Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-20T05:35:19.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Taking the New Year's Resolution Test seriously: eliciting individuals’ judgements about self-control and spontaneity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2022

Kevin P. Grubiak
Affiliation:
School of Business, Economics and Information Systems, University of Passau, Passau, Germany
Andrea Isoni*
Affiliation:
Behavioural Science Group, Warwick Business School, Coventry, UK University of Cagliari, Cagliari, Italy
Robert Sugden
Affiliation:
School of Economics and Centre for Behavioural and Experimental Social Science, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
Mengjie Wang
Affiliation:
Cardiff Business School, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK
Jiwei Zheng
Affiliation:
Lancaster University Management School, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
*
*Correspondence to: E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Self-control failure occurs when an individual experiences a conflict between immediate desires and longer-term goals, recognises psychological forces that hinder goal-directed action, tries to resist them but fails in the attempt. Behavioural economists often invoke assumptions about self-control failure to justify proposals for policy interventions. These arguments require workable methods for eliciting individuals’ goals and for verifying occurrences of self-control failure, but developing such methods confronts two problems. First, it is not clear that individuals’ goals are context-independent. Second, facing an actual conflict between a desire and a self-acknowledged goal, a person may consciously choose not to resist the desire, thinking that spontaneity is more important than self-control. We address these issues through an online survey that elicited individuals’ self-reported judgements about the relative importance of self-control and spontaneity in conflicts between enjoyment and health-related goals. To test for context-sensitivity, the judgement-elicitation questions were preceded by a memory recall task which directed participants’ attention either to the enjoyment of acting on desires or to the satisfaction of achieving goals. We found little evidence of context-sensitivity. In both treatments, however, judgements that favoured spontaneity were expressed with roughly the same frequency and strength as judgments that favoured self-control.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In offering guidance about public policy, behavioural economists often use the concept of self-control failure to support claims that, in specific contexts, individuals’ actual choices are contrary to their best interests, as judged by those individuals themselves. Policy interventions are proposed with the aim of helping people to reach goals that they acknowledge as theirs, but which forces within their own psychology hinder them from achieving.Footnote 1 An individual experiences self-control failure if he or she is aware of such forces, tries to resist them, but fails in the attempt. Policies that are designed to counter such failures can be defended as non-paternalistic on the grounds that the policy-maker's intention is to satisfy individuals’ self-acknowledged goals.Footnote 2

If this kind of justification is to be used to guide public policy, behavioural economics needs workable methods for eliciting individuals’ goals and for verifying occurrences of self-control failure. Surprisingly little attention has been given to the design of such methods. Potential questions about people's goals are often treated as having self-evident answers, as when Thaler and Sunstein (Reference Thaler and Sunstein2008, p. 73), advocating nudging people towards healthier lifestyles, introduce the New Year's Resolution Test: ‘[H]ow many people vow to smoke more cigarettes, drink more martinis, or have more chocolate donuts in the morning next year?’ For Thaler and Sunstein, this question is merely rhetorical, but the premise of our article is that the actual content of people's self-acknowledged goals, and the normative significance that people actually attach to those goals, are research topics that require real answers.

It is uncontroversial that many people profess long-term goals from which they sometimes deviate when facing more immediate desires. However, recognition of such inconsistencies leaves open the question of how they should be interpreted from a normative point of view, or in the formation of public policy. Advocates of interventions to counter self-control failures often appeal to dual-self interpretations. For example, Thaler and Sunstein (Reference Thaler and Sunstein2008, pp. 41–2) represent self-control problems in terms of a person's two selves, a far-sighted ‘Planner’ and a myopic ‘Doer’. Describing the roles of these two selves, Thaler and Sunstein say: ‘The Planner is trying to promote your long-term welfare but must cope with the feelings, mischief, and strong will of the Doer, who is exposed to the temptations that come with arousal.’ There is an implicit assumption that the Planner's judgements about goals retain their normative authority – they represent what the person ‘truly’ values – even when the actual human being is consciously choosing to ignore them. On this view, public policy should help the Planner to maintain control when the Doer is trying to rebel.Footnote 3 But if policymakers want to respect individuals’ own judgements, they need to ask how far, and how consistently, real human beings identify with their Planners rather than with their Doers. Our aim in this article is to place these questions on the agenda of behavioural public policy and to take some preliminary steps towards answering them.

