect depression and anxiety in established adulthood, with body dissatisfaction and age at sexual initiation as unique developmental mechanisms.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Pubertal development has a profound influence on psychological functioning, particularly internalizing behaviors. Indeed, the biopsychosocial experience of puberty can actuate psychopathology (e.g. depression and anxiety) in youth who are vulnerable. Variations in pubertal development, especially its timing in comparison to peers, are also associated with individual differences in psychological health. For instance, it is well-established that early puberty in female youth is associated with internalizing problems in adolescence (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023; Ullsperger & Nikolas, Reference Ullsperger and Nikolas2017). There is also increasing suggestion that off-time puberty has adverse consequences for male youth, with late maturers reporting heightened depression and anxiety (Graber, Reference Graber2013). Moreover, increasing evidence shows that some puberty-linked internalizing problems persist into adulthood (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023).

Yet, crucial questions remain unanswered about the so-called long arm of pubertal development. Are persisting links from pubertal timing to adult depression and anxiety straightforward extensions of adolescent – or even childhood – internalizing problems? What mechanisms maintain, exacerbate, or eliminate long-term associations of puberty with depression and anxiety symptoms? Finally, are these processes similar across the sexes? (We investigated associations with puberty (a biological and sex-related process), and participants were identified by assigned sex in the parent study.)

Although answers to these questions require full longitudinal data from male and female participants spanning childhood to established adulthood, some insights can be gleaned from recent empirical work on the persistence of depression. Hypothesized mediators focus on negative body image and life experiences resulting from self- and other reactions to early physical development in female youth. In one longitudinal investigation, links between early menarche and past-week adult depression symptoms (~28 years old) were mediated by discontinued high school education or ever experiencing physical or sexual assault (Mendle, Ryan, & McKone, Reference Mendle, Ryan and McKone2019). Studies of adults providing retrospective reports of pubertal timing and mediators assessed at the same time as outcome measures also show some associations: Depression in male late-maturing young adults (~20 years old) statistically occurred via low self-perceived masculinity, including body image (Beltz, Reference Beltz2018); internalizing problems in very early-maturing young adult females (~21 years old) were mediated by body image dysphoria and low best friend intimacy (Thériault, Otis, Hébert, Gurreri, & Lambert, Reference Thériault, Otis, Hébert, Gurreri and Lambert2019). Other studies using longitudinal or retrospective designs have failed to find mediation (e.g. teen pregnancy did not mediate the link between early menarche and adult problems; Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Beltz, Carter and Dorn2019).

It is difficult to draw strong inferences from these studies. No study used a true longitudinal design with contemporaneous assessment of adolescent pubertal development (in fact, all used retrospective reports with two only assessing menarche), internalizing outcomes assessed in established adulthood, control for adolescent psychological health (as adolescent internalizing problems are strongly associated with internalizing disorders in adulthood, including major depressive disorder [MDD] and generalized anxiety disorder [GAD]; van der Ende, Verhulst, & Tiemeier, Reference van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2020), and mediating mechanisms assessed between puberty and adulthood. Furthermore, only one study included male participants (Beltz, Reference Beltz2018).

We aimed to address this substantial knowledge gap by using full longitudinal data to determine the extent to which variations in pubertal timing are linked to adult depression and anxiety in both sexes; we covaried adolescent (and childhood) internalizing problems, and considered multiple intervening mechanisms in emerging adulthood (approximately late teenage years into late twenties) that might account for both recent and lifetime symptoms. We focused on mechanisms previously suggested to be important in prior research, which have established gender-related links with both adult internalizing symptoms and puberty (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023; Graber, Reference Graber2013). First, low self-esteem in adolescence has been linked to an increased risk of depression and anxiety in adulthood (Boden, Fergusson, & Horwood, Reference Boden, Fergusson and Horwood2008; Steiger, Allemand, Robins, & Fend, Reference Steiger, Allemand, Robins and Fend2014), as well as to early maturation in female youth (Reynolds & Juvonen, Reference Reynolds and Juvonen2012; Stojković, Reference Stojković2013). Second, body dissatisfaction has been shown to contribute to anxiety (Vannucci & Ohannessian, Reference Vannucci and Ohannessian2018) and depression (Sharpe et al., Reference Sharpe, Patalay, Choo, Wall, Mason, Goldschmidt and Neumark-Sztainer2018), and is associated with early maturation in female youth (Duncan, Ritter, Dornbusch, Gross, & Merrill Carlsmith, Reference Duncan, Ritter, Dornbusch, Gross and Merrill Carlsmith1985; Shope, Freeman, & Culbert, Reference Shope, Freeman and Culbert2022). Third, educational attainment has been linked to positive well-being (Hergenrather, Zeglin, McGuire-Kuletz, & Rhodes, Reference Hergenrather, Zeglin, McGuire-Kuletz and Rhodes2015; Sareen, Afifi, McMillan, & Asmundson, Reference Sareen, Afifi, McMillan and Asmundson2011), with potential pubertal timing influences that differ by sex (lower education is associated with late maturation in male youth, but with early maturation in female youth; Koivusilta & Rimpelä, Reference Koivusilta and Rimpelä2004; Cavanagh, Riegle-Crumb, & Crosnoe, Reference Cavanagh, Riegle-Crumb and Crosnoe2007). Fourth, early sexual initiation has been associated with adolescent depression and earlier pubertal timing in both sexes, though these associations appear stronger in female than male youth (Baams, Dubas, Overbeek, & van Aken, Reference Baams, Dubas, Overbeek and van Aken2015; Spriggs & Halpern, Reference Spriggs and Halpern2008; Wesche, Kreager, Lefkowitz, & Siennick, Reference Wesche, Kreager, Lefkowitz and Siennick2017).

We hypothesized that (a) in female participants, pubertal timing would be negatively associated with internalizing symptoms (recent and lifetime depression and anxiety), and (b) in male participants, pubertal timing would be positively associated with symptoms (e.g. late maturation would confer higher risk than early maturation for depression) – and that these associations would persist after controlling for adolescent (and childhood) internalizing problems. For persisting associations, we then studied potential mechanisms occurring during emerging adulthood, hypothesizing that persisting internalizing symptoms would be mediated by low self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, lack of college degree, and for female participants, early sexual initiation.

