Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-19T16:32:25.021Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Frailty and neuropathology in relation to dementia status: the Cambridge City over-75s Cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2021

Lindsay Wallace
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada Faculty of Graduate Studies, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada
Sally Hunter
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Cambridge Public Health, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Olga Theou
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada
Jane Fleming
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Cambridge Public Health, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Kenneth Rockwood
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada
Carol Brayne*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Cambridge Public Health, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Carol Brayne, Cambridge Public Health, University of Cambridge, School of Clinical Medicine, Forvie Site, Cambridge Biomedical Cambus, Cambridge, CB2 OSR, UK. Phone: +44 1223 330321. Email: [email protected].

Abstract

Objective:

To examine the relative contributions of frailty and neuropathology to dementia expression in a population-based cohort study.

Design:

Cross-sectional analysis of observational data.

Setting:

Population-representative clinicopathological cohort study.

Participants:

Adults aged 75+ recruited from general practice registries in Cambridge, UK, in 1985.

Measurements:

A 39-item frailty index and 15-item neuropathological index were used to operationalize frailty and neuropathology, respectively. Dementia status was ascertained by clinical consensus at time of death. Relationships were evaluated using logistic regression models in participants with autopsy records (n = 183). Model fit was assessed using change in deviance. Population attributable fraction for frailty was evaluated in relation to dementia incidence in a representative sample of the survey participants (n = 542).

Results:

Participants with autopsy were 92.3 ± 4.6 years at time of death, and mostly women (70%). Average frailty index value at last survey before death was 0.34 ± 0.16. People with dementia (63% of the sample) were frailer, had lower MMSE scores, and a higher burden of neuropathology. Frailty and neuropathological burden were significantly and independently associated with dementia status, without interaction; frailty explained an additional 3% of the variance in the model. Assuming a causal relationship and based on population-attributable fraction analyses, preventing severe frailty (Frailty Index ≥ 0.40) could have avoided 14.2% of dementia cases in this population-based cohort.

Conclusions:

In the very old, frailty contributes to the risk for dementia beyond its relationship with the burden of traditional dementia neuropathologies. Reducing frailty could have important implications for controlling the burden of dementia. Future research on frailty interventions should include dementia risk as a key outcome, public health interventions and policy decisions should consider frailty as a key risk factor for dementia, and biomedical research should focus on elucidating shared mechanisms of frailty and dementia development.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2021

Background

As treatments for clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease continue to fail in clinical trials, evidence is accumulating to suggest that diverse risk factors and mechanistic pathways are important, especially in late-life dementia (Canevelli etal., Reference Canevelli, Bruno and Cesari2017). Many studies have now shown that single-protein abnormalities (e.g. plaques and tangles) are not highly correlated with the clinical expression of dementia, especially in the oldest old (Boyle etal., Reference Boyle2013; Brayne etal., Reference Brayne2009; Jansen etal., Reference Jansen2018; MRC CFAS, 2001; Wallace etal., Reference Wallace, Theou, Godin, Andrew, Bennett and Rockwood2019). The research paradigm for tackling dementia has assumed that Alzheimer’s disease pathology is responsible for the majority of clinical expression of dementia (Jack etal., Reference Jack2018), though increasingly evidence suggests that the majority of late-life dementia is associated with mixed pathology, including vascular abnormalities, and hippocampal sclerosis among others (Boyle etal., Reference Boyle, Yu, Wilson, Leurgans, Schneider and Bennett2018; Wilson etal., Reference Wilson, Leurgans, Boyle, Schneider and Bennett2010). In the general population, age remains the number one risk factor for dementia (Livingston etal., Reference Livingston2017), and understanding the context and contributions of aging in the development of dementia may provide insight into its complex etiology (Canevelli etal., Reference Canevelli, Cesari and van Kan2015; Searle and Rockwood, Reference Searle and Rockwood2015). Age-related diseases, such as heart disease and osteoarthritis, not only accumulate with age but also appear to be the result of small-scale (i.e. molecular) deficits which scale up to affect whole bodily systems in the form of frailty (Castell etal., Reference Castell2015; Rockwood etal., Reference Rockwood, Mitnitski and Howlett2015; Wallace etal., Reference Wallace2014). Internal or external insults are usually repaired easily by redundant repair mechanisms before becoming deficits, but as the body ages, the repair mechanisms fail and lead to the accumulation of deficits (Mitnitski etal., Reference Mitnitski, Song and Rockwood2013; Mitnitski and Rockwood, Reference Mitnitski and Rockwood2015; Mitnitski etal., Reference Mitnitski, Mogilner and Rockwood2001).

