Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-nt87m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-23T03:51:10.467Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Differential formal childcare uptake amongst migrants and their descendants in Europe and Australia: the role of socio-economic status, employment and work–family attitudes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 April 2025

Jonas Wood*
Affiliation:
University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The provision of formal childcare services holds significant potential benefits in addressing challenges posed by population ageing, labour shortages and welfare dependency. However, existing literature indicates persistent differentials in formal childcare uptake by migration background, with limited understanding of underlying demand-side factors. This study addresses this gap by comprehensively examining demographic, socio-economic, employment-related and attitudinal characteristics as potential explanations for these disparities. Utilising data from the Generations and Gender Survey across seven high-income countries, our findings reveal that whereas differentials for migrants’ descendants are limited and insignificant even without controlling for background variables, the negative differential for migrants disappears almost completely. Socio-economic status and particularly employment potential emerge as a key explanatory factors alongside differential attitudes towards maternal employment, which seem to play a role in some contexts, yet not in others. Cross-country differences in the results are discussed in the face of socio-economic gradients in formal childcare uptake, migrant-native gaps in the labour market and below-demand supply of formal childcare, yet also plead for future research interacting demand- and supply-side factors for a larger set of countries. In conclusion, this study reveals the intricate interplay of demographic, socio-economic and attitudinal factors underlying migrant–native disparities in formal childcare uptake.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In an era of population ageing, labour shortages and high pension costs, the availability of formal childcare is a policy offering multiple potential benefits. Formal childcare services support parental employment, thereby reducing welfare dependency and poverty risks (Troger and Verwiebe, Reference Troger and Verwiebe2015; Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018). Furthermore, formal childcare may stimulate household gender equality, realisation of fertility intentions and improve satisfaction with family life (Isaksen and Bikova, Reference Isaksen and Bikova2019; Schober and Schmitt, Reference Schober and Schmitt2017). For young children, formal childcare is believed to promote cognitive development, fostering educational and economic performance (Havnes and Mogstad, Reference Havnes and Mogstad2011; Hansen and Hawkes, Reference Hansen and Hawkes2009; Felfe and Lalive, Reference Felfe and Lalive2012; Leseman and Slot, Reference Leseman and Slot2014; Burger, Reference Burger2010). Additionally, formal childcare may alleviate pressure on informal childcare providers, often grandparents, further supporting employment.

For such effects to materialise, formal childcare must reach children in various population sub-groups. However, previous studies document strong socio-economic differences in formal childcare uptake (e.g. by income or education) in high-income countries, with some Nordic exceptions (Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Abrassart and Bonoli, Reference Abrassart and Bonoli2015; Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2016; Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2012; Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Van Lancker, Reference Van Lancker2017; Van Lancker, Reference Van Lancker2013; Ghysels and Van Lancker, Reference Ghysels and Van Lancker2011; Mamolo et al., Reference Mamolo, Coppola and Di Cesare2011; Meagher and Szebehely, Reference Meagher, Szebehely, Kvist, Fritzell and Hvinden2012; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005; Greenberg, Reference Greenberg2011; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023). Unlike a longer tradition of studying ethnic differences in formal childcare usage in the USA (e.g. Fuller et al., Reference Fuller, Holloway and Liang1996; Greenberg and Hofferth, Reference Hofferth1994; Greenberg and Kahn, Reference Greenberg and Kahn2012; Liang et al., Reference Liang, Fuller and Singer2000; West et al., Reference West, Wright and Germino-Hausken1995), our understanding of differential childcare uptake by migration background is more recent in other regions. Despite exceptions (e.g. higher daycare use by migrant mothers in Italy [Mussino and Ortensi, Reference Mussino and Ortensi2023]), a handful of studies generally find lower usage amongst groups with a migration background (e.g. Eremenko and Unterreiner, Reference Eremenko and Unterreiner2023; Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021).

Research examining sub-group disparities in formal childcare uptake identifies both macro-level supply-side and micro-level demand-side factors as complementary explanations. Macro-level analyses compare countries, regions or time periods to address supply-side factors such as coverage (Maes et al., Reference Maes, Neels, Biegel and Wood2023b), affordability and fee structures (Abrassart and Bonoli, Reference Abrassart and Bonoli2015; Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Huston et al., Reference Huston, Chang and Gennetian2002; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005), accessibility (Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018), priority criteria (European Commission, 2014), government spending (Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2016; Van Lancker, Reference Van Lancker2017) and policy coherence (Krapf, Reference Krapf2014). Additionally, a significant body of literature examines micro-level factors influencing formal childcare uptake, potentially serving as demand-side explanations for sub-group differentials (Mamolo et al., Reference Mamolo, Coppola and Di Cesare2011; van Gameren and Ooms, Reference van Gameren and Ooms2009; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005; Huston et al., Reference Huston, Chang and Gennetian2002; Greenberg, Reference Greenberg2011).

Unfortunately, hitherto it is largely unclear whether differential formal childcare usage by migration background can be explained by demand-side factors, such as household characteristics (e.g. cohabiting grandparents, residential location) (Burchinal et al., Reference Burchinal, Nelson and Carlson2008; Vandenbroeck et al., Reference Vandenbroeck, De Visscher and Van Nuffel2008), socio-economic status (e.g. income, housing) (Sprong and Skopek, Reference Sprong and Skopek2023), labour market positions (e.g. employment) (Bonizzoni, Reference Bonizzoni2014; Maes et al., Reference Maes, Wood and Neels2023a; Wall and Jose, Reference Wall and Sao Jose2004; Sprong and Skopek, Reference Sprong and Skopek2023) or attitudes (Seibel and Hedegaard, Reference Seibel and Hedegaard2017). The literature is lacking empirical tests of many demand-side explanations, studies considering the aforementioned explanations simultaneously and comparisons between countries. Consequently, this study addresses the question: To what extent can differences in formal childcare uptake between natives without a migration background, migrants and migrants’ descendants be explained by household characteristics, socio-economic status, employment and work–family attitudes in Europe and Australia?

This study contributes to the existing literature in four ways. First, we offer a relatively comprehensive examination of potential demand-side explanations, including demographic, socio-economic, employment-related and attitudinal characteristics. Whilst traditional attitudes towards work–family and formal childcare correlate negatively with formal childcare uptake (Steiber and Haas, Reference Steiber and Haas2012; van Gameren and Ooms, Reference van Gameren and Ooms2009; Fortin, Reference Fortin2005), their role as explanations for differential formal childcare uptake by migration background remains unknown. Second, we assess differential uptake between migrants and their descendants, as our understanding of such intergenerational differences remains limited. Third, some demand-side determinants of formal childcare uptake may also be consequences of formal childcare uptake (e.g. parental employment). To address this, our study estimates employment potential and predicted pre-parenthood attitudes towards maternal employment, which are unaffected by childcare uptake, as potential determinants of formal childcare uptake (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023). Fourth, previous research on the extent to which demand-side predictors explain differences in formal childcare uptake has produced inconclusive findings, which may be attributed to not only contextual differences, but also varying research designs. To address this, our study examines formal childcare uptake by migration background in six European countries (Austria, Belgium, France, Germany, Russia, Sweden) and Australia, using comparable data and a fixed research design.