Method

Overview

Participants were enrolled in the Colorado Adoption/Twin Study of Lifespan behavioral development and cognitive aging (CATSLife) (Wadsworth et al., Reference Wadsworth, Corley, Munoz, Trubenstein, Knaap, DeFries and Reynolds2019), an extension of two longitudinal studies: the Colorado Longitudinal Twin Study (LTS) and the Colorado Adoption Project (CAP). The LTS sample consisted of monozygotic and dizygotic infant twins and their families (original sample: N = 966 individuals from 483 twin pairs), and the CAP sample consisted of adoptee and matched nonadoptee infants and their families (original sample: N = 732 individuals from 490 families). Both studies aimed to longitudinally assess genetic and environmental contributions to development from birth through adolescence and had similar data collection procedures before merging for the CATSLife. See additional details in Plomin and DeFries (Reference Plomin and DeFries1985); Rhea, Gross, Haberstick, and Corley (Reference Rhea, Gross, Haberstick and Corley2006); and Wadsworth et al. (Reference Wadsworth, Corley, Munoz, Trubenstein, Knaap, DeFries and Reynolds2019).

The current study utilized data from puberty and behavioral assessments conducted nearly annually from childhood to mid-adolescence and subsequently during emerging adulthood and established adulthood. Pubertal development was assessed yearly in the LTS/CAP from grades 3 through 9 (ending around age 15, with a few participants assessed until age 17). Depression and anxiety symptoms in established adulthood were assessed in the CATSLife at a median age of 29 years (range 28–43). Psychological mediators were assessed in the LTS/CAP at a median age of 20 years (range 16–33). The adolescent internalizing problems covariate was assessed in the LTS/CAP at age 16 years (range 16–18). The childhood internalizing problems covariate used for sensitivity analyses was assessed in the LTS/CAP at age 7 years.

Participants

The final sample consisted of 879 participants (54% female, from 559 families) who had data on pubertal development (described in Beltz, Corley, Bricker, Wadsworth, & Berenbaum, Reference Beltz, Corley, Bricker, Wadsworth and Berenbaum2014), and at least one behavioral measure of interest at established adulthood, emerging adulthood, and adolescence. Of the 1458 participants with data on pubertal timing, 331 had missing adolescent behavioral data, and an additional 248 had no adult outcome measure; the resulting sample had data on at least one mediator. The sample was 92% White, 3% Multiracial, 2% Asian, 2% American Indian/Alaskan Native, <1% other (not reported separately due to identifiability), and 1% unknown/not reported; about 5% was Hispanic/Latine.

The procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, revised in 2008.

Measures

All measures met standard psychometric criteria, including reliability and validity in other samples, as well as internal consistency reliability in the current sample (Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics by sex

Note: Numeric superscripts indicate measure validity references. Sex differences (0 = female, 1 = male) were assessed with random intercepts-only multilevel models to adjust for family status; b is the unstandardized coefficient, with its standard error (SE).

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Pubertal timing

Age at mid-puberty

Timing of mid-puberty was estimated via growth curve analyses from annual reports on the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988), a widely used measure of pubertal status. Youth completed five items about the development of secondary sex characteristics: three items for both sexes (body hair, skin changes, and growth spurt) and two sex-specific items (breast development and menarche for female adolescents; facial hair growth and deepening voice for male adolescents). All items except menarche were rated on a 4-point scale (1 = No development to 4 = Development completed). Menarche was rated as 1 = Absent or 4 = Completed. Items were averaged to generate a composite at each assessment. Each adolescent’s age at mid-puberty as measured by the PDS (score = 2.5) was then calculated via Bayes estimation from sex-stratified random effects logistic growth curve models (Beltz et al., Reference Beltz, Corley, Bricker, Wadsworth and Berenbaum2014). Estimates were provided for all youth with at least two puberty assessments, but participants were excluded (as in other reports: Beltz et al., Reference Beltz, Corley, Bricker, Wadsworth and Berenbaum2014; Beltz, Corley, Wadsworth, DiLalla, & Berenbaum, Reference Beltz, Corley, Wadsworth, Dilalla and Berenbaum2020; Chaku et al., Reference Chaku, Berenbaum, Qian, Corley, Wadsworth, Reynolds and Beltz2024) if their estimated age at mid-puberty was three or more standard deviations from the mean.

Age at menarche

Female adolescents who indicated on the PDS that they had reached menarche were asked to report the age (years and months) at which it occurred.

Outcomes: internalizing symptoms in established adulthood

Analyses focused on assessments of the number of recent and lifetime MDD and GAD clinical symptoms. The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS; Robins et al., Reference Robins, Cottler, Bucholz, Compton, North and Rourke1999) was used to assess the number of past-year and lifetime MDD symptoms. The DIS was also used to assess the number of lifetime GAD symptoms. Assessment of past year GAD symptoms was not precise (e.g. symptom presence/absence versus count), so the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire General Distress Anxiety subscale (MASQ GDA; Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991) was used to assess symptoms in the past 2 weeks.

Mediators: measures in emerging adulthood

Hypothesized mediators available in the CATSLife were generally assessed in emerging adulthood (self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, and education level). Age at sexual initiation was not tightly developmentally constrained, as it generally occurs between puberty and emerging adulthood.

Self-esteem

Participants completed the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1965) between the ages of 16 and 21 (M = 17.32, SD = 0.57). Scores were averages of 10 items (rated on a four-point Likert scale).

Body dissatisfaction

Body dissatisfaction was assessed with the 8-item Shape Concern subscale of the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (Fairburn & Beglin, Reference Fairburn and Beglin1994), a 28-item self-report measure of the frequency and severity of behaviors associated with eating disorders in the past 28 days. It was completed between the ages of 21 and 33 (M = 23.65, SD = 2.24).

Education level

Participants reported on the level of education obtained by early adulthood (21–25 years) (McClelland, Acock, Piccinin, Rhea, & Stallings, Reference McClelland, Acock, Piccinin, Rhea and Stallings2013; Wadsworth et al., Reference Wadsworth, Corley, Munoz, Trubenstein, Knaap, DeFries and Reynolds2019). Education was scored to reflect the presence or absence of a bachelor’s degree or higher.

