Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T17:25:53.147Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cumulative lifetime acute stressor exposure interacts with reward responsiveness to predict longitudinal increases in depression severity in adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2022

Kreshnik Burani*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
C. J. Brush
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
Grant S. Shields
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Science, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, USA
Daniel N. Klein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Brady Nelson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
George M. Slavich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Greg Hajcak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Kreshnik Burani, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Life stress and blunted reward processing each have been associated with the onset and maintenance of major depressive disorder. However, much of this work has been cross-sectional, conducted in separate lines of inquiry, and focused on recent life stressor exposure, despite the fact that theories of depression posit that stressors can have cumulative effects over the lifespan. To address these limitations, we investigated whether acute and chronic stressors occurring over the lifespan interacted with blunted reward processing to predict increases in depression over time in healthy youth.

Method

Participants were 245 adolescent girls aged 8–14 years old (Mage = 12.4, s.d. = 1.8) who were evaluated at baseline and two years later. The reward positivity (RewP), an event-related potential measure of reward responsiveness, was assessed at baseline using the doors task. Cumulative lifetime exposure to acute and chronic stressors was assessed two years later using the Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adolescents (Adolescent STRAIN). Finally, depressive symptoms were assessed at both baseline and follow-up using the Children's Depression Inventory.

Results

As hypothesized, greater lifetime acute stressor exposure predicted increases in depressive symptoms over two years, but only for youth exhibiting a blunted RewP. This interaction, however, was not found for chronic stressors.

Conclusions

Lifetime acute stressor exposure may be particularly depressogenic for youth exhibiting a blunted RewP. Conversely, a robust RewP may be protective in the presence of greater acute lifetime stressor exposure.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a pervasive and recurrent psychiatric condition that causes a substantial disease burden in adolescents and adults (Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014). In the United States, the lifetime prevalence is approximately 19%, with similar estimates documented in other countries (Bromet et al., Reference Bromet, Andrade, Hwang, Sampson, Alonso, de Girolamo and Kessler2011). Individuals with MDD often report considerable social and occupational impairment (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Heeringa, Lakoma, Petukhova, Rupp, Schoenbaum and Zaslavsky2008; Kupferberg, Bicks, & Hasler, Reference Kupferberg, Bicks and Hasler2016). The economic burden of depression is also high. For example, in 2010, adults with MDD incurred approximately $100 billion in direct costs related to medical and pharmaceutical services sought for MDD, and another $100 billion in work-related costs attributable to days missed at work and lack of productivity (Greenberg, Fournier, Sisitsky, Pike, & Kessler, Reference Greenberg, Fournier, Sisitsky, Pike and Kessler2015). Among those who have experienced one lifetime major depressive episode (MDE), 60% are likely to develop a second MDE, with recurrence rates increasing as an individual experiences more MDEs (Monroe & Harkness, Reference Monroe and Harkness2011; Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, Keller, Leon, Mueller, Lavori, Shea and Endicott2000). MDD is thus a relatively common and highly burdensome disorder.

Major life stressors are one of the strongest known precipitants of MDD (Hammen, Reference Hammen2005; Kessler, Reference Kessler1997; Slavich, Reference Slavich2016), with their occurrence substantially increasing the risk for developing depressive symptoms and MDD in both adolescents and adults (Bouma, Ormel, Verhulst, & Oldehinkel, Reference Bouma, Ormel, Verhulst and Oldehinkel2008; Fried, Nesse, Guille, & Sen, Reference Fried, Nesse, Guille and Sen2015; Ge, Lorenz, Conger, Elder, & Simons, Reference Ge, Lorenz, Conger, Elder and Simons1994; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999; Mazure, Reference Mazure1998; van Praag, Reference van Praag2004). Stressor duration also has been found to be an important dimension associated with risk for depressive symptoms and MDD (Carter & Garber, Reference Carter and Garber2011; Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields, & Auerbach, Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019). Both acute and chronic stressors have been related to the onset of a depressive episode (Hammen, Kim, Eberhart, & Brennan, Reference Hammen, Kim, Eberhart and Brennan2009); however, research suggests that acute stressors may be particularly depressogenic (Monroe, Slavich, & Georgiades, Reference Monroe, Slavich, Georgiades, I. H. and C. L.2009; Slavich, O'Donovan, Epel, & Kemeny, Reference Slavich, O'Donovan, Epel and Kemeny2010).

Despite the abundance of research examining the association between life stressor exposure and depression, much of this work has focused on recent life stressors. In addition, the neurobiological mechanisms linking stress with depression remain unclear. One possible pathophysiological mechanism involves stress-induced reductions in reward-related brain activity that may lead to increases in depression (Pizzagalli, Reference Pizzagalli2014). For example, experimental research using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) provides support indicating that laboratory stressors blunt activity in the mesolimbic and mesocortical reward pathways (Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Berghorst, Nickerson, Dutra, Goer, Greve and Pizzagalli2014; Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Pisoni, Bondy, Kumar, Singleton, Hajcak and Auerbach2019; Porcelli, Lewis, & Delgado, Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012). Moreover, electroencephalogram (EEG) research has identified the reward positivity (RewP) as an index of reward processing evident approximately 250 ms to 350 ms following feedback indicating rewards compared to losses (Proudfit, Reference Proudfit2015). Prior research has shown that the RewP has good internal consistency in both adolescents and adults (Levinson, Speed, Infantolino, & Hajcak, Reference Levinson, Speed, Infantolino and Hajcak2017; Luking, Nelson, Infantolino, Sauder, & Hajcak, Reference Luking, Nelson, Infantolino, Sauder and Hajcak2017) and that it relates to other measures of reward processing. Specifically, a larger RewP has been associated with greater self-reported reward responsiveness, ventral striatum activation, and reward response bias (Becker, Nitsch, Miltner, & Straube, Reference Becker, Nitsch, Miltner and Straube2014; Bress & Hajcak, Reference Bress and Hajcak2013; Carlson, Foti, Mujica-Parodi, Harmon-Jones, & Hajcak, Reference Carlson, Foti, Mujica-Parodi, Harmon-Jones and Hajcak2011). Overall, the RewP has good psychometric properties and construct validity.