Background

Potential demand-side explanations for gaps in formal childcare uptake

On the basis of micro-economic theory emphasising trade-offs between childcare costs and potential gains concerning income and children’s development (Blau and Currie, Reference Blau and Currie2006), but also ideational theories highlighting the role of internalised preferences and socialisation (Steiber and Haas, 2012; van Gameren and Ooms, Reference van Gameren and Ooms2009; Fortin, Reference Fortin2005; Goerres and Tepe, Reference Goerres and Tepe2012), we discuss four demand-side explanations for differential formal childcare uptake by migration background.

First, differential formal childcare uptake may reflect demographic characteristics. The number of children in the household potentially affects the probability of formal childcare uptake, as grandparents, for instance, are presumably less able to care for multiple grandchildren. Previous research also interprets higher formal childcare uptake for one- or two-year-olds in comparison with younger children in terms of preferences for parental care for young babies and parental leave uptake (Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Huston et al., Reference Huston, Chang and Gennetian2002; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005; Morgan and Zippel, Reference Morgan and Zippel2003). Demographic variation in formal childcare uptake by age and location (e.g. urban–rural differences) are also repeatedly identified (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023; Abrassart and Bonoli, 2015; Pennerstorfer and Pennerstorfer, Reference Pennerstorfer and Pennerstorfer2021; Yerkes and Javornik, Reference Yerkes and Javornik2019). At the household level, co-resident grandparents are often considered a source of informal care, which might function as a substitute for formal childcare (Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023; Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021). The aforementioned demographic determinants are likely to play a role as partial explanations for differential uptake of formal childcare by migration background. Migrants in our data are more likely to have more children and live in urban areas with higher unmet need for childcare. Furthermore, migrants’ descendants are more likely to live in multi-generation households with potential availability of informal childcare, which contrasts with the low prevalence of multi-generation households in groups without migration background or migrants. As a result of such compositional differences between the three groups considered, and the likely impact on formal childcare uptake, we expect that differential formal childcare use by migration background can be partly explained by these demographic characteristics (hypothesis 1).

Second, the available literature provides numerous empirical indications for differential formal childcare uptake by socio-economic status (e.g. education, wealth) (Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Abrassart and Bonoli, Reference Abrassart and Bonoli2015; Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2016; Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2012; Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Van Lancker, Reference Van Lancker2017; Van Lancker, Reference Van Lancker2013; Ghysels and Van Lancker, Reference Ghysels and Van Lancker2011; Mamolo et al., Reference Mamolo, Coppola and Di Cesare2011; Meagher and Szebehely, Reference Meagher, Szebehely, Kvist, Fritzell and Hvinden2012; Kreyenfeld et al., 2003; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005; Greenberg, Reference Greenberg2011; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023). Positive effects of educational attainment are interpreted in terms of employment opportunities which already accumulate in early careers (Maes et al. 2021; Wood et al. Reference Wood, Neels, Kil and De Wachter2016; Maes et al., Reference Maes, Wood and Neels2019), or more positive attitudes towards outsourcing of childcare. In addition, household wealth impacts the affordability of childcare uptake (Del Boca and Vuri, Reference Del Boca and Vuri2007). Given that most migrant groups, and to a lesser degree their descendants, are lower educated and less wealthy than natives without a migration background, we expect that differential formal childcare use by migration background can be partly explained by socio-economic status (hypothesis 2).

Third, favourable employment opportunities entail high opportunity costs of taking care of young children (e.g. forgone wages) and enhance the affordability of formal childcare (Abrassart and Bonoli, Reference Abrassart and Bonoli2015; Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018), whilst unstable precarious employment hinders planning of future care needs and restricts access to formal childcare (e.g. waiting lists). Migrants are on average more prone to unemployment or precarious employment characterised by flexible schedules, non-standard hours and short-term contracts, and research has underscored the challenges to accessing formal childcare faced by migrants in such situations (Bonizzoni, Reference Bonizzoni2014; Wall and Jose, Reference Wall and Sao Jose2004; Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021). As a result, we expect that differential formal childcare use by migration background can be partly explained by employment potential (hypothesis 3).

Fourth, differences in formal childcare uptake between migrants and natives may be partially attributed to attitudes. Available literature indicates that traditional views on maternal employment and formal childcare services are associated with lower childcare uptake (Steiber and Haas, 2012; van Gameren and Ooms, Reference van Gameren and Ooms2009; Fortin, Reference Fortin2005) and also provides some indications of more traditional attitudes amongst migrants (Seibel and Hedegaard, Reference Seibel and Hedegaard2017), though evidence seems inconclusive with strong heterogeneity within migrant communities (De Valk, Reference de Valk2008; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Van den Berg, Neels, Timmerman, Fadil and Goddeeris2017; Wood, Reference Wood2022). Given the theoretical possibility of differential attitudes, we hypothesise that differential formal childcare uptake by migration background can be partly accounted for by attitudes towards maternal employment (hypothesis 4).

Cross-country differences

The degree of differential formal childcare uptake by migration background is likely to vary by country due to compositional and supply-side differences. First, the group composition of migrants and their descendants with young children varies between countries in terms of reason of migration (e.g. labour, education, family, humanitarian) and country of origin due to differential migration histories and migration policies. Additionally, cross-country variation in differential formal childcare uptake by migration background might also be related to variation in socio-economic compositional differences. Available literature indicates the largest foreign-native born gaps in employment in Western and Northern European countries (e.g. Belgium, Germany, France, Sweden), and more limited gaps in Anglo-Saxon countries (e.g. Australia) and Central and Eastern European countries (e.g. Russia) (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Rendall, Rabinovich, Tsang, van Oranje-Nassau and Janta2008; OECD, 2020). Second, differential uptake of formal childcare by migration background might also depend on supply-side factors. The extent to which lower socio-economic position and employment potential drive differential patterns of uptake by migration background potentially depends on the supply level which has been shown to affect social inclusiveness in uptake (Maes et al., Reference Maes, Neels, Biegel and Wood2023b). Supply meets demand in Nordic countries, yet not in other European countries or Australia (European Commission et al., 2014).

An assessment of all potential underlying explanations for cross-country differentiations in formal childcare uptake by migration background lies beyond the scope of this study, as low cell frequencies hamper the assessment of compositional features, such as origin country and reason of migration, and the number of countries included is too low to explain variation at the country-level reliably using contextual indicators. However, benefitting from the fact that this study is novel in using a standardised approach to estimate differential formal childcare uptake by migration background, we expect that differentials in the uptake of formal childcare by migration background vary depending on the country considered (hypothesis 5).