Age at sexual initiation

At age 17 or 21, participants reported the age of their first sexual experience if it had occurred (Bricker et al., Reference Bricker, Stallings, Corley, Wadsworth, Bryan, Timberlake and DeFries2006). Age at sexual initiation was categorized to be early or not, with age 16 serving as the cut-off based on the sample mode. Consistent with other work (Beltz et al., Reference Beltz, Corley, Wadsworth, Dilalla and Berenbaum2020; Chaku et al., Reference Chaku, Berenbaum, Qian, Corley, Wadsworth, Reynolds and Beltz2024; Liang & Chikritzhs, Reference Liang and Chikritzhs2013; Upchurch, Lillard, Aneshensel, & Li, Reference Upchurch, Lillard, Aneshensel and Li2002), participants reporting ages younger than 13 (n = 8) were excluded.

Covariate: internalizing problems in adolescence and childhood

To determine whether pubertal timing was uniquely linked to adult depression and anxiety and mechanisms that might mediate links, we controlled for parallel adolescent internalizing problems associated with pubertal timing. Caregivers reported on adolescent internalizing behavior problems at age 16 using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991). The CBCL internalizing broadband scale raw score, which includes anxiety, depression, and withdrawn behaviors, was used, although CBCL is caregiver-reported and the adult measures are self-reported. In sensitivity analyses controlling for childhood internalizing problems (as described in Sensitivity Analyses), the caregiver-reported CBCL internalizing broadband scale raw score at age 7 was used.

Analytic plan

First, descriptive information was examined: (a) sex differences in study variables were tested using multilevel models, with nesting in families accounted for with random intercepts; (b) zero-order correlations among the study variables were calculated for descriptive purposes.

Second, hypotheses were tested with a series of multilevel models accounting for nesting in families via random intercepts for recent and lifetime depression and anxiety in established adulthood (Figure 1). All multilevel models were conducted in SPSS 29, separately by sex, controlling for initial study (due to some sample differences between included participants from the LTS and CAP; Supplementary Table 1) and age, and estimated with maximum likelihood. Type I error was .05.

Figure 1. Multilevel models testing hypotheses of pubertal timing associations (X) with outcomes in established adulthood (Y) via mediators in emerging adulthood (M), and covarying behavior problems in adolescence (C). Note: (a) Model 1 assesses the links between pubertal timing and established adulthood outcomes (solid arrow). Model 2 incorporates an adolescent behavior covariate (dashed arrow). (b) Model 3 assesses emerging adulthood mediations of the associations pubertal timing and established adulthood outcome links (solid arrows). Model 4 adds an adolescent behavior covariate (dashed arrows). There was minor overlap in assessment periods due to scheduling constraints in the original studies, with 4% of participants having their adolescent internalizing problems covariate assessment overlap with their self-esteem mediator assessment. All four models controlled for age and initial study. MDD, ‘major depressive disorder’; GAD, ‘generalized anxiety disorder’.

For each outcome, several models were considered. Model 1 (XY; Figure 1a) provided a test of pubertal timing (X) associations with internalizing symptoms in established adulthood (Y). If significant (showing continuing links with pubertal timing), three additional multilevel models were tested to examine potential persistence and underlying mechanisms (mediators). Model 2 (X adding CY; Figure 1a) added adolescent internalizing problems; and (C) as a covariate to determine whether pubertal timing predicted adulthood depression and anxiety above and beyond adolescent behavior. Models 3 and 4 added potential mediators (M: self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, education level, and age at sexual initiation) of pubertal timing associations with internalizing symptoms. The measure of adolescent internalizing problems was not included in Model 3 (XMY; Figure 1b), but it was added in Model 4 (X adding CMY; Figure 1b) as a covariate. Two equations are estimated in mediation: In the first (MdlM: mediator model), X predicts M; in the second (MdlY: outcome model), X and M predict Y. As pubertal timing is a continuous predictor (i.e. age), its multilevel model coefficients are interpreted in yearly increments (e.g. ‘early’ puberty reflects an increase/decrease in symptoms with every one-year decrease in pubertal timing).

Multilevel mediation models were tested using MLmed in SPSS (Rockwood & Hayes, Reference Rockwood and Hayes2017), estimating within-family and between-family effects, and using the Monte Carlo method to estimate the sampling distribution of indirect effects, with inferences made via 95% confidence intervals; if the intervals did not include zero, then mediation was inferred (in the context of covariates). The primary hypotheses concerned between-family mediation. Within-family mediation results are also reported for completeness in the Supplementary Material; hypothesized mechanisms may be amplified within families, but power is reduced.

Sensitivity analyses

Sensitivity analyses controlling for both adolescent and childhood internalizing problems (Supplementary Figure 1) were conducted to ensure that significant pubertal timing links with adulthood outcomes were not explained by prepubertal behaviors. The age 7 CBCL internalizing broadband scale raw score was used to measure childhood internalizing problems, ensuring that these problems were captured before adrenarche and thus would not be impacted by puberty.

Missing data

Potential influences of missingness on results were assessed by comparing the 879 participants included in this study to the 579 participants from the parent study excluded from analyses. Participants with available data did not differ from those with missing data in terms of race or ethnicity but were more likely to be female and nonadoptees (Supplementary Table 2). Female participants included in analyses had later menarche, were less likely to have early age at sexual initiation, and had lower levels of adolescent internalizing problems than those excluded (Supplementary Table 3). Male participants included in analyses had more recent MDD symptoms and were less likely to have early age at sexual initiation than those excluded (Supplementary Table 4).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive data are shown in Table 1. Most expected sex differences were found: Compared to male participants, female participants had earlier pubertal timing, higher levels of major depressive and generalized anxiety symptoms in established adulthood, higher body dissatisfaction and likelihood of attaining a bachelor’s degree during emerging adulthood, and higher levels of internalizing problems in adolescence. Zero-order correlations among study variables are shown in Supplementary Table 5.

Persistence and mechanisms of pubertal timing associations with internalizing symptoms in established adulthood

The results of hypothesis testing using multilevel models to predict internalizing symptoms and between-family mediation are shown in Tables 2 (MDD) and 3 (GAD) for female participants and in Supplementary Table 6 for male participants. Within-family mediation results for female participants are in Supplementary Tables 7 and 8. As noted, for outcomes with nonsignificant associations with pubertal timing (Model 1), Models 2–4 were not tested.

