Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T01:59:57.604Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nutritional aspects of commercially prepared infant foods in developed countries: a narrative review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 May 2017

Kate Maslin*
Affiliation:
School of Health Sciences and Social Work, James Watson West Building, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth PO1 2FR, UK
Carina Venter
Affiliation:
Division of Allergy and Immunology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Centre, Cincinnati, OH 45229, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Kate Maslin, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Nutritional intake during infancy is a critical aspect of child development and health that is of significant public health concern. Although there is extensive research on breast-feeding and timing of solid food introduction, there is less evidence on types of solid foods fed to infants, specifically commercially prepared infant foods. The consumption of commercially prepared infant foods is very prevalent in many developed countries, exceeding the consumption of homemade foods in some situations. Although these food products may have practical advantages, there are concerns about their nutritional composition, sweet taste, bioavailability of micronutrients, diversity of ingredients and long-term health effects. The extent that the manufacturing, fortification and promotion of these products are regulated by legislation varies between countries and regions. The aim of the present narrative review is to investigate, appraise and summarise these aspects. Overall there are very few studies directly comparing homemade and commercial infant foods and a lack of longitudinal studies to draw firm conclusions on whether commercial infant foods are mostly beneficial or unfavourable to infant health.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
© The Authors 2017 

Introduction

It is well established that infancy is a critical time for the development of health in later life and that early nutrition plays a significant role in physical and cognitive development( 1 , Reference Langley-Evans 2 ). Specifically there is considerable concern that exposure to and consumption of sweet foods early in life will have metabolic consequences on children’s health( Reference Goran 3 ). The WHO recommends exclusive breast-feeding until 6 months, with introduction of solid food at 6 months( 1 ). Following on from a milk-based diet, the introduction of solid food to infants’ diets, known as complementary feeding, enables infants to meet their nutritional requirements, whilst continuing to provide exposure to new tastes and textures. This period is an important developmental milestone. Ideally, it should provide a gradual transition progressing from a solely milk-based diet to a varied diet, providing foods that are both nutritious and safe. The use of home-prepared baby foods is encouraged by several international organisations( 4 8 ).

Complementary feeding has been debated extensively over the past few decades, specifically the most appropriate age of introduction of solid food and allergenic foods( Reference Muraro, Halken and Arshad 9 Reference Perkin, Logan and Marrs 11 ), timeframes for the introduction of different tastes and textures( Reference Northstone and Emmett 12 , Reference Mennella 13 ), use of organic foods( Reference Jiwan, Duane and O’Sullivan 14 ) and baby-led weaning( Reference Brown and Lee 15 , Reference Cameron, Heath and Taylor 16 ). The use of homemade v. commercially produced infant foods is implicated in all of these aspects. Commercially prepared infant foods, also known as ‘readymade’ infant foods, are typically mass produced and purchased in a pre-prepared format requiring minimal, if any, cooking or heating before consumption. In comparison, homemade infant foods are generally prepared in households by parents/carers, using fresh ingredients. Tracking of eating behaviours and preferences throughout life has been demonstrated( Reference Lioret, McNaughton and Spence 17 Reference Skinner, Carruth and Bounds 19 ). Specifically it has been shown that consumption of commercially prepared baby food at age 6 months is associated with consumption of ready-to-eat foods at 2 years of age( Reference Smithers, Golley and Mittinty 20 ), underlying the significance of this topic.

Traditionally baby foods were made at home, typically puréed or mashed, with mass production first reported to have occurred in 1928( Reference Bentley 21 ). Advice regarding infant feeding changed from the late nineteenth to the mid-twentieth centuries, meaning solids were introduced at earlier ages, at approximately 4–6 weeks old in the 1950s( Reference Bentley 21 ). This change in advice, combined with an increasing birth rate in the post-Second World War era, led to a growth in the mass production of commercial baby foods by manufacturers as part of the canned goods industry, particularly in the USA( Reference Bentley 21 ). Although initially commercially produced infant foods were a means to provide fruit and vegetables all year round, over the years products have diversified significantly. Currently a broad range of commercially prepared products exist across a number of categories according to stage/age range and type of food (for example, cereal products, baby snacks, desserts). To illustrate the number of products now available, recent studies of commercial infant foods identified 479 different products in the UK market( Reference García, Raza and Parrett 22 ) and 657 in the US market( Reference Cogswell, Gunn and Yuan 23 ). Although there are an extensive number of products available, it is difficult to say whether the variety of ingredients used has changed over time.

Concerns have been raised regarding commercially produced infant food, specifically diversity of ingredients used( Reference Mesch, Stimming and Foterek 24 ), the taste profile( Reference Garcia, McLean and Wright 25 ), nutritional content( Reference Cogswell, Gunn and Yuan 23 , Reference Elliott 26 ), bioavailability of micronutrients( Reference Mir-Marqués, González-Masó and Cervera 27 Reference Bosscher, Van Cauwenbergh and Van der Auwera 29 ) and toxicity( Reference Carbonell-Barrachina, Ramirez-Gondolfo and Wu 28 ). Together these factors cumulatively create a significant change in early food exposure, with potential implications for the development of non-communicable diseases, namely allergy( Reference Björkstén, Sepp and Julge 30 , Reference Grimshaw, Maskell and Oliver 31 ) and obesity( Reference Luoto, Kalliomäki and Laitinen 32 ). There are also concerns generally regarding the role of infant feeding practices in the development of early tooth decay( Reference Colak, Dulgergil and Dalli 33 , Reference Peres, Sheiham and Liu 34 ), although there is no evidence that commercial infant food in particular contributes to this issue. Additionally there are claims that commercially produced infant foods may displace or reduce the duration of breast-feeding( Reference Tzioumis, Kay and Wright 35 ).

Given the widespread availability of commercial baby food in developed countries and debate regarding the impact this could have on infant diet and long-term health outcomes, a summary of the evidence is warranted. The present review aims to address and critically appraise the literature regarding nutritional implications of commercial infant food consumption, in addition to broader aspects such as taste, ingredient variety and parental perception. Fig. 1 illustrates the factors, which will be discussed. The review will not include the wider topics of complementary feeding in developing countries, studies that include toddler foods( Reference Cogswell, Gunn and Yuan 23 , Reference Dunford, Louie and Byrne 36 Reference Carstairs, Craig and Marais 38 ) or infant beverages, which are considered outside the remit.

Fig. 1 Current issues regarding commercial infant food intake in developed countries.