Data and method

Data

We use data from the Generations and Gender Survey (GGS), a multi-purpose cross-country survey under the Generations and Gender Programme (GGP), for seven countries: Australia (2005-2006), Austria (2008–2009), Belgium (2008–2010), France (2005), Germany (2005), Russia (2004) and Sweden (2012–2013). The survey employed face-to-face standardised interviews with rigorous random probability sampling procedures, targeting non-institutionalised residents aged 18–79 years. Response rates were comparable to other international surveys (Fokkema et al., Reference Fokkema, Kveder and Hiekel2016). Focussing on individuals residing with at least one biological child under the age of 3 years, and excluding participants with missing data on covariates (518 individuals), a sample of 4,056 parents was obtained. We use sample design weights to restore potential error (e.g. due to sampling designs or selective response patterns), and additional weights to ensure that each country sample equals one-seventh of the total sample.

Due to varying immigration histories, the seven countries exhibit different minority groups in the analytical sample. In the Belgian and French samples, groups with a migration background include 68 per cent and 74 per cent of non-European origin, respectively, many of which originate from North-African or Turkish post-Second World War labour migration. The German and Austrian samples include, respectively, 45 per cent and 42 per cent non-European origin groups, a non-EU European origin group (e.g. Russian Federation) in Germany, and 66 per cent of all migrants originating from Turkey, Germany, Bosnia and Herzegovina, Romania, Serbia, Kosovo and Croatia in Austria. The amount of migrants who have been residing in the host country for more than 15 years is 48 per cent in France and Austria, and 25 per cent in Germany and Belgium. As Sweden witnessed post-Second World War immigration from Nordic countries, Germany and the Baltics, and later immigration peaks related to conflicts and humanitarian crises in the Balkan, West Asia or Africa, the sample with a migration background includes 46 per cent non-European origin groups, the majority of which originates from North Africa and West Asia. A total of 41 per cent of the migrants in the Swedish analytical sample were already residing in Sweden for 15 years or longer. Since Russia experienced immigration former soviet states from the 1990s onwards, 75 per cent of the sample of migrants and their descendants originate from Ukraine, Kazakhstan, Belarus, Azerbaijan and Moldova, with 35 per cent of migrants residing in the country for 15 years or longer. Finally, in line with Australian migration history including large Asian groups (e.g. Chinese origin), and immigrants from the commonwealth or Europe, 56 per cent of the migrants in the Australian sample are of European descent, and 27 per cent originate from Asian countries. The Australian analytical sample stands out, with 60 per cent of migrants residing in the country for 15 years or longer.

The GGS data allow us to distinguish natives from first-generation migrants and their descendants (see Table A1 in appendix for sample sizes). Unfortunately, low cell counts do not allow for the examination of migrant origin and generation simultaneously. However, we do perform additional robustness checks distinguishing natives without a migration background, migrants and their descendants from higher-income and lower-income countries or European versus non-European countries, without considering intergenerational differences between migrants and their descendants.

Method

The dependent variable of interest is the uptake of formal childcare, using the question, ‘Do you get regular help with childcare from a day care centre, a nursery or pre-school, an afterschool care-centre, a self-organised childcare group, a babysitter, or from some other institutional or paid arrangement?’ Although capturing a broader spectrum of practices than commonly used definitions of early childhood education and care services (ECEC), it complements standard definitions (e.g. Eurostat [EU-SILC]) of informal care by emphasising payment (OECD, 2019). The percentage of respondents reporting uptake of formal childcare in our analytical sample of individuals residing with at least one biological child under 3 years indicates that Belgium and France typically approximate the high levels of childcare coverage in Nordic countries during the 2000s, in line with the available literature. The percentage is the highest in Sweden (69 per cent), France (54 per cent) and Belgium (51 per cent). Austria, Germany and Australia exhibit lower levels, at 38–39 per cent, and the lowest percentage is found in Russia (29 per cent). Although these percentages, unlike official statistics, cannot be interpreted as reliable estimates due to differences in definition, self-reporting and sampling variation, this is not considered problematic in this study which addresses within-country differences by migration background, rather than cross-national differences in overall uptake. There are no indications that the aforementioned sources of bias might function differentially by migration background.

We employ a country fixed-effects logit model to address differential formal childcare use by migration background within countries. This method incorporates country dummies as fixed effects into the regression model, allowing for within-country interpretations of covariate effects and reducing bias in estimated predictor effects resulting from different distributions across countries. The null model includes country fixed effects, migration background (natives without a migration background, migrants, migrants’ descendants) and interactions between country and migration background. Using effects coding for country dummies implies that the main effect of migration background is the grand mean of the country-specific differentials. Additionally, the null model also includes sex, as the sampling design targeted both male and female respondents and available literature documents sex differences in reporting of topics related to (un)paid work (e.g. Press and Townsley, Reference Press and Townsley1998).

We compare five nested regression models with the null model to evaluate whether differential childcare uptake by migration background alters when various potential statistical explanations are considered. As logit model covariate estimates are incomparable across nested models due to unobserved heterogeneity, we calculate average marginal effects (Mood, Reference Mood2009). First, model 1 controls for demographic background variables known to influence the demand for formal childcare (Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Huston et al., Reference Huston, Chang and Gennetian2002; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005). With respect to children’s characteristics, we control for the number of resident children (one, two, three, four or more) and the age of the youngest child (0, 1 or 2 years). At the parental level, we control for age (linear and square). At the household level, we control for presence of co-resident grandparents, residency in an urban and/or capital region and type of household (distinguishing married couples, from unmarried couples and single parent households).

Second, model 2 estimates differential formal childcare use by migration background, incorporating socio-economic status indicators. We distinguish low education (ISCED 0–2) from medium levels (ISCED 3–4) and higher tertiary degrees (ISCED 5–6). In addition, to capture wealth which might affect the affordability of formal childcare, we use commonly adopted proxy variables for wealth (e.g. Gadeyne, Reference Gadeyne2006): home ownership and the amount of rooms relative to the household size.

Model 3 tests the explanatory power of employment potential for gaps in formal childcare uptake by migration background. We follow previous research (Maes et al. Reference Maes, Wood and Neels2023a; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023) and estimate ‘employment potential’ representing respondents’ employment probability if childless, which cannot be affected by formal childcare uptake (see appendix for equations). Using a sample of 19,552 childless respondents, two logistic regressions estimate employment probabilities of male and female respondents separately as a function of (interactions between) country, subnational region, age, educational attainment (low, medium, high), migration background (native, first generation, second generation), number of siblings (1, 2, 3, 4 or more), whether respondents lived with their parents until the age of 15 years and educational attainment of respondents’ father and mother (low, medium, high, unknown). Similarly, using a sample of 16,205 childless respondents with a cohabiting partner, two logistic regressions are performed to estimate the employment probabilities of male and female respondents’ partners. Subsequently, matching for the covariates included in the models for employment potential, the estimated probabilities for childless individuals are attributed to their counterparts with children as a measure of their employment potential.