Frailty is conceptualized as multisystem impairment giving rise to physiologic vulnerability to adverse health outcomes (Clegg etal., Reference Clegg, Young, Iliffe, Rikkert and Rockwood2013) and is most commonly operationalized as a health state characterized by the accumulation of health deficits (Mitnitski etal., Reference Mitnitski, Mogilner and Rockwood2001), or as a phenotypic syndrome (Fried etal., Reference Fried2001). Frailty is recognized as contributing to the dementia syndrome (Song etal., Reference Song, Mitnitski and Rockwood2014; Sterniczuk etal., Reference Sterniczuk, Theou, Rusak and Rockwood2015), brain atrophy (Gallucci etal., Reference Gallucci, Piovesan and Di Battista2018), mild cognitive impairment (Trebbastoni etal., Reference Trebbastoni2017), cognitive decline (Thibeau etal., Reference Thibeau, McDermott, McFall, Rockwood and Dixon2019) and predicts dementia incidence (Rogers etal., Reference Rogers, Steptoe and Cadar2017). This evidence suggests that it is possible that the expression of dementia, even in the face of neuropathology, may be modified by frailty as measured by deficit accumulation (Anstey etal., Reference Anstey and Dixon2014). Therefore, the objective of the current study was to examine the relative contributions of frailty and neuropathology to dementia expression in a population-based representative cohort study and to build on earlier work where frailty and neuropathology contributed independently to dementia risk in a sample of older adults in retirement homes (Wallace etal., Reference Wallace2020; Reference Wallace, Theou, Godin, Andrew, Bennett and Rockwood2019).

Methods

Sample/participants

The Cambridge City over-75s Cohort study was initiated in 1985 as a population representative sample (95% response rate) of people aged 75 or above on general practice registers in Cambridge, UK, including those living in care (Fleming etal., Reference Fleming, Zhao, O’Connor, Pollitt and Brayne2007). It aimed to study cognition and function in older adults and enrolled 2610 participants of whom 2166 (all excluding one practice) were followed-up until their death (10 surveys over 28 years; see Additional Supplementary File 1 for study design). Each survey included questions on demographics, activities of daily living, and health problems. In cases where participants were unable to respond, proxy informants were sought. Early surveys were supplemented with additional CAMDEX (Cambridge Mental Disorders of the Elderly Examination) psychiatric assessments which included mental state examination, psychiatric history, performance-based cognitive testing, and a proxy informant interview. A brain donation program was initiated in survey 2 (year 2), and donation was agreed to and fulfilled by 242 participants, with known representation from the base population.

For the purpose of this cross-sectional study, we used the survey 3 (7 years after the initial survey) as our baseline as surveys from this wave on had the largest number of relevant variables to create a frailty index which could be used consistently across the remaining surveys.

At survey 3, 714 participants were interviewed, of whom 175 became brain donors; of these, 170 had complete neuropathological and frailty data, as well as known dementia status were included in the cross-sectional analysis. Some participants (n = 68) missed survey 3 but were followed-up in subsequent surveys and donated their brains, of these, 13 had complete neuropathological and frailty data, and known dementia status, providing a total sample of 183 for cross-sectional analysis (Additional Supplementary File 2).

For the population-attributable risk analyses, we extended our inclusion criteria to participants who were not brain donors (as we were not assessing neuropathology, but rather the attributable risk of frailty to dementia). Here, we sampled the original 714 participants at survey 3, excluded those with prevalent dementia (n = 153) or missing prevalence data at survey 3 (n = 2), and incomplete frailty data (n = 17) leaving us with a sample of 542 (Additional Supplementary File 2).

Measures

Dementia status

All clinical study records for brain donors were reviewed post-mortem and dementia status was ascertained by consensus by at least two clinicians using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 4th Edition (DSM-IV) criteria and blinded to neuropathological data (Brayne etal., Reference Brayne2009; Fleming etal., Reference Fleming, Zhao, O’Connor, Pollitt and Brayne2007).