Usage of commercial infant foods internationally

The usage of commercial infant foods has been reported by national feeding and cohort studies in several developed countries, although dietary collection methods, sampling and timeframes differ between studies, so direct comparison is not always possible. Additionally, as the focus of these studies is often breast- and formula-feeding practices and the age of introduction of solid food, not all published research specifically differentiates between commercially prepared and homemade infant food, so precise data are not always available.

National feeding data from the UK indicate that when questioned about the previous day’s dietary intake, a greater proportion of infants aged 4–6 months had been fed commercially prepared baby food than homemade baby food (38 % compared with 28 %)( Reference McAndrew, Thompson and Fellows 39 ). In addition, almost half (45 %) of mothers of 8- to 10-month-old infants use commercially prepared baby foods at least once per day( Reference McAndrew, Thompson and Fellows 39 ). Differences were observed according to maternal occupational status and ethnicity( Reference McAndrew, Thompson and Fellows 39 ), with those in the ‘managerial/professional’ job categories and the ‘Chinese and other’ ethnic groups less likely to use commercially prepared infant foods. However, in contrast, research from a large infant cohort study in the South of England that used principal component analyses to analyse food frequency data found clear differences in preference for wet and dry commercial infant food at age 6 months, but the pattern was not associated with many of the maternal and family characteristics considered( Reference Robinson, Marriott and Poole 40 ). The heterogeneous results reported are probably due to the differences in population sampling, dietary collection and statistical analysis methods used.

Similar trends are evident in other European countries. In Ireland, a birth cohort study found that 63·2 % of 6-month-old infants consumed a sweetened commercially produced cereal for breakfast, with 30–31 % consuming commercially prepared products at lunch and evening times( Reference Tarrant, Younger and Sheridan-Pereira 41 ). Another Irish birth cohort study reported that 49 % of foods eaten by infants in the first 6 weeks of weaning were homemade( Reference O’Donovan, Murray and Hourihane 42 ). In France commercial infant foods are estimated to account for 27–28 % of energy intake at ages 6–11 months, the majority of parents (63 %) offer their child commercially prepared baby foods 4–7 d/week, with only 24 % never using them( Reference Bresson and Le Bris 43 ). In a German birth cohort study, analysis of all food diaries completed indicated that 94·4 % of infants consumed at least one commercially prepared baby food product within a 3 d period, whereas homemade complementary food was eaten exclusively by only 5·6 % of participants( Reference Foterek, Hilbig and Alexy 44 , Reference Foterek, Hilbig and Alexy 45 ). Participants with higher consumption of commercial infant food were significantly older, breastfed for a shorter duration and were more likely to have mothers with a lower educational status. In Italy, a birth cohort study of 400 infant and mother pairs reported that commercially prepared infant foods were consumed in significantly higher quantities by infants who were breastfed than non-breastfed( Reference Pani, Carletti and Knowles 46 ). By using an estimated food diary approach, this study was able to quantify that commercial baby foods contributed a higher energy content than that of homemade foods. However, it must be stated that of these three studies, only one recruited( Reference Bresson and Le Bris 43 ) a cross-sectional nationally representative sample.

From an economic and availability perspective, sales of commercial infant food in the twenty-seven countries of the European Union were reported to be 1271 million Euro in 2011( Reference Ghisolfi, Bocquet and Bresson 47 ). In Germany, the number of commercial infant products on sale increased between 2010 and 2012 from 276 to 309 jarred vegetable–potato–meat meals, demonstrating increased availability of products.

In developed countries outside of Europe, there is a similar pattern of consumption. Although the national infant feeding survey in Australia focused on breast and formula feeding and did not specifically collect data on commercial infant food( 48 ), the baby and toddler food market is reported to have grown by a rate of 4·8 % per year in the last 5 years( Reference Dunford, Louie and Byrne 36 ). In the USA, 73–95 % of infants between the ages of 4 and 12 months consumed commercially produced baby foods when a national cross-sectional feeding survey was conducted in 2002( Reference Briefel, Reidy and Karwe 49 ). When repeated in 2008, the five most frequently consumed vegetables by infants aged 4–9 months were commercially prepared, rather than fresh( Reference Siega-Riz, Deming and Reidy 50 ). Although the proportion of infants aged 6–9 months consuming commercially prepared fruit products decreased from 66·4 to 50·2 %, between 2004 and 2008, respectively, four of the top five most frequently consumed fruits were commercially prepared rather than fresh in 2008( Reference Siega-Riz, Deming and Reidy 50 ). A limitation of this study, however, is that the data were collected using a 24 h recall, which may not be reflective of food group intake over a longer time period. Looking at sales figures in the USA, they have risen from 36·7 to 55 billion US dollars per annum from 2010 to 2015( 51 ).

Perceptions of commercial infant foods

Studies assessing maternal perceptions of commercial infant foods have taken place in developed countries including Scotland( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 , Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards 53 ), England( Reference Maslin, Galvin and Shepherd 54 , Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington 55 ), France( Reference Betoko, Charles and Hankard 56 Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 58 ), USA( Reference Kim, Whaley and Gradziel 59 ), Australia( Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare 60 ) Germany, Italy, Spain and Sweden( Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards 53 ). All of these studies, with the exception of Kim et al. ( Reference Kim, Whaley and Gradziel 59 ), used a focus group or structured interview approach, which enables in-depth analysis of opinions and attitudes.

Perceptions of infant feeding and commercial baby food are influenced by educational level, parity, previous experience of weaning and cultural factors( Reference Maslin, Galvin and Shepherd 54 , Reference Betoko, Charles and Hankard 56 , Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 58 ), with second-time mothers and those from lower socio-economic groups more likely to perceive commercially prepared infant foods positively than first-time mothers or those from higher socio-economic groups( Reference Maslin, Galvin and Shepherd 54 ). Homemade foods are generally viewed as the ideal food by most mothers, due to the freshness of ingredients, taste( Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards 53 ), ‘avoidance of chemical in jars’( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 ) and low cost; however, there is disagreement whether fresh or commercially prepared products are cheaper( Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare 60 ). In most studies commercial baby foods are perceived negatively; as ‘bland’ and ‘unauthentic’( Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken 57 ) or only used in ‘an emergency’( Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington 55 ), with some participants saying they felt ‘a bit guilty’ using prepared foods to feed their baby( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 ). Similarly a questionnaire-based study in the USA indicated that many mothers have a preference for fresh fruits and vegetables over jarred baby foods, with mothers of older infants (9–11 months) reporting a significantly higher preference than mothers of younger infants( Reference Kim, Whaley and Gradziel 59 ). However, the study by Kim et al. ( Reference Kim, Whaley and Gradziel 59 ) recruited a sample of low-income mothers who received supplemental food package and overall 83·7 % of respondents were ‘very satisfied’ with the jarred fruit and vegetables received.