Model 4 estimates gradients in formal childcare uptake, controlling for attitudes towards maternal employment, measured with the statement ‘A pre-school child is likely to suffer if his or her mother works’, with response options ranging from ‘strongly agree to ‘strongly disagree’ in a 5-point Likert scale. This question has also been used in previous research (Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023). Given evidence that working mothers who use formal childcare develop more positive attitudes over time (Steiber and Haas, 2012), we use predicted childless attitudes. These attitudes are estimated separately for men and women for the aforementioned childless sample as a function of the same vector of indicators as the estimation of respondents’ employment potential. The estimated probabilities for childless individuals are attributed to their counterparts with children. Consequently, predicted attitudes are likely to reflect an individual’s attitudes towards maternal employment, net of any adjustments made after childbearing and (non-)uptake of formal childcare.

Finally, the full model (model 5) includes demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes.

Results

Differential formal childcare use and demand-side characteristics

Figure 1 illustrates deviations in the probability of formal childcare uptake amongst migrants and their descendants, in comparison with natives without a migration background. As the average probability of formal childcare uptake varies across countries, the average marginal effects of migration background are expressed as a percentage of the mean uptake per country. A corresponding figure with absolute deviations is provided in the appendix (see Figure A1).

Figure 1. Relative deviations in the probability of formal childcare uptake (i.e. average marginal effects/average probability by country) amongst first-generation migrants (1.1) and second-generation migrants (1.2) in comparison with natives.

Source: Generations & Gender Survey (GGS) wave 1, calculations by authors M0 (null model); M1 (demographic control variables); M2 (controlling for socio-economic status); M3 (controlling for employment potential); M4 (controlling for predicted attitudes towards maternal employment); M5 (full model controlling for all covariates).

On average across all countries, the null model indicates a significantly negative differential for first-generation migrants (Figure 1.1). The mean gap equals 20 per cent of the average probability of uptake in a country. However, this differential is not significant in Russia, Austria and Australia. Sweden stands out with a significantly higher probability of formal childcare uptake for first-generation migrants. The largest negative differentials occur in Germany and Belgium, exhibiting a gap between natives without a migration background and first-generation migrants amounting to 51 per cent and 44 per cent, respectively, of the mean probability of uptake.

With respect to migrants’ descendants (Figure 1.2), estimations indicate that there is much less consistency in the direction of the differential, but also that the magnitude of the deviations is smaller. For all countries but Belgium, the deviation does not reach statistical significance. The Belgian sample, however, indicates a negative differential in formal childcare uptake for migrants’ descendants similar to the results for migrants. Although comparing these results with previous studies using different indicators is cumbersome, it is noteworthy that the relative positioning of the countries aligns with studies focussing on socio-economic differentiation, with relatively strong differentials in Belgium and France; weaker gradients in Germany and Austria; and the weakest to virtually non-existent gradients in Sweden (Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Ghysels and Van Lancker, Reference Ghysels and Van Lancker2011; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023).

Micro-level factors as explanation for differential formal childcare uptake

This section presents the results of multivariate regression models taking into account demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment. With respect to demographic characteristics, Table 1 indicates that – when using linear regression models to estimate gaps in demographic composition controlling for between-country differences – first-generation migrants are on average interviewed slightly later in calendar time, but also have more resident children and are more likely to reside in highly urban and/or capital regions. When comparing migrants’ descendants to natives, the only statistically significant difference is that migrants’ descendants are more likely to live in highly urban and/or capital regions. Controlling for a set of demographic characteristics (Figure 1) – on average across countries – entails a weakening of the negative differential formal childcare uptake from 20.4 per cent to 18.8 per cent of the average probability of uptake for first-generation migrants, and from 8.7 per cent to 5.5 per cent for migrants’ descendants. The degree to which differential uptake of formal childcare by migration background can be accounted for by these demographic characteristics varies markedly between countries. With respect to first-generation migrants, controlling for demographic composition entails a weakening of the insignificant differential in Russia, and the significant negative differential in Belgium, Sweden and France, with the latter turning statistically insignificant. In contrast, results for Germany, Austria and Australia indicate stable or even increasing differentials after controlling for demographic composition. With respect to migrants’ descendants, the aforementioned limited differentials in most countries remain small, whereas the relatively large negative differential in Belgium largely persists after controlling for demographic characteristics. Regression estimates provided in Table 2 identify age of the youngest child and number of resident children as significant predictors of formal childcare uptake, with markedly higher childcare uptake amongst two- and three-child parents.

Table 1. Migrant native differentials in demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment, effects from fixed-effects linear regression models, 7 GGS countries 2004–2013

Notes: As a result of the usage of effects coding for country fixed effects, the main effects of education can be interpreted as the average effect across countries. Significance levels: *p < 0.050, **p < 0.010, ***p < 0.001. Source: GGS wave 1, calculations by authors.

Table 2. Average marginal effects from country fixed-effects logit models of couples’ formal childcare uptake, fifteen GGS countries, 2002–2013

Notes: As a result of the usage of effects coding for country fixed effects, the main effects of education can be interpreted as the average effect across countries. Significance levels: *p < 0.050, **p < 0.010, ***p < 0.001 Source: GGS wave 1, calculations by authors.

Regarding socio-economic status, Table 1 presents that groups with a migration background, and particularly first-generation migrants, exhibit lower socio-economic status. First-generation migrant groups are on average more likely to be lower educated. This negative differential in terms of educational attainment partly extends over to migrants’ descendants, who are also less likely to be highly educated in comparison with natives without a migration background. Additionally, both migrants and their descendants exhibit a significantly lower probability of home ownership and less rooms per capita. The multivariate regression results of model 2 (Table 2) indicate that highly educated parents are more likely to use formal childcare, and that homeowners also exhibit higher probabilities of formal childcare uptake, in addition to an unexpected negative association between formal childcare use and rooms relative to household size. The latter association can be explained by the impact of the number of children on household size, an interpretation which is supported by the fact that this association disappears when controlling for number of children in the full model (model 5). Regarding differential formal childcare usage (Figure 1), findings show that variation in socio-economic status accounts for a considerable part of the gaps in formal childcare uptake. Compared with the null model, the differential uptake for first-generation migrants shifts from 20.4 per cent to 13.6 per cent when controlling for level of education and housing characteristics, whereas the corresponding gap for migrants’ descendants shifts from 8.7 per cent to 3.8 per cent. This average finding masks variation between countries. The clear explanatory power in reducing or even reversing negative differentials in France and Belgium, and to a lesser degree in Australia and Sweden, contrasts with little-to-no explanatory power of socio-economic status in Russia, Germany and Austria.