Frailty Index

The frailty index is a health state measure that reflects vulnerability to adverse health outcomes (Clegg etal., Reference Clegg, Young, Iliffe, Rikkert and Rockwood2013). The frailty index = (number of health deficits present)/(number of health deficits measured). For example, a person with 5 of 30 potential deficits measured has a frailty index score of 5/30 = 0.17. Candidate variables from respondent interviews were indicative of poor health and included symptoms, signs, functional impairments, and comorbidities. These variables were screened against four criteria: (1) Relationship with age; (2) Prevalence of at least 1%; (3) Less than 5% missing data across participants at any survey; (4) No more than 80% prevalence (saturation). A total of 39 items metall criteria and were included in the index. The index demonstrated properties consistent with frailty indices from similar samples (i.e. normal distribution with right skewed tail, higher frailty index in women than men, increase with age). As the goal of the regression analyses was to examine the cross-sectional relationship between frailty, neuropathology, and dementia, but neuropathology could only be obtained post-mortem via autopsy, we used a frailty index and dementia measurements obtained from participants’ last survey prior to death. Baseline frailty index (survey 3) was used to predict population-attributable risk. Refer to Additional Supplementary File 3 for list of variables included in the frailty index. The frailty index was categorized into tertiles for the descriptive analyses, using cut-points of 0.27, 0.43, and a cut-point of 0.40 (corresponding to severely frail; Guaraldi etal., Reference Guaraldi2019) was used for the population-attributable risk analyses.

Neuropathological Index

Neuropathological data were obtained at autopsy by semi-quantitative scoring by trained neuropathologists according to the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) protocol (Mirra, Reference Mirra1997; Mirra etal., Reference Mirra1991). Fifteen neuropathological variables were included in the index including: (1) atrophy, (2) pallor of substantia nigra and/or locus coerulus, (3) significant obstruction of large cerebral vessels, (4) gross parenchymal vascular lesions, (5) small vessel disease, (6) microinfarcts, (7) white matter pallor, (8) neuritic plaques, (9) amyloid deposits, (10) neurofibrillary tangles, (11) vascular amyloid, (12) granulovacuolar degeneration, (13) severe neuronal loss, (14) severe gliosis, (15) Lewy bodies. Other neuropathologies available in this cohort relating to Pick’s disease, leukoencephalopathy, lobar atrophy, Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease, spongiform encephalopathy, neoplasms/tumors, Hirano bodies, ubiquitin, and Huntington’s disease were not included in the index because they were absent in all cases. TDP-43 was excluded as there were too many missing cases. Full information on regional inclusions and coding are detailed in Additional Supplementary File 4. Included neuropathological variables were summed and divided by the number of valid variables (according to the deficit accumulation approach detailed above). The neuropathological index was then categorized into tertiles for the descriptive analyses, using cut-points of 0.30, 0.42. Details of autopsy and neuropathological assessment can be found at cc75c.group.cam.ac.uk and Brayne etal. (Reference Brayne2009).

Other measures

Age, sex, education, post-mortem interval, and time from frailty evaluation to death were evaluated as confounders. Education (in years) was the only covariate found to be a significant independent predictor of the outcome, but age, sex, and education were conserved in final model for conceptual reasons.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive analysis techniques (including analysis of variance and χ2 tests) were used to describe the characteristics of the sample and assess distributions of frailty, neuropathology, and dementia in the sample.

Logistic regression models were used to explore the effect of frailty (frailty index) and neuropathology (neuropathological index) on dementia status independently and in the same model. Their interaction was also evaluated. Model fit was evaluated by change in deviance (χ2 of −2LogLikelihood values). Model assumptions including binary outcome, independent observations, no collinearity among independent variables, linearity of independent variables with log odds (according to Box Tidwell test), and large enough sample size based on least frequent outcome (Stoltzfus, Reference Stoltzfus2011) were tested. Nagelkerke Pseudo R 2 was reported to show goodness of fit.

Complete-case analysis was employed, though 56 participants were excluded as they were missing more than 20% of the variables needed to calculate a frailty index. We undertook two sensitivity analyses to attempt to evaluate the effect of the missing data on our findings. First, we used an extreme case sensitivity analysis where missing frailty index values were allocated the highest level of frailty. Further, we attempted multiple imputations (chained equations algorithm) for frailty index values.