A reduced RewP has been shown to be evident among children and adults with increased depressive symptoms and MDD (Belden et al., Reference Belden, Irvin, Hajcak, Kappenman, Kelly, Karlow and Barch2016; Bowyer et al., Reference Bowyer, Joyner, Yancey, Venables, Hajcak and Patrick2019; Bress, Smith, Foti, Klein, & Hajcak, Reference Bress, Smith, Foti, Klein and Hajcak2012; Brush, Ehmann, Hajcak, Selby, & Alderman, Reference Brush, Ehmann, Hajcak, Selby and Alderman2018; Foti & Hajcak, Reference Foti and Hajcak2009; Klawohn, Burani, Bruchnak, Santopetro, & Hajcak, Reference Klawohn, Burani, Bruchnak, Santopetro and Hajcak2020; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Wang, Shang, Shen, Li, Cheung and Chan2014). Moreover, a reduced RewP has been observed prior to the onset of depression, suggesting that it may be a pre-clinical marker of subsequent risk for MDD (Bress, Foti, Kotov, Klein, & Hajcak, Reference Bress, Foti, Kotov, Klein and Hajcak2013; Nelson, Perlman, Klein, Kotov, & Hajcak, Reference Nelson, Perlman, Klein, Kotov and Hajcak2016). Collectively, the evidence indicates that the RewP is a reliable neural indicator of reward processing that is frequently blunted in individuals with elevated depressive symptoms and MDD and reduced among those at increased risk for depression (Proudfit, Reference Proudfit2015).

In addition, prior research has indicated that reward processing, as measured by the RewP, interacts with life stressor exposure to predict increases in depressive symptoms in adolescents and adults. In a cross-sectional study of young adults, Pegg et al. (Reference Pegg, Ethridge, Shields, Slavich, Weinberg and Kujawa2019) examined whether social v. non-social lifetime stressor severity as measured by the Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adults (STRAIN) interacted with the RewP, measured during a social reward task, to predict depressive symptoms. The authors found that greater lifetime social stressor severity interacted with a blunted RewP to social rewards to predict greater increases in depressive symptoms; however, this was not found for non-social stress severity. In a separate longitudinal study using the Adolescent Life Events Questionnaire, Burani et al. (Reference Burani, Klawohn, Levinson, Klein, Nelson and Hajcak2021) found that experiencing more recent stressful life events interacted with a blunted RewP to predict greater increases in depressive symptoms one year later in adolescent girls. Similarly, Goldstein et al. (Reference Goldstein, Kessel, Kujawa, Finsaas, Davila, Hajcak and Klein2020) found that experiencing more stressful life events, as measured using the UCLA Life Stress Interview, interacted with a blunted RewP to predict greater increases in depressive symptoms three years later. Collectively, these cross-sectional and longitudinal studies are consistent with a diathesis-stress model of depression (Monroe & Simons, Reference Monroe and Simons1991), wherein life stressors are most likely to precipitate depression for persons exhibiting a blunted RewP. To our knowledge, however, no studies have examined how stressors occurring over the entire life course interact with this neural marker indicator of reward-related brain activity to predict changes in depressive symptoms in adolescents over time.

The absence of lifetime stressor studies on this topic is notable because cumulative lifetime stressor exposure has been hypothesized to have adverse effects on neurobiological systems such as the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009), the reward system (Casement, Shaw, Sitnick, Musselman, & Forbes, Reference Casement, Shaw, Sitnick, Musselman and Forbes2015), and the immune system (Slavich, Reference Slavich2020; Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014), each of which have been associated with depression. Stressor exposure activates the HPA axis, which leads to the production of cortisol that helps to mobilize the body's response to stressors; however, repeated exposure to stressors can cause prolonged hyperactivation of the HPA axis that has health-damaging effects (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). Similarly, repeated exposure to stressors during adolescence predicts blunted activation of the ventral striatum – a brain region linked to the RewP – during adulthood (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Albert, Iselin, Carré, Dodge and Hariri2016). Likewise, chronic and repeated stressor exposure has been found to contribute to increased inflammatory activity (Furman et al., Reference Furman, Campisi, Verdin, Carrera-Bastos, Targ, Franceschi and Slavich2019), which has been implicated in depression (Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014). Therefore, multiple findings underscore the importance of investigating how cumulative lifetime stressor exposure interacts with the RewP to shape the risk for stress-related disorders such as depression.

The present study addresses this issue by examining whether acute and chronic stressors occurring over the life course prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms in the context of a blunted RewP in a large community sample of 8- to- 14-year-old girls who were assessed longitudinally over two years. Acute stressors were defined as those lasting a few days (e.g. hearing bad news, getting into an accident), whereas chronic stressors were defined as those lasting at least one month (e.g. persistent housing, financial, or relationship problems; Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019). Recent evidence suggests that acute stressors are stronger proximal predictors of MDD as compared to chronic stressors (Monroe et al., Reference Monroe, Slavich, Georgiades, I. H. and C. L.2009; Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, O'Donovan, Epel and Kemeny2010). Therefore, we were particularly interested in whether acute stressor exposure interacts with the RewP to predict subsequent increases in depressive symptoms.

To investigate this possibility, at baseline, our sample of adolescent females completed a reward task while EEG data were collected; they also completed the Children's Depression Inventory (CDI) to assess their depressive symptoms. Two years later, these participants again completed the CDI as well as the Adolescent STRAIN to assess their lifetime stressor exposure. Based on the literature summarized above, we hypothesized that the RewP assessed at baseline would interact with participants' lifetime stressor exposure to predict subsequent increases in depression. Given prior research on the topic (e.g. Monroe et al., Reference Monroe, Slavich, Georgiades, I. H. and C. L.2009; Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, O'Donovan, Epel and Kemeny2010), we expected that acute lifetime stress exposure, in particular, would be most strongly related to increases in depression for youth exhibiting a blunted RewP.

Method

Sample

Participants were recruited from Long Island, New York, as part of a longitudinal study examining processes associated with developmental changes in reward processing. The complete sample included 317 adolescent girls between 8 and 14 years old (M age = 12.4, s.d. = 1.8). Of these 317 participants, three did not complete the reward task and nine were excluded due to poor EEG data quality. Therefore, the final sample at baseline included 305 participants (M age = 12.4, s.d. = 1.8). At baseline, participants completed the reward task and the CDI (see below).

