Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T03:31:29.842Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Constructing a New Measure of Macropartisanship Disaggregated by Race and Ethnicity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2022

Joshua J. Dyck
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, USA
Gregg B. Johnson*
Affiliation:
Valparaiso University, Valparaiso, IN, USA
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Macropartisanship is a measure of aggregate trends in party identification in the mass public that allows researchers to track partisanship dynamically. In previous research, macropartisanship was found to vary in concert with major political events and forces like presidential approval and the economy. However, studying macropartisanship as an aggregate trend assumes that group dynamics within the measure are equivalent. We present a series of new measures of macropartisanship using Stimson’s (2018) dyad ratio approach disaggregated by race and ethnicity. We detail the creation of measures for White, Latino, and Black macropartisanship from 1983 to 2016 using more than 500 surveys from CBS News and CBS/New York Times. The resulting data collection is publicly available and can be downloaded in monthly, quarterly, or yearly format. Our initial analysis of these data show that thinking about macropartisanship as a single aggregate measure masks important and significant variation in our understanding of party identification. Change in the measures are uncorrelated. Latino macropartisanship is more volatile and responds more to economic conditions, Black macropartisanship is very stable and has become more Democratic in response to increased polarization, while White macropartisanship has become less responsive to economic conditions as has become more Republican as Republicans have moved to the right.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Race, Ethnicity, and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

1. Introduction

The importance of the development of party identification to the study of American political behavior cannot be overstated. Often acquired early in life, party identification acts as a great regulator and predictor of political action. It provides shortcuts to voters who are uncertain and organizing mechanisms for information to highly informed voters. And, it is generally quite stable. Changes to an individual’s party identification are the exception rather than the rule. While static identification is the norm for individuals, changes observed in macropartisanship in many instances move in predictable and meaningful ways (MacKuen et al. Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989).

Aggregate measures like macropartisanship, however, miss important realities about American history and democracy. According to the 2018 Current Population Survey, just over 60% of the U.S. population identifies as White, not Hispanic, which means 4 in 10 Americans come from a historically underrepresented racial or ethnic group. Notably, American society is often mischaracterized as fitting within the liberal democracy paradigm, and mono-theorizing in this way ignores rampant structures of inequality that have been critical to American political development. One cannot understand American democracy without a critical theory of inequality that accounts for race, ethnicity, and gender and the structures that created and perpetuated that inequality (Smith Reference Smith1993). Indeed, in at least one account of party change, it is White Americans’ attitudes about race that lead to durable change in party coalitions for decades (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1986, Reference Carmines and Stimson1989). While disputes occur in this literature about a variety of topics ranging from the stability of party identification (e.g. MacKuen et al. Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989; but see Green et al. Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998; Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler2002) to the nature of issue evolutions (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989; but see Abramowitz and Saunders Reference Abramowitz and Saunders1998), little in these articles addresses the apparent stability in the partisanship of African Americans, or the apparent instability of the partisanship of Latinos, and how they differ fundamentally from those of non-Latino White Americans because of identity, shared history, and the politics of race, ethnicity and inequality. To be sure, a great deal of research exists on the political behavior of African Americans and Latinos. But our aggregate measures of macropartisanship need to be disaggregated if we are to be honest about the fact that (1) these communities differ in their partisanship, its stability and change, and (2) we want macropartisanship and its responsiveness to represent something other than White macropartisanship.

In this paper, we rectify this problem by introducing a new dataset of macropartisanship, disaggregated by race and ethnicity such that yearly, quarterly, and monthly calculations of White macropartisanship, Black macropartisanship, and Latino macropartisanship can be presented from 1983 to 2016 for the United States. These data were created through an aggregation of every CBS News and CBS/New York Times survey containing party ID from 1983 to 2016 and by applying Stimson’s (Reference Stimson2018) dyad ratio algorithm. After demonstrating the measure’s validity, our initial analysis shows that traditional movers of macropartisanship, presidential approval and consumer confidence, as well as elite polarization, affect racial and ethnic groups quite differently. We provide for a public release of the dataFootnote 1 and provide checks on external validity to deal with some potential pitfalls of using this data and time period.

2. Black and Latino macropartisanship—the case for disaggregation

We situate our inquiry at the intersection of the literature on party identification, notably its stability, and the literature on party identification for Blacks and Latinos. The conventional wisdom in American political behavior dates back to the development of the Michigan model (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Converse, Miller and Stokes1960). Individuals develop party identification through a preadult socialization process. Lacking information about most basic matters of politics, party identification becomes a central organizing factor in personal identity for determining political behavior. Research has tended to show that party identification is stable over the life cycle (Green et al. Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998; Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler2002; Niemi and Jennings Reference Niemi and Jennings1991; Sears and Funk Reference Sears and Funk1999).

However, MacKuen et al. (Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989) argue that the aggregate trend in macropartisanship is sensitive to short-term fluctuations in observable parts of the political environment. Most notably, they argue that changes in election results, presidential approval, economic conditions, and dramatic events can change party identification trends in predictable ways at the macro level—and that those changes can have long-lasting effects, particularly changes related to economic fluctuations. While Green et al. (Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998) and others (Campbell Reference Campbell2010) dispute the volatility in party identification observed in macropartisanship, the measurement strategy opens up the notion that we can measure aggregate trends in party identification and that their fluctuations are meaningful.

While this literature created important new insights into political behavior, we argue there are several reasons that these expectations are on faulty footing for both Blacks and Latinos. For Blacks, we have for decades observed partisan identification and voting rates for the Democratic Party in the 85–95% range. Other factors also distinguish Black voting behavior. For instance, higher levels of religiosity among African Americans do not lead Blacks to be more likely to support Republican candidates as it does for Whites (McDaniel and Ellison Reference McDaniel and Ellison2008). Furthermore, economic differences are not apparent in Black party identification (Dawson Reference Dawson1994). Dawson (Reference Dawson1994), whose theory underlies much of what is understood about Black partisanship and voting behavior today, argued that Blacks as individuals make voting decisions based on a community utility heuristic that considers the fate of the community as a whole. When we understand this, we come to understand how Black party ID may differ from White party ID—theories of White partisanship rooted in either the socialization (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Converse, Miller and Stokes1960) or running tally (Fiorina Reference Fiorina1981) framework do not use a group heuristic that focuses identification and voting behavior nearly as clearly and would allow for greater flexibility in responses to variations in the political environment (presidential approval, economic performance, etc.) A micro change in the economy will likely not be a persuasive as the fact that the Democratic Party is associated as the party of civil rights (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989) and that the Republican Party is viewed as the party of racial conservatism and often has used racialized appeals to court support from White voters (Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004).

And if the foundations of Black and White party ID are different, there is good reason to believe that Latino party ID may be less firmly rooted than the party ID of Whites and African Americans. There are two primary reasons why we might expect more volatile patterns of partisan change among Latinos (Dyck et al. Reference Dyck, Johnson and Wasson2012). The first explanation is a party conversion story. While it is true that party ID is generally stable for most Americans, comparatively speaking, immigrants have weaker party attachments (Cain et al. Reference Cain, Kiewiet and Uhlaner1991; Uhlaner and Garcia Reference Uhlaner and Garcia2005) and are less likely to have socialized bonds of party identification that span generations. The second explanation is a story of population change and immigration. According to the Pew Research Center, the median age among Latinos is 15 years younger than the median age for White Americans (Patten Reference Patten2016). Therefore, any observed changes in party identification may be driven by changes in the underlying population. To the extent that we observe any changes in Latino party ID, individuals may be changing party ID, but the process of generational replacement, or perhaps more accurately, generational growth, may be the underlying story. As Hajnal and Lee (Reference Hajnal and Lee2011, 89) write, “we maintain that the unique context of immigration implicates three distinct factors: information uncertainty, ideological ambivalence, and identity formation.” Under these conditions, we would expect that Latinos may not have the firmly rooted partisanship (Green et al. Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler2002) of non-Latino Whites or of African Americans. In short, we would expect greater stability in Black party identification and greater volatility in Latino party identification, but we can only examine this by disaggregating macropartisanship data by race and ethnicity.