Population-attributable fraction estimates the proportion of cases that hypothetically could be avoided if the exposure were eliminated or reduced. Here, we used this to determine the fraction of dementia cases (ascertained at time of death) that could be avoided if severe frailty (frailty index ≥ 0.40) was “eliminated” or avoided at baseline. Population-attributable fraction was calculated using the following formula (Lin and Chen, Reference Lin and Chen2019):

$$\eqalign{ {\rm{Population \, attributable \, fraction }}\cr \quad {{= }}{{\left[ {{\rm{proportion \,exposed \,in \,whole \,sample }} \times {\rm{ }}\left( {{\rm{RR}} - {\rm{1}}} \right)} \right]} / }\cr \quad \hskip7.3pt{\left[ {{\rm{proportion \,exposed \,in \,whole \,sample }} \times {\rm{ }}\left( {{\rm{RR}} - {\rm{1}}} \right){\rm{ \; +\;1}}} \right]}}} \right$$

where RR = relative risk. As the exposure was rare, the hazard ratio approximates the relative risk and was used in order to control for time from baseline. By using a Cox proportional hazards model, we were able to control for time from baseline to death, as well as control for the contributions of baseline age, sex, and education. For this analysis, we extended our sample to include participants who did not have autopsy to achieve a larger and population-representative sample. Statistical analyses were performed in SPSS version 25.0 and R version 3.5.2.

Results

Autopsy sample participants (n = 183) were aged 92.3 (SD 4.6; normally distributed) years on average at time of death, and mostly women (69.4%). Average frailty index at last survey before death was 0.34 (SD 0.16; normally distributed). People with dementia were frailer, had lower Mini-Mental State Examination scores, and a higher burden of neuropathology (Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of sample

a Normally distributed.

* p < 0.05.

Very few participants demonstrated little to no neuropathology at death (n = 7; 4%). Among people with no dementia, 7.1% had a high burden of neuropathology. Among people with dementia, 16.3% demonstrated a low burden of neuropathology. Within each level of neuropathological burden, those with dementia generally had higher frailty (Figure 1), though dose-response was not consistent (likely due to small sample size).

Figure 1. Proportion of participants with dementia according to tertiles of neuropathological index and frailty tertiles. Note: Frailty and dementia status were assessed at last survey before death (median 1.9 years pre-mortem), neuropathological burden was assessed at time of death. Numbers within the bars represent sample size.

The proportion of people with dementia was highest among people with a high frailty index and high burden of neuropathology, low among those with low frailty index and low burden of neuropathology, and in between for intermediate or either high frailty index or high neuropathology, suggesting these risk factors may be additive (Figure 1).

Logistic regression models satisfied assumptions and demonstrated that both frailty (at last survey before death) and neuropathological burden (at death) were significantly and independently associated with dementia status at last survey (Table 2), although they did not interact (p = 0.81). Addition of the frailty index to the model with the neuropathological index significantly improved model fit X 2(1) 5.44, p < 0.01). Pseudo R2 increased from 0.085 to 0.123 suggesting that the addition of frailty to the model increased the explained variance by 3.8%. Sensitivity analysis using extreme case imputation did not significantly change the results (Additional Supplementary File 5). Multiple Imputation by Chained Equations (MICE) was not used in this analysis as the only significant predictors of missing frailty index data were related to pathological measures that would be used in an interaction with the frailty index and would produce nonsensical results.

Table 2. Logistic regression models for dementia status (n = 183; all models adjusted for age, sex, education) demonstrating that the frailty index and neuropathological index are independently associated with dementia status, even when included in the same model. Model fit is significantly improved when both frailty index and neuropathological index are included in a model for dementia status

OR, Odds Ratio.

Based on a hazard ratio of 2.15 (95% CI 1.01–4.60, p = 0.048) and proportion of exposed (0.142) among 542 participants with no dementia at baseline (survey three), the PAF = 0.142, indicating that preventing severe frailty (frailty index ≥ 0.40) on average 5.6 years before death would avoid 14.2% of dementia cases in this cohort.

Discussion

Almost a quarter of the sample (23.4%) demonstrated a mismatch between neuropathological burden and clinical dementia (i.e. either dementia with low neuropathology or no dementia with high neuropathology), similar to previous reports (Brayne etal., Reference Brayne2009; Savva etal., Reference Savva, Wharton, Ince, Forster, Matthews and Brayne2009). Frailty explained additional variance and significantly predicted dementia status, even after controlling for the neuropathological index, but did not interact with the neuropathological index. Taken together, these results suggest that frailty and neuropathology may be additive risk factors that independently are neither necessary nor sufficient but are largely responsible for creating the conditions in which the clinical syndrome of dementia is experienced. Given the independent risk conferred by frailty, we investigated what the scale of reduction of incidence of dementia would be, assuming a causal relationship. If this is the case, preventing severe frailty could reduce dementia risk by 14.2%. This indicates that frailty treatment and management is a worthwhile area to focus on, not only in its own right but also for its consequences as part of societal attempts to reduce the impact of dementia in populations.