Table 2. Multilevel model results for female participants: between-family associations between pubertal timing (X) and major depressive disorder symptoms in established adulthood (Y) via mediators in emerging adulthood (M), covarying adolescent internalizing problems (C), initial study (S), and age (A)

Note: Estimates are unstandardized. All significant fixed direct (p < .05) and indirect (confidence intervals that do not include 0) effects are bolded. Alternating models are shaded grey for ease of distinguishing them. Initial study (LTS or CAP) and age links with model outcomes are only noted if significant; all significant study and age links were in positive directions (+). Residual effects ranged from 0.08 to 25.04 and all but four were, as expected, larger than random effects. Mdl, model; Model 1: XY; Model 2: X and CY; Model 3: XM ➔ Y; Model 4: X and CMY; M, emerging adulthood mediator; C, covariate; Y, established adulthood outcome; S, initial study; A, age at established adulthood; SE, self-esteem; BD, body dissatisfaction; EL, education level; AS, age at sexual initiation; MdlM, mediator model within mediation analysis; MdlY, outcome model within mediation analysis.

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 3. Multilevel model results for female participants: between-family associations between pubertal timing (X) and generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in established adulthood (Y) via mediators in emerging adulthood (M), covarying adolescent internalizing problems (C), initial study (S), and age (A)

Note: Estimates are unstandardized. All significant fixed direct (p < .05) and indirect (confidence intervals that do not include 0) effects are bolded. Alternating models are shaded grey for ease of distinguishing them. Initial study (LTS or CAP) and age links with model outcomes are only noted if significant; all significant study and age links were in positive directions (+). Residual effects ranged from 0.08 to 33.27 and all but three were, as expected, larger than random effects. Mdl, model; Model 1: XY; Model 2: X and CY; Model 3: XMY; Model 4: X and CMY; M, emerging adulthood mediator; C, covariate; Y, established adulthood outcome; S, initial study; A, age at established adulthood; SE, self-esteem; BD, body dissatisfaction; EL, education level; AS, age at sexual initiation; MdlM, mediator model within mediation analysis; MdlY, outcome model within mediation analysis.

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Predicting female participants’ internalizing symptoms

Major depressive disorder

Recent (past year) MDD symptoms were predicted by age at mid-puberty, with younger age associated with higher adult means (Model 1). This remained significant after controlling for adolescent internalizing problems (Model 2). Age at menarche showed a similar pattern of results but was not significant in Model 1.

The link between age at mid-puberty and past-year MDD symptoms was significantly mediated by age at sexual initiation (Model 3): early timing was associated with early sexual initiation, which, in turn, was associated with more MDD symptoms. After adjusting for adolescent internalizing problems (Model 4), age at sexual initiation remained a significant mediator. The direct effect (XY) of age at mid-puberty on MDD symptoms remained significant in Models 3 and 4 in which age at sexual initiation was a significant mediator. Self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, and education level were not significant mediators. Covariates (initial study, age) were rarely significant, except for models including education level.

Similar patterns arose for lifetime MDD symptoms. Lifetime MDD symptoms were predicted by age at mid-puberty (Model 1), and this remained significant after controlling for adolescent internalizing problems (Model 2). Age at menarche did not significantly predict lifetime MDD symptoms but showed a similar pattern of results.

The link between age at mid-puberty and lifetime MDD symptoms was significantly mediated by both body dissatisfaction and age at sexual initiation (Model 3): early timing was associated with heightened body dissatisfaction and early sexual initiation, both of which, in turn, were associated with more MDD symptoms. After adjusting for adolescent internalizing problems (Model 4), age at sexual initiation remained a significant mediator, but body dissatisfaction did not. The direct effect of age at mid-puberty on MDD symptoms was no longer significant in Model 3, in which body dissatisfaction was a significant mediator. In contrast, the direct effect (XY) of age at mid-puberty on MDD symptoms remained significant in Models 3 and 4 in which age at sexual initiation was a significant mediator. Neither self-esteem nor education level was significant mediators. Covariates were not significant, except for models including education level.

In sensitivity analyses controlling for both adolescent and childhood internalizing problems, results for MDD symptoms generally showed the same pattern of findings as those controlling for only adolescent internalizing problems (Supplementary Tables 9 and 10; Supplementary Figure 2), despite their smaller sample size (N main = 473 versus N sensitivity = 385).

Generalized anxiety disorder

Both recent (past 2 weeks) and lifetime GAD symptoms were predicted by age at mid-puberty, with younger age associated with more anxiety in established adulthood (Model 1). These associations were no longer significant after controlling for adolescent internalizing problems (Model 2), although the coefficients were similar between Models 1 and 2 for each outcome. Age at menarche showed similar patterns of results for both past 2 weeks and lifetime GAD symptoms but was not significant in Model 1.

The links between age at mid-puberty and both the past 2 weeks and lifetime GAD symptoms were significantly mediated by body dissatisfaction (Model 3): early timing (each one-year decrease of the continuous measure) was associated with heightened body dissatisfaction, which, in turn, was associated with more GAD symptoms. Body dissatisfaction remained a significant mediator after adjusting for adolescent internalizing problems (Model 4). The direct effects (XY) of age at mid-puberty on both the past 2 weeks and lifetime GAD symptoms became nonsignificant in Models 3 and 4. Self-esteem, education level, and age at sexual initiation did not significantly mediate the links between age at mid-puberty and past 2 weeks and lifetime GAD symptoms. Covariates were not significant for past 2 weeks symptoms, except in models including education level; they were significant in all models of lifetime symptoms.

Results for GAD symptoms changed in sensitivity analyses controlling for both adolescent and childhood internalizing problems (Supplementary Table 11). Age at mid-puberty was no longer significantly associated with adult outcomes in Model 1, although the change in probability level for lifetime GAD (i.e. p = .046 increased to p = .058) likely reflects the reduced sample size (N main = 473 versus N sensitivity = 385).

Predicting male participants’ internalizing symptoms

None of the symptom outcomes (i.e. past year or lifetime MDD, past 2 weeks or lifetime GAD) were predicted by pubertal timing in Model 1.

Discussion

This study with full longitudinal data reveals persisting links between internalizing symptoms in adulthood and variations in pubertal development in female, but not male, participants; it also provides novel evidence about mechanisms accounting for the sex-dependent associations. Early puberty (i.e. each one-year decrease in a continuous measure) continued to be associated with psychological problems in established adulthood, above and beyond associations seen before puberty and in adolescence, through links with behaviors in emerging adulthood. The results support most of our hypotheses concerning depression symptoms (Figure 2); hypotheses concerning anxiety symptoms were sensitive to childhood behavior problems (Supplementary Figure 2). Specifically, persisting depression symptoms were mediated by body dissatisfaction for lifetime depression and by early sexual initiation for both past year and lifetime depression; past 2 weeks and lifetime anxiety symptoms were mediated by body dissatisfaction, but this association was reduced when childhood internalizing problems were considered.