3. Constructing the dataset

In order to disaggregate macropartisanship by race and ethnicity, we first need to determine a source for constructing such a measure. In the present day, we can turn to some large-scale data collection efforts like the Cooperative Congressional Election Survey (CCES), which surveys around 25,000–50,000 Americans every year. Other organizations like Latino Decisions do an admirable job of accurately surveying Latinos. The CCES, however, has only been around since 2006 and Latino Decisions was founded in 2007. Other projects like National Black Election Survey and the Latino National Survey have been fielded in 1 or 2 years. Pew, likewise, has some data trends, like the 2000–2006 series of surveys of Latinos.

Given the literature’s expectations on the stickiness of partisan identification, studying changes across race and ethnicity requires a longer time series than these datasets can provide. Consequently, we make use of the best available data from the 1980s and 1990s to construct an aggregate overtime measure of macropartisanship for Whites, Blacks, and Latinos. In fact, we turn to the same CBS/NYTimes polls used across a number of studies of macropartisanship (e.g. Green et al. Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998). The CBS and CBS/NYTimes data archive is best suited to this project for a variety of reasons. First, CBS/NYTimes has long asked a question on race and started asking respondents if they identified as Hispanic in 1983.Footnote 2 Second, unlike Gallup, CBS/NYTimes uses the classic wording of party ID consistent with the American National Election Study/Michigan Survey Research Center. The Gallup wording has been found in previous research to produce a noisier measure of partisanship subject to considerably more short-term variation, that is, an artifact of measurement error (Abramson and Ostrom Reference Abramson and Ostrom1991). Finally, while there are some other long-standing data collections available in either the ICPSR or Roper archives (e.g. ABC/Washington Post), only the CBS News and CBS/NYTimes polls go back to the early 1980s allowing for the construction of a longer time series.

We gathered all available CBS News and CBS/NYTimes surveys housed at the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research and the Roper Center for Public Opinion Research for the period 1983–2016. We found a total of 392 CBS/NYTimes and 181 CBS News datasets that cover this period that asked respondents both their party ID and race/ethnicity, for a total of 573. We encountered data errors in a 12 of the datasets, giving us a total of 561 usable datasets.Footnote 3 We also note that scores of early datasets failed to ask respondents if they identified as Latino or Hispanic, so these surveys could not be incorporated into our analysis. However, these 561 datasets allow us to construct a disaggregated time series of party identification by race/ethnicity that dates back more than three decades. Footnote 4 For our study period, we have usable data for Whites, African Americans, and Latinos. We also collected data for those identifying as Asian and “Other” in their response to race, but their numbers in many of the surveys was very low. Furthermore, CBS/NYT surveys only regularly added Asian as a racial category in the late 1990s, so much of our sample lacks historical data for the fastest growing immigrant group today.

Our approach has some obvious limitations. First, each survey has a limited number of Black and Latino respondents. Across the 561 datasets, we have a total of 54,409 Black and 33,476 Latino respondents, that is an average of 97.0 Blacks and 58.4 Latinos per survey, meaning the CBS/NYT polls surveyed about 1650 Blacks and just over 1100 Latino respondents in an average year.

While the literature does expect higher volatility for Latino party identification due to the distinct socialization process for newer immigrant groups, we cannot completely rule out the possibility that a portion of this volatility could be a function of the small number of Latinos per survey. Second, the number of surveys per year is inconsistent and the time of year in which the surveys are conducted also varies considerably. In 2 years, 1983 and 1985, we have only two surveys with complete data, whereas after 2000 we have at least 12 surveys each year. Similarly, far more surveys are conducted in election years, while the months immediately preceding an election have the highest number of surveys. A final limitation of the data is that the CBS/NYTimes and CBS News survey is a three-point measure of party identification (Campbell Reference Campbell2010). Using a more comprehensive seven-point scale found in the ANES demonstrates that weak party identifiers and independent leaners are typically indistinguishable (Keith et al. Reference Keith, Magleby, Nelson, Orr and Westlye1992). Consequently, our measure may hide shifts in macropartisanship that take place between pure independents and independent leaners. Due to their distinct process of political socialization, Latinos may simply be more likely to identify with these independent leaners, despite behaving like weak partisans.

Nevertheless, these data provide the most comprehensive way to construct a measure of macropartisanship that can be disaggregated by race and ethnicity.Footnote 5 As noted above, the number of Black and Latino respondents was often small in a given survey, but as we will show in the next section, when we aggregate the data, this concern is greatly reduced. For example, if we look at quarterly data then we typically have over 400 African American respondents and about 280 Latino respondents. These samples are larger than those of racial and ethnic subgroups in seminal micro-level analyses of group interest and shared fate (c.f. Chong and Kim’s Reference Chong and Kim2006).Footnote 6 Additionally, we find that while macropartisanship changes for all racial and ethnic groups, the standard deviations do not vary substantially over time, meaning changes are not likely related simply to changing sample sizes. In sum, we are confident that changes in macropartisanship across racial and ethnic groups is not simply a function of sample size.

4. Measuring macropartisanship

4.1 The standard measure

The standard measure of macropartisanship reflects the relative distribution of the two-party partisan identifiers at any given moment. This is simply calculated as the number of Democrats in a sample divided by the number of Democrats and Republicans in the sample multiplied by 100 (Erikson et al. Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson1998; Green et al. Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998; MacKuen et al. Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989). We find that the Democratic Party held a small, consistent advantage with a median of 53.7% of the two-party identifiers. We also see that the Republican Party gained ground in the 1980s, lost ground in the middle of Clinton’s term, gained ground in the early part of the presidency of George W. Bush, but again lost ground in the Obama years (see Figure 1). That macropartisanship slightly favors the Democratic Party and appears to track with presidential approval and economic fortunes is consistent with expectations. We also see that overall macropartisanship is fairly stable, typically hovering around 54% and very rarely going below 50% or over 58%. Essentially, overall macropartisanship indicates a narrowly divided American public that makes only slight course corrections over time.

Figure 1. Overall Macropartisanship using CBS/NYTimes Data. June 1983 to August 2016Footnote 7

While aggregate macropartisanship demonstrates both the overall stability and subtle shifts in the population, it fails to examine whether stability and shifts hold across racial and ethnic divisions. Using the same methodology mentioned above, we calculated macropartisanship measures for Blacks, Latinos, and Whites and plotted them over the course of our study (see Figure 2). For African Americans, we see an enormous gap in macropartisanship, with more than 90% of partisan identifiers falling in the Democratic camp. Furthermore, even though there are more than eight times as many non-Latino Whites than African Americans across all of our datasets, they have similar standard deviations. This provides prima facie evidence that African American partisanship is not only distinct from overall macropartisanship but is also more stable. This is consistent with the Dawson’s (Reference Dawson1994) contention that Black party identification is strongly predicted by linked fate, which downplays short-term forces. One of the remarkable things to observe here is that, running counter to conventional wisdom, the nomination and election of Barack Obama in 2008 produced little movement in Black macropartisanship, as group identity had already placed 9 out of every 10 African American partisans squarely within the Democratic Party. If anything, African Americans began trending even more toward the Democratic Party in the early 1990s.