Although the sample was drawn from a population-representative cohort, those who participated in the autopsy subset (i.e. brain donors) were more likely to be diagnosed with dementia than those who did not have autopsy, as they were more likely to be followed-up and undergo clinical testing (see Additional Supplementary File 6 for comparisons). Nevertheless, the autopsy subset has shown to have cognitive score distributions that closely match those of the full cohort (Brayne etal., Reference Brayne2009; EClipSE Collaboration, 2009). The expected impact on our results here is minor, and likely reflects that frailty management would be slightly more important to control dementia in this group due to their other intersecting risks.

Another limitation was the sample size. While the sample size for the overall cohort study is quite large, by Survey 3 there had been significant attrition from this “older old” cohort due primarily to mortality, and missingness is to be expected as individuals become frail (Brayne etal., Reference Brayne1999; Chatfield etal., Reference Chatfield, Brayne and Matthews2005; Matthews etal., Reference Matthews, Chatfield and Brayne2006; Reference Matthews, Chatfield, Freeman, McCracken and Brayne2004). In an effort to determine the impact of the missing data on our results, we used an extreme sensitivity analysis in which imputed cases missing frailty index data were allocated the highest level of frailty. This sensitivity analysis did not change our results. Further, when we attempted multiple imputation (chained equations algorithm) for frailty index values we found the only predictors of missing frailty index data were related to pathological measures, suggesting that frailty is highly associated with informative dropout in this sample. Our future work will aim to investigate this relationship.

The goal of the cross-sectional regression analyses was to examine the relationship between pathology, frailty, and dementia as close to death as possible, therefore data on frailty and dementia were obtained from the last survey prior to death. This was done to minimize the effect of the autopsy results reflecting worse pathology than was present at the time of the survey from which frailty and dementia measurements were obtained. Even so, the median time from last survey to autopsy was about 2 years and it is likely we were not able to capture terminal decline that would influence frailty. Future investigations should address the issues of terminal decline.

Assessment of neuropathology was not stereological, being based on only one tissue section from each brain area for each staining method. This may lead to under- or over-estimates of pathology in a few cases. However, given the sample size, we assume that the effects of any discrepancies will be minor and cancel out.

Our results are consistent with other reports which show that the vast majority community-dwelling people are not free of neuropathology at time of death (Boyle etal., Reference Boyle, Yu, Wilson, Leurgans, Schneider and Bennett2018; Brayne etal., Reference Brayne2009), and neuropathologies typically occur together. In other words, not only are pathological substrates of dementia rarely singular or “pure” in nature but also a “clean” or “unburdened” brain from a pathological perspective is almost unseen in the oldest old (MRC CFAS and CC75C etal., 2012). A few groups have also examined the combined effect of pathology on disease and demonstrated generally, that the more pathology is considered, the better the prediction of dementia (Boyle etal., Reference Boyle, Yu, Wilson, Leurgans, Schneider and Bennett2018). While this may not be surprising it is a fact that has been ignored by those seeking a specific treatment for a specific pathology. It is important to consider the combined small effects of all such pathologies as an indicator of the overall health of the system, rather than focus on which one is the most predictive as has been done with the amyloid hypothesis, and now Limbic-predominant Age-related TDP-43 Encephalopathy Neuropathologic Changes (LATE-NC) (Nelson etal., Reference Nelson2019).

Previous work by our groups has shown not only that frailty is associated with biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease (Wallace etal., Reference Wallace, Theou, Andrew and Rockwood2018) but also that the relationship between Alzheimer’s-specific neuropathology and dementia changes over levels of frailty and age (Savva etal., Reference Savva, Wharton, Ince, Forster, Matthews and Brayne2009; Wallace etal., Reference Wallace, Theou, Godin, Andrew, Bennett and Rockwood2019). While our results were similar in that we see a significant mismatch in neuropathology and dementia status, we did not find an interaction between neuropathology and frailty in relation to dementia status. There are a few differences to take into account when considering the implications of such findings. Perhaps the most influential is the type of pathology measured. The original analysis (Wallace etal., Reference Wallace, Theou, Godin, Andrew, Bennett and Rockwood2019) included very specific “hallmark” features of AD (i.e. plaques and tangles) and examined AD-specific dementia as the outcome, whereas the analyses presented here combine several forms of neuropathology in an index with respect to all-cause dementia as an outcome. The mixed neuropathological index may represent a brain-specific frailty index, and may act as an indicator of overall deficit accumulation in the brain, and thus would not interact with the original frailty index (indicating bodily health) because it would be a reflection of it (with some expected variation).