Two years later, participants returned for a follow-up visit, during which time STRAIN and CDI data were collected (n = 245; M age = 14.4, s.d. = 1.8). Participants who completed both the baseline visit and follow-up visit were younger than those who only completed the baseline visit (M diff = 0.52, t(303) = 2.06, p = 0.040). Importantly, these two groups did not differ on baseline CDI or RewP scores (ps = 0.667).

At the follow-up visit, participants were predominantly White (87%) with the remaining participants self-identifying as African American (6.5%), American Indian/Alaskan Native (0.8%), or Other (5%). Nine percent of the sample at the follow-up visit reported being Hispanic. All participants also completed the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (KSADS) at baseline. Of the 245 participants included in the final sample, 6 individuals (~2.4% of the sample) met the diagnostic criteria for MDD according to the Diagnostic Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV. The average household income of the final sample was $127 190 (s.d. = $75 159). All participants and their parents provided informed assent and consent, respectively, as approved by the Institutional Review Board at Stony Brook University.

Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adolescent STRAIN

Lifetime stressor exposure was assessed at the follow-up session (n = 245) using the Adolescent STRAIN (Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019; see https://www.strainsetup.com). Participants reported on 33 acute life events and 42 chronic difficulties for a total of 75 stressors that spanned 12 primary life domains (i.e. Housing, Education, Work, Treatment/Health, Marital/Partner, Reproduction, Financial, Legal/Crime, Other Relationships, Guardian/Parent Relationships, Death, Life-Threatening Situations) and five core social-psychological characteristics (i.e. Interpersonal Loss, Physical Danger, Humiliation, Entrapment, Role Change/Disruption). For each stressor endorsed, the STRAIN generates additional questions assessing the stressor's severity, frequency, timing of exposure, and duration.

Acute life events (i.e. acute stressors) in the STRAIN system are defined as stressors typically lasting a few days, such as learning of a death, getting fired, or being physically attacked (Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019). Chronic difficulties (i.e. chronic stressors), in turn, typically last a minimum of one month though many last longer, such as persistent educational, housing, or financial problems (Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019). Based on participants' responses, the STRAIN produces cumulative lifetime stressor indices summarizing each individual's total lifetime stressor exposure (i.e. number of stressors experienced) and severity, the latter of which is based on a five-point Likert stressor severity scale. These scores can then be calculated separately for acute and chronic stressors. The STRAIN assesses a variety of stressors across 12 life domains. Higher scores always represent greater lifetime stressor exposure. The Adolescent STRAIN has been validated against clinical, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes (Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019) as has its sister instrument, the Adult STRAIN (Banica, Sandre, Shields, Slavich, & Weinberg, Reference Banica, Sandre, Shields, Slavich and Weinberg2020, Reference Banica, Sandre, Shields, Slavich and Weinberg2021; Cazassa, da Oliveira, Spahr, Shields, & Slavich, Reference Cazassa, da Oliveira, Spahr, Shields and Slavich2020; Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018; Sturmbauer, Shields, Hetzel, Rohleder, & Slavich, Reference Sturmbauer, Shields, Hetzel, Rohleder and Slavich2019).

Reward task

The doors task is a simple monetary reward paradigm in which participants are presented with two doors displayed side-by-side and are instructed to select the door they believe will yield a prize (i.e. money) using the left or right mouse button. Once participants make a decision, a fixation cross is presented for 1500 ms followed by feedback indicating whether they won (i.e. green arrow pointing upwards signifies +$0.50) or lost (i.e. red arrow pointing downwards signifies −$0.25); this feedback was presented for 2000 ms. Following each trial, text on the screen instructed participants to ‘Click for next round,’ followed by a fixation cross presented for 1000 ms. There was a total of 30 gain and 30 loss trials, presented pseudorandomly using Presentation version 17.0 (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA). Participants were told they had a chance to earn up to $15; all participants were given $8 at the end of the task.

Children's Depression Inventory (CDI)

The CDI assesses depressive symptoms over the last two weeks and has been well-validated in children between 7 and 17 years old (Kovacs, Reference Kovacs1992). It consists of 27 items rated on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from 0 to 2, with 0 indicating an absence of a symptom and 2 indicating the definite presence of the symptom. Total scores range from 0 to 54, with higher scores indicating greater depressive symptom severity. Participants completed the CDI at baseline (n = 305; α = 0.89) and two years later (n = 255; α = 0.90).

EEG processing

Continuous EEG data were recorded while participants completed the doors task using the ActiveTwo BioSemi system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands) using an elastic cap containing 34 electrode sites placed according to the 10/20 system (i.e. 32 channels plus Iz and FCz). Facial electrodes were placed above and below the left eye, and near the outer canthi of the left and right eyes to monitor horizontal and vertical electrooculographic activity. Two additional electrodes were placed on the left and right mastoids. The EEG signal was pre-amplified at the electrode to improve signal-to-noise ratio, and data were digitized at a 24-bit resolution with a sampling rate of 1024 Hz using a low-pass fifth-order sinc filter with a half-power cutoff of 204 Hz. Active electrodes were measured online with reference to a common mode sense active electrode constructing a monopolar channel.

EEG data were processed offline using BrainVision Analyzer 2.1 (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany). Raw EEG data were re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids. The re-referenced data were filtered from 0.1 Hz to 30 Hz using a 2nd order filter. The EEG data were then segmented from −200 ms prior to the onset of feedback and up to 1000 ms following feedback. Eyeblinks and ocular movement correction was performed using the Gratton, Coles, and Donchin (Reference Gratton, Coles and Donchin1983) regression-based method. Prior to averaging the data as a function of feedback type, segments containing a voltage greater than 50 μV between consecutive sample points, a voltage difference of 300 μV within a segment, or a maximum voltage difference of less than 0.5 μV within 100-ms intervals were identified as artifacts and automatically rejected. The 200-ms pre-feedback interval was used for baseline correction.