Figure 2. Macropartisanship by Race and Ethnicity. June 1983 to August 2016Footnote 8

For Latinos, we also find that the Democratic Party enjoys a significant advantage, though the advantage is not nearly so great, with almost 7 in 10 partisans identifying with the Democrats. We also note that Latino macropartisanship appears less stable. The passage of the IRCA in 1986 seems to have garnered the party of Reagan some additional support, though Latinos drifted back to the Democratic Party for most of the Clinton administration; this also coincides with major shocks to the party system that occurred in California, where the Republican Party targeted Latinos and paid the price in terms of a generation of lost party identifiers (Bowler et al. Reference Bowler, Nicholson and Segura2006; Dyck et al. Reference Dyck, Johnson and Wasson2012). The Republican Party again picked up support in the wake of 9/11, before Latinos moved away from the Republican Party during the Bush administration in the later years.Footnote 9 Democrats gained their biggest advantage in the wake of the Great Recession and the election of Obama but lost ground as the Spanish-language media and the activist community became increasingly concerned over deportations during Obama’s first term in office. However, support for the Democratic Party among Latinos appears to spike in presidential election years. The greater variation observed among Latinos is consistent with the notion that many from the Latino community lack firm roots and ties that span generations with one of the American parties, yet return to the Democratic Party in important election years, all of which is consistent with Hajnal and Lee (Reference Hajnal and Lee2011) core arguments.Footnote 10 The Democratic advantage, the volatility in Latino partisanship, and the recent strengthening of this advantage are consistent with other recent work in the field (see Hajnal Reference Hajnal2020). Regardless, Figure 2 supports our contention that Latino macropartisanship is distinct from overall macropartisanship.

For the non-Latino White population, we find a small advantage for the Republican Party. Non-Latino Whites drift away from the Democratic Party in the Reagan years consistent with the movement of White men away from the Democratic Party and the creation of the durable gender gap (Kaufmann and Petrocik Reference Kaufmann and Petrocik1999). White macropartisanship ebbs toward the Democratic Party a bit in the George W. Bush administration’s later years, and then slightly away from the Democrats during the Obama years, reflecting the thermostatic nature of public opinion trends more generally (Erikson et al. Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson2002). However, all of these shifts are small. In short, non-Latino Whites and African Americans seem to shift gradually, supporting Green et al. (Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998) contention that macropartisanship changes slowly, as if eroded by the wind, not quickly, as if eroded by the tide (see Figure 2). White partisans are simply far more Republican than African Americans or Latinos, at times appear to drift in the opposite direction of African Americans, and are more stable than Latino partisans.Footnote 11 In short, the overall measure of macropartisanship found in Figure 1 masks important variations by race and ethnicity found in Figure 2.

4.2 Dyad ratio algorithm

While we have over 500 surveys to construct this 34 year time series of data, the inconsistent time between surveys has to be considered, and the data transformed in some way to correct for the fact that at different times the precision of our estimates differ and that there are some time windows where there are holes in the data. Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm (1991, Reference Stimson2015, Reference Stimson2018) presents a solution for this problem. This factor-analytic algorithm compiles time series data by weighting each individual survey by how well it correlates with the latent concept—macropartisanship (Carlin et al. Reference Carlin, Hartlyn, Hellwig, Love, Martínez-Gallardo and Singer2018; Claassen Reference Claassen2019). Point estimates are then exponentially smoothed (see Stimson Reference Stimson2018 for a detailed explanation). Previous macropartisanship studies and policy mood studies used this approach in the United States (see Enns Reference Enns2016; Stimson, MacKuen, and Erikson Reference Stimson, Mackuen and Erikson1995; Erikson et al. Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson2002), while comparativists use this approach to create cross-national measures of presidential approval (see Arnold et al. Reference Arnold Christian and Wiesehomeier2017; Carlin and Hellwig Reference Carlin and Hellwig2019).

Described by Stimson as an algorithm “for building a continuous regular time series from the scraps of dated survey results that are typically available for public opinion analysis,” the algorithm stores data in a sparse matrix, with variable-indexed rows and time-indexed columns (Stimson Reference Stimson2015, 1). Therefore, within each column, there is a time step with a corresponding variable value. In this case, that variable is macropartisanship as the Democratic percentage of two-party PID. The matrix is sparse because surveys are only performed every so often or clustered around certain events (like elections), so most of the time steps will have no data. A dyad is the values for the same variable at different time steps. The dyad ratio, then, is a ratio of one of these dyads, or the ratio of the value of the variable at one time step to the value at a different time step. The algorithm uses both forward and backward recursion to calculate two dyad ratios at each time step. These dyad ratio estimates in both directions are then averaged to produce an estimate that uses all the available information and that weights all the items equally. Stimson’s implementation of the algorithm also smooths the data and accounts for validity estimation.

This is important because it allows us to generate monthly, quarterly, and annual estimates of macropartisanship from 1983 to 2016, even when the frequency of polling varies considerably over time. Here, we see immense value in having 561 surveys over 34 years and we go well beyond other available estimates of macropartisanship that might be gleaned from a single number taken from the American National Election Study or the CCES.

Drawing on Stimson (Reference Stimson2018), we start by recalculating macropartisanship for the entire population; here we present the monthly version of the data. A simple line graph of our measure of macropartisanship created by Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm sees that these data closely matche what we found in the previous section where we plotted cubic splines of the raw data (see Figure 3). In the raw data, we found the mean of macropartisanship was 53.84, while in our dyad ratio algorithm data have a mean of 53.91, meaning the Democratic Party tended to hold a small advantage. The Republican Party reached approximate parity in the late 1980s until the mid-1990s, again in the immediate wake of 9/11, and during the height of the Tea Party in 2010 and 2011. The Democratic Party held advantages early in the Reagan Administration, in the second term of the Clinton Administration, the second Bush II term, and in Obama’s second term. Again, these ebbs and flows in this measure of macropartisanship largely track with the larger literature, with Republicans gaining ground when a president of their party is popular or when a Democratic President loses popular support, while the Democratic Party gains ground under the opposite conditions.

Figure 3. Overall Macropartisanship using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm. June 1983 to August 2016

Now that we have an overall measure of macropartisanship, we can also use Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm to recalculate macropartisanship by race and ethnicity (see Figure 4). Again, we see the Stimson method produces a figure that closely tracks the cubic spline plots of the raw data found in the previous section (see Figure 2). Using the dyad ratio method, we find that over 9 out of 10 African American partisans identify with the Democratic Party, with a mean macropartisanship score of 93.31, compared to 93.22 in the raw data. The small shift toward the Republican Party that took place in the 1980s to the mid-1990s was overwhelmed by a shift back to the Democratic Party starting in the mid-1990s. That stabilized even before the nomination and inauguration of Barack Obama.

Figure 4. Macropartisanship by Race and Ethnicity using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm June 1983 to August 2016

Latinos also follow the same pattern when we compare the data using the dyad ratio algorithm (see Figure 4) and the raw data (see Figure 2). We find a mean macropartisanship score of 68.58 for Latinos using the dyad ratio algorithm, while the raw data produced a mean of 68.49. We also see the same movements of Latinos over time, as well as the higher rate of volatility among Latinos compared with African Americans or non-Latino Whites.Footnote 12

Finally, we turn to the non-Latino White segment of the population. Again, the use of dyad ratio algorithm (see Figure 4) results in similar trends as when we plotted the raw data earlier (see Figure 2). The Democratic Party’s advantage among the White population evaporated during the Reagan Administration and, with the exception of the latter years of the second Bush II Administration, the Republican Party held a consistent advantage. The macropartisanship score of 46.80 in the raw data is matched by a score of 46.90 in the monthly data using Stimson’s algorithm. We can also see that non-Latino Whites do not necessarily follow the same patterns as the other groupings. While African Americans stuck with the Democratic Party during the Obama Administration and Latinos trended toward the Democrats, the White population moved toward the Republican Party. In sum, Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm produces substantially similar results to what was seen in the plots found in the previous section and allows us to develop monthly, quarterly, and annual measures of macropartisanship by race and ethnicity.Footnote 13

4.3 Independents

One limitation of the preceding analysis is that it calculates macropartisanship as a relative measure of the two-party PID. This is interesting if the variation in those identifying as independent is stagnant. However, macropartisanship largely ignores the dealignment/realignment debate found in the literature (Dalton and Flanagan Reference Dalton and Flanagan2017; Hawley and Sagarzazu Reference Hawley and Sagarzazu2012; Lawrence Reference Lawrence2018). Additionally, the volatility of Latino macropartisanship and the theoretical roots of that instability, we believe that this new dataset also unveils other interesting patterns worthy of further exploration. As has been argued by Hajnal and Lee (Reference Hajnal and Lee2011), independent party identification among groups with less rooted partisanship, particularly Latinos, reflects non-partisan or apolitical origins that come from a group feeling disenfranchised and disconnected from the political process. Therefore, beyond the relative strength of voting behavior for the Democratic party as captured by the share of the two-party PID, variation in independent identification may also give us an insight into macropartisanship among Latinos.