Several of the same studies have also noted positive perceptions of commercial infant food by some participants. Perceived advantages of commercially produced infant foods are portability and convenience, with preparation of homemade food viewed as laborious and wasteful by some( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 , Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards 53 , Reference Schwartz, Madrelle and Vereijken 57 ). This is in agreement with the overall trend towards increased reliance on readymade foods across all ages generally( Reference Jabs and Devine 61 ). Access to fresh fruit and vegetables, leading to availability and perishability concerns, is also noted to differ depending on urban or rural location( Reference Boak, Virgo-Milton and Hoare 60 ). Betoko et al. ( Reference Betoko, Charles and Hankard 56 ) reported that increased use of commercially prepared vegetables and fruit purées was explained by an awareness of nutritional advice about infant feeding, coupled with a lack of time and culinary skills to implement the advice. Indeed, some research has reported that commercial infant foods are perceived as superior to homemade foods by some mothers, describing them as ‘safer’ and possibly composed of better ingredients( Reference Maslin, Galvin and Shepherd 54 ). This is especially applicable to organic foods, which are viewed as ‘natural’( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 ). Commercially prepared infant foods are also seen as providing an opportunity to try out new foods that the family would not normally consume (for example, pumpkin)( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 ).

Two European studies( Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards 53 , Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 58 ) that recruited mothers and infants from different countries enabled exploration of cultural influences on commercial infant foods. Maier et al. ( Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 58 ) conducted structured interviews with two groups of mothers of infants aged 4–9 months: one group in Dijon, France, the other group in Aalen, Germany. Clear between- and within-group differences in weaning practices were found, with 68 % of Aalen mothers reported to prepare baby food at home more than once per week compared with 46 % of Dijon mothers. Distinct cultural differences were also reported by Synnott et al. ( Reference Synnott, Bogue and Edwards 53 ), who compared mothers from five different European countries. For example, all of the Italian participants chose to prepare homemade food for their infants compared with Swedish parents who were more likely to supplement home-prepared foods with commercially prepared foods. The study also highlighted that different factors were influential for each country when purchasing food for the infant. In Germany, Scotland and Sweden, health was considered the most important issue, followed by taste and organic ingredients. In Italy, the priorities were health, followed by method of production and brand compared with Spain, where the three most important factors were health, taste and brand.

Limitations of these studies are that they generally have used a small sample size, due to the qualitative approach. With the exception of Hoddinott et al. ( Reference Hoddinott, Craig and Britten 52 ), the cited studies have only included mothers as participants, rather than fathers or other caregivers and as with all health-related studies, selection bias and/or social desirability bias cannot be ruled out, with only those interested in infant feeding and nutrition likely to have taken part. However, overall these studies do provide rich insight into reasons why commercially prepared infant food is used so broadly, as well as the perceived disadvantages, such as taste and nutritional content.

Taste and variety of ingredients used in commercial infant foods

By incorporating a wide variety of fresh foods, ideally complementary feeding should provide a platform for the establishment of balanced taste preferences. It is known that newborn infants have an innate preference for sweet tastes and innate rejection of bitter tastes, which has developed from an evolutionary perspective to seek out energy and reject toxins( Reference Beauchamp and Mennella 62 ). However, these innate preferences can be manipulated with exposure to different tastes in the early stages of weaning; hence it is recommended that bitter-tasting vegetables may need to be offered several times before acceptance is achieved( Reference Birch and Doub 63 , Reference Nicklaus 64 ). The exposure effect has been described as consistent, powerful and universal( Reference Schwartz, Scholtens and Lalanne 65 ). A landmark study demonstrated that repeated exposure to similar foods in the early stages of complementary feeding can increase preference within a period of 10 d( Reference Birch, Gunder and Grimm-Thomas 66 ), although this study used banana and peas, rather than commercially prepared foods.

Research in the UK by Garcia et al. ( Reference García, Raza and Parrett 22 , Reference Garcia, McLean and Wright 25 ) reported that nearly two-thirds of the 329 commercial baby foods studied were sweet, with a distinct lack of bitter vegetables. The six most common fruit and vegetables used were sweet (apple, banana, tomato, mango, carrot and sweet potato), with green vegetables such as broccoli or spinach rarely incorporated into products. In total, fruit juice was added to 18 % of products and 8·5 % of savory products had added fruit, giving them a sweet taste. This ubiquitious use of sweet flavours to mask the taste of bitter vegetables may be due to commercial pressure to manufacture instantly palatable foods( Reference Garcia, McLean and Wright 25 ).

It is unclear whether increased reliance on commercial infant foods reduces or increases the diversity of foods introduced during the weaning period. In theory, consumption of a varied diet should reduce the risk of developing a deficiency or excess of any particular nutrient( Reference Ruel 67 ), with dietary variety shown to correlate strongly with dietary adequacy in toddlers( Reference Cox, Skinner and Carruth 68 ). Less food diversity, defined broadly as the consumption of a narrow range of foods( Reference Ruel 67 ), in early life has also been associated with increased risk of any asthma, atopic asthma, wheeze and allergic rhinitis in a large birth cohort study( Reference Nwaru, Takkinen and Kaila 69 ).

A German study investigating food diversity in commercial infant foods reported that homemade infant meals used twenty-six different vegetables, compared with seventeen different vegetables used in commercially prepared food, with the majority of meals based on carrot( Reference Mesch, Stimming and Foterek 24 ), a finding also reported in a UK study( Reference Carstairs, Craig and Marais 38 ). Despite this, there was no difference in the variety of vegetables consumed at 6 or 9 months of age. Indeed, by 12 months of age, those fed commercial meals consumed a greater variety of vegetables. This was attributed to maternal confusion around infant feeding guidelines and that for practical food preparation reasons, infants who are fed homemade food may be fed the same homemade meal on three consecutive days, due to food being prepared in bulk.