As our population ages, a growing number of people will live long enough to accumulate several neuropathologies, but many of these people will reach older ages without necessarily experiencing dementia before their deaths. The implications of the current strategies for early detection of specific pathologies is that to “prevent” dementia may actually create more harm than good, in that younger and fitter people will be screened and “disease” will be detected in people who would not have necessarily gone on to develop symptoms. In this way, our work can inform a more public health oriented, preventative approach, by targeting frailty as a means of effective behavioral intervention for dementia risk. We hope this work will inform research and clinical approaches in considering dementia as a multi-determined disease that occurs in the aging body, which in essence suggests that the interaction of many mechanisms leading to many diverse pathways that give rise to dementia are likely. Single-mechanism treatments are therefore unlikely to be widely successful, and broad pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical therapies such as anti-aging compounds (Keller etal., Reference Keller, Kane, Heinze-Milne, Grandy and Howlett2019) and exercise (Ahlskog etal., Reference Ahlskog, Geda, Graff-Radford and Petersen2011) should be explored more deeply for use in this population.

Conclusions

The analyses presented here suggest that frailty in its own right contributes to risk for dementia in the oldest old and reduction of frailty can contribute meaningfully to dementia risk. This suggests that future research on frailty interventions should include dementia risk as a key outcome, public health interventions and policy decisions should consider frailty as a key risk factor for dementia (Norton etal., Reference Norton, Matthews, Barnes, Yaffe and Brayne2014), and biomedical research should focus on elucidating shared mechanisms of frailty and dementia development.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Each phase of the CC75C study has been approved by Cambridge Research Ethics Committee. Secondary analyses of these data have been approved by the Dalhousie University/Nova Scotia Health Authority Research Ethics Board.

Availability of data and materials

The data analysed here are available to be requested from www.cc75c.group.cam.ac.uk.

Conflict of interest

KR is President and Chief Science Officer of DGI Clinical, which in the last 5 years has contracts with pharma and device manufacturers (Baxter, Baxalta, Biogen, Shire, Hollister, Nutricia, Roche, Otsuka) on individualized outcome measurement. In 2017 he attended an advisory board meeting with Lundbeck. Otherwise any personal fees are for invited guest lectures, rounds and academic symposia, received directly from event organizers, for presentations on frailty. He is Associate Director of the Canadian Consortium on Neurodegeneration in Aging, which is funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CAN-137,794), with additional funding from the Alzheimer Society of Canada and several other charities, as well as from Pfizer Canada and Sanofi Canada (in Phase 1, 2014-2019). The remaining authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Source of funding

We thank all the past CC75C sponsors for financial support spanning two decades (see http://www.cc75c.group.cam.ac.uk/pages/grant/default.htm for full list of project grants) from the Medical Research Council’s funding the survey in this analysis to most recently the Abbeyfield Research Foundation, British Medical Association Foundation for Medical Research and General Nursing Council Trust. SH is currently supported by an ARUK Network Support Grant. The funders had no role in the study design, analysis or writing this paper.

Description of authors’ roles

LW and KR conceived idea with input from CB. SH prepared and advised on the neuropathological data and analyses and contributed to final draft. LW conceived of design, undertook analyses, interpreted the data, wrote first draft and revised all subsequent drafts. JF advised on methodological design and analysis. OT aided in interpretation and analysis, and revised all drafts. CB is the custodian of the data, and contributed to design, analyses, interpretation, and revised all drafts.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank particularly the study respondents, their families, friends and the staff in many care homes and collaborating general practices without whose help none of this research would be possible. The current research team gratefully acknowledge the contributions of previous investigators, past research team members, Addenbrooke’s Hospital mortuary staff and the helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper from current collaborators (see full list on CC75C study website: www.cc75c.group.cam.ac.uk/pages/studypersonnel/default.htm). All neuropathology data was generated by Consultant Neuropathologists at Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge, UK. All slides were prepared by the Cambridge Brain Bank. The Cambridge Brain Bank is supported by the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre. LW was generously supported by a Mitacs Globalink fellowship which facilitated this collaboration. The authors would also like to thank Emily Zhao for her assistance in data preparation and management.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610220003932

Footnotes

*

Shared last authorship.