We begin with a presentation of dyad ratio estimates of independent identification for the overall sample (see Figure 5). Unlike changes in Democratic or Republican Party identification, or even macropartisanship, independent identification is remarkably stable in the overall sample. After an initial decline, independents consistently form a bit over one-third of the sample from 1987 for about 20 years, with a mean of 36.73%. However, starting around 2007, we start to see a sustained upward trend in independent identification. For the last 5 years of our data, we find independent identification averaged 41.87%, a five-point rise from the overall average.

Figure 5. Overall Independent Identification using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm. June 1983 to August 2016

Next, we present the dyad ratio estimates of independent identification for African Americans, Latinos, and non-Latino Whites in Figure 6. Here, we start with the non-Latino White independent identification, as it most closely follows the overall trend, with an average of 37.21%. It also follows the initial decline, 20 years of stability, and then uptick overall the last decade of our data. The Black population consistently demonstrates lower independent identification, with an average of only 26.43%. They also demonstrate a sharp upward trend starting in around 2012, after the uptick seen in the overall population and among non-Latino Whites. Turning to the Latino population, we see a fair amount of overlap with the White population, averaging 37.72%. More importantly, we see a sharp upward trend in independent identification starting around 2008, with more than 50% of Latinos identifying as independents in the final 3 years of the surveys. This provides prima facie evidence that dealignment is a substantial trend among Latino respondents in the last decade.

Figure 6. Independents by Race and Ethnicity using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm. June 1983 to August 2016

This decline in party identification in both major parties is hidden in macropartisanship data, and especially hidden without considering variation by race and ethnicity. To be sure, one limitation of our data is that we do not have information on the partisan leanings of independents over the study period. As has been well established (Keith et al. Reference Keith, Magleby, Nelson, Orr and Westlye1992), independents who lean toward the Democratic or Republican parties tend to have voting behavior patterns that look much more similar to those of weak party identifiers than they do to pure independents. Therefore, we do not wish to overstate these trends, as picking “leaning Independent” instead of “weak Partisan” can be viewed as a social desirability effect (Klar and Krupnikov Reference Klar and Krupnikov2016). Still, as Hajnal and Lee (Reference Hajnal and Lee2011) have argued, independent identification means something different for Latinos and we are especially interested by the fact that as Latino macropartisanship has trended toward the Democratic Party, we also observe a higher share of independent identification. While it is beyond the scope of this paper, we believe that this merits further investigation.

5. Validity

In this section, we seek to test the validity of our measures. We do this by (1) comparing the aggregated measure of macropartisanship to the measure constructed by Erikson et al. (Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson2002), (2) comparing the disaggregated measure to the bi-annual data points from the American National Election Study archive,Footnote 14 and (3) by validating that macropartisanship varies with model covariates from previous work (notably presidential approval and consumer confidence). In this last section, we can also observe how the disaggregated models for Whites, Latinos, and Blacks differ from the overall model.

5.1 The original measure

The most direct way to (1) assess the validity and (2) assess the utility of our new measure of macropartisanship is to compare it to the extended data presented by Erikson et al. (Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson2002) available from Stimson’s (Reference Stimson2017) website. While in the previous section we used monthly dyad ratio calculations, in this section we use quarterly dyad ratio calculations, as the MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s dataset contains quarterly measures. In Figure 7, we see that our measure of macropartisanship tracks with MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s original dataset.Footnote 15 We do find that MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s measure is slightly more Democratic than our measure, with means of 54.82% and 53.84, respectively.Footnote 16

Figure 7. MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson (MES) versus CBS/CBSNYTimes Quarterly Measures of Macropartisanship June 1983 to August 2016

In addition to comparing our overall measure of macropartisanship with the original measure created by MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson, we also compare our measure of macropartisanship for non-Latino Whites with MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s original measure. We compare the two because the literature on macropartisanship fails to account for variations by race and ethnicity and the traditional measure was developed when the White population accounted for a far larger proportion of the population. In Figure 8, we see that our measure of macropartisanship for Whites also tracks closely with MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s data.Footnote 17 In sum, our measure of macropartisanship based on CBS and CBS/NYTimes polls, as well as our subset of the non-Latino White population, closely tracks MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s original measure.

Figure 8. MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson (MES) versus CBS/CBSNYTimes Quarterly Measures of White Macropartisanship June 1983 to August 2016

5.2 Validity by comparison with the ANES

A careful reader might wonder how we are certain that our disaggregated measures of macropartisanship are actually measuring what we assert they are measuring. Here, we might be concerned that data collection techniques, particularly for interviewing Latinos, were not as good in the 1980s and 1990s as they are today. While the CBS/NYT surveys did complete Spanish interviews, we know little about their early sampling procedures and can only assume that they are a far cry from what are considered best practices for reaching the Latino population today, particularly the Spanish-speaking Latino population.Footnote 18 As a check on external validity, we check our yearly estimates of macropartisanship for White, Latinos and Blacks against existing data points available from the American National Election Studies.

Figure 9. ANES versus CBS/CBSNYTimes Annual Measures of Macropartisanship 1983 to 2016

The ANES is suitable for this task, as they have consistently taken biennial surveys of approximately 2000 Americans since the 1990s, with many interviews taking place in person for much of the data collection period. Given the ANES is only conducted every 2 or 4 years, here we used Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm to calculate annual measures of macropartisanship.Footnote 19 Remarkably, the data track very well with our annual estimates from CBS News and CBS/New York Times surveys (see Figure 9). For African American respondents, we observe a slight decline in the 1980s and then a small but sustained upward trend identification in the Democratic Party. Even though we are looking at a very small sample size (N = 14), we see a positive and significant correlation (r = 0.48, p = 0.08). Similarly, Latinos show a dip in Democratic Party identification during the 1980s, a return in the Clinton years, another dip in the early part of the Bush II administration, and a sustained move toward the Democrats in both datasets. Again, despite the small sample size, we find a strong statistical correlation (r = 0.78, p < 0.01). Interestingly, we find similar levels of White identification in both datasets but find the weakest statistical relationship (r = 0.28, p = 0.34). Nevertheless, starting in the later years of the Clinton era, we see White macropartisanship track much more closely in both datasets, with a small upward trend in the Bush II administration and a slight downward trend in the Obama years. Overall, a comparison of our new dataset to the ANES data provides strong evidence of external validity, particularly for African American and Latino samples.

5.3 Disaggregated macropartisanship and the known predictors of macropartisanship

Erikson et al. (Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson2002) establish a series of correlates of macropartisanship—notably presidential approval (direct) and economic indicators such as consumer confidence (indirect). As a means of establishing the external and construct validity of disaggregating macropartisanship by race and ethnicity, we examine the relationships between White, Latino, and Black macropartisanship and (1) presidential approval, (2) consumer confidence, (3) political polarization, and (4) presidential administration. Our examination here reveals five central observations. First, there is strong evidence that we are presenting a new measure of macropartisanship that is valid and performs like the previous measure presented by Erikson et al. (Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson2002). Second, we find evidence of a more muted relationship between consumer confidence and macropartisanship, consistent with literature finding that partisan motivated reasoning has subsumed economic voting (Duch et al. Reference Duch, Palmer and Anderson2000). Third, it appears that there is a notable exception to this trend among Latinos, whose partisanship we have shown to be the least stable/most variable; Latino macropartisanship appears most responsive to economic conditions, while macropartisanship for Blacks, and possibly for Whites, is not.Footnote 20 Fourth, increased political polarization, particularly the Republican Party’s sharp shift to the right, has moved Whites to the GOP and away from the Democratic Party. Finally, we also see mixed evidence of administration effects, with the greatest effect seeming to be a substantial shift among Latinos toward the Democratic Party in the Obama years. This shift appears to have occurred even when controlling for traditional factors and polarization.