We start from the idea that self-control – constraining yourself to act on long-term goals – can be contrasted with spontaneity – leaving yourself scope to respond to situations as they arise, rather than following pre-scripted plans. As an informal illustration of this contrast, consider a different New Year's Resolution Test, in which individuals are asked to rate the importance they attach to keeping their New Year's resolutions. A high rating might be interpreted as a personal judgement in favour of self-control relative to spontaneity; a low rating might be interpreted as the opposite. Normatively, neither of these attitudes seems self-evidently superior to the other; one might associate self-control with the ‘Big Five’ trait of conscientiousness, and spontaneity with openness to experience. Thus, acting contrary to a professed goal should not be treated as sufficient evidence of a failed attempt to exercise self-control or of a desire to be helped: as viewed by the actor, it might be a conscious expression of spontaneity.

In this article, we report an online survey which investigated the relative importance that individuals attach to self-control and spontaneity in relation to conflicts between immediate enjoyment and long-term health goals. We focus on three issues that need to be faced if public policies are to be justified as non-paternalistic responses to self-control failures.

First, do people generally judge self-control to be more important than spontaneity? That this is the case is an implicit assumption of many behavioural policy recommendations, but might turn out to be false.

Second, how homogeneous are people's judgements about these attitudes? Even if the judgements of a majority of people favoured self-control, policymakers might want to take some account of contrary judgements held by significant minorities.

Third, are those judgements context-independent? Much of the evidence that is cited in support of the hypothesis of self-control failure is about the context-dependence of revealed preferences – the fact that preferences between given consumption possibilities differ systematically according to the presence or absence of cues that draw attention to particular temptations. It is also known that when people report their overall satisfaction with their lives, the implicit weights they give to different aspects of life can depend on what is currently the focus of their attention (Schkade & Kahneman, Reference Schkade and Kahneman1998) – an effect summed up in Kahneman's (Reference Kahneman2011, p. 402) ‘fortune cookie’ maxim: ‘Nothing in life is as important as you think it is when you are thinking about it.’ If preferences and life satisfaction judgements are often context-dependent, the same might be true of people's attitudes to self-control and spontaneity.

Since our respondents were not recruited as a representative sample, our survey must be treated as an exploratory study. Our main findings were as follows. Overall, judgements that favoured spontaneity were expressed with roughly the same frequency and strength as judgments that favoured self-control. When asked to express judgements about what was important in life, most participants maintained both that it was important to make long-term plans and stick to them and that there was no harm in occasionally taking small enjoyments rather than sticking to those plans. We found strong evidence of individual heterogeneity. Because of this, we defined an ordinal scale for measuring individuals’ judgements about the relative importance of self-control and spontaneity in relation to everyday conflicts between enjoyment and health-related goals. Using this scale, we found little evidence of context-dependence.

We conclude that identifying when and where individuals want to be helped to avoid self-control failures is more difficult – both empirically and conceptually – than many behavioural economists seem to think. We believe that our findings point to the importance of treating desires for spontaneity as equally deserving of attention as desires for self-control, and as suggesting interesting lines of further research.

As a preliminary to describing our study, we briefly discuss some of the ways in which the concept of self-control failure is used in behavioural economics, and review some existing evidence about people's everyday experience of self-control problems.

Self-control failure in behavioural economics

The idea that deviations from neoclassical rationality can be explained in terms of self-control failure is common in behavioural economics. Such explanations are not confined to modes of behaviour that might reasonably be viewed as addictions, such as problem drinking, compulsive gambling, or reckless borrowing. For example, the possibility of self-control failure has been invoked in explanations of inconsistencies in time preference (O'Donoghue & Rabin, Reference O'Donoghue and Rabin2003), of why people buy gym memberships when it would be cheaper to pay for each visit separately (Della Vigna & Malmendier, Reference DellaVigna and Malmendier2006),Footnote 4 of why shoppers use inconvenient entrances to stores rather than walk past charity collectors (Andreoni et al., Reference Andreoni, Rao and Trachtman2017), and of why experimental subjects show extreme risk aversion when choosing between low-prize lotteries (Fudenberg & Levine, Reference Fudenberg and Levine2006).