RewP quantification

The RewP was calculated as the mean activity within a 100-ms time window around the most positive peak of the gain (M trials = 29.3; s.d. = 1.8) minus loss (M trials = 29.2; s.d. = 1.9) difference waveform extracted from a 200 ms to 400 ms time window at FCz for each participant (Burani et al., Reference Burani, Klawohn, Levinson, Klein, Nelson and Hajcak2021). No participants were excluded for having a low number of trials.

Data analysis

All analyses were conducted in R version 4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2020). First, bivariate Pearson correlations were conducted to determine associations between the Adolescent STRAIN variables, Baseline RewP, Baseline Age, and CDI scores at baseline and follow up using the psych R package (Revelle, Reference Revelle2020). Internal consistency was calculated using the Psychometric package (Fletcher, Reference Fletcher2010). To test our primary hypothesis, a multiple linear regression was conducted. CDI scores at follow up were predicted from Baseline RewP, Baseline Age, Baseline CDI scores, Lifetime Acute Stressor Exposure, Lifetime Chronic Stressor Exposure, the Lifetime Acute Stressor Exposure × Baseline RewP interaction, and the Lifetime Chronic Stressor Exposure × Baseline RewP interaction. All variables were mean-centered in the regression analyses. For the multiple linear regression, we used the stats R package (R Core Team, 2020), and follow-up simple slopes analyses were performed and plotted using the interactions package (Long, Reference Long2019). The Johnson-Neyman technique was used to identify the region of significance wherein the simple slope analyses showed a significant interaction (Johnson & Neyman, Reference Johnson and Neyman1936). The Johnson-Neyman plot thus provides values of the moderator for which the relation between the predictor and outcome are both non-significant and significant. The final sample size for the regression analyses was 245.

To test for the robustness of any significant effect observed, we also performed sensitivity analyses that included household income and ethnicity as additional covariates. The household income variable was recoded into a categorical variable prior to being entered into the regression such that an increase of $ 30 000 placed participants into a higher category (e.g. 0 = less than $29900, 1 = between 29 900 and 59 000, etc., up to 299 900). All participants who reported an income of more than $ 299 900 were all placed in one category. Given that the sample consisted largely of White participants, ethnicity was dichotomized into White (0) and non-White (1). The car R package was used to generate the variance inflation factor (Fox, Reference Fox2020) which indicates the degree of multicollinearity.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Figure 1 displays the event-related potential to wins and losses as well as the RewP (i.e. difference waveform) and the scalp topography of the sample. Demographics for the sample, and descriptives of all study variables, are presented in Table 1. Correlations between the variables are presented in Table 2. Participants experienced an average of 13.9 lifetime stressors (s.d. = 10.63; range 0–56), with 8.1 acute stressors (s.d. = 6.52; range 0–34) and 5.8 chronic stressors (s.d. = 4.90; range 0–28). Average RewP amplitude at baseline was 5.53 μV (s.d. = 6.2). Of note, the RewP did not correlate with any lifetime stressor exposure variables (see Table 2).

Fig. 1. The event-related potential (ERP) to wins (red) and losses (black) as well as the difference waveform (RewP) at electrode FCz (top graph) and the scalp topography (bottom graph) (N = 245).

Table 1. Descriptives for the study variables, and demographics of the sample (N = 245).

STRAIN, Stress and Adversity Inventory; RewP, Reward Positivity; CDI, Children's Depression Inventory.

Note: Of the 245 participants in the final sample, data on ethnicity were available for 238 participants.

Table 2. Correlations among the main study variables

STRAIN, Stress and Adversity Inventory; RewP, Reward Positivity; CDI, Children's Depression Inventory.

**p < 0.001. *p < 0.05. Pearson's r correlations are reported.

Lifetime chronic and acute stressor exposure

We examined whether lifetime acute stressor exposure and lifetime chronic stressor exposure interacted with baseline RewP to predict depressive symptoms at follow up, controlling for baseline depressive symptoms and baseline age (Table 3). Higher baseline depressive symptoms and greater lifetime chronic stressor exposure predicted higher depressive symptoms at follow up. Baseline RewP, baseline age and lifetime acute stressor exposure, however, did not predict elevations in depressive symptoms at follow up. Finally, lifetime acute, but not chronic, stressor exposure interacted with baseline RewP to predict greater depressive symptoms at follow up. Simple slopes analyses indicated that lifetime acute stressor exposure predicted higher depressive symptoms at follow up for youth with a low (−1 s.d.) but not average or high (+1 s.d.) RewP (see Table 4 and Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Conditional effect of lifetime acute stressor exposure (STRAIN) on follow-up depressive severity (CDI). Johnson-Neyman plot of the Lifetime Acute Stressor Exposure × Reward Positivity interaction predicting depressive symptom scores on the Children's Depression Inventory at follow up. The dotted line indicates the value of the RewP where the confidence intervals cross zero. Lifetime Acute Stressor Exposure predicted depressive symptom scores at follow up at amplitudes of RewP below the dotted line. The shaded area represents the confidence interval of the conditional effect, with the dark gray area representing values of Baseline RewP for which the effect is significant, and the light gray area representing values of Baseline RewP for which the effect is non-significant. Baseline RewP, Age, CDI scores and Lifetime Chronic Stressor Exposure were included as covariates (N = 245).

Table 3. Regression results using depressive symptoms at follow up as the dependent variable predicted from lifetime acute stressor exposure, lifetime chronic stressor exposure, baseline age, RewP, depressive symptoms, the lifetime acute stressor exposure × baseline RewP interaction, and the lifetime chronic stressor exposure × baseline RewP interaction

RewP, Reward Positivity; CDI, Children's Depression Inventory; STRAIN, Stress and Adversity Inventory; b, unstandardized regression weights; CI, 95% confidence interval; LL and UL indicate the lower and upper limits of the CI, respectively; VIF, Variance Inflation Factor. ^ = p < 0.10; * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01.

Table 4. Simple slopes of the relation between lifetime acute stressor exposure (STRAIN) and depression severity (CDI) scores two years later at different levels of baseline RewP

RewP, Reward Positivity; CI, 95% confidence interval.

***p < 0.001.

Note: Beta coefficients are adjusted for baseline depressive symptoms and baseline age.