First, we follow the methodology laid out in MacKuen et al. (Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989) to calculate the mean deviates for macropartisanship. This is simply an observed value for macropartisanship minus the mean for macropartisanship. We calculated this for overall macropartisanship, for Latinos, for Whites, and for African Americans. A score above zero signifies that Democratic Party identification was above average for the observed time period, while a score below zero indicates Democratic identification was below average.

Second, we collected monthly presidential approval data from Gallup (UCSB 2018). Footnote 21 Again, we calculated the mean deviate for presidential approval, so a higher score means a president is more popular than the overall average, while a negative score means a president is less popular. Next, we lag these scores by 1 month so that last month’s presidential approval would predict this month’s macropartisanship. We also multiply this measure of approval by –1 in the case of Republican presidents so that higher approval rates for Republican presidents should lead to lower Democratic Party identification and lower macropartisanship numbers.

Third, we gathered another of the original factors linked with changes in macropartisanship, consumer confidence. Consumer confidence is designed to capture the effect of the economy on changes in partisanship. A good economy will presumably lead to not only more popular presidents, but a shift in partisanship toward the incumbent party (Erikson et al. Reference Erikson, MacKuen and Stimson1998; Green et al. Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler1998; MacKuen et al. Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989). The Consumer Confidence Index (CCI) is collected monthly via survey and surveys were lagged 1 month. Again, we calculated the mean deviate, so a score above zero indicates above average confidence, while a score below zero shows below average consumer confidence. Again, we multiply this economic measure by −1 for Republican administrations.

Fourth, we assembled data on political polarization. Since the advent of Nixon’s Southern strategy, the parties have increasingly diverged and Congress has become increasingly polarized (Lewis Reference Lewis2020). This led to a long process of rebooting the American political process so that liberals identified as Democrats and conservatives as Republicans (Levendusky Reference Levendusky2009). At the elite level, this polarization was asymmetrical, with Republicans in Congress moving much further to the right than Democrats moved to the left. Similarly, Poole and Rosenthal’s (Reference Poole and Rosenthal1997) well-known claim regarding a two-dimensional spatial model, where the second dimension captured social issues, including race, evaporated during the period under study (Hare and Poole Reference Hare and Poole2014; Voteviewblog 2017). Consequently, we calculate elite polarization using the median of first dimension NOMINATE estimates, where we take the median Republican score in a given congress and subtract the median Democratic score to create our measure of polarization (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Poole, Rosenthal, Boche, Rudkin and Sonnet2021).Footnote 22 Based on the polarization literature and the race/ethnicity literature, we expect Latinos, African Americans, and Whites will respond to this polarization accordingly, with Latinos and African Americans increasingly identifying with the Democratic Party and Whites moving to the Republican camp.

Finally, we added a series of presidential dummy variables for each administration included in our study: Reagan, Bush I, Clinton, Bush II, and Obama. Again, this harkens back to MacKuen et al. (Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989) original model that showed substantial variation across administration. We decided not to include events dummies, given this analysis is simply designed to provide additional validation of our measures of macropartisanship.

We start with simple Pearson’s correlation coefficients, presented in Table 1. We look to see if presidential approval or CCI are correlated with overall macropartisanship, or with macropartisanship broken down by race or ethnicity. First, we see that presidential approval is a reliable predictor of macropartisanship across all groups. However, we see that it is substantially weaker for Latinos and African Americans than it is for Whites.

Table 1. Presidential Approval, Consumer Confidence, and Macropartisanship

Pearson’s correlation coefficients. *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01

We conducted both a Hotelling’s T-Test and a Steiger’s Z-Test to determine if any of these correlations were different from one another (Weiss Reference Weiss2011). We found that the relationship between White macropartisanship and presidential approval is significantly stronger than the relationship is for Latino macropartisanship or African American macropartisanship.Footnote 23 The White population simply seems to respond to presidential approval cues more than other groups. Second, we see that CCI is not significant for Whites but is significant for Latinos and African Americans. For Latinos, the effect of a good economy is as predicted. Good economic times lead to shifts in macropartisanship toward the president’s party, while bad times lead to shifts away. However, for African Americans, the reverse is true. Good economic times actually move people away from the president’s party, while bad times move them toward the president. This unusual finding might be a function of who oversaw economic good times, or it might be a function of the distribution of gains during recent economic good times. Either way, this is worth additional investigation in future works. Third, we see that polarization has differential effects across racial and ethnic groups, with the correlation being positive for Latinos and African Americans and negative for Whites. As the parties diverged on policies over the last several decades, we see that African Americans moved even more sharply into the Democratic camp, Latinos moved somewhat to the Democratic camp, while Whites moved substantially toward the Republican camp.Footnote 24 Taken together, presidential approval provides additional evidence for the validity of our data, while consumer confidence introduces some questions.

However, Mackuen et al. (Reference MacKuen, Erikson and Stimson1989) argue that the effect of economic performance on macropartisanship is likely to be indirect, working through its connection to presidential approval. Additionally, we cannot discount the fact that (1) the importance of economic evaluations in macropartisanship may vary by race and (2) may be muted by considerations in individuals and the political environment (see Anderson Reference Anderson2007 for a review of this literature). To consider the effect of presidential approval, consumer confidence, and presidential administration, we present two types of analysis: (1) regression models with a lagged dependent variable (Achen Reference Achen2000; Keele and Kelly Reference Keele and Kelly2006) and (2) Newey–West (Newey and West Reference Newey and West1987) models with six lags (see Table 2). The use of a lagged dependent variable is quite common in time series models given the likelihood of serial autocorrelation. Newey–West estimators not only account for serial autocorrelation but also for heteroskedasticity that is common in time series data.Footnote 25 In these models, we include the variables for presidential approval, consumer confidence, and polarization but also add in presidential administration dummies. We use the Bush II administration as the excluded category as its macropartisanship scores average closest to the mean.Footnote 26

Table 2. Presidential approval, consumer confidence, political polarization, administration, and macropartisanship.

Standard errors in parentheses.

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.10.

These models produce interesting results. We see that presidential approval is positive and significant in all models. A popular president simply brings all racial and ethnic groups toward the president’s party, while an unpopular president hurts the president’s party. It is with economic evaluations and political polarization that differential effects appear. A strong economy matters for Latinos, may matter for Whites, but has no effect on African Americans. This is consistent with our expectations, as weaker Latino partisanship allows for a greater role for the economy. Similarly, whether it is the result of group identity or the skewed distribution of economic gains over the last several decades, African Americans appear unresponsive to economic ups and downs.

Our findings related to political polarization may be the most interesting. Polarization makes no difference for Latinos, clearly matters for Whites, and may also matter for Blacks. Once we controlled for other factors, the positive correlation between polarization and Latinos shifting to the Democratic Party evaporates. However, we see a strong, negative effect for Whites. As the associations between the Democratic Party and civil rights and the Republican Party and racial conservatism accelerated, Whites moved substantially away from the Democratic Party (see Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989; Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004). The effect for African Americans is positive and significant, but only in the Newey–West model. This initial analysis cannot definitively resolve the effects of polarization on macropartisanship but does provide important insights that merit study in future works.