In two founding manifestos of behavioural welfare economics, Camerer et al. (Reference Camerer, Issacharoff, Loewenstein, O'Donoghue and Rabin2003, pp. 1217 and 1238–42) and Sunstein and Thaler (Reference Sunstein and Thaler2003, pp. 1168 and 1184) cite inconsistencies in time preference as examples of self-control failures that public policy should aim to counter. The emphasis on self-control failure is particularly marked in Sunstein and Thaler's paper, which uses the composite phrase ‘bounded rationality and bounded self-control’ as a generic term for the class of errors or biases that individuals should be helped to avoid. Or, as Sunstein & Thaler (Reference Sunstein and Thaler2003, p. 1162) put it in a programmatic passage, their concern is with cases in which ‘individuals make inferior decisions in terms of their own welfare – decisions that they would change if they had complete information, unlimited cognitive abilities, and no lack of self-control’. When they present their now-famous cafeteria story, they say that their proposal that the fruit should be placed before the desserts on the cafeteria counter is designed ‘to help people to solve their self-control problems’ (Sunstein & Thaler, Reference Sunstein and Thaler2003, p. 1184).

In their later book Nudge, Thaler and Sunstein repeatedly emphasise that their recommendations are designed to ‘make choosers better off, as judged by themselves’ (Reference Thaler and Sunstein2008, p. 5, italics in original; see also pp. 10, 12, and 80). The implication is that Sunstein and Thaler's concept of self-control failure involves acting contrary to one's own judgement. But when they set out the justification for nudging people towards healthy eating, their main arguments are not based on evidence about people's actual judgements:

Consider the issue of obesity. Rates of obesity in the United States are now approaching 20 per cent, and more than 60 per cent of Americans are considered either obese or overweight. There is overwhelming evidence that obesity increases risks of heart disease and diabetes, frequently leading to premature death. […] We do not claim that everyone who is overweight is necessarily failing to act rationally, but we do reject the claim that all or almost all Americans are choosing their diet optimally. […] With respect to diet, smoking, and drinking, people's current choices cannot reasonably be claimed to be the best means of promoting their well-being. Indeed, many smokers, drinkers and overeaters are willing to pay third parties to help them make better decisions. (p. 7)

Notice that, except in the final sentence (which surely applies only to a small minority of the relevant population), the evidence that Thaler and Sunstein present is about the harmful effects of obesity, not about the mental processes that underlie the choices that cumulatively cause it. As Sugden (Reference Sugden2017) points out, self-control failure is only one of a wide range of psychological mechanisms that are potential explanations of an individual's tendency to eat unhealthy but enjoyable food. Many of these mechanisms – for example, procrastination, social proof, availability, cognitive dissonance, intrapersonal empathy gaps, and self-deception – do not depend on the assumption that, in the individual's own judgement, there is a conflict between his immediate desires and his long-term well-being. Because what is at issue here is not whether people's choices are rational or optimal or the best means of promoting their well-being, but whether they are the result of self-control failure, interventions that are designed to make people better-off as judged by themselves must be based on information about people's actual judgements.

Many behavioural-economic discussions of self-control problems use the distinction between System 1 and System 2 mental operations, as proposed by Wason and Evans (Reference Wason and Evans1975) and developed by Kahneman (Reference Kahneman2003). System 1 operations are fast and automatic; System 2 operations are slow and under conscious control. This distinction is used by Thaler and Sunstein (Reference Thaler and Sunstein2008, pp. 19–39) and Kahneman (Reference Kahneman2011) as a way of organising behavioural findings. In both cases, the suggestion is that System 1 is liable to induce preferences and judgements that are systematically biased and that System 2 is capable of correcting. As Kahneman and Sunstein (Reference Kahneman, Sunstein, Changeux, Damasio, Singer and Christen2006, p. 92), endorsing Kahneman and Frederick (Reference Kahneman, Frederick, Gilovich, Griffin and Kahneman2002, p. 51), put it: ‘System 1 quickly proposes intuitive answers to judgment problems as they arise, and System 2 monitors the quality of these proposals, which it may endorse, correct, or override.’ Self-control problems can be fitted into this framework by treating a person's immediate desires as unmediated products of System 1, and her judgements about her goals as products of System 2 operations (to which System 1 may have provided inputs). However, we see no good reason to assume that System 2 is always able to produce context-independent judgements. (Recall the possibly analogous finding that judgements about life satisfaction can be context-dependent.) Nor is it necessarily the case that, as Kahneman (Reference Kahneman2011, p. 21) suggests, ‘[w]hen we think of ourselves, we identify with System 2, the conscious, reasoning self that has beliefs, makes choices, and decides what to think about and what to do.’ A person who values spontaneity might sometimes identify with her System 1 feelings and override goal-directed thoughts issuing from her System 2.