Similarly, in a low-income sample of mothers and infants from the USA, infants aged 6–12 months who received commercial baby foods consumed a greater variety of fruits and vegetables, than those who did not( Reference Hurley and Black 70 ), even when adjusting for infant age, maternal education and ethnicity. Looking at longer-term outcomes, a longitudinal UK study reported that feeding home-cooked fruit or vegetables during infancy was associated with increased uptake and variety of fruit and vegetables eaten at the age of 7 years, whereas feeding commercially prepared fruit and vegetables during infancy was not( Reference Coulthard, Harris and Emmett 71 ). A proposed explanation for this was that commercially prepared fruit and vegetables are likely to have a uniform taste and texture, whereas those cooked at home or eaten raw will vary according to whether they are in season and the cooking method. It is also possible that the specific combination of ingredients in commercially prepared baby food may mask or interfere with learning about the particular flavour of single vegetables( Reference Caton, Ahern and Hetherington 55 ). A German study found no association between commercially prepared food intake in infancy and fruit and vegetable variety intake at preschool age in girls; however, in boys there was an association with reduced vegetable variety score( Reference Foterek, Hilbig and Alexy 45 ). The reason for this was unclear.

In summary, the existing research base underlines the fact that the development of dietary variety and taste preference is complex and multifactorial and it is not yet clear what role commercial infant food plays in either the short or long term.

Meat and fish content of commercial infant foods

Looking more broadly at food groups other than fruit and vegetables, concerns have also been raised about the limited inclusion of fish in commercially prepared infant foods( Reference Mesch, Stimming and Foterek 24 ). A study in Scotland highlighted the lack of infant seafood-based foods in the UK, finding that of 341 main meals available, only 3·8 % were seafood-based, compared with 30·2 % poultry-based, 35·5 % meat-based and 30·5 % vegetable-based( Reference Carstairs, Marais and Craig 72 ). This is seen as an important issue, as underexposure to the distinctive taste of fish may lead to reduced preference in child- and adulthood. When the study was updated in 2015, the proportion of meals containing seafood had increased to 6·3 %. However, it must be noted that this study only focused on one country, and availability of seafood-based meals in different parts of the world may be different and influenced by cultural preferences.

Fish, specifically oily fish, is of particular nutritional relevance in infancy due to the iodine( Reference Zimmerman 73 ) and long-chain PUFA content and associated health outcomes( Reference Koletzko, Lien and Agostoni 74 , Reference Riediger, Othman and Suh 75 ). Although the iodine content of infant formula milk is regulated, there is no recommendation regarding minimum iodine fortification of commercial infant food in the European Union( 6 , 76 ). In terms of essential fatty acids, the concern regarding a lack of sufficient long-chain PUFA in commercial infant food was noted more recently by Loughrill & Zand( Reference Loughrill and Zand 77 ). The contribution of fish-based meals to essential fatty acid intake was found to be low, providing only 19·9 and 3·41 % of requirements for EPA and DHA, respectively, which may be because the meals analysed were only composed of approximately 10 % of fish by weight( Reference Loughrill and Zand 77 ).

On the contrary, it could also be argued that non-consumption of fish is common in all infants and young children, regardless of whether they are fed predominantly homemade or prepared baby food. National UK dietary data reported that after disaggregation of composite dishes, mean consumption of fish from all sources ranged from 1 g/d for children aged 4–6 months to 6 g/d for those aged 12–18 months. The proportion of infants and young children consuming fish and fish products increased with age from 13 % at 4–6 months to 53 % at 12–18 months. This delayed introduction may be due to confusion and change in infant feeding guidelines for allergy prevention( Reference Mesch, Stimming and Foterek 24 ).

Red meat is also a source of long-chain PUFA, with lamb often recommended as a first meat for infants in some countries, including Italy( Reference Nudda, McGuire and Battacone 78 ). A study comparing the n-3 PUFA content of fresh lamb with a lamb-containing commercially prepared infant meal found a threefold higher content in fresh lamb. This may be due to the common use of vegetable oil as an ingredient in homogenised infant meat products which modifies the fatty acid composition, or due to the origin of lamb meat used in commercially prepared products( Reference Nudda, McGuire and Battacone 78 ). Of note, the quantity of long-chain PUFA in lamb-based commercially prepared foods was higher than that previously identified in beef-based products.

In terms of quantifying the amount of meat in an infant food product, a European Union directive( 6 ) states that if meat, poultry or fish are mentioned first in the name of the product, whether or not the product is presented as a meal, then the named meat, poultry or fish shall constitute not less than 10 % by weight of the total product. If meat, poultry or fish are mentioned, but not first in the name of the product, then it shall constitute not less than 8 % by weight of the total product. Following on from this stipulation, a German study concluded that the low meat composition of many commercial infant meals may increase the risk of marginal Fe status in older infants who were breastfed for 4–6 months( Reference Dube, Schwartz and Mueller 79 ). In Australia, Mauch et al. ( Reference Mauch, Perry and Magarey 80 ) reported that commercial infant foods were the most common source of meat/meat alternatives consumed at age 5·5 months, but by 14 months mixed meals such as Bolognese were more common. The study concluded that parents should encourage meat ‘in a recognisable form’ and as one of the first complementary foods.

Nutritional content of commercial infant foods

Tables 1 and 2 provide a summary of studies that have investigated the nutritional content of commercial infant foods from 1997 to 2016. Studies were heterogeneous in design, assessing different types and numbers of food (main meals, desserts, snacks), obtained from several different countries. The studies have been divided into two tables, broadly dependent on the objectives of the study.

Table 1 Summary of studies on macronutrient composition with emphasis on sugar, fat and salt content

RNI, reference nutrient intake.

Table 2 Summary of studies on micronutrient and trace element composition

RNI, reference nutrient intake.

Contrasting methods have been used dependent on the objective of the study, which limits the ability to directly compare results and generalise findings. The majority of the studies investigating energy, sugar and salt content relied on nutritional content information provided by food labels( Reference García, Raza and Parrett 22 , Reference Ruel 67 ), which could be subject to substantial error, depending on the accuracy of the labelling information provided. Research investigating micronutrient, trace element or toxicity levels which undertook independent laboratory analysis of samples is arguably more objective( Reference Mir-Marqués, González-Masó and Cervera 27 Reference Bosscher, Van Cauwenbergh and Van der Auwera 29 , Reference Melø, Gellein and Evje 81 Reference Loughrill, Govinden and Zand 84 ). However, the methods, criteria and analysis standards may differ between studies. Overall there is noted to be a paucity of studies directly comparing the nutritional content of commercial and homemade infant foods, with only two studies identified that directly compared equivalent products using laboratory analysis( Reference van den Boom, Kimber and Morgan 85 , Reference Randhawa, Kakuda and Wong 86 ). The same conclusion was reached by a recent report, which reported that the overall evidence on nutritional composition was of low quality and direct comparison of commercially prepared infant foods with homemade foods was often lacking( Reference Tzioumis, Kay and Wright 35 ). In recognising the paucity of studies that directly compare commercially prepared infant with homemade infant foods, it must be highlighted that infant eating patterns are not necessarily dichotomous, i.e. that infants may be fed a combination of commercially prepared and homemade products and the proportion of each may vary at different developmental stages. Additionally, there are ethical concerns regarding infant feeding studies and therefore randomised controlled trials are probably not a suitable or practical study design to implement.