With respect to employment potential, migrants’ descendants and particularly migrants themselves display lower estimated employment potential (Table 1), in line with available literature (OECD, 2020). Model estimates indicate positive associations formal childcare uptake and employment potential, particularly the employment potential of female respondents or female partners (Table 2, model 3), but also that migrant–native gaps decrease when taking the effect of employment potential into account (Figure 1). Compared with the null model, the negative differential in formal childcare uptake for migrants and their descendants shifts from 20.4 per cent to 7.2 per cent and from 8.7 per cent to 3.1 per cent, respectively, when controlling for employment potential. It is noteworthy that, when focussing on the average pattern across countries, the model controlling for employment potential is the only model (except the full model) in which differential formal childcare uptake for migrants vis-à-vis natives without a migration background is no longer statistically significant. However, the extent to which differentials in formal childcare uptake can be explained by differences in employment potential also varies strongly by country. In most countries the explanatory value of employment potential as explanation for differential formal childcare uptake by migration background is similar or even stronger than socio-economic status, such as is the case in Russia, Germany, Belgium, Australia, Sweden and migrants’ descendants in Austria, whilst the explanatory value of employment potential is weaker yet still notable for France. The gap between natives without a migration background and first-generation migrants in Austria even reverses from a negative gap to a positive difference when controlling for employment potential.

Regarding attitudes, migrants’ descendants and particularly migrants themselves on average exhibit significantly lower predicted scores on a seven-point scale of progressive attitudes towards maternal employment (Table 1). The multivariate regression results of model 4 (Table 2) indicate that predicted progressive attitudes towards maternal employment associate positively with formal childcare uptake. Estimates of formal childcare uptake by migration background (Figure 1) indicate that the average degree of differential formal childcare uptake across countries decreases when comparing the null model with a model controlling for predicted attitudes towards maternal employment. For migrants the gap decreases from 20.4 per cent to 10.1 per cent and for migrants’ descendants it decreases from 8.7 per cent to 2.6 per cent. However, the differential uptake amongst migrants remains statistically significant. With respect to cross-country variation in the explanatory power of attitudes, the reduction of negative differentials (or strengthening of positive differentials) in uptake is similar or weaker in comparison with socio-economic position and/or employment potential in Russia, France, Austria, Australia and Sweden. The results for migrants and their descendants in Belgium and migrants in Germany stand out, as the weakening of the differential is larger than for all other aforementioned models in these two countries.

When combining demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment in one multivariate model (model 5), findings (Table 2) indicate higher formal childcare uptake amongst single parents, parents with more than one child, parents with a slightly older under-three year old in the household, homeowner parents and parents with a higher employment potential. Additionally, Figure 1 illustrates the remaining differentials in formal childcare usage by migration background. On average across all seven countries, uptake gaps decrease from 20.4 to 2.0 per cent lower uptake amongst first generations migrants and 8.7 per cent lower to 6.4 per cent higher uptake amongst migrants’ descendants. Hence, the already limited negative differential for migrants’ descendants has been reversed, whereas only a marginal negative gap for first-generation migrants persists after controlling for the combined explanatory power of demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment.

However, country-specific results indicate cross-country variation in the persistence of differential formal childcare usage by migration background. Differential uptake for migrants and descendants in Australia and migrants in Russia seem resistant to our control variables. Results for Germany show that the negative differential for migrants is halved, whereas the original negative differential for migrants’ descendants is reversed. In France the negative differential for migrants is mostly explained. In Austria we find a reversal of the gap for migrants, and strengthening of a positive differential for their descendants. In Belgium the original gaps without controls are approximately triple the size of the remaining gaps after including all control variables. Finally, results for Sweden indicate that the negative differential in formal childcare uptake for migrants is almost completely explained, whereas the moderate negative gap for their descendants is reversed when including all control variables.

Sensitivity analyses

To test the robustness of our findings, we ran five (groups of) sensitivity checks. First, the main analyses include both educational attainment and housing characteristics as indicators for socio-economic status. However, spending power and the ability to purchase a (large) home could be endogenous to formal childcare uptake (via employment, wages and access to credit). Results of model 2 without housing characteristics (see Figure A2, model 2, excl. H, in appendix) indicate slightly stronger migrant–native gaps in formal childcare uptake compared with the model including housing factors for migrants (−13.6 versus −16.9 per cent) and their descendants (−3.8 versus −5.9 per cent).

Second, as it is theoretically possible that lower formal childcare amongst migrants and their descendants results from using informal childcare and/or parental leave, we ran models including a dummy indicator for the informal childcare use (see Figure A2, model 6 [INF] in appendix) and a model including a dummy indicator for parental leave uptake (see Figure A2, model 7 [LEAVE] in appendix). On average across countries, but also within most countries, these indicators seem to explain a very large part of negative differentials in formal childcare uptake, and also drive reversals of differentials. However, as the use of informal childcare and parental leave are likely to be endogenous, that is, the result of (non-)take up of formal childcare, these control variables are not used in the main analyses.

Third, as the GGS for Belgium, France, Russia, Austria and Sweden also includes the question (five-point Likert scale) whether care for pre-school children is a task for society, both or the family, we assessed the explanatory power of this indicator (see Figure A2, model 8 in appendix). On average across countries, this indicator explained a share of differential uptake for migrants similar to the model including predicted attitudes to maternal employment, yet the explanatory power is more limited for Belgium and drives an even stronger reversal to a positive gradient in Sweden.

Fourth, we re-ran all models distinguishing natives without a migration background from European country origin groups, and non-European origin groups, without comparing generations, which is not possible due to low cell frequencies. The results (see Figure A3 in appendix) in terms of statistical explanation of demographic composition, socio-economic status, employment potential and work–family attitudes are similar to the main results. Analyses distinguishing high-income from lower-income origin countries similarly do not alter the main findings.

Fifth, several variations of the estimation procedure for employment potential were tested. We omitted migration background from the equations for employment potential. In addition, instead of the main analyses in which single parents are assigned the employment probability of a partner on the basis of similar profiles of childless individuals with a partner, we also re-ran the models using an indicator of partners’ employment potential, which is set to zero for all single parents. Additionally, we ran all models using an estimation of employment potential at the household level, modelling whether any present partner is not working. None of these variations alter the main results of the study.