Online Supplementary Table S1 contains a multiple regression model predicting depressive symptoms at follow up with the same predictors as the main regression model but, in addition, includes household income and ethnicity as covariates. With household income and ethnicity included, lifetime acute stressor exposure and the RewP interacted to predict depressive symptoms at follow up at a trend level. These results suggest that income and ethnicity may partially explain this interactive effect on depression.

Lifetime chronic and acute stressor severity

Online Supplementary Table S2 contains the results of a multiple regression model predicting CDI scores at follow up from the lifetime acute stressor severity and lifetime chronic stressor severity variable while controlling for baseline RewP, baseline CDI and baseline age. This analysis was conducted to examine the specificity of effects for stressor exposure v. stressor severity. Baseline age, baseline RewP, and lifetime acute stressor severity were not significant predictors of depression at follow up; however, greater baseline depressive symptoms and lifetime chronic stressor severity predicted higher follow-up depressive symptoms. Finally, lifetime acute, but not chronic, stressor severity interacted with the RewP to predict increases in depressive symptoms at follow up such that lifetime acute stressor severity predicted depressive symptoms but only for those with a blunted RewP (see online Supplementary Table S2a). Therefore, the results for lifetime acute stressor exposure and severity were similar.

Residualized wins and residualized losses

To examine whether these results were driven by wins or losses, we used residualized difference scores for both wins and losses (Meyer, Lerner, De Los Reyes, Laird, & Hajcak, Reference Meyer, Lerner, De Los Reyes, Laird and Hajcak2017) in separate regression models. Overall, the results indicated that only residualized wins interacted with lifetime acute stressor exposure to predict depressive symptoms at follow up. In sum, therefore, these data suggest that the results may be driven by neural responses to rewards instead of losses (see online Supplementary Tables S3, S3a and S4).

Discussion

The present study examined whether stressors occurring across the life course moderated the association between the RewP and longitudinal increases in depressive symptoms in a large sample of 8-to-14-year-old girls. We found that exposure to chronic (but not acute) stressors over the lifespan predicted increases in depressive symptoms over time. As hypothesized, however, only acute lifetime stressor exposure was moderated by the RewP, such that increases in depressive symptoms over the two-year study period were evident only for youth exhibiting a blunted RewP.

These results are consistent with diathesis-stress models of depression (Monroe & Simons, Reference Monroe and Simons1991) insofar as acute life stressors appeared to be particularly depressogenic for youth with blunted reward processing. These findings are also consistent with prior studies, which have found that life stress and the RewP interact to predict depression (Burani et al., Reference Burani, Klawohn, Levinson, Klein, Nelson and Hajcak2021; Goldstein et al., Reference Goldstein, Kessel, Kujawa, Finsaas, Davila, Hajcak and Klein2020). Our results add to this prior work by suggesting that this interaction between life stressor exposure and reward functioning may be evident only for acute stressors occurring over the life course. The RewP may thus be a stress vulnerability marker insofar as individuals with a blunted RewP exhibit less responsivity to reward and reward-seeking behavior (Bress & Hajcak, Reference Bress and Hajcak2013), which may lead to a greater impact of acute stressor exposure through less engagement with rewarding activities and stimuli in the environment.

One possible biological pathway by which stressor exposure may impact reward processing leading to depression involves the HPA axis. When activated, the HPA axis produces corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and glucocorticoids. Several brain regions implicated in reward processing are rich in receptors for CRF and glucocorticoids, including the prefrontal cortex, the ventral striatum, and the ventral tegmental area (Novick et al., Reference Novick, Levandowski, Laumann, Philip, Price and Tyrka2018; Van Pett et al., Reference Van Pett, Viau, Bittencourt, Chan, Li, Arias and Sawchenko2000). Increased CRF in the reward system may in turn attenuate dopamine transmission leading to reduced reward-related behavior (Bryce & Floresco, Reference Bryce and Floresco2016). A second possibility involves the immune system. Greater lifetime stressor exposure has been found to predict higher levels of inflammatory activity (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Lind, Horn, Mills, Nelson, Barnes and Allen2021), and a core biobehavioral effect of inflammation involves the reduction of reward-related neural signaling and behavior (Felger et al., Reference Felger, Li, Haroon, Woolwine, Jung, Hu and Miller2016; Slavich, Reference Slavich2020, Reference Slavich2022). Additional research is needed to investigate these and other biological pathways that may link greater lifetime stressor exposure with reward-related neural function and depression.

Consistent with prior research, these results also demonstrate that a robust RewP may potentially confer resilience to stress (Burani et al., Reference Burani, Klawohn, Levinson, Klein, Nelson and Hajcak2021; Goldstein et al., Reference Goldstein, Kessel, Kujawa, Finsaas, Davila, Hajcak and Klein2020; Pegg et al., Reference Pegg, Ethridge, Shields, Slavich, Weinberg and Kujawa2019). Individuals with greater acute lifetime stressor exposure did not exhibit increases in depressive symptoms two years later if they had a larger RewP response to reward. This finding suggests that youth who respond to rewarding stimuli robustly may be relatively protected against the depressogenic effects of life stress. Importantly, hypersensitivity to rewards may also function as a risk factor for other forms of psychopathology. Increased reward processing, for example, could lead to excessive approach-related affect that is characteristic of hypomania and mania (Alloy & Nusslock, Reference Alloy and Nusslock2019; Nusslock, Young, & Damme, Reference Nusslock, Young and Damme2014). Nevertheless, future research may benefit from developing interventions that aim to enhance reactivity to pleasant stimuli/rewards among those with a blunted RewP, which may increase resiliency – something that is currently done in cognitive behavior therapy (e.g. promoting behavioral activation, scheduling pleasurable activities) but which could be perhaps further emphasized for those with deficient reward system functioning.