Figure 2. Summary of results for female participants: pubertal timing associations with MDD and GAD recent and lifetime outcomes and mechanisms, and controls for adolescent behavior. Note: Checkmarks indicate significant pubertal timing associations with the outcome (leftmost column). Xs indicate nonsignificant pubertal timing associations. All four models controlled for age and initial study. Covariate models (Models 2 and 4) also controlled for adolescent behavior. Only significant mediators are indicated under the ‘Persistence Mechanism’ and ‘Mechanism w/ Covariate’ columns. MDD, ‘major depressive disorder’; GAD, ‘generalized anxiety disorder’.

Our results extend the literature, for the first time establishing these links using full longitudinal data, with psychological symptoms measured in adulthood controlled for problems in childhood and adolescence, puberty measured contemporaneously throughout the pubertal transition, and mediators assessed between puberty and established adulthood. Pubertal timing predicted depression and anxiety in female adults, above and beyond links present in adolescence (and for depression, even when controlling for childhood problems) as well as when controlling for study-related and age covariates. Body dissatisfaction and age at sexual initiation mediated select links between pubertal timing and internalizing symptoms in female adults, again beyond associations in adolescence (and beyond associations in childhood for depression). Finally, patterns of results for recent versus lifetime symptoms were also largely similar, including in the context of adolescent covariates and mediators, highlighting that persisting downstream links between puberty and internalizing symptoms are experienced in the day-to-day lives of established female adults.

Findings about developmental mediators importantly extend previous work using cross-sectional retrospective reports showing that body dysphoria measured at puberty mediated links between early puberty and internalizing problems in female young adults (Thériault et al., Reference Thériault, Otis, Hébert, Gurreri and Lambert2019) and is consistent with broader hypotheses about links between pubertal development and body image as well as sexual activity. For example, early maturation has been consistently linked to lower body satisfaction in female youth and higher body satisfaction in male youth (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023); female young adults who retrospectively reported early maturation also reported greater body surveillance, greater sex appeal self-worth, and less body appreciation (Grower, Ward, & Beltz, Reference Grower, Ward and Beltz2019). These findings broadly align with speculations about the psychological impacts of cultural sexual objectification of the female body (Fredrickson & Roberts, Reference Fredrickson and Roberts1997). Early (versus on-time or late) pubertal timing could increase the likelihood of objectification within a peer group and potentially prolong the sensitive period during which female youth engage in negative cognitions about their bodies, ultimately accentuating internalizing problems in adulthood. In comparison, cultural sexual objectification is not as prevalent for the male body, which could explain the present study’s null findings for male participants.

Nevertheless, not all investigated links were significant. Persisting associations between pubertal timing and anxiety symptoms were less evident when childhood internalizing problems were included in analyses, consistent with work questioning the developmental origins of adolescent anxiety (e.g. Reardon, Leen-Feldner, & Hayward, Reference Reardon, Leen-Feldner and Hayward2009) and the significance of the study covariates (e.g. age) on lifetime anxiety symptoms (Table 3). Self-esteem and education level were not significant mediators of any pubertal timing – adult outcome links, possibly because our assessment used an age-related dichotomous indicator of college completion (as a potential socioeconomic status indicator), whereas past research focused on high school completion (as an indicator of externalizing problems; Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Beltz, Carter and Dorn2019). There were also limitations of the measures, including assessment ages of the mediators and method variance (i.e. reports of psychological problems were obtained from parents at early ages but participants themselves at later ages). Estimates that were similar in magnitude were significant in some cases but not others because of varying sample size (e.g. pubertal timing links with lifetime anxiety in female participants).

It is important to note that we focused on a range of symptoms measured continuously, not categorical diagnoses, consistent with a dimensional approach to psychopathology (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013). This reflected the community nature of the sample (with low rates of diagnoses) and the likelihood that early puberty acts to increase problems but not necessarily to trigger frank psychopathology, as past work in this sample has shown that childhood problems do not predict pubertal timing, but that both problems and timing have largely unique influences on adolescent problems (Beltz et al., Reference Beltz, Corley, Wadsworth, Dilalla and Berenbaum2020). This also means that our results are not likely to change with any changes in diagnostic criteria (Chmielewski, Clark, Bagby, & Watson, Reference Chmielewski, Clark, Bagby and Watson2015).

This study is novel in several ways. First, these are the only findings using a full longitudinal design – with assessments from four distinct developmental periods – to investigate pubertal timing associations with adult recent and lifetime behavioral outcomes, whether those associations reflect continuity or potential emergence (using covariates of both childhood and adolescent psychological health), and the mechanisms underlying them (using mediators measured between adolescence and established adulthood). It also leveraged sophisticated multilevel mediation analyses necessary for accounting for within-family dependencies when estimating indirect effects via confidence intervals (Rockwood & Hayes, Reference Rockwood and Hayes2017).

Second, we extended work on mediators of pubertal timing links to include male participants. We did not replicate findings of late puberty associations with adult depression in male participants, perhaps because previous work focused on early adulthood versus our focus on established adulthood, and we also had difficulty differentiating on-time from late-maturing male youth because puberty assessments ended at age 15 in the CAP/LTS.

Third, pubertal timing was described with growth curve models based on multiple assessments, leveraging past work in this sample and the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS) without making assumptions about Tanner Stages (Beltz et al., Reference Beltz, Corley, Bricker, Wadsworth and Berenbaum2014; Shirtcliff, Dahl, & Pollak, Reference Shirtcliff, Dahl and Pollak2009). Most previous studies relied on a single measure of menarche in female youth, thus excluding male youth. Indeed, it is surprising that persisting links between age at menarche and established adult outcomes were not detected. This might reflect the enhanced sensitivity of the age at mid-puberty measure or systematic missing data linked to menarche, as participants with earlier menarche were less likely to be included in analyses.

Limitations and considerations

The interpretation of findings should be considered in light of some limitations often shared by similar studies. First, puberty was assessed by self-reports on the PDS rather than physical exam (e.g. Tanner staging), and it is unknown if mid-puberty as assessed via the PDS is equivalent to mid-puberty as assessed by other methods. However, there are several strengths of using the PDS, including its reliability and validity (especially with longitudinal assessment (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023; Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Dahl and Pollak2009)), as well as its incorporation of menarche status.