Discussion

Notwithstanding a relative scarcity of empirical research on formal childcare uptake by migration background in comparison with the large body of research addressing gradients in uptake by income or level of education, a handful of studies report lower uptake rates for migrants and their descendants in comparison with natives (Maes et al., Reference Maes, Neels, Biegel and Wood2023b; Eremenko and Unterreiner, Reference Eremenko and Unterreiner2023; Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021; Sprong and Skopek Reference Sprong and Skopek2023), similarly to ethnic gaps in the uptake of parental leave in many high-income countries (Kil et al., Reference Kil, Wood and Neels2018; Marynissen et al. Reference Marynissen, Wood and Neels2021). Given the numerous potential benefits of formal childcare enrolment for parents, children and society as a whole, this pattern is cause for concern amongst policy-makers and social policy scholars. As the available literature provides little information on potential explanations for differential formal childcare uptake by migration background, this study assesses the statistical explanatory power of four groups of demand-side characteristics: demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment. Using comparable data for seven countries, this study reports negative differentials in formal childcare uptake for first-generation migrant groups except in Sweden, which resonates with previous cross-national research on socio-economic differentiation in formal childcare uptake, finding the weakest to virtually non-existent gradients in Sweden (Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Ghysels and Van Lancker, Reference Ghysels and Van Lancker2011; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023). Migrants’ descendants exhibit relatively weak differential formal childcare uptake compared with natives without a migration background, despite strong negative gradients in Belgium, in line with previous research (Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021).

There are three main lessons to be drawn from this study. First, the overarching question is: Can differentials in formal childcare uptake by migration background be explained by the observed demand-side factors? Our findings indicate that – whereas differentials for migrants’ descendants are limited and insignificant even without controlling for background variables – the average negative differential for migrants across all countries disappears almost completely and is no longer statistically significant after controlling for demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment.

Second, there is no clear dominant demand-side statistical explanation for differential formal childcare use by migration background. Overall, demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and attitudes towards maternal employment collectively contribute to understanding lower formal childcare uptake amongst migrants’ descendants and particularly migrants themselves. Controlling for demographic composition reduces the gap in formal childcare usage, particularly amongst first-generation migrants (hypothesis 1). Socio-economic status also plays a significant role, as migrants generally exhibit lower educational attainment and homeownership rates, and highly educated parents and homeowners are found more likely to use formal childcare (hypothesis 2), in line with previous research (Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023). Contributing to the existing literature on the linkage between employment and formal childcare uptake (Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2012; Van Lancker, Reference Van Lancker2013; Krapf, Reference Krapf2014; Abrassart and Bonoli, Reference Abrassart and Bonoli2015; Hirshberg et al., Reference Hirshberg, Huang and Fuller2005; Mamolo et al., Reference Mamolo, Coppola and Di Cesare2011; Greenberg, Reference Greenberg2011), we also show that the employment potential of migrants and their descendants also influences formal childcare uptake, with lower predicted employment probabilities associated with lower uptake. On average across countries, the explanatory power of employment potential seems stronger than all other background characteristics as potential explanations for gaps in uptake between natives without a migration background and migrants (hypothesis 3). In line with previous research (Steiber and Haas, 2012; van Gameren and Ooms, Reference van Gameren and Ooms2009; Fortin, Reference Fortin2005), attitudes towards maternal employment also impact childcare uptake, with migrants exhibiting less progressive attitudes on average, and weakening negative differential uptake for migrants and their descendants after controlling for such attitudes (hypothesis 4).

Third, in line with expectations (hypothesis 5), country-specific results indicate cross-country variation in differential uptake by migration background and the explanatory power of different demand-side factors. Despite the fact that future research on such cross-sectional variation using a larger set of countries should be encouraged, we speculate on potential reasons for such cross-country differences. Given that employment potential explains most of the differential formal childcare uptake between migrants and natives without a migration background, and the country-ranking of the identified gaps seems to align with available literature on gradients by income or socio-economic status (Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Ghysels and Van Lancker, Reference Ghysels and Van Lancker2011; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Neels and Maes2023), we speculate that cross-national variation in labour market inequalities and design features of formal childcare might play a role. This assumption resonates with previous research highlighting socio-economic position and employment as key explanations for unequal uptake of formal childcare by migration background (Thil et al., 2023; Eremenko and Unterreiner, Reference Eremenko and Unterreiner2023, Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021). We find the strongest significant negative differential for formal childcare uptake for migrants in some Western European countries – Belgium, Germany and to a lesser degree, France – contexts with strong employment differentials for migrants (OECD, 2020; Rubin 2008). These findings align with available research highlighting the importance of socio-economic position and employment as explanations for differential formal childcare uptake by migration background in Belgium and France (Eremenko and Unterreiner, Reference Eremenko and Unterreiner2023, Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021). We find more limited gradients in Austria, Australia and Russia, which are also countries with more limited differences in labour market outcomes (OECD, 2020).

Additionally, our speculation that the design features of formal childcare also affect the degree of differential uptake by migration background seems supported by the positive differential for migrants in Sweden, a context with high coverage and accessibility and weak socio-economic differentiation in formal childcare uptake (Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Ghysels and Van Lancker, Reference Ghysels and Van Lancker2011). Hence the relatively large employment gaps between natives without a migration background and migrants in Sweden presumably do not translate into negative gradients such as in the Western European countries, as formal childcare supply typically meets demand (European Commission et al., 2014). As such, this study complements research suggesting that supply-side factors such as policy design features, access to information or geographic differences in childcare shortages should also be taken into account to fully grasp inequalities in formal childcare usage (Abrassart and Bonoli, Reference Abrassart and Bonoli2015; Pavolini and Van Lancker, Reference Pavolini and Van Lancker2018; Vandenbroeck et al., Reference Vandenbroeck, Geens and Berten2014).

In addition to the importance of socio-economic position and employment, it is noteworthy that the explanatory power of attitudes seems largest in Belgium and Germany, also the countries with the largest differentials in formal childcare uptake by migration background. Despite the fact that this finding aligns with previous research indicating that migrants are less in favour of formal childcare than natives in Germany (Seibel and Hedegaard, Reference Seibel and Hedegaard2017), the available literature on migrants’ attitudes concerning these topics in different countries is too premature to speculate on the reasons for cross-sectional variation in the importance of attitudes as a potential explanation of formal childcare uptake by migration background.

The findings of this study hold significant implications for policy discussions surrounding the social investment paradigm, social inclusion and inequalities in formal childcare coverage (e.g. Hummel et al., Reference Hummel, Yerkes and Bal2023; Pennerstorfer and Pennerstorfer, Reference Pennerstorfer and Pennerstorfer2021; Bonoli, Reference Bonoli, Careja, Emmenegger and Giger2020; Yerkes and Javornik, Reference Yerkes and Javornik2019; Van Lancker and Ghysels, Reference Van Lancker and Ghysels2012). In addressing differential formal childcare use by migration background, the study sheds light on the multi-faceted nature of demand-side factors influencing access to formal childcare services. By comprehensively examining demographic, socio-economic, employment-related and attitudinal characteristics, the study underscores the complexity of addressing such gaps, which might contrast with stereotypical mono-causal explanations in popular debates.