Sugar, salt and fat content

Several studies have focused on the ‘healthy eating’ aspect of infant nutrition, assessing either sugar, salt or fat content. In terms of sugar, the overall trend was that products had an inappropriately high sugar content compared with nationally recognised standards and recommendations( Reference García, Raza and Parrett 22 , Reference Randhawa, Kakuda and Wong 86 , Reference Hilbig, Foterek and Kersting 87 ); however, this claim is difficult to disentangle as fruit, and therefore fructose, was a primary ingredient in many of the products investigated. Some studies specified added sugar content( Reference Cogswell, Gunn and Yuan 23 , Reference Hilbig, Foterek and Kersting 87 , Reference Walker and Goran 88 ), whereas others reported only total sugar content( Reference García, Raza and Parrett 22 ) which makes comparisons problematic. Both total sugar and added sugar are an essential factor to consider, given the recently published recommendations regarding reducing consumption of added sugars( Reference Moynihan and Kelly 89 ).

In contrast to sugar and added sugar, few products had Na levels of concern according to information provided on the food label( Reference Hilbig, Foterek and Kersting 87 ). In support of this viewpoint, Maalouf et al. ( Reference Maalouf, Cogswell and Yuan 90 ) determined that only 2·2 % of dietary Na was derived from commercial baby food using nationally representative data in infants aged 0–6 months in the USA, which increased to 8·8 % in infants aged 6–12 months. However, this study relied on data using a 24 h recall method, and therefore may not necessarily be reflective of usual daily intake. One study that conducted laboratory analysis reported Na content exceeded the maximum permitted level( Reference Mir-Marqués, González-Masó and Cervera 27 ). As commercially prepared infant foods are widely used, the Na content is important, as salt preference may be established due to exposure in infancy( Reference Stein, Cowart and Beauchamp 91 ).

There was disagreement whether overall the nutritional composition of infant food was acceptable. A UK study concluded that total daily intake of fat from the consumption of commercial complementary food may be in excess of the recommended guidelines if the intakes of dessert and snacks are incorporated( Reference Zand, Chowdhry and Pollard 92 ). In terms of energy intake, van den Boom et al. ( Reference van den Boom, Kimber and Morgan 85 ) reported that homemade foods have a lower energy density than commercially prepared; however, this was later contradicted by Garcia et al. ( Reference García, Raza and Parrett 22 ), although different methods were used by each study.

Micronutrient content and adequacy

Studies of micronutrient content overall did not reach a consensus whether homemade or commercially prepared baby food had a nutritionally superior content. This may in part be due to different regulations on micronutrient fortification in different countries. A summary of studies is shown in Table 2.

Mineral and trace element content

Concentrations of Fe, Zn and Ca in commercially prepared infant foods were raised as a concern by some studies( Reference Mir-Marqués, González-Masó and Cervera 27 , Reference Carbonell-Barrachina, Ramirez-Gondolfo and Wu 28 , Reference van den Boom, Kimber and Morgan 85 ). Overall a systematic review found no evidence that commercially prepared infant foods improved anaemia or micronutrient status, but only two studies were included in the review, which were deemed to have a moderate risk of bias( Reference Tzioumis, Kay and Wright 35 ). In contrast to the systematic review, Melø et al. ( Reference Melø, Gellein and Evje 81 ) reported that a diet based solely on commercially prepared foods would provide a sufficient intake of Ca, Cu, Fe, K, Mg and Zn for a 6-month-old infant, whether breast or formula fed. However, this conclusion was drawn based on nutritional analysis of a sample menu recommended by an infant food manufacturer and therefore may be subject to bias. By using dietary pattern scores, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a large prospective study in the UK, demonstrated that between 6 and 8 months of age, Ca and Fe intakes increased across infants who scored highly in the commercially prepared baby food patterns. This could be because in the 1990s when the study took place, most commercially prepared infant foods were fortified with ferrous sulfate, unlike current times when many unfortified organic products are available( Reference Smithers, Golley and Brazionis 93 ).

Looking at subtypes of commercial infant foods, no difference was found in Fe, Zn, Mg and K levels between vegetable and meat meals( Reference Zand, Chowdhry and Wray 83 ). Overall trace elements were at acceptable levels when compared with available national and international guidelines( Reference Mir-Marqués, González-Masó and Cervera 27 , Reference Carbonell-Barrachina, Ramirez-Gondolfo and Wu 28 , Reference Zand, Chowdhry and Wray 83 ), although baby rice contained excessive Pb, As, Ni and Cr in some countries( Reference Carbonell-Barrachina, Ramirez-Gondolfo and Wu 28 ).

Vitamin content

Few studies evaluated vitamin content, although Randhawa et al. ( Reference Randhawa, Kakuda and Wong 86 ) reported that mean vitamin B1, B2 and C contents were comparable across commercial, laboratory-prepared and homemade recipes. One study identified vitamin C as the most commonly fortified micronutrient( Reference Dunford, Louie and Byrne 36 ). More recently, Loughrill et al. ( Reference Loughrill, Govinden and Zand 84 ) have suggested that commercial infant food may supply excess levels of vitamin A in infants; however, this calculation was made on the basis of a theoretical daily menu consisting of only commercially prepared foods and infant formula, so cautious interpretation is required.

Limitations of the review

Although the present article has aimed to appraise the current literature, there are limitations and restrictions to this review. As set out in the introduction, the review has focused on complementary infant feeding in children under the age of 1 year old in developed countries. We have not included studies related to infant beverages, including infant formula or juice, as the remit would have been too broad. There are also varying degrees of legislation governing the composition, fortification and marketing of commercial infant foods in different countries; therefore findings and summaries reported may not be relevant to other countries. As previously mentioned, a wide range of categories of foods is available, which may have changed over time. Some of the studies cited have calculated the nutritional content of commercially prepared foods based on theoretical daily intakes using estimated portion sizes; however, the use and consumption of commercial and homemade foods are not dichotomous. There is also a paucity of data regarding the contribution of energy, macro- and micronutrients from commercial and homemade foods. Fundamentally, infant feeding practices are complicated by confounding variables including socio-economic and cultural factors, beliefs, attitudes and maternal diet; therefore it is not always possible to explain dietary patterns. As this is a narrative review, rather than a systematic review, it cannot be guaranteed that all the existing literature has been explored; however, an extensive literature search was undertaken.