Evidence of everyday self-control

Hofmann et al. (Reference Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster and Vohs2012a, Reference Hofmann, Vohs and Baumeister2012b) report a seminal study that uses experience sampling to elicit individuals’ experiences of desires over the course of a week of ordinary life. Each participant was asked to carry a smartphone for seven consecutive days. Each day, the participant received seven calls, at randomised intervals, and reported desires that she was currently feeling or had felt in the last half hour. For each of these desires, she reported: (1) its content, (2) its strength, (3) whether she had attempted to resist it, (4) whether she had enacted it, (5) whether it conflicted with a personal goal, and if so, (6) the goal with which it conflicted. This study can be interpreted as sampling individuals’ everyday experiences of self-control problems, and the authors themselves (Reference Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster and Vohs2012a, p. 1319) interpret it in relation to a four-stage psychological model of ‘intentional’ and ‘effortful’ self-control. In the first stage, the individual experiences some desire. In the second stage, she assesses whether the desire conflicts with a goal. If she recognises such a conflict, the desire is problematic. If so, there is a third stage in which she decides whether to resist the desire. If she decides to resist, she attempts self-control. If so, there is a fourth stage in which the attempt may succeed (the desire is not enacted) or fail (it is enacted).

Delaney and Lades (Reference Delaney and Lades2017) report a study that is closely modelled on that of Hofmann et al., but uses the day reconstruction method (Kahneman et al., Reference Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz and Stone2004) in place of experience sampling. Each participant was first asked to complete a time-use diary of the previous day, breaking the day down into a set of discrete episodes, and then responded to a series of questions about each episode. Participants reported up to three desires that they had felt during the episode. For each desire, they answered questions similar to those used by Hoffman et al. to elicit properties (1)–(6).

The two studies produced broadly similar results. Hofmann et al.'s 205 participants reported a total of 7827 desire episodes, of which 47% were problematic. Of all desires (problematic or not), 42% were actively resisted. Of those that were resisted, 17% were enacted (Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster and Vohs2012a, p. 1325).Footnote 5 Delaney and Lades's 142 participants reported a total of 2059 desires. 69.1% of these desires were problematic. Participants attempted to resist 56.1% of problematic desires. In 35.1% of cases in which such an attempt was made, the attempt failed (p. 1161). Expressing Delaney and Lades's data in more stylised form: on an average sampled day, an average participant faced 10 self-control problems, attempted self-control five or six times, and experienced self-control failure twice. Hofmann et al. (Reference Hofmann, Vohs and Baumeister2012b, pp. 584 and 585) report that the categories of goals that most commonly conflicted with desires were ‘health-related’ (directed at health or fitness) and ‘time use’ (directed at using time efficiently and getting things done); self-control failure rates were highest for desires for ‘engaging in media use’ and for ‘work’ (not, interestingly, for avoiding work). In Delaney and Lades's study, the most common self-control failures were in relation to ‘postponing a task’ and ‘using social media’ (p. 1161).

The most frequently reported self-control problems might seem rather mundane [Hofmann et al. (Reference Hofmann, Vohs and Baumeister2012b, p. 585) express mild surprise at not finding more evidence of ‘disastrous failures to control sexual impulses and urges to spend money’], but this is likely to be the effect of the everyday sampling frame. It seems clear that self-control problems, successes, and failures are all common features of everyday life. To this extent, the evidence is consistent with a System 1/System 2 model in which automatic processes produce desires which cognitive processes sometimes but not always override.

However, the observation of self-control attempts does not imply that individuals’ judgements about their goals, or about the importance of being goal-directed, are stable or context-independent. Indeed, Hofmann et al. (Reference Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster and Vohs2012a, pp. 1328–9) report some evidence to the contrary. Some of their participants responded to questions about the presence or absence of other people when the desires were experienced. The presence of other people had no significant effect on the strength of desires or on perceptions of conflict between desires and goals, but individuals were more likely to attempt to resist highly conflicted desires if other people were around. There was also a tendency for the presence of other people to inhibit the enactment of desires in general, whether problematic or not.