The administration dummies produce some interesting insights. When comparing administrations to the Bush II administration, we see that Whites and Blacks were a bit less Democratic during the Bush I and Clinton administrations, though the effect for Blacks is only significant in the Newey–West model. Interestingly, the Obama administration may have been a boon to Democrats among Latinos and Whites, but not among African Americans, with positive and significant coefficients in the Newey–West models. For Whites, it may just mean that Obama was able to make small gains among Whites despite the Republican Party’s gains related to polarization. Again, this merits less exploratory analysis in future works.

6. Discussion

Macropartisanship is an aggregate measure of American partisanship that varies over time. But political science theories about macropartisanship—whether it is stable or variable, by how much, and what moves it—are caught up in a vision of American politics that thinks centrally about the political behavior of White Americans. There are scholarly traditions which have defined different expectations for Black political behavior—whereby we might expect greater overtime partisan stability, and different expectations for Latino political behavior—whereby we might expect greater responsiveness to apolitical trends.

In this paper, using Stimson’s (Reference Stimson2018) dyad ratio algorithm to aggregate more than 500 CBS News and CBS/New York Times surveys from 1983 to 2016, we have presented a new measure of macropartisanship that can be disaggregated by race and ethnicity to create valid and reliable estimates of Black, Latino and non-Latino White macropartisanship. These data, which are available for download monthly, quarterly, or yearly, are presented as a free public download.

We view this paper as the first step to investigating the divergent trends in disaggregated macropartisanship, but indeed, the differential variation needs to be explored further. Can we connect theories of linked fate to the relative stability in Black macropartisanship? Is Latino macropartisanship, which exhibits considerably more overtime variation, reacting to protest events and changes in the partisan disposition toward issues like citizenship and immigration? Finally, should models of White macropartisanship account for the growing presence of White identity in recent work? These questions, all beyond the scope of this project, are all worthy of future investigation and research we hope will be spurred by this project and data release.

The exploratory analyses that are presented are driven by the theoretic traditions of the Black and Latino political behavior literatures. We find, consistent with expectations, that Black macropartisanship is remarkably stable. Interestingly, the overtime variation in Black, White, and Latino macropartisanship is predicted by variation in presidential approval, but evaluations of the economy, measured by consumer sentiment, matter mostly for Latinos. We also find that political polarization led Whites to abandon the Democratic Party for the Republican Party, while having a mixed effect on Blacks and no apparent effect on Latinos. These findings help put into context the importance of increasingly ideological extremism and White identity within the Republican Party (Jardina Reference Jardina2019).

However, as we observe Latino macropartisanship to be considerably more variable, we also find that the economy affects the swings in Latino macropartisanship to a greater degree. We also note that Latino macropartisanship, despite its variation, has trended toward a Democratic advantage since 2008. In the same era, however, we have also seen a dramatic rise in Latino independent identification, at a rate that far outpaces both Blacks and Whites. These findings are of particular note given the research in California that has found that Latinos moved away from the Republican Party in response to a nativist policy response from the Republican Party (Barreto et al. Reference Barreto, Ramírez and Woods2005; Bowler et al. Reference Bowler, Nicholson and Segura2006; Dyck et al. Reference Dyck, Johnson and Wasson2012; Korey and Lascher Reference Korey and Lascher2006, but see Hui and Sears Reference Hui and Sears2018). Given the nativism surrounding the national Republican Party under Trump, it remains to be seen if we will see the emerging trend in Latino macropartisanship that favors the Democrats continue. While Democrats have made gains in the last decade in the data, the sharp rise in independent identification is equally worthy of investigation in future research.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2021.35

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for research assistance from Dan Herschel, Gabriel Carvajal, and Jack Cordell. Additionally, Shanna Pearson-Merkowitz, Ryan Carlin and Greg Love provided helpful advice related to using Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm in R. Finally, we appreciate the helpful advice our anonymous reviewers provided to strengthen the manuscript.

Appendix A

Table A1. Testing Whether the Correlations between Presidential Approval and Macropartisanship Vary by Race and Ethnicity

Hotelling's T-Test reports a t-statistic. Steiger's Z-Test reports a Z-statistic; p-values in parentheses. *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.

Table A2. Testing Whether the Correlations between Polarization and Macropartisanship Vary by Race and Ethnicity

Hotelling's T-Test reports a t-statistic. Steiger's Z-Test reports a Z-statistic; p-values in parentheses. *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.

Footnotes

1 See our dataverse at Johnson and Dyck (Reference Johnson and Dyck2021).

2 Though in early years, the survey was inconsistent in regard to asking this question.

3 The list of datasets with their accompanying ICPSR and Roper Center identification numbers and date of the survey is included in the dataverse. Some surveys listed the month of the survey, but not the date the survey started. When no date was listed, we listed the start of the month as the date the survey started. If more than one survey was conducted, we listed the start of the month as the date of the first survey, the 15th as the date of the second survey, and the 25th as the date of any month in the event of a third survey.

4 Barreto and Pedraza (Reference Barreto and Pedraza2009) note that the ANES pooled cluster sampling is problematic when trying to select a subsample of Latinos. However, the CBS/NYTimes poll uses random digit dialing using primary sampling units. Similarly, we know that recent CBS/NYTimes polls were conducted in both English and Spanish. Techniques for surveying in Spanish are obviously better today than they were in the 80s and 90s, but these data at least allow us to attempt to create a reasonable overtime measure that spans four decades.

5 While it would also be desirable to be able to measure macropartisanship for Asian-Americans and other racial and ethnic subgroups, our sample sizes are only large enough to compute time trend estimates for non-Latino Whites, Latinos, and African Americans.

6 A random sample of 400 carries a margin of error of +/-4.9% and a random sample of 280 carries a margin of error of +/-5.9%. Additionally, we generated scatterplots and local polynomial smooth plots that show Latino, White, and Black macropartisanship over time with confidence intervals. These figures do show Latino macropartisanship is more volatile than White or (especially) African American macropartisanship (see Online Appendix Figures 3-5).

7 Here, we present the combined scatterplot and the median spline plots; median spline plots are used to illustrate trends. Median splines smooth out functional shapes and their use is exceptionally common in biostatistics and medical research (Perperoglou et al. Reference Perperoglou, Sauerbrei, Abrahamowicz and Schmid2019).

8 Here, we again use median splines to plot macropartisanship, this time by race and ethnicity. We have suppressed the scatterplots to make the figures easier to read.

9 This likely had far more to do with hard line immigration stances taken by Congressional Republicans and by policies enacted in the states than by Bush himself, who advocated for a pathway to citizenship for undocumented immigrants and was by all accounts, an immigration moderate within his party.

10 Despite the fact that our samples for Latinos have the smallest number of respondents, the variation in sample sizes over the study period gives us some leverage over understanding the trends in the data. We observe fewer respondents in the first half of our time period than in the second half. However, mean Latino macropartisanship for the first half is 68.4% with a standard deviation of 6.9 and for the second half is 69.7% with a standard deviation of 7.7. If changes for Latino macropartisanship were due to small sample sizes, we should expect a much higher standard deviation in the first half compared with the second half, yet we actually find a slightly higher standard deviation in the second half of our sample. This gives us a great deal of confidence that the volatility we are observing is not a function of sampling error, but instead consistent with the theoretical arguments we have outlined. See also footnote 12 regarding this point in relation to Stimson’s dyad ratio algorithm.

11 The correlations for macropartisanship across race and ethnicity are all positive and statistically significant at p < 0.10. However, these correlations are quite low, ranging from 0.11 for Blacks and Whites to 0.21 for Latinos and Whites.

12 The estimation report from the dyad ratio algorithm shows higher standard deviations for the Latino series than for the White or Black series. However, the dyad ratio algorithm’s estimation report also shows a higher eigen estimate for the Latino series (0.63) than the White series (0.60), while the Latino series also shows a higher percent of the variance explained (45.23) compared with the White series (42.47). Only the African American series has a higher eigen estimate (0.77) and percent of variance explained (54.88). These all speak to the validity of the measures despite the smaller sample sizes for Latinos and African Americans, as Stimson (Reference Stimson2018) notes that mood indicators of policy preferences are typified by percent of variance explained in the high 30s.