Several limitations of this study are worth noting. First, we only sampled adolescent girls given their relatively high risk for developing depression during this developmental period. Moreover, this sample was largely White. Future research should thus examine if these results generalize across both sexes and genders as well as to older adolescents, adults, and racially and ethnically diverse populations. This is particularly important as the nature of depressive symptoms differs across ethnicities (Assari & Moazen-Zadeh, Reference Assari and Moazen-Zadeh2016; Wight, Aneshensel, Botticello, & Sepúlveda, Reference Wight, Aneshensel, Botticello and Sepúlveda2005) as well in adolescents v. adults, who display higher levels of anhedonia (Rice et al., Reference Rice, Riglin, Lomax, Souter, Potter, Smith and Thapar2019). Therefore, the age range and racial/ethnic composition of this sample may limit the depressive symptoms that were assessed. Along these same lines, additional research is needed to examine the possibility that any sex and/or gender differences detected in the future may vary as a function of biological and/or social factors that have been associated with depression. Second, participants were recruited from the community and thus exhibited relatively few major life stressors and depressive symptoms. It should be noted that even modest elevations in depressive symptoms in this age range are strong predictors of subsequent increases in depressive symptoms or onset of MDD (Klein, Shankman, Lewinsohn, & Seeley, Reference Klein, Shankman, Lewinsohn and Seeley2009). Nevertheless, future studies are needed to study these processes in at-risk and depressed populations. Specifically, recruiting individuals who have experienced more lifetime stressors, or who are at risk for developing depression, would permit an examination of whether such stressors interact with blunted reward processing to predict caseness of MDD, rather than increases in depressive symptoms. In addition, future studies could investigate whether the timing of acute or chronic stressors interacts with the RewP to predict depressive symptoms.

Third, future studies are needed to examine the time course characteristics of both lifetime acute stressor exposure and blunted reward processing, and how they interact to impact the development of depressive symptoms. Fourth, we used baseline RewP as predictor of changes in depressive symptoms over time. This approach has both limitations and benefits. Regarding the limitations, although clinical interview data suggest that this sample was relatively healthy, it is unknown whether these adolescents experienced depressive symptoms prior to the baseline visit that may have impacted their RewP. Future longitudinal research is thus needed to examine this issue. Fifth, retrospective self-reporting of life stressors can be biased by depressive symptoms, which can make it difficult to determine if high lifetime stressor scores are attributable to experiencing more stressors v. having more depressive symptoms (Harkness & Monroe, Reference Harkness and Monroe2016). We addressed this issue by using an instrument for assessing stressor exposure (i.e. the STRAIN), which is not sensitive to negative mood or social desirability (Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018).

Notwithstanding these points, the present study is the first to demonstrate that greater lifetime acute stressor exposure interacts with the RewP to predict longitudinal increases in depressive symptoms over a two-year period. Framed in terms of resilience, individuals with an increased RewP appeared to be protected from the depressogenic effects of acute life stressor exposure. In contrast with our prior work, we did not find a main effect of RewP in prospectively predicting increases in depressive symptoms (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Perlman, Klein, Kotov and Hajcak2016). However, the present study differed in several ways, including a different age range and different symptom measures (i.e. interview-based v. self-report), and a longer duration between baseline and follow up. In doing so, the present study extends our understanding of how the RewP interacts with lifetime stressor exposure to predict changes in depression over time. Collectively, these data provide novel evidence regarding possible pathways to depression, insofar as they suggest that adolescents who experience greater acute stressor burden over the lifespan, and who do not exhibit a strong neural response to reward, may be at the greatest risk of developing depression.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722001386.

Author contributions

K. B. analyzed the data under the supervision of C. J. B., G. M. S., and G. H. The article was drafted by K. B., and all co-authors provided critical revisions. G. M. S. created the STRAIN and prepared the stressor data for analyses, led by G. S. S. The study was designed by G. H., D. N. K., and B. N., and G. H. acquired the primary study funding.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant MH097767 to G. H., NIH grant 5T32 MH093311-09 to K. B., and NIH grant F32 MH125504 to C. J. B. G. M. S. was supported by grant #OPR21101 from the California Initiative to Advance Precision Medicine and by contract #21-10317 from the Office of the California Surgeon General and California Department of Health Care Services, which supports the UCLA-UCSF ACEs Aware Family Resilience Network. These organizations had no role in designing or planning this study; in collecting, analyzing, or interpreting the data; in writing the article; or in deciding to submit this article for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