Finally, we present three avenues for future research. First, future research on explanations for differential formal childcare use by migration background should compare different generations and origin groups simultaneously, whilst addressing differentiation by reason for migration and length of stay, which requires sample sizes considerably larger than those available in the GGS. Second, the GGS data provide variables which would be unavailable in other types of data (e.g. lack of attitudes in administrative data). However, the data do not include information on intensity or timing of uptake, knowledge of formal childcare systems (Hummel et al., Reference Hummel, Yerkes and Bal2023), local-level contextual variables (e.g. childcare provision or regional level care cultures) (Pennerstorfer and Pennerstorfer, Reference Pennerstorfer and Pennerstorfer2021) or the proximity of potential informal care (e.g. grandparents) (Biegel et al., Reference Biegel, Wood and Neels2021), which require further attention. Third, future research should address potential explanations for cross-country differences in the gaps and underlying explanations due to compositional and contextual differences, providing more information on how demand-side (e.g. employment position) and supply-side features (e.g. priority rules or childcare shortages) (e.g. EllingsÆTer and Gulbrandsen, Reference EllingsÆTer and Gulbrandsen2007) might interact in shaping subgroup differentials in formal childcare uptake. Such a study requires larger sample sizes to study the composition of groups with a migration background in more detail, and a larger set of countries to employ contextual indicators.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S004727942500008X

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO grant no. 12ZA821N: COPE Project, G096321N and G0AHS24N), and the Research Council of the University of Antwerp (BOF: SEP-ERC Wood).