13 The measures are indeed distinct. We calculated the pairwise correlations in change in macropartisanship for Whites, Latinos and Blacks and found that Latino-White was .071, Latino-Black was .005, and Black-White was −0.050. None of them reached standard levels of statistical significance.

14 Data from the American National Election Studies.

15 A simple correlation shows a robust relationship (r=0.69, p<0.0001), though perhaps not as strong as we might have expected. This is likely due to distinct methodologies in survey techniques and questions, as well as how MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson built their measure.

16 This measurement, difference, however, is to be expected as Erikson, MacKuen, and Stimson noted that in-person Gallup surveys were more Democratic than telephone surveys that Gallup moved to exclusively in the post-1991 era. To correct for this in the data, they adjusted the telephone surveys to conform to the in-person surveys, creating a pro-Democratic adjustment in the data. All of the CBS and CBS/New York Times surveys used here are from telephone surveys and no adjustments were made to the final data.

17 As we saw with the overall measure of macropartisanship, we find MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson’s measure and our measure of White macropartisanship are significantly correlated (r = 0.56, p < 0.001), while our measure of White macropartisanship also correlates with our overall measure at a very high rate (r = 0.79, p < 0.001).

18 See for instance the procedures detailed from Latino Decisions (2020) on best practices for getting an accurate sample of the American Latino population.

19 ANES data cover 1984–2016, with no data for 2006, 2010, or 2014.

20 Given these findings related to consumer confidence, we also ran models with presidential approval excluded (see Online Appendix Table 1). Here, we find that consumer confidence is significant in most models, meaning that a good economy does appear to cause shifts in macropartisanship. However, we do find the effects appear much stronger for Latinos than for Whites, while the effects for African Americans are small and even statistically insignificant in the Newey–West models.

21 Gallup data were gathered at The American Presidency Project (2021). These data usually provide multiple surveys per month, so we took the monthly average approval score. A few months did not produce estimates, so we used linear interpolation to fill in data for these months.

22 If we use the mean, rather than median scores on the first dimension, we find substantively and statistically similar results. While Hare and Poole (Reference Hare and Poole2014) demonstrate, NOMINATE’s second dimension evaporates during the period of our study and we double checked. We find a strong, positive correlation between first and second dimensions (r=0.97, p<0.0001) on our measure of polarization, so we did not include the second dimension in our analysis. Finally, we also ran analyses with Republican NOMINATE scores in one set of models and Democratic Nominate scores in another (see Online Appendix Tables 2 and 3). A positive coefficient indicates that as a party becomes more conservative the group became more Democratic, while a negative coefficient indicates the group became more Republican as the party moved to the right. In the table with only Republican NOMINATE scores, we see little change in the effect of presidential approval and consumer confidence, while the effect for Republican conservatism is only significant for Whites. As the Republican Party moved to the right, Whites moved away from the Democratic Party to the Republican Party (see Online Appendix Table 2). In the table with only Democratic NOMINATE scores, we also see little change in the effect of presidential approval and consumer confidence. However, we see a significant effect for Whites and African Americans in the Newey–West models. A more conservative Democratic Party made Whites more Democratic and Blacks less Democratic, which fits expectations. Further study of the effects of elite and mass divergence on issues of policy, particularly issues of race and ethnicity, is warranted in future studies.

23 See Appendix Tables 1 and 2 for the results of Hotelling’s T-Test and Steiger’s Z-Test.

24 These correlations are significantly different from one another (see Appendix Table 2).

25 The precise structure of the serial autocorrelation can be deduced by plotting autocorrelations and partial autocorrelations (see Online Appendix Figures 1 and 2). The first figure shows strong time serial components, with significant autocorrelations lasting over a year. However, the second figure shows that an AR1 structure largely captures concerns related to autocorrelation. When we conducted Durbin–Watson tests on the OLS models with the lagged dependent variable, we determined that only the model for Blacks produced residuals that violated an AR1 process (p=0.06). Still, we followed the advice of leading scholars and also included Newey–West estimators to examine the robustness of our findings. While we included six lags in these models, even going out to 12 lags makes not statistical or substantive difference.

26 Based on the advice of a reviewer, we also ran ARMA (1, 1) models (see Online Appendix Table 4) to address possible errors–in–variables bias due to survey sampling error given the time series nature combined with the subdivision of the data by race and ethnicity (Beck Reference Beck1991). Our analysis produced somewhat distinct findings regarding the effects of presidential approval and consumer confidence.