References

Ahlskog, J. E., Geda, Y. E., Graff-Radford, N. R., Petersen, R. C. (2011). Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 86, 876884. https://doi.org/10.4065/mcp.2011.0252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstey, K. J. and Dixon, R. A. (2014). Applying a cumulative deficit model of frailty to dementia: progress and future challenges. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 6, 84. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-014-0084-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A. etal. (2013). Much of late life cognitive decline is not due to common neurodegenerative pathologies: cognitive decline. Annals of Neurology, 74, 478489. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.23964 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Wilson, R. S., Leurgans, S. E., Schneider, J. A. and Bennett, D. A. (2018). Person-specific contribution of neuropathologies to cognitive loss in old age: neuropathologies and cognition. Annals of Neurology, 83, 7483. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.25123 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brayne, C. etal. (2009). Neuropathological correlates of dementia in over-80-year-old brain donors from the population-based Cambridge City over-75s Cohort (CC75C) Study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 18, 645658. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2009-1182 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brayne, C. etal. (1999). Estimating the true extent of cognitive decline in the old old. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 12831288. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb07426.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canevelli, M., Bruno, G. and Cesari, M. (2017). The sterile controversy on the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 472473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canevelli, M., Cesari, M. and van Kan, G. A. (2015). Frailty and cognitive decline: how do they relate? Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition & Metabolic Care, 18, 4350. https://doi.org/10.1097/MCO.0000000000000133 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castell, M. V. etal. (2015). Osteoarthritis and frailty in elderly individuals across six European countries: results from the European Project on OSteoArthritis (EPOSA). BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders, 16, 359. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-015-0807-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatfield, M. D., Brayne, C. E. and Matthews, F. E. (2005). A systematic literature review of attrition between waves in longitudinal studies in the elderly shows a consistent pattern of dropout between differing studies. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 58, 1319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clegg, A., Young, J., Iliffe, S., Rikkert, M. O. and Rockwood, K. (2013). Frailty in elderly people. The Lancet, 381, 752762. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62167-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
EClipSE collaborative members (2009). Cohort profile: Epidemiological Clinicopathological studies in Europe (EClipSE). Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 18, 659663. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2009-1181 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleming, J., Zhao, E., O’Connor, D. W., Pollitt, P. A. and Brayne, C. (2007). Cohort profile: the Cambridge City over-75s Cohort (CC75C). International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 4046. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl293 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, L. P. etal. (2001). Frailty in older adults evidence for a phenotype. Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56, M146M157. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallucci, M., Piovesan, C. and Di Battista, M. E. (2018). Associations between the frailty index and brain atrophy: the Treviso Dementia (TREDEM) Registry. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 62, 16231634. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170938 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guaraldi, G. etal. (2019). Compression of frailty in adults living with HIV. BMC Geriatrics, 19, 229. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-019-1247-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, C. R. etal. (2018). NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 14, 535562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jansen, W. J. etal. (2018). Age and the association of dementia-related pathology with trajectories of cognitive decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 61, 138145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.08.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, K., Kane, A., Heinze-Milne, S., Grandy, S. A. and Howlett, S. E. (2019). Chronic treatment with the ACE inhibitor enalapril attenuates the development of frailty and differentially modifies pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokines in aging male and female C57BL/6 mice. Journals of Gerontology Series A, 74, 11491157. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/gly219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, C.-K. and Chen, S.-T. (2019). Estimation and application of population attributable fraction in ecological studies. Environmental Health, 18, 52. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12940-019-0492-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G. etal. (2017). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. The Lancet, 390, 26732734. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, F. E., Chatfield, M., Brayne, C. and Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS) (2006). An investigation of whether factors associated with short-term attrition change or persist over ten years: data from the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS). BMC Public Health, 6, 185. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-6-185 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, F. E., Chatfield, M., Freeman, C., McCracken, C., Brayne, C. and CFAS, M. (2004). Attrition and bias in the MRC cognitive function and ageing study: an epidemiological investigation. BMC Public Health, 4, 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-4-12 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirra, S.S. (1997). The CERAD neuropathology protocol and consensus recommendations for the postmortem diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: a commentary. Neurobiology of Aging, 18, S91S94. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00058-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirra, S. S. etal. (1991). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) Part II. Standardization of the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 41, 479479. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.41.4.479 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitnitski, A. and Rockwood, K. (2015). The rate of aging: the rate of deficit accumulation does not change over the adult life span. Biogerontology, 17, 199204. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10522-015-9583-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitnitski, A., Song, X. and Rockwood, K. (2013). Assessing biological aging: the origin of deficit accumulation. Biogerontology, 14, 709717. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10522-013-9446-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitnitski, A. B., Mogilner, A. J. and Rockwood, K. (2001). Accumulation of deficits as a proxy measure of aging. Scientific World Journal, 1, 323336. https://doi.org/10.1100/tsw.2001.58 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, P. T. etal. (2019) Limbic-predominant age-related TDP-43 encephalopathy (LATE): consensus working group report. Brain, 142, 15031527. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awz099 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, S., Matthews, F. E., Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K. and Brayne, C. (2014). Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. The Lancet Neurology, 13, 788794. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neuropathology Group. Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Aging Study (MRC CFAS) (2001). Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre, community-based population in England and Wales. The Lancet, 357, 169175. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(00)03589-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MRC CFAS and CC75C, etal. (2012). Impact of less common and “Disregarded” neurodegenerative pathologies on dementia burden in a population-based cohort. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 28, 485493. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2011-111268 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rockwood, K., Mitnitski, A. and Howlett, S. E. (2015). Frailty: scaling from cellular deficit accumulation? Interdisciplinary Topics in Gerontology and Geriatrics, 41, 114. https://doi.org/10.1159/000381127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, N. T., Steptoe, A. and Cadar, D. (2017). Frailty is an independent predictor of incident dementia: evidence from the English longitudinal study of ageing. Scientific Reports, 7, 17. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-16104-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savva, G. M., Wharton, S. B., Ince, P. G., Forster, G., Matthews, F. E. and Brayne, C. (2009). Age, neuropathology, and dementia. New England Journal of Medicine, 360, 23022309. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa0806142 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Searle, S. D. and Rockwood, K. (2015). Frailty and the risk of cognitive impairment. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-015-0140-3 Google ScholarPubMed
Song, X., Mitnitski, A. and Rockwood, K. (2014). Age-related deficit accumulation and the risk of late-life dementia. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 6, 54. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-014-0054-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterniczuk, R., Theou, O., Rusak, B. and Rockwood, K. (2015). Cognitive test performance in relation to health and function in 12 European Countries: the SHARE Study. Canadian Geriatrics Journal, 18, 144151. https://doi.org/10.5770/cgj.18.154 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoltzfus, J. C. (2011). Logistic regression: a brief primer. Academic Emergency Medicine: Official Journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine, 18, 10991104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2011.01185.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thibeau, S., McDermott, K., McFall, G. P., Rockwood, K. and Dixon, R. A. (2019). Frailty effects on non-demented cognitive trajectories are moderated by sex and Alzheimer’s genetic risk. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 11, 55. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-019-0509-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trebbastoni, A. etal. (2017). The impact of frailty on the risk of conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: evidences from a 5-year observational study. Frontiers of Medicine, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fmed.2017.00178 Google ScholarPubMed
Wallace, L. M. K., Theou, O., Andrew, M. K. and Rockwood, K. (2018). Relationship between frailty and Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers: a scoping review. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 10, 394401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2018.05.002 Google ScholarPubMed
Wallace, L. M. K. etal. (2020). Neuropathological burden and the degree of frailty in relation to global cognition and dementia. Neurology, https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000010944 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, L. M. K., Theou, O., Godin, J., Andrew, M. K., Bennett, D. A. and Rockwood, K. (2019). Investigation of frailty as a moderator of the relationship between neuropathology and dementia in Alzheimer’s disease: a cross-sectional analysis of data from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. The Lancet Neurology 18, 10. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30371-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, L. M. K. etal. (2014). Accumulation of non-traditional risk factors for coronary heart disease is associated with incident coronary heart disease hospitalization and death. PLoS ONE, 9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090475 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S., Leurgans, S. E., Boyle, P. A., Schneider, J. A. and Bennett, D. A. (2010). Neurodegenerative basis of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology, 75, 10701078. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f39adc CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of sample

Figure 1

Figure 1. Proportion of participants with dementia according to tertiles of neuropathological index and frailty tertiles. Note: Frailty and dementia status were assessed at last survey before death (median 1.9 years pre-mortem), neuropathological burden was assessed at time of death. Numbers within the bars represent sample size.

Figure 2

Table 2. Logistic regression models for dementia status (n = 183; all models adjusted for age, sex, education) demonstrating that the frailty index and neuropathological index are independently associated with dementia status, even when included in the same model. Model fit is significantly improved when both frailty index and neuropathological index are included in a model for dementia status

Supplementary material: File

Wallace et al. supplementary material

Wallace et al. supplementary material

Download Wallace et al. supplementary material(File)
File 217 KB