Competing interests

The author(s) declare none

References

Abrassart, A., & Bonoli, G. (2015). Availability, cost or culture? Obstacles to childcare services for low-income families. Journal of Social Policy, 44, 787806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biegel, N., Wood, J., & Neels, K. (2021). Migrant-native differentials in the uptake of (in)formal childcare in Belgium: the role of mothers’ employment opportunities and care availability. Journal of Family Research, 33(2), 467–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blau, D., & Currie, J. (2006). Pre-school, day care, and after-school care: who’s minding the kids? Handbook of the Economics of Education, 2, 11631278.Google Scholar
Bonizzoni, P. (2014). Immigrant working mothers reconciling work and childcare: the experience of Latin American and Eastern European women in Milan. Social Politics: International Studies in Gender, State and Society, 21, 194217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonoli, G. (2020) Social investment, active labour market policies and migration. In: Careja, R., Emmenegger, P. and Giger, N. (Eds.), The European Social Model under Pressure: Liber Amicorum in Honour of Klaus Armingeon. Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden, 193–206.Google Scholar
Burchinal, M, Nelson, L, Carlson, M, et al. (2008). Neighborhood characteristics, and child care type and quality. Early Education and Development, 19, 702725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burger, K (2010). How does early childhood care and education affect cognitive development? An international review of the effects of early interventions for children from different social backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(2), 140165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Valk, H. (2008). Parental influence on work and family plans of adolescents of different ethnic backgrounds in the Netherlands. Sex Roles, 59(9), 738751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Boca, D, & Vuri, D. (2007). The mismatch between employment and child care in Italy: the impact of rationing. Journal of Population Economics, 20, 805832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EllingsÆTer, A. L., & Gulbrandsen, L. (2007). Closing the childcare gap: the interaction of childcare provision and mothers’ agency in Norway. Journal of Social Policy, 36(4), 649669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eremenko, T, & Unterreiner, A. (2023). Access to formal childcare among families of newly arrived migrants from non-EU countries in France. Genus, 79, 26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
European Commission, European Education and Culture Executive Agency, Eurostat, & Eurydice. (2014). Key data on early childhood education and care in Europe – Eurydice and Eurostat report – 2014 edition. Publications Office.Google Scholar
Felfe, C., & Lalive, R. (2012). Early child care and child development: For whom it works and why. IZA Discussion Papers: (No. 7100).Google Scholar
Fokkema, T, Kveder, A, Hiekel, N, et al. (2016). Generations and gender programme wave 1 data collection: an overview and assessment of sampling and fieldwork methods, weighting procedures, and cross-sectional representativeness. Demographic Research, 34, 499524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fortin, NM. (2005). Gender role attitudes and the labour-market outcomes of women across OECD countries. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 21, 416438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, B, Holloway, SD, & Liang, X. (1996). Family selection of child-care centers: the influence of household support, ethnicity, and parental practices. Child Development, 67, 33203337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadeyne, S. (2006). The ultimate inequality: socio-economic differences in all-cause and cause-specific mortality in Belgium in the first part of the 1990s (Vol. 39). Brussels and the Hague: NIDI-CBGS Publications. Google Scholar
Ghysels, J., & Van Lancker, W. (2011). The unequal benefits of activation: an analysis of the social distribution of family policy among families with young children. Journal of European Social Policy, 21, 472485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goerres, A., & Tepe, M. (2012). Doing it for the kids? The determinants of attitudes towards public childcare in unified Germany. Journal of Social Policy, 41(2), 349372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. P. (2011). The impact of maternal education on children’s enrollment in early childhood education and care. Children and Youth Services Review, 33, 10491057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. P., & Kahn, J. M.. (2012). Early childhood education and care use: differences by race/ethnicity and age. Journal of Children and Poverty, 18, 2354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, K., & Hawkes, D. (2009). Early childcare and child development. Journal of Social Policy, 38(2), 211239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Havnes, T., & Mogstad, M. (2011). No child left behind: subsidized child care and children’s long-run outcomes. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy, 3, 97129.Google Scholar
Hirshberg, D, Huang, DS-C, & Fuller, B. (2005). Which low-income parents select child-care?: family demand and neighborhood organizations. Children and Youth Services Review, 27, 11191148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofferth, S.L. (1994). Access to early childhood programs for children at risk: Department of Education Office of Educational.Google Scholar
Hummel, B., Yerkes, M. A., & Bal, M. (2023). ‘Unprecedented injustice’: digitalisation and the perceived accessibility of childcare benefits. Journal of Social Policy, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huston, A. C., Chang, Y. E., & Gennetian, L. (2002). Family and individual predictors of child care use by low-income families in different policy contexts. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17, 441469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isaksen, L.W., & Bikova, M. (2019). Egalitarian ideologies on the move: changing care practices and gender norms in Norway. Journal of European Social Policy, 29, 627639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kil, T., Wood, J., & Neels, K. (2018). Parental leave uptake among migrant and native mothers: can precarious employment trajectories account for the difference? Ethnicities, 18(1), 106141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krapf, S. (2014). Who uses public childcare for 2-year-old children? Coherent family policies and usage patterns in Sweden, Finland and Western Germany. International Journal of Social Welfare, 23, 2540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leseman, P.P.M., & Slot, P.L. (2014) Breaking the cycle of poverty: challenges for European early childhood education and care. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 22, 314326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liang, X., Fuller, B., & Singer, J. D. (2000). Ethnic differences in child care selection: the influence of family structure, parental practices, and home language. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 357384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maes, J., Neels, K., Biegel, N., & Wood, J. (2023b). Uptake of formal childcare among second generation and native mothers in Belgium: can increasing local childcare availability narrow migrant-native gaps? Genus, 79(1), 7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maes, J., Wood, J., & Neels, K. (2019). Early labour market trajectories of intermediate and second generation Turkish and Maghreb women in Belgium. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 61, 6578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maes, J., Wood, J. & Neels, K. (2023a). Path-dependencies in employment trajectories around motherhood: comparing native versus second-generation migrant women in Belgium. Journal of International Migration & Integration, 24(Suppl 1), 281344 (2023).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mamolo, M, Coppola, L, & Di Cesare, M. (2011). Formal childcare use and household socio-economic profile in France, Italy, Spain and UK. Population Review, 50, 170194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marynissen, L., Wood, J., & Neels, K. (2021). Mothers and Parental Leave in Belgium: Social Inequalities in Eligibility and Uptake. Social Inclusion, 9(No 2 (2021): 13.Google Scholar
Meagher, G., & Szebehely, M. (2012). Equality in the social service state: Nordic childcare models in comparative perspective. In: Kvist, J., Fritzell, J., Hvinden, B., et al. (Eds.), Changing social equality. The Nordic welfare model in the 21st century. Bristol: Policy Press.Google Scholar
Mood, C. (2009). Logistic regression: why we cannot do what we think we can do, and what we can do about it. European Sociological Review, 26, 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, K. J., & Zippel, K. (2003). Paid to care: the origins and effects of care leave policies in Western Europe. Social Politics: International Studies in Gender, State and Society, 10, 4985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mussino, E., & Ortensi, L. E.. (2023). Childcare in Italy among migrants and natives: who uses which type and why? Genus, 79, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
OECD. (2019). OECD Family Database (http://www.oecd.org/els/family/database.htm).Google Scholar
OECD. (2020). Dataset: Employment, unemployment, and participation rates by place of birth and sex. Data extracted on 6 Sep 2022 06:39 UTC (GMT) from OECD.Stat: OECD.Google Scholar
Pavolini, E., & Van Lancker, W. (2018). The Matthew effect in childcare use: a matter of policies or preferences? Journal of European Public Policy, 25, 878893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennerstorfer, A., & Pennerstorfer, D. (2021). Inequalities in spatial accessibility of childcare: the role of non-profit providers. Journal of Social Policy, 50(1), 122147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Press, J. E., & Townsley, E. (1998). Wives’ and husbands’ housework reporting: gender, class, and social desirability. Gender & Society, 12(2), 188218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, J., Rendall, M. S., Rabinovich, L., Tsang, F., van Oranje-Nassau, C., & Janta, B. (2008). Migrant women in the European labour force. Current situation and future prospects. RAND Europe 2008Google Scholar
Schober, P., & Schmitt, C. (2017). Day-care availability, maternal employment and satisfaction of parents: evidence from cultural and policy variations in Germany. Journal of European Social Policy, 27, 433446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibel, V., & Hedegaard, T. F.. (2017). Migrants’ and natives’ attitudes to formal childcare in the Netherlands, Denmark and Germany. Children and Youth Services Review, 78, 112121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sprong, S., & Skopek, J. (2023). Childcare utilisation by migration background: evidence from a nationally representative Irish cohort study. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 84, 100773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiber, N., & Haas, B. (2012) Advances in explaining women’s employment patterns. Socio-Economic Review, 10, 343367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troger, T., & Verwiebe, R. (2015) The role of education for poverty risks revisited: couples, employment and profits from work–family policies. Journal of European Social Policy, 25, 286302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gameren, E., & Ooms, I. (2009). Childcare and labor force participation in the Netherlands: the importance of attitudes and opinions. Review of Economics of the Household, 7, 395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lancker, W. (2013). Putting the child-centred investment strategy to the test: evidence for the EU27. European Journal of Social Security, 15, 427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lancker, W. (2017). Reducing inequality in childcare service use across European countries: what (if any) is the role of social spending? Social Policy and Administration, 52, 271–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lancker, W, & Ghysels, J. (2012). Who benefits? The social distribution of subsidized childcare in Sweden and flanders. Acta Sociologica, 55, 125142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lancker, W., & Ghysels, J. (2016). Explaining patterns of inequality in childcare service use across 31 developed economies: a welfare state perspective. International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 57, 310337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandenbroeck, M., De Visscher, S., Van Nuffel, K., et al. (2008). Mothers’ search for infant child care: the dynamic relationship between availability and desirability in a continental European welfare state. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 245258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandenbroeck, M., Geens, N., & Berten, H. (2014) The impact of policy measures and coaching on the availability and accessibility of early child care: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Social Welfare, 23, 6979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall, K, & Sao Jose, J. (2004). Managing work and care: a difficult challenge for immigrant families. Social Policy and Administration, 38, 591621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, J, Wright, D, & Germino-Hausken, E. (1995). Child care and early education program participation of infants, toddlers, and preschoolers: National Center for Education Statistics.Google Scholar
Wood, J. (2022). Childcare ideals among second generation Muslim Moroccan immigrants in Flanders. Community, Work & Family, 1-18Google Scholar
Wood, J., Neels, K., Kil, T., & De Wachter, D. (2016). Family formation and labour force participation. A decomposition of educational differentials in maternal employment. Population, 71(1), 5382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, J., Neels, K., & Maes, J. (2023). A closer look at demand-side explanations for the Matthew effect in formal childcare uptake in Europe & Australia. Journal of European Social Policy33(4), 451–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, J., Van den Berg, L., & Neels, K. (2017) Household division of labour and family formation in the Moroccan First and Second Generations at the turn of the 21st Century. In: Timmerman, C., Fadil, N., Goddeeris, I., et al. (Eds.), Moroccan Migration in Belgium. More than 50 Years of Settlement. Leuven: Leuven University Press.Google Scholar
Yerkes, M.A., & Javornik, J. (2019). Creating capabilities: childcare policies in comparative perspective. Journal of European Social Policy, 29, 529544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Relative deviations in the probability of formal childcare uptake (i.e. average marginal effects/average probability by country) amongst first-generation migrants (1.1) and second-generation migrants (1.2) in comparison with natives.Source: Generations & Gender Survey (GGS) wave 1, calculations by authors M0 (null model); M1 (demographic control variables); M2 (controlling for socio-economic status); M3 (controlling for employment potential); M4 (controlling for predicted attitudes towards maternal employment); M5 (full model controlling for all covariates).

Figure 1

Table 1. Migrant native differentials in demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, employment potential and predicted attitudes towards maternal employment, effects from fixed-effects linear regression models, 7 GGS countries 2004–2013

Figure 2

Table 2. Average marginal effects from country fixed-effects logit models of couples’ formal childcare uptake, fifteen GGS countries, 2002–2013

Supplementary material: File

Wood supplementary material

Wood supplementary material
Download Wood supplementary material(File)
File 327.3 KB