Conclusion and future research needs

It is clear that usage of commercially prepared infant foods is very pervasive in many developed countries. Research has highlighted concerns about the altered nutritional intake, sweet taste, food diversity and toxicity of commercial infant food and the effect this could have on long-term dietary intake and health. However, the evidence base is unequivocal, complicated by different regulations between countries and a lack of randomised controlled trials. Commercially prepared baby foods have practical advantages and may improve nutritional intake and dietary variety in some situations and population groups. Overall, there are very few studies directly comparing homemade and commercial infant foods and a lack of longitudinal studies to draw firm conclusions on whether commercial infant foods are predominantly beneficial or unfavourable to infant health. It is therefore important for further high-quality research to be conducted.

Acknowledgements

The present research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

K. M. planned and drafted the manuscript. C. V. assisted with drafting the manuscript and approved the final version.

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

1. World Health Organization (2002) Infant and young child nutrition: Global strategy on infant and young child feeding. Fifty Fifth World Health Assembly, 16 April 2002. http://apps.who.int/gb/archive/pdf_files/WHA55/ea5515.pdf (accessed March 2017).Google Scholar
2. Langley-Evans, SC (2015) Nutrition in early life and the programming of adult disease: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet 28, Suppl. 1, 114.Google Scholar
3. Goran, M (2013) How growing up sweet can turn sour. Pediatr Obes 8, 237241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Food Safety Authority of Ireland (2011) Best Practice for Infant Feeding in Ireland. A Guide for Health Care Professionals Based on the Scientific Recommendations for National Infant Feeding Policy, 2nd ed. Dublin: Food Safety Authority of Ireland.Google Scholar
6. Commission of the European Communities (2006) Commission Directive 2006/125/EC of 5 December 2006 on processed cereal-based foods for infants and young children. https://www.fsai.ie/uploadedFiles/Dir2006_125.pdf Google Scholar
7. United States Department of Agriculture Food and Nutrition Service (2009) Infant Nutrition and Feeding. https://wicworks.fns.usda.gov/infants/infant-feeding-guide Google Scholar
8. Australian Government National Health and Medical Research Council Department of Health and Ageing (2012) Eat for Health: Infant Feeding Guidelines: Information for Health Workers. https://www.eatforhealth.gov.au/sites/default/files/files/the_guidelines/n56_infant_feeding_guidelines.pdf Google Scholar
9. Muraro, A, Halken, S, Arshad, SH, et al. (2014) EAACI Food Allergy and Anaphylaxis Guidelines. Primary prevention of food allergy. Allergy Eur J Allergy Clin Immunol 69, 590601.Google Scholar
10. Perkin, MR, Logan, K, Tseng, A, et al. (2016) Randomized trial of introduction of allergenic foods in breast-fed infants. N Engl J Med 374, 17331743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Perkin, MR, Logan, K, Marrs, T, et al. (2016) Enquiring About Tolerance (EAT) study: feasibility of an early allergenic food introduction regimen. J Allergy Clin Immunol 137, 14771486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Northstone, K & Emmett, P (2013) The associations between feeding difficulties and behaviours and dietary patterns at 2 years of age: The ALSPAC cohort. Matern Child Nutr 9, 533542.Google Scholar
13. Mennella, JA (2014) Ontogeny of taste preferences: basic biology and implications for health. Am J Clin Nutr 99, 704711.Google Scholar
14. Jiwan, MA, Duane, P, O’Sullivan, L, et al. (2010) Content and bioaccessibility of carotenoids from organic and non-organic baby foods. J Food Compos Anal 23, 346352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. Brown, A & Lee, M (2011) A descriptive study investigating the use and nature of baby-led weaning in a UK sample of mothers. Matern Child Nutr 7, 3447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16. Cameron, SL, Heath, ALM & Taylor, RW (2012) How feasible is Baby-Led Weaning as an approach to infant feeding? A review of the evidence. Nutrients 4, 15751609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Lioret, S, McNaughton, SA, Spence, AC, et al. (2013) Tracking of dietary intakes in early childhood: the Melbourne InFANT Program. Eur J Clin Nutr 67, 275281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Northstone, K & Emmett, PM (2008) Are dietary patterns stable throughout early and mid-childhood? A birth cohort study. Br J Nutr 100, 10691076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Skinner, JD, Carruth, BR, Bounds, W, et al. (2002) Children’s food preferences: a longitudinal analysis. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 16381647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Smithers, LG, Golley, RK, Mittinty, MN, et al. (2012) Dietary patterns at 6, 15 and 24 months of age are associated with IQ at 8 years of age. Eur J Epidemiol 27, 525535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21. Bentley, A (2006) Booming baby food: infant food and feeding in post-World War II America. Mich Hist Rev 32, 6387.Google Scholar
22. García, AL, Raza, S, Parrett, A, et al. (2013) Nutritional content of infant commercial weaning foods in the UK. Arch Dis Child 98, 793797.Google Scholar
23. Cogswell, ME, Gunn, JP, Yuan, K, et al. (2015) Sodium and sugar in complementary infant and toddler foods sold in the United States. Pediatrics 135, 416423.Google Scholar
24. Mesch, CM, Stimming, M, Foterek, K, et al. (2014) Food variety in commercial and homemade complementary meals for infants in Germany. Market survey and dietary practice. Appetite 76, 113119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Garcia, AL, McLean, K & Wright, CM (2016) Types of fruits and vegetables used in commercial baby foods and their contribution to sugar content. Matern Child Nutr 12, 838847.Google Scholar
26. Elliott, CD (2010) Sweet and salty: nutritional content and analysis of baby and toddler foods. J Public Health (Oxf) 33, 6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Mir-Marqués, A, González-Masó, A, Cervera, ML, et al. (2015) Mineral profile of Spanish commercial baby food. Food Chem 172, 238244.Google Scholar
28. Carbonell-Barrachina, A, Ramirez-Gondolfo, A, Wu, X, et al. (2012) Essential and toxic elements in infant foods from Spain, UK, China and USA. J Environ Monit 14, 24472455.Google Scholar