Second, the sample is limited in several ways, consisting of siblings in families within a narrow demographic range, so it is important to extend the work to more heterogeneous samples especially in light of racial/ethnic differences in pubertal timing (Deardorff, Hoyt, Carter, & Shirtcliff, Reference Deardorff, Hoyt, Carter and Shirtcliff2019; Deardorff et al., Reference Deardorff, Marceau, Johnson, Reeves, Biro, Kubo and Hiatt2021; Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023; Keenan, Culbert, Grimm, Hipwell, & Stepp, Reference Keenan, Culbert, Grimm, Hipwell and Stepp2014). The sample was also likely selected for psychological health both in the foundation sample (given criteria for adoptive parents and longitudinal study demands), and in the participants with full data for this study (excluded participants had more psychological problems than those included); this reduces the prevalence of psychological problems in this sample and underestimates potential links with pubertal variations. Further, missing data resulted in different subsamples for different outcomes and lower power to test mediation than direct effects. It also produced variations in standard errors around similar-sized associations and, therefore, levels of statistical significance. Overall, these sample limitations likely resulted in the underestimation of associations.

Third, we considered only some aspects of the long-term consequences of pubertal timing. We did not include all mediators likely to be important, such as sexual assault (Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Beltz, Carter and Dorn2019) and peer interactions (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023; Negriff & Susman, Reference Negriff and Susman2011); our measure of age at sexual initiation also could have been interpreted in different ways by participants (e.g. not only as intercourse). This reflected the nature of the sample (e.g. few adverse experiences), and our reliance on existing data using a strict criterion that the mediator be assessed between puberty and established adulthood. Due to the importance of having mediators occur temporally between pubertal timing and adult symptoms (O’Laughlin, Martin, & Ferrer, Reference O’Laughlin, Martin and Ferrer2018), we were unable to consider adolescent problems as a mediator, though it is likely that pubertal timing impacts adolescent psychosocial health, which could, in turn, influence later outcomes. We also examined a selected set of internalizing outcomes and did not consider other adult psychological outcomes, such as substance abuse or other adolescent externalizing problems also shown to be related to variations in pubertal timing (e.g. risk taking and antisocial behavior; Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023; Ullsperger & Nikolas, Reference Ullsperger and Nikolas2017). We only examined persisting associations with pubertal timing because we did not have data on potential mediators of other developmental changes, such as desisting links mediated by parental monitoring (Dorn & Beltz, Reference Dorn and Beltz2023).

Fourth, we could not identify whether early pubertal timing increased risk, late pubertal timing was protective, or both. Although we took a dimensional approach in assessing continuous pubertal timing links via linear relations with outcomes, this assumes a similar association exists across the range of mid-puberty ages; this is a perpetual issue (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt1991; Hoyt, Niu, Pachucki, & Chaku, Reference Hoyt, Niu, Pachucki and Chaku2020).

Furthermore, we did not correct for multiple comparisons, given the dearth of information about the long-term links between puberty, potential mediators, and adult internalizing symptoms. Although this may have increased Type I error, we opted to minimize Type II error in this novel investigation of multiple mediators and outcomes (Rothman, Reference Rothman1990). Confidence in our findings is increased by consistency of prediction across mediators and both recent and lifetime symptoms; nonetheless, our findings require replication.

Future directions

Our results converge with others to emphasize the long-term associations between pubertal timing and psychological health, while also highlighting important opportunities for future work. These opportunities include exploring how associations extend to other psychological problems, particularly externalizing problems, and other aspects of pubertal development (e.g. variations in tempo). Additionally, future work could examine whether these associations reflect continuity or developmental change in nature or size (e.g. emerging or persisting with potential augmentation or reduction, or desist) or in manifestation (e.g. shifting from adolescent depression to adult substance use). Such work may require unique assessments, including of potential mediators temporally close to puberty and using idiographic approaches (e.g. intensive and/or personalized questions and assessment schedules; Chaku & Beltz, Reference Chaku, Beltz, Gilmore and Lockman2022). Few studies will have the full or intensive longitudinal data ideal to address these questions. Fortunately, some aspects of pubertal development can be retrospectively assessed using a simple measure in which adults report their pubertal timing relative to their peers. This measure has been shown to provide a good approximation to timing measured contemporaneously, with similar links to adolescent behavior (Chaku et al., Reference Chaku, Berenbaum, Qian, Corley, Wadsworth, Reynolds and Beltz2024).

Conclusions

Our findings reveal that early puberty’s links with experiences of early sexual activity and poor feelings about one’s own body matter for mental health, especially for depression symptoms, in female established adults. We uniquely delineate how pubertal timing continues to influence psychological health decades later in sex-differentiated ways, using longitudinal data from four periods of the lifespan to extend other work to show that some adverse links may persist, and a mediating role of body dissatisfaction and age at sexual initiation in maintaining some links. Understanding these mechanisms linking pubertal variations to psychological health in adulthood may ultimately lead to interventions to reduce the risk for psychological health problems.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000820.