References

Alloy, L. B., & Nusslock, R. (2019). Future directions for understanding adolescent bipolar spectrum disorders: A reward hypersensitivity perspective. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(4), 669683. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2019.1567347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assari, S., & Moazen-Zadeh, E. (2016). Ethnic variation in the cross-sectional association between domains of depressive symptoms and clinical depression. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7, 53. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banica, I., Sandre, A., Shields, G. S., Slavich, G. M., & Weinberg, A. (2020). The error-related negativity (ERN) moderates the association between interpersonal stress and anxiety symptoms six months later. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 153, 2736. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.03.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banica, I., Sandre, A., Shields, G. S., Slavich, G. M., & Weinberg, A. (2021). Associations between lifetime stress exposure and the error-related negativity (ERN) differ based on stressor characteristics and exposure timing in young adults. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 118. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-021-00883-z.Google ScholarPubMed
Becker, M. P., Nitsch, A. M., Miltner, W. H., & Straube, T. (2014). A single-trial estimation of the feedback-related negativity and its relation to BOLD responses in a time-estimation task. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(8), 30053012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belden, A. C., Irvin, K., Hajcak, G., Kappenman, E. S., Kelly, D., Karlow, S., … Barch, D. M. (2016). Neural correlates of reward processing in depressed and healthy preschool-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(12), 10811089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouma, E. M. C., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Oldehinkel, A. J. (2008). Stressful life events and depressive problems in early adolescent boys and girls: The influence of parental depression, temperament and family environment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 105(1–3), 185193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.05.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowyer, C. B., Joyner, K. J., Yancey, J. R., Venables, N. C., Hajcak, G., & Patrick, C. J. (2019). Toward a neurobehavioral trait conceptualization of depression proneness. Psychophysiology, 56(7), e13367. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bress, J. N., Foti, D., Kotov, R., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts depression in adolescent girls. Psychophysiology, 50(1), 7481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bress, J. N., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Self-report and behavioral measures of reward sensitivity predict the feedback negativity. Psychophysiology, 50(7), 610616. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bress, J. N., Smith, E., Foti, D., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Neural response to reward and depressive symptoms in late childhood to early adolescence. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 156162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromet, E., Andrade, L. H., Hwang, I., Sampson, N. A., Alonso, J., de Girolamo, G., … Kessler, R. C. (2011). Cross-national epidemiology of DSM-IV major depressive episode. BMC Medicine, 9(1), 90. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-9-90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brush, C. J., Ehmann, P. J., Hajcak, G., Selby, E. A., & Alderman, B. L. (2018). Using multilevel modeling to examine blunted neural responses to reward in major depression. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(12), 10321039.Google ScholarPubMed
Bryce, C. A., & Floresco, S. B. (2016). Perturbations in effort-related decision-making driven by acute stress and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(8), 21472159. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burani, K., Klawohn, J., Levinson, A. R., Klein, D. N., Nelson, B. D., & Hajcak, G. (2021). Neural response to rewards, stress and sleep interact to prospectively predict depressive symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 50(1), 131140. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2019.1630834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, M. L., Lind, M. N., Horn, S. R., Mills, K. L., Nelson, B. W., Barnes, M. L., … Allen, N. B. (2021). Using mobile sensing data to assess stress: Associations with perceived and lifetime stress, mental health, sleep, and inflammation. Digital Health, 7, 20552076211037228. https://doi.org/10.1177/20552076211037227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, J. M., Foti, D., Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Harmon-Jones, E., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Ventral striatal and medial prefrontal BOLD activation is correlated with reward-related electrocortical activity: A combined ERP and fMRI study. Neuroimage, 57(4), 16081616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, J. S., & Garber, J. (2011). Predictors of the first onset of a major depressive episode and changes in depressive symptoms across adolescence: Stress and negative cognitions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(4), 779796. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casement, M. D., Shaw, D. S., Sitnick, S. L., Musselman, S. C., & Forbes, E. E. (2015). Life stress in adolescence predicts early adult reward-related brain function and alcohol dependence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(3), 416423. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cazassa, M. J., da Oliveira, M. S., Spahr, C. M., Shields, G. S., & Slavich, G. M. (2020). The stress and adversity inventory for adults (adult STRAIN) in Brazilian Portuguese: Initial validation and links with executive function, sleep, and mental and physical health. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 3083. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felger, J. C., Li, Z., Haroon, E., Woolwine, B. J., Jung, M. Y., Hu, X., & Miller, A. H. (2016). Inflammation is associated with decreased functional connectivity within corticostriatal reward circuitry in depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(10), 13581365. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, T. D. (2010). psychometric: Applied psychometric theory (version R package version 2.2). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psychometric.Google Scholar
Foti, D., & Hajcak, G. (2009). Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 81(1), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. (2020). Companion to applied regression (version 3.0-10). Retrieved from https://r-forge.r-project.org/projects/car/, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=car.Google Scholar
Fried, E. I., Nesse, R. M., Guille, C., & Sen, S. (2015). The differential influence of life stress on individual symptoms of depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 131(6), 465471. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furman, D., Campisi, J., Verdin, E., Carrera-Bastos, P., Targ, S., Franceschi, C., … Slavich, G. M. (2019). Chronic inflammation in the etiology of disease across the life span. Nature Medicine, 25(12), 18221832. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-019-0675-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., & Simons, R. L. (1994). Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 467. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.4.467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, B. L., Kessel, E. M., Kujawa, A., Finsaas, M. C., Davila, J., Hajcak, G., & Klein, D. N. (2020). Stressful life events moderate the effect of neural reward responsiveness in childhood on depressive symptoms in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 50(9), 15481555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55(4), 468484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, P. E., Fournier, A.-A., Sisitsky, T., Pike, C. T., & Kessler, R. C. (2015). The economic burden of adults with major depressive disorder in the United States (2005 and 2010). The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 76(02), 155162. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.14m09298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1(1), 293319. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C., Kim, E. Y., Eberhart, N. K., & Brennan, P. A. (2009). Chronic and acute stress and the prediction of major depression in women. Depression and Anxiety, 26(8), 718723. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, J. L., Albert, D., Iselin, A.-M. R., Carré, J. M., Dodge, K. A., & Hariri, A. R. (2016). Cumulative stress in childhood is associated with blunted reward-related brain activity in adulthood. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 405412. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harkness, K. L., & Monroe, S. M. (2016). The assessment and measurement of adult life stress: Basic premises, operational principles, and design requirements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(5), 727. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, P. O., & Neyman, J. (1936). Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their application to some educational problems. Statistical Research Memoirs, 1, 5793.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(6), 837841. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.6.837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C. (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 191214. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Heeringa, S., Lakoma, M. D., Petukhova, M., Rupp, A. E., Schoenbaum, M., … Zaslavsky, A. M. (2008). Individual and societal effects of mental disorders on earnings in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 703711. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klawohn, J., Burani, K., Bruchnak, A., Santopetro, N., & Hajcak, G. (2020). Reduced neural response to reward and pleasant pictures independently relate to depression. Psychological Medicine, 51(5), 741749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., Shankman, S. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (2009). Subthreshold depressive disorder in adolescents: Predictors of escalation to full-syndrome depressive disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(7), 703710. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181a56606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M. (1992). Childrens depression inventory. New York: Multi-Health Systems North Tonawanda.Google Scholar
Kumar, P., Berghorst, L. H., Nickerson, L. D., Dutra, S. J., Goer, F. K., Greve, D. N., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Differential effects of acute stress on anticipatory and consummatory phases of reward processing. Neuroscience, 266, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.01.058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupferberg, A., Bicks, L., & Hasler, G. (2016). Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 313332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, A. R., Speed, B. C., Infantolino, Z. P., & Hajcak, G. (2017). Reliability of the electrocortical response to gains and losses in the doors task. Psychophysiology, 54(4), 601607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, S. H., Pisoni, A., Bondy, E., Kumar, P., Singleton, P., Hajcak, G., … Auerbach, R. P. (2019). Altered reward processing following an acute social stressor in adolescents. PLoS One, 14(1), e0209361. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, W., Wang, L., Shang, H., Shen, Y., Li, Z., Cheung, E. F. C., & Chan, R. C. K. (2014). The influence of anhedonia on feedback negativity in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychologia, 53, 213220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, J. A. (2019). Interactions: comprehensive, user-friendly toolkit for probing interactions. R package ver., 1.1.0.Google Scholar
Luking, K. R., Nelson, B. D., Infantolino, Z. P., Sauder, C. L., & Hajcak, G. (2017). Internal consistency of functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography measures of reward in late childhood and early adolescence. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(3), 289297.Google ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazure, C. M. (1998). Life stressors as risk factors in depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5(3), 291313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1998.tb00151.x.Google Scholar
Meyer, A., Lerner, M. D., De Los Reyes, A., Laird, R. D., & Hajcak, G. (2017). Considering ERP difference scores as individual difference measures: Issues with subtraction and alternative approaches. Psychophysiology, 54(1), 114122. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2011). Recurrence in major depression: A conceptual analysis. Psychological Review, 118(4), 655. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., Slavich, G. M., & Georgiades, K. (2009). The social environment and life stress in depression. In I. H., Gotlib, & C. L., Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (2nd edn., pp. 340360). The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Nelson, B. D., Perlman, G., Klein, D. N., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2016). Blunted neural response to rewards as a prospective predictor of the development of depression in adolescent girls. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(12), 12231230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novick, A. M., Levandowski, M. L., Laumann, L. E., Philip, N. S., Price, L. H., & Tyrka, A. R. (2018). The effects of early life stress on reward processing. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 101, 80103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nusslock, R., Young, C. B., & Damme, K. S. F. (2014). Elevated reward-related neural activation as a unique biological marker of bipolar disorder: Assessment and treatment implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 62, 7487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.08.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pegg, S., Ethridge, P., Shields, G. S., Slavich, G. M., Weinberg, A., & Kujawa, A. (2019). Blunted social reward responsiveness moderates the effect of lifetime social stress exposure on depressive symptoms. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 178. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Depression, stress, and anhedonia: Toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10(1), 393423. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porcelli, A. J., Lewis, A. H., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Acute stress influences neural circuits of reward processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 157. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Proudfit, G. H. (2015). The reward positivity: From basic research on reward to a biomarker for depression. Psychophysiology, 52(4), 449459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Revelle, W. (2020). Psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research (Version R package version 2.0.7). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.Google Scholar
Rice, F., Riglin, L., Lomax, T., Souter, E., Potter, R., Smith, D. J., … Thapar, A. (2019). Adolescent and adult differences in major depression symptom profiles. Journal of Affective Disorders, 243, 175181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.09.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M. (2016). Life stress and health: A review of conceptual issues and recent findings. Teaching of Psychology, 43(4), 346355. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628316662768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slavich, G. M. (2020). Social safety theory: A biologically based evolutionary perspective on life stress, health, and behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), annurev-clinpsy-032816-045159. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M. (2022). Social safety theory: Understanding social stress, disease risk, resilience, and behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic and beyond. Current Opinion in Psychology, 45, 101299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: A social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 774. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., O'Donovan, A., Epel, E. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2010). Black sheep get the blues: A psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 3945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slavich, G. M., & Shields, G. S. (2018). Assessing lifetime stress exposure using the stress and adversity inventory for adults (adult STRAIN): An overview and initial validation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(1), 1727. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., Stewart, J. G., Esposito, E. C., Shields, G. S., & Auerbach, R. P. (2019). The stress and adversity inventory for adolescents (adolescent STRAIN): Associations with mental and physical health, risky behaviors, and psychiatric diagnoses in youth seeking treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(9), 9981009. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, D. A., Keller, M. B., Leon, A. C., Mueller, T. I., Lavori, P. W., Shea, M. T., … Endicott, J. (2000). Multiple recurrences of major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(2), 229233. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturmbauer, S. C., Shields, G. S., Hetzel, E.-L., Rohleder, N., & Slavich, G. M. (2019). The stress and adversity inventory for adults (adult STRAIN) in German: An overview and initial validation. PLoS One, 14(5), e0216419. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Pett, K., Viau, V., Bittencourt, J. C., Chan, R. K. W., Li, H.-Y., Arias, C., … Sawchenko, P. E. (2000). Distribution of mRNAs encoding CRF receptors in brain and pituitary of rat and mouse. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 428(2), 191212. https://doi.org/10.1002/1096-9861(20001211)428:2<191::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-U.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Praag, H. M. (2004). Can stress cause depression? Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 28(5), 891907. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.05.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wight, R. G., Aneshensel, C. S., Botticello, A. L., & Sepúlveda, J. E. (2005). A multilevel analysis of ethnic variation in depressive symptoms among adolescents in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 60(9), 20732084. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. The event-related potential (ERP) to wins (red) and losses (black) as well as the difference waveform (RewP) at electrode FCz (top graph) and the scalp topography (bottom graph) (N = 245).