29. Bosscher, D, Van Cauwenbergh, R, Van der Auwera, JC, et al. (2002) Calcium, iron and zinc availability from weaning meals. Acta Paediatr 91, 761768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30. Björkstén, B, Sepp, E, Julge, K, et al. (2001) Allergy development and the intestinal microflora during the first year of life. J Allergy Clin Immunol 108, 516520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Grimshaw, KEC, Maskell, J, Oliver, EM, et al. (2014) Diet and food allergy development during infancy: birth cohort study findings using prospective food diary data. J Allergy Clin Immunol 133, 511519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Luoto, R, Kalliomäki, M, Laitinen, K, et al. (2011) Initial dietary and microbiological environments deviate in normal-weight compared to overweight children at 10 years of age. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 52, 9095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
33. Colak, H, Dulgergil, CT, Dalli, M, et al. (2013) Early childhood caries update: a review of causes, diagnoses and treatments. J Nat Sci Biol Med 4, 2938.Google Scholar
34. Peres, M, Sheiham, A, Liu, P, et al. (2016) Sugar consumption and changes in dental caries from childhood to adolescence. J Dent Res 95, 388394.Google Scholar
35. Tzioumis, E, Kay, M, Wright, M, et al. (2016) Health effects of available complementary foods: a systematic review. www.who.int/nutrition/topics/CF_health_effects_commercially_systematicreview.pdf Google Scholar
36. Dunford, E, Louie, JCY, Byrne, R, et al. (2015) The nutritional profile of baby and toddler food products sold in Australian supermarkets. Matern Child Health J 19, 25982604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Elliott, CD & Conlon, M (2014) Packaged baby and toddler foods: questions of sugar and sodium. Pediatr Obes 10, 149155.Google Scholar
38. Carstairs, SA, Craig, LC, Marais, D, et al. (2016) A comparison of preprepared commercial infant feeding meals with home-cooked recipes. Arch Dis Child 101, 10371042.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. McAndrew, F, Thompson, J, Fellows, L, et al. (2012) Infant Feeding Survey 2010. Leeds: Health and Social Care Information Centre.Google Scholar
40. Robinson, S, Marriott, L, Poole, J, et al. (2007) Dietary patterns in infancy: the importance of maternal and family influences on feeding practice. Br J Nutr 98, 10291037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Tarrant, RC, Younger, KM, Sheridan-Pereira, M, et al. (2010) Factors associated with weaning practices in term infants: a prospective observational study in Ireland. Br J Nutr 104, 15441554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42. O’Donovan, SM, Murray, DM, Hourihane, JO, et al. (2015) Adherence with early infant feeding and complementary feeding guidelines in the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study. Public Health Nutr 18, 110.Google Scholar
43. Bresson, J & Le Bris, M (2012) Nouvelles données sur l’alimentation des enfants ages de 4 à 24 mois en France (New data on feeding children aged 4–24 months in France). www.nutripro.nestle.fr Google Scholar
44. Foterek, K, Hilbig, A & Alexy, U (2013) Breastfeeding and weaning practices in the DONALD Study – age and time trends. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 58, 361367.Google Scholar
45. Foterek, K, Hilbig, A & Alexy, U (2015) Associations between commercial complementary food consumption and fruit and vegetable intake in children. Results of the DONALD study. Appetite 85, 8490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46. Pani, P, Carletti, C, Knowles, A, et al. (2014) Patterns of nutrients’ intake at six months in the northeast of Italy: a cohort study. BMC Pediatr 14, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47. Ghisolfi, J, Bocquet, A, Bresson, J-L, et al. (2013) Les aliments industriels (hors laits et céréales) destinés aux nourrissons et enfants en bas âge: un progrès diététique? (Processed baby foods for infants and young children: a dietary advance? A position paper by the Committee on Nutrition of the French Society of Paediatrics). Arch Pediatr 20, 523532.Google Scholar
48. Australian Institute of Health & Welfare (2011) 2010 Australian National Infant Feeding Survey. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.Google Scholar
49. Briefel, RR, Reidy, K, Karwe, V, et al. (2004) Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study: improvements needed in meeting infant feeding recommendations. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 3137.Google Scholar
50. Siega-Riz, AM, Deming, DM, Reidy, KC, et al. (2010) Food consumption patterns of infants and toddlers: where are we now? J Am Diet Assoc 110, S38S51.Google Scholar
51. Statistica (2016) Statistics and facts on the baby food market in the U.S. https://www.statista.com/topics/1218/baby-food-market/ Google Scholar
52. Hoddinott, P, Craig, L, Britten, J, et al. (2010) A prospective study exploring the early infant feeding experiences of parents and their significant others during the first 6 months of life: what would make a difference? http://www.healthscotland.com/documents/4720.aspx Google Scholar
53. Synnott, K, Bogue, J, Edwards, CA, et al. (2007) Parental perceptions of feeding practices in five European countries: an exploratory study. Eur J Clin Nutr 61, 946956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54. Maslin, K, Galvin, AD, Shepherd, S, et al. (2015) A qualitative study of mothers’ perceptions of weaning and the use of commercial infant food in the United Kingdom. Matern Paediatr J 1, 1000103.Google Scholar
55. Caton, SJ, Ahern, SM & Hetherington, MM (2011) Vegetables by stealth. An exploratory study investigating the introduction of vegetables in the weaning period. Appetite 57, 816825.Google Scholar
56. Betoko, A, Charles, MA, Hankard, R, et al. (2013) Infant feeding patterns over the first year of life: influence of family characteristics. Eur J Clin Nutr 67, 631637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57. Schwartz, C, Madrelle, J, Vereijken, CMJL, et al. (2013) Complementary feeding and “donner les bases du gout” (providing the foundation of taste). A qualitative approach to understand weaning practices, attitudes and experiences by French mothers. Appetite 71, 321331.Google Scholar
58. Maier, A, Chabanet, C, Schaal, B, et al. (2007) Food-related sensory experience from birth through weaning: contrasted patterns in two nearby European regions. Appetite 49, 429440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59. Kim, LP, Whaley, SE, Gradziel, PH, et al. (2013) Mothers prefer fresh fruits and vegetables over jarred baby fruits and vegetables in the new Special Supplemental Nutrition Program For Women, Infants, and Children food package. J Nutr Educ Behav 45, 723727.Google Scholar
60. Boak, R, Virgo-Milton, M, Hoare, A, et al. (2016) Choosing foods for infants: a qualitative study of the factors that influence mothers. Child Care Health Dev 42, 359369.Google Scholar
61. Jabs, J & Devine, CM (2006) Time scarcity and food choices: an overview. Appetite 47, 196204.Google Scholar
62. Beauchamp, GK & Mennella, JA (2009) Early flavor learning and its impact on later feeding behavior. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 48, Suppl. 1, S25S30.Google Scholar