Acknowledgments

We thank Sally-Ann Rhea for overseeing data collection and management. We are grateful to the CATSLife participants whose investment in research made this important longitudinal investigation possible.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (grants AG046938, HD010333, and DA011015). HP was supported by the Penn State Prevention and Methodology Training Program, funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (T32 DA017629). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests exist.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist: 4–18 and 1991 profile. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Allen, D. N., & Becker, M. L. (2019). Diagnostic and symptom interviews for adults. In Gerald, Goldstein, Allen, Daniel N., John, DeLuca, Handbook of psychological assessment (pp. 355393). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-802203-0.00012-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baams, L., Dubas, J. S., Overbeek, G., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2015). Transitions in body and behavior: A meta-analytic study on the relationship between pubertal development and adolescent sexual behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health, 56(6), 586598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.11.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beeber, L., Seeherunwong, A., Schwartz, T., Funk, S., & Vongsirimas, N. (2007). Validity of the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale in young women from Thailand and the USA. Thai Journal of Nursing Research, 11, 240250.Google Scholar
Beltz, A. M. (2018). Gendered mechanisms underlie the relation between pubertal timing and adult depressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescent Health, 62(6), 722728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.12.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beltz, A. M., Corley, R. P., Bricker, J. B., Wadsworth, S. J., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2014). Modeling pubertal timing and tempo and examining links to behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 50(12), 27152726. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038096CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beltz, A. M., Corley, R. P., Wadsworth, S. J., Dilalla, L. F., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2020). Does puberty affect the development of behavior problems as a mediator, moderator, or unique predictor? Development and Psychopathology, 32(4), 14731485. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941900141XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, K. C., Peterson, C. B., Frazier, P., & Crow, S. J. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of the eating disorder examination and eating disorder examination-questionnaire: A systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45(3), 428438. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20931CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2008). Does adolescent self-esteem predict later life outcomes? A test of the causal role of self-esteem. Development and Psychopathology, 20(1), 319339. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bricker, J. B., Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., Wadsworth, S. J., Bryan, A., Timberlake, D. S., … DeFries, J. C. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on age at sexual initiation in the Colorado Adoption Project. Behavior Genetics, 36(6), 820832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-006-9079-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks-Gunn, J., Warren, M. P., Rosso, J., & Gargiulo, J. (1987). Validity of self-report measures of girls’ pubertal status. Child Development, 58(3), 829. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1991). Individual differences are accentuated during periods of social change: The sample case of girls at puberty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 157168. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, S. E., Riegle-Crumb, C., & Crosnoe, R. (2007). Puberty and the education of girls. Social Psychology Quarterly, 70(2), 186198. https://doi.org/10.1177/019027250707000207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaku, N., & Beltz, A. M. (2022). Using temporal network methods to reveal the idiographic nature of development. In Gilmore, R. O. & Lockman, J. J. (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (pp. 159190). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2021.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaku, N., Berenbaum, S. A., Qian, Y., Corley, R. P., Wadsworth, S. J., Reynolds, C. A., & Beltz, A. M. (2024). Pubertal timing in adolescence and adulthood: Relations among contemporaneous and retrospective measures. Developmental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001784CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chmielewski, M., Clark, L. A., Bagby, R. M., & Watson, D. (2015). Method matters: Understanding diagnostic reliability in DSM-IV and DSM-5. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 764769. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000069CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(3), 316336. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 11(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-11-126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deardorff, J., Hoyt, L. T., Carter, R., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2019). Next steps in puberty research: Broadening the lens toward understudied populations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(1), 133154. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12402CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deardorff, J., Marceau, K., Johnson, M., Reeves, J. W., Biro, F. M., Kubo, A., … Hiatt, R. A. (2021). Girls’ pubertal timing and tempo and mental health: A longitudinal examination in an ethnically diverse sample. Journal of Adolescent Health, 68(6), 11971203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.01.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (2015). Measures of self-esteem. In Boyle, Gregory J., Saklofske, Donald H., Gerald, Matthews, Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs (pp. 131157). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00006-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorn, L. D., & Beltz, A. M. (2023). Puberty: Foundations, findings, and the future. In APA handbook of adolescent and young adult development. (pp. 319). Washington: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000298-001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, P. D., Ritter, P. L., Dornbusch, S. M., Gross, R. T., & Merrill Carlsmith, J. (1985). The effects of pubertal timing on body image, school behavior, and deviance. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14(3), 227235. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02090320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eklund, M., Bäckström, M., & Hansson, L. (2018). Psychometric evaluation of the Swedish version of Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 72(5), 318324. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039488.2018.1457177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 920958. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2020). Multitrait-multimethod analyses of change of internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence: Predicting internalizing and externalizing DSM disorders in adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(4), 343354. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairburn, C. G., & Beglin, S. J. (1994). Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self-report questionnaire? The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16(4), 363370. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/78664153.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T.-A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(2), 173206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graber, J. A. (2013). Pubertal timing and the development of psychopathology in adolescence and beyond. Hormones and Behavior, 64(2), 262269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grower, P., Ward, L. M., & Beltz, A. M. (2019). Downstream consequences of pubertal timing for young women’s body beliefs. Journal of Adolescence, 72(1), 162166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hergenrather, K. C., Zeglin, R. J., McGuire-Kuletz, M., & Rhodes, S. D. (2015). Employment as a social determinant of health: A review of longitudinal studies exploring the relationship between employment status and mental health. Rehabilitation Research, Policy, and Education, 29(3), 261290. https://doi.org/10.1891/2168-6653.29.3.261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoyt, L. T., Niu, L., Pachucki, M. C., & Chaku, N. (2020). Timing of puberty in boys and girls: Implications for population health. SSM – Population Health, 10, 100549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, K., Culbert, K. M., Grimm, K. J., Hipwell, A. E., & Stepp, S. D. (2014). Timing and tempo: Exploring the complex association between pubertal development and depression in African American and European American girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(4), 725736. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivusilta, L., & Rimpelä, A. (2004). Pubertal timing and educational careers: A longitudinal study. Annals of Human Biology, 31(4), 446465. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460412331281719CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koopman-Verhoeff, M. E., Gredvig-Ardito, C., Barker, D. H., Saletin, J. M., & Carskadon, M. A. (2020). Classifying pubertal development using child and parent report: Comparing the Pubertal Development Scales to Tanner staging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 66(5), 597602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.11.308CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, W., & Chikritzhs, T. (2013). Errors in recall of age at first sex. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e72947. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072947CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marti, F. A., Pourat, N., Lee, C., & Zima, B. T. (2022). Overview of ten child mental health clinical outcome measures: Testing of psychometric properties with diverse client populations in the U.S. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research, 49(2), 197225. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10488-021-01157-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martín-Albo, J., Núñez, J. L., Navarro, J. G., & Grijalvo, F. (2007). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and validation in university students. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10(2), 458467. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600006727CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., Piccinin, A., Rhea, S. A., & Stallings, M. C. (2013). Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 314324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendle, J., Beltz, A. M., Carter, R., & Dorn, L. D. (2019). Understanding puberty and its measurement: Ideas for research in a new generation. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(1), 8295. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendle, J., Ryan, R. M., & McKone, K. M. P. (2019). Early menarche and internalizing and externalizing in adulthood: Explaining the persistence of effects. Journal of Adolescent Health, 65(5), 599606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.06.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Negriff, S., & Susman, E. J. (2011). Pubertal timing, depression, and externalizing problems: A framework, review, and examination of gender differences. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(3), 717746. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00708.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Laughlin, K. D., Martin, M. J., & Ferrer, E. (2018). Cross-sectional analysis of longitudinal mediation processes. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 53(3), 375402. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2018.1454822CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537962CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado Adoption Project. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Reardon, L. E., Leen-Feldner, E. W., & Hayward, C. (2009). A critical review of the empirical literature on the relation between anxiety and puberty. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, B. M., & Juvonen, J. (2012). Pubertal timing fluctuations across middle school: Implications for girls’ psychological health. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(6), 677690. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9687-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhea, S.-A., Gross, A. A., Haberstick, B. C., & Corley, R. P. (2006). Colorado Twin Registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(6), 941949. https://doi.org/10.1375/183242706779462895CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robins, L. N., Cottler, L. B., Bucholz, K. K., Compton, W. M., North, C. S., & Rourke, K. M. (1999). The diagnostic interview schedule for DSM-IV. St. Washington University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Croughan, J., & Ratcliff, K. S. (1981). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic interview schedule: Its history, characteristics, and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(4), 381. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780290015001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rockwood, N. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2017). MLmed: An SPSS macro for multilevel mediation and conditional process analysis. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association of Psychological Science (APS), Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES). https://doi.org/10.1037/t01038-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, K. J. (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology, 1(1). Retrieved from https://journals.lww.com/epidem/fulltext/1990/01000/no_adjustments_are_needed_for_multiple_comparisons.10.aspxCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sareen, J., Afifi, T. O., McMillan, K. A., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2011). Relationship between household income and mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(4), 419. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitz, K. E., Hovell, M. F., Nichols, J. F., Irvin, V. L., Keating, K., Simon, G. M., … Jones, K. L. (2004). A validation study of early adolescents’ pubertal self-assessments. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 24(4), 357384. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431604268531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharpe, H., Patalay, P., Choo, T.-H., Wall, M., Mason, S. M., Goldschmidt, A. B., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2018). Bidirectional associations between body dissatisfaction and depressive symptoms from adolescence through early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 14471458. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001663CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirtcliff, E. A., Dahl, R. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2009). Pubertal development: Correspondence between hormonal and physical development. Child Development, 80(2), 327337. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01263.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shope, M. M., Freeman, A. J., & Culbert, K. M. (2022). Elucidating early pubertal timing effects on disordered eating symptoms in young adult women. Eating Behaviors, 45, 101602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2022.101602CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, S. J., Blais, M. A., Gansler, D. A., Sandberg, E., Bistis, K., & LoCicero, A. (2010). Psychometric properties of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Overall and across demographic groups living within the United States. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 33(1), 5680. https://doi.org/10.1177/0163278709356187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spriggs, A. L., & Halpern, C. T. (2008). Sexual debut timing and depressive symptoms in emerging adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37(9), 10851096. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9303-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steiger, A. E., Allemand, M., Robins, R. W., & Fend, H. A. (2014). Low and decreasing self-esteem during adolescence predict adult depression two decades later. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(2), 325338. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stojković, I. (2013). Pubertal timing and self-esteem in adolescents: The mediating role of body-image and social relations. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(3), 359377. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.682145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thériault, J., Otis, J., Hébert, M., Gurreri, S., & Lambert, J. (2019). Exploring the mediating and moderating roles of body-related concerns and social interactions on the association between early puberty and psychological distress in young adult women. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 110(5), 606615. https://doi.org/10.17269/s41997-019-00213-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullsperger, J. M., & Nikolas, M. A. (2017). A meta-analytic review of the association between pubertal timing and psychopathology in adolescence: Are there sex differences in risk? Psychological Bulletin, 143(9), 903938. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Upchurch, D. M., Lillard, L. A., Aneshensel, C. S., & Li, N. F. (2002). Inconsistencies in reporting the occurrence and timing of first intercourse among adolescents. The Journal of Sex Research, 39(3), 197206. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490209552142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vannucci, A., & Ohannessian, C. M. (2018). Body image dissatisfaction and anxiety trajectories during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47(5), 785795. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1390755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadsworth, S. J., Corley, R. P., Munoz, E., Trubenstein, B. P., Knaap, E., DeFries, J. C., … Reynolds, C. A. (2019). CATSLife: A study of lifespan behavioral development and cognitive functioning. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22(6), 695706. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wesche, R., Kreager, D. A., Lefkowitz, E. S., & Siennick, S. E. (2017). Early sexual initiation and mental health: A fleeting association or enduring change? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(3), 611627. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics by sex