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptives for the study variables, and demographics of the sample (N = 245).

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations among the main study variables

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Conditional effect of lifetime acute stressor exposure (STRAIN) on follow-up depressive severity (CDI). Johnson-Neyman plot of the Lifetime Acute Stressor Exposure × Reward Positivity interaction predicting depressive symptom scores on the Children's Depression Inventory at follow up. The dotted line indicates the value of the RewP where the confidence intervals cross zero. Lifetime Acute Stressor Exposure predicted depressive symptom scores at follow up at amplitudes of RewP below the dotted line. The shaded area represents the confidence interval of the conditional effect, with the dark gray area representing values of Baseline RewP for which the effect is significant, and the light gray area representing values of Baseline RewP for which the effect is non-significant. Baseline RewP, Age, CDI scores and Lifetime Chronic Stressor Exposure were included as covariates (N = 245).

Figure 4

Table 3. Regression results using depressive symptoms at follow up as the dependent variable predicted from lifetime acute stressor exposure, lifetime chronic stressor exposure, baseline age, RewP, depressive symptoms, the lifetime acute stressor exposure × baseline RewP interaction, and the lifetime chronic stressor exposure × baseline RewP interaction

Figure 5

Table 4. Simple slopes of the relation between lifetime acute stressor exposure (STRAIN) and depression severity (CDI) scores two years later at different levels of baseline RewP

Supplementary material: File

Burani et al. supplementary material

Burani et al. supplementary material

Download Burani et al. supplementary material(File)
File 34.7 KB