63. Birch, LL & Doub, AE (2014) Learning to eat: birth to age 2 y. Am J Clin Nutr 99, 723728.Google Scholar
64. Nicklaus, S (2011) Children’s acceptance of new foods at weaning. Role of practices of weaning and of food sensory properties. Appetite 57, 812815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65. Schwartz, C, Scholtens, PA, Lalanne, A, et al. (2011) Development of healthy eating habits early in life. Review of recent evidence and selected guidelines. Appetite 57, 796807.Google Scholar
66. Birch, LL, Gunder, L, Grimm-Thomas, K, et al. (1998) Infants’ consumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods. Appetite 30, 283295.Google Scholar
67. Ruel, MT (2003) Operationalizing dietary diversity: a review of measurement issues and research priorities. J Nutr 133, 3911S3926S.Google Scholar
68. Cox, DR, Skinner, JD, Carruth, BR, et al. (1997) A food Variety Index for Toddlers (VIT): development and application. J Am Diet Assoc 97, 13821386.Google Scholar
69. Nwaru, BI, Takkinen, HM, Kaila, M, et al. (2014) Food diversity in infancy and the risk of childhood asthma and allergies. J Allergy Clin Immunol 133, 10841091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70. Hurley, KM & Black, MM (2010) Commercial baby food consumption and dietary variety in a statewide sample of infants receiving benefits from the special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 15371541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
71. Coulthard, H, Harris, G & Emmett, P (2010) Long-term consequences of early fruit and vegetable feeding practices in the United Kingdom. Public Health Nutr 13, 20442051.Google Scholar
72. Carstairs, S, Marais, D, Craig, L, et al. (2016) Seafood inclusion in commercial main meal early years’ food products. Matern Child Nutr 12, 860868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73. Zimmerman, M (2016) The importance of adequate iodine during pregnancy and infancy. In Hidden Hunger: Malnutrition and the First 1,000 Days of Life: Causes, Consequences and Solutions. World Review of Nutrition and Dietetics. vol. 115, pp. 118124 [HK Biesalski and RE Black, editors]. Basel: Karger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
74. Koletzko, B, Lien, E, Agostoni, C, et al. (2008) The roles of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy, lactation and infancy: review of current knowledge and consensus recommendations. J Perinat Med 36, 514.Google Scholar
75. Riediger, ND, Othman, RA, Suh, M, et al. (2009) A systemic review of the roles of n-3 fatty acids in health and disease. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 668679.Google Scholar
76. Commission of the European Communities (2006) Commission Directive 2006/141/EC of 22 December 2006 on infant formulae and follow-on formulae and amending Directive 1999/21/EC. https://www.fsai.ie/uploadedFiles/Dir2006_141.pdf Google Scholar
77. Loughrill, E & Zand, N (2016) An investigation into the fatty acid content of selected fish-based commercial infant foods in the UK and the impact of commonly practice re-heating treatments used by parents for the preparation of infant formula milks. Food Chem 197, 783789.Google Scholar
78. Nudda, A, McGuire, MK, Battacone, G, et al. (2011) Documentation of fatty acid profiles in lamb meat and lamb-based infant foods. J Food Sci 76, 4347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79. Dube, K, Schwartz, J, Mueller, MJ, et al. (2010) Complementary food with low (8%) or high (12%) meat content as source of dietary iron: a double-blinded randomized controlled trial. Eur J Nutr 49, 1118.Google Scholar
80. Mauch, CE, Perry, RA, Magarey, AM, et al. (2015) Dietary intake in Australian children aged 4–24 months: consumption of meat and meat alternatives. Br J Nutr 113, 17611772.Google Scholar
81. Melø, R, Gellein, K, Evje, L, et al. (2008) Minerals and trace elements in commercial infant food. Food Chem Toxicol 46, 33393342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
82. Zand, N, Chowdhry, BZ, Zotor, FB, et al. (2011) Essential and trace elements content of commercial infant foods in the UK. Food Chem 128, 123128.Google Scholar
83. Zand, N, Chowdhry, BZ, Wray, DS, et al. (2012) Elemental content of commercial “ready to-feed” poultry and fish based infant foods in the UK. Food Chem 135, 27962801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
84. Loughrill, E, Govinden, P & Zand, N (2016) Vitamins A and E content of commercial infant foods in the UK: a cause for concern? Food Chem 210, 5662.Google Scholar
85. van den Boom, S, Kimber, AC & Morgan, JB (1997) Nutritional composition of home-prepared baby meals in Madrid. Comparison with commercial products in Spain and home-made meals in England. Acta Paediatr 86, 5762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
86. Randhawa, S, Kakuda, Y, Wong, CL, et al. (2012) Microbial safety, nutritive value and residual pesticide levels are comparable among commercial, laboratory and homemade baby food samples: a pilot study. Open Nutr J 6, 8996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
87. Hilbig, A, Foterek, K, Kersting, M, et al. (2015) Home-made and commercial complementary meals in German infants: results of the DONALD study. J Hum Nutr Diet 28, 613622.Google Scholar
88. Walker, R & Goran, M (2015) Laboratory determined sugar content and composition of commercial infant formulas, baby foods and common grocery items targeted to children. Nutrients 7, 58505867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
89. Moynihan, P & Kelly, S (2014) Effect of caries on restricting sugar intake: systematic review to update WHO guidelines. J Dent Res 93, 818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
90. Maalouf, J, Cogswell, ME, Yuan, K, et al. (2015) Top sources of dietary sodium from birth to age 24 mo, United States, 2003–2010. Am J Clin Nutr 101, 10211028.Google Scholar
91. Stein, LJ, Cowart, BJ & Beauchamp, GK (2012) The development of salty taste acceptance is related to dietary experience in human infants: a prospective study. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 123129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
92. Zand, N, Chowdhry, BZ, Pollard, LV, et al. (2015) Commercial “ready-to-feed” infant foods in the UK: macro-nutrient content and composition. Matern Child Nutr 11, 202214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
93. Smithers, LG, Golley, RK, Brazionis, L, et al. (2012) Dietary patterns of infants and toddlers are associated with nutrient intakes. Nutrients 4, 935948.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Current issues regarding commercial infant food intake in developed countries.

Figure 1

Table 1 Summary of studies on macronutrient composition with emphasis on sugar, fat and salt content

Figure 2

Table 2 Summary of studies on micronutrient and trace element composition