Figure 1

Figure 1. Multilevel models testing hypotheses of pubertal timing associations (X) with outcomes in established adulthood (Y) via mediators in emerging adulthood (M), and covarying behavior problems in adolescence (C). Note: (a) Model 1 assesses the links between pubertal timing and established adulthood outcomes (solid arrow). Model 2 incorporates an adolescent behavior covariate (dashed arrow). (b) Model 3 assesses emerging adulthood mediations of the associations pubertal timing and established adulthood outcome links (solid arrows). Model 4 adds an adolescent behavior covariate (dashed arrows). There was minor overlap in assessment periods due to scheduling constraints in the original studies, with 4% of participants having their adolescent internalizing problems covariate assessment overlap with their self-esteem mediator assessment. All four models controlled for age and initial study. MDD, ‘major depressive disorder’; GAD, ‘generalized anxiety disorder’.

Figure 2

Table 2. Multilevel model results for female participants: between-family associations between pubertal timing (X) and major depressive disorder symptoms in established adulthood (Y) via mediators in emerging adulthood (M), covarying adolescent internalizing problems (C), initial study (S), and age (A)

Figure 3

Table 3. Multilevel model results for female participants: between-family associations between pubertal timing (X) and generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in established adulthood (Y) via mediators in emerging adulthood (M), covarying adolescent internalizing problems (C), initial study (S), and age (A)

Figure 4

Figure 2. Summary of results for female participants: pubertal timing associations with MDD and GAD recent and lifetime outcomes and mechanisms, and controls for adolescent behavior. Note: Checkmarks indicate significant pubertal timing associations with the outcome (leftmost column). Xs indicate nonsignificant pubertal timing associations. All four models controlled for age and initial study. Covariate models (Models 2 and 4) also controlled for adolescent behavior. Only significant mediators are indicated under the ‘Persistence Mechanism’ and ‘Mechanism w/ Covariate’ columns. MDD, ‘major depressive disorder’; GAD, ‘generalized anxiety disorder’.

Supplementary material: File

Pham et al. supplementary material

Pham et al. supplementary material
Download Pham et al. supplementary material(File)
File 429.7 KB