References

Abramowitz, AI and Saunders, KL (1998) Ideological realignment in the US electorate. Journal of Politics 60, 634652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, PR and Ostrom, CW Jr (1991) Macropartisanship: an empirical reassessment. American Political Science Review 85, 198–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Achen, CH (2000) Why Lagged Dependent Variables Can Suppress the Explanatory Power of Other Independent Variables.” Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Political Methodology Section, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
American National Election Studies (www.electionstudies.org) These materials are based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under grant numbers SES 1444721, 2014-2017, the University of Michigan, and Stanford University. (accessed 7 February 2020).Google Scholar
Anderson, CJ (2007) The end of economic voting? Contingency dilemmas and the limits of democratic accountability. Annual Review of Political Science 10, 271296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold Christian, DD and Wiesehomeier, N (2017) Presidents, policy compromise, and legislative success. Journal of Politics 79, 380395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, MA and Pedraza, FI (2009) The renewal and persistence of group identification in American politics. Electoral Studies 28, 595605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, MA, Ramírez, R and Woods, ND (2005) Are naturalized voters driving the California Latino Electorate? Measuring the effect of IRCA citizens on Latino Voting*. Social Science Quarterly 86, 792811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, N (1991) Comparing dynamic specifications: the case of presidential approval. Political Analysis 3, 5187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowler, S, Nicholson, SP and Segura, GM (2006) Earthquakes and aftershocks: race, direct democracy, and partisan change. American Journal of Political Science 50, 146159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, BE, Kiewiet, DR and Uhlaner, CJ (1991) The acquisition of partisanship by Latinos and Asian Americans. American Journal of Political Science 35, 390422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, A, Converse, PE, Miller, WE and Stokes, DE (1960) The American Voter. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, JE (2010) Explaining politics, not polls: reexamining macropartisanship with recalibrated NES data. Public Opinion Quarterly 74, 616642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlin, RE, Hartlyn, J, Hellwig, T, Love, GJ, Martínez-Gallardo, C and Singer, MM (2018) Public support for Latin American presidents: the cyclical model in comparative perspective. Research & Politics 5, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlin, RE and Hellwig, T (2019) Policy regimes and economic accountability in Latin America. Comparative Political Studies 52, 20322060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmines, EG and Stimson, JA (1986) On the structure and sequence of issue evolution. American Political Science Review 80, 901920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmines, EG and Stimson, JA (1989) Issue evolution: Race and the Transformation of American Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D and Kim, D (2006) The experiences and effects of economic status among racial and ethnic minorities. American Political Science Review 100, 335351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claassen, C (2019) Estimating smooth country–year panels of public opinion. Political Analysis 27, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalton, RJ and Flanagan, SE (2017) Electoral Change in Advanced Industrial Democracies: Realignment or Dealignment? Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, MC (1994) Behind the Mule: Race and Class in African-American Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Duch, RM, Palmer, HD and Anderson, CJ (2000) Heterogeneity in perceptions of national economic conditions. American Journal of Political Science 44, 635652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyck, JJ, Johnson, GB and Wasson, JT (2012) A blue tide in the golden state: ballot propositions, population change, and party identification in California. American Politics Research 40, 450475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enns, PK (2016) Incarceration Nation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, RS, MacKuen, MB and Stimson, JA (1998) What moves macropartisanship? A response to Green, Palmquist, and Schickler. American Political Science Review 92, 901912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, RS, MacKuen, MB and Stimson, JA (2002) The Macro Polity. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fiorina, MP (1981) Retrospective Voting in American National Elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Green, D, Palmquist, B and Schickler, E (1998) Macropartisanship: a replication and critique. American Political Science Review 92, 883899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D, Palmquist, B and Schickler, E (2002) Partisan Hearts and Minds: Political Parties and the Social Identities of Voters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hajnal, ZL (2020) Dangerously Divided: How Race and Class Shape Winning and Losing in American Politics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajnal, ZL and Lee, T (2011) Why Americans Don't Join the Party: Race, Immigration, and the Failure (of Political Parties) to Engage the Electorate. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, C and Poole, KT (2014) The polarization of contemporary American politics. Polity 46, 411429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawley, G and Sagarzazu, I (2012) Where did the votes go? Reassessing American party realignments via vote transfers between major parties from 1860 to 2008. Electoral Studies 31, 726739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hui, I and Sears, DO (2018) Reexamining the effect of racial propositions on Latinos’ partisanship in California. Political Behavior 40, 149174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchings, VL and Valentino, NA (2004) The centrality of race in American politics. Annual Review of Political Science 7, 383408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jardina, A (2019) White Identity Politics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, G and Dyck, JJ (2021) Data and Codebook for “Constructing a New Measure of Macropartisanship Disaggregated by Race and Ethnicity”. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Dataverse, V1. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/MIKNXJ CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufmann, KM and Petrocik, JR (1999) The changing politics of American men: understanding the sources of the gender gap. American Journal of Political Science 43, 864887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keele, L and Kelly, JJ (2006) Dynamic models for dynamic theories: the ins and outs of lagged dependent variables. Political Analysis 14, 1862005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keith, BE, Magleby, DB, Nelson, CJ, Orr, EA and Westlye, MC (1992) The Myth of the Independent Voter. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Klar, S and Krupnikov, Y (2016) Independent Politics: How American Disdain for Parties Leads to Political Inaction. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korey, JL and Lascher, EL Jr (2006) Macropartisanship in California. Public Opinion Quarterly 70, 4865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latino Decisions (2020) Methodology Statement, 2020 American Election Eve Poll. Available at https://latinodecisions.com/wp-content/uploads/2020/11/Methodology-Statement-AEE-2020.pdf (accessed 7 February 2020).Google Scholar
Lawrence, DG (2018) The Collapse of the Democratic Presidential Majority: Realignment, Dealignment, and Electoral Change from Franklin Roosevelt to Bill Clinton. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levendusky, M (2009) The Partisan Sort: How Liberals Became Democrats and Conservatives Became Republicans. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, J (2020) Polarization in Congress. Available at https://voteview.com/articles/party_polarization (accessed 6 December 2021).Google Scholar
Lewis, JB, Poole, K, Rosenthal, H, Boche, A, Rudkin, A and Sonnet, L (2021) Voteview: Congressional Roll-Call Votes Database. https://voteview.com/ (accessed 6 December 2021).Google Scholar
MacKuen, MB, Erikson, RS and Stimson, JA (1989) Macropartisanship. American Political Science Review 83, 11251142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, EL and Ellison, CG (2008) God's party? Race, religion, and partisanship over time. Political Research Quarterly 61, 180191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newey, WK and West, KD (1987) A simple, positive semi-definite, heteroskedasticity and autocorrelation consistent covariance matrix. Econometrica 55, 703708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, RG and Jennings, MK (1991) Issues and inheritance in the formation of party identification. American Journal of Political Science 35, 970988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patten, E (2016) The Nation’s Latino population is defined by its youth. Pew Research Center. http://www.pewhispanic.org/2016/04/20/the-nations-latino-population-is-defined-by-its-youth/ (accessed 7 February 2020).Google Scholar
Perperoglou, A, Sauerbrei, W, Abrahamowicz, M and Schmid, M (2019) A review of spline function procedures in R. BMC Medical Research Methodology 19, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poole, KT and Rosenthal, H (1997) Congress: A Political-economic History of Roll Call Voting. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sears, DO and Funk, CL (1999) Evidence of the long-term persistence of adults' political predispositions. Journal of Politics 61, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, RM (1993) Beyond Tocqueville, Myrdal, and Hartz: the multiple traditions in America. American Political Science Review 87, 549566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stimson, JA (2015) Using Wcalc6 and the Dyad Ratios Algorithm. https://stimson.web.unc.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/9919/2015/08/Wcalc6.pdf (accessed 7 February 2020).Google Scholar
Stimson, JA (2017) Macropartisanship, Quarterly 1952-2017.4. https://stimson.web.unc.edu/data/ (accessed 7 February 2020).Google Scholar
Stimson, JA (2018) The dyad ratios algorithm for estimating latent public opinion: estimation, testing, and comparison to other approaches. Bulletin of Sociological Methodology/Bulletin de Méthodologie Sociologique 137, 201218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stimson, JA, Mackuen, MB and Erikson, RS (1995) Dynamic representation. The American Political Science Review 89, 543565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The American Presidency Project (2021) Presidential Job Approval. University of California Santa Barbara. https://www.presidency.ucsb.edu/statistics/data/presidential-job-approval (accessed 7 February 2020).Google Scholar
Uhlaner, CJ and Garcia, FC (2005) Learning which party fits: experience, ethnic identity, and the demographic foundations of Latino party identification. In Segura GM and Bowler S (eds), Diversity in Democracy: Minority Representation in the United States. Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press, pp. 72101.Google Scholar
Voteviewblog (2017) The Collapse of the Voting Structure—Possible Big Trouble Ahead. https://voteviewblog.com/2017/01/12/the-collapse-of-the-voting-structure-possible-big-trouble-ahead/ (accessed 6 December 2021).Google Scholar
Weiss, BA (2011) Hotelling’s t-Test and Steiger’s Z test calculator [Computer software]. https://blogs.gwu.edu/weissba/teaching/calculators/hotellings-t-and-steigers-z-tests/ (accessed 7 January 2021).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Overall Macropartisanship using CBS/NYTimes Data. June 1983 to August 20167

Figure 1

Figure 2. Macropartisanship by Race and Ethnicity. June 1983 to August 20168

Figure 2

Figure 3. Overall Macropartisanship using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm. June 1983 to August 2016

Figure 3

Figure 4. Macropartisanship by Race and Ethnicity using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm June 1983 to August 2016

Figure 4

Figure 5. Overall Independent Identification using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm. June 1983 to August 2016

Figure 5

Figure 6. Independents by Race and Ethnicity using Stimson’s Dyad Ratio Algorithm. June 1983 to August 2016

Figure 6

Figure 7. MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson (MES) versus CBS/CBSNYTimes Quarterly Measures of Macropartisanship June 1983 to August 2016

Figure 7

Figure 8. MacKuen, Erikson, and Stimson (MES) versus CBS/CBSNYTimes Quarterly Measures of White Macropartisanship June 1983 to August 2016

Figure 8

Figure 9. ANES versus CBS/CBSNYTimes Annual Measures of Macropartisanship 1983 to 2016

Figure 9

Table 1. Presidential Approval, Consumer Confidence, and Macropartisanship

Figure 10

Table 2. Presidential approval, consumer confidence, political polarization, administration, and macropartisanship.

Figure 11

Table A1. Testing Whether the Correlations between Presidential Approval and Macropartisanship Vary by Race and Ethnicity

Figure 12

Table A2. Testing Whether the Correlations between Polarization and Macropartisanship Vary by Race and Ethnicity

Supplementary material: File

Dyck and Johnson supplementary material

Dyck and Johnson supplementary material

Download Dyck and Johnson supplementary material(File)
File 1.1 MB