In relation to our research questions, it is particularly interesting that many problematic desires (44% in Delaney and Lades's study, possibly around 30% in Hofmann et al.'s study – see note 5) were not resisted. These were cases in which a participant recognised that a current desire conflicted with a personal goal, but chose not to attempt self-control. In other words, acting contrary to a self-acknowledged goal is not equivalent to a failure of self-control.

Our survey design

Our survey was designed to investigate individuals’ judgements about the personal importance of being disposed to resist desires that conflict with goals. We focused on conflicts between desires for immediately enjoyable activities and goals for good health – a domain in which, as evidenced by Hofmann et al.'s findings, conflicts between desires and goals are particularly common.

It is important to recognise that our objective was not to elicit individuals’ current preferences for or against imposing constraints on their future choices. We take as given that, as implied by Planner/Doer models and as shown in the everyday self-control studies reviewed in the preceding section, people's preferences with respect to self-control are often temporally inconsistent. Acting on the preferences of her Planner, a person may choose to impose constraints on her possible future actions, but when the future arrives, she may act on the preferences of her Doer in trying to evade those constraints. Our intention was not to add to existing knowledge about the content of these two types of preferences: it was to investigate how people adjudicate between them – how far they identify with their Planners, and how far with their Doers. Participants were compensated for the time spent in answering the survey questions but, since the purpose of those questions was to elicit individuals’ personal judgments about their own lives, there was no way of rewarding ‘correct’ or ‘successful’ responses.

To allow an investigation of context-dependence (or independence), survey respondents were divided randomly between two treatments. All respondents answered the same questions about attitudes to self-control and spontaneity. In one treatment, these questions were preceded by a task that was intended to direct attention to the enjoyable properties of activities that might conflict with health-related goals. In the other, they were preceded by a task that was intended to direct attention to the sense of satisfaction derived from achieving health-related goals through activities that might not be immediately enjoyable.

In pre-registering the study, we did not propose any hypothesis about the direction in which individuals’ judgements about the importance of self-control might be differentially affected by cues that directed their attention either to enjoyment or to goals. The most obvious possibility is that cues that direct attention to enjoyment induce judgements that give a low weight to self-control. But we recognised an alternative possibility: thoughts about enjoyment might act as reminders to exert self-control, and so induce judgements favouring self-control. Either effect, if observed, would cast doubt on the idea that individuals have stable attitudes to self-control problems. Because each participant's attention has to be directed in only one direction, we must use between-subject tests. A corollary of this is that there is little scope for investigating whether some individuals are susceptible to one effect and some to the other.

The survey had three parts – an attention-focusing task, a judgement elicitation task, and a short questionnaire. We used a pre-registered pilot study to determine the form of the attention-focusing task.Footnote 6

The design of the pilot was very similar to that of the main study, except that there were two pairs of attention-focusing treatments rather than the single pair used in the main survey. In the pilot, 241 participants were randomly distributed between the resulting four treatments; 229 completed the survey. One pair of treatments used memory recall tasks. In the memory/enjoyment treatment, participants recalled memories relating to enjoyments that might be thought to conflict with health-related goals. In the memory/goals treatment, they recalled memories of being satisfied about efforts to achieve health-related goals. We will describe these treatments in more detail later. The other pair of treatments used pictures to activate thoughts about enjoyment or long-term goals. In the pictures/enjoyment treatment, participants were shown 14 pictures, comprising 6 enjoyable foods, 6 enjoyable activities, 1 healthy food, and 1 healthy activity, and for each picture answered ‘How much would you enjoy this?’ on a 7-point scale. In the pictures/goals treatment, participants were shown 14 pictures, comprising 6 healthy foods, 6 healthy activities, 1 enjoyable food, and 1 enjoyable activity, and for each picture answered ‘How good do you think this would be for your health?’ on a 7-point scale. We pre-committed to using whichever type of attention-focusing task induced more significant differences (irrespective of direction) in responses to the judgement elicitation task. The judgement elicitation task comprised 32 questions divided by topic into four blocks – ‘self-control wishes’, ‘spontaneity wishes’, ‘regrets’, and ‘importance in life’– each of eight questions. When pictures were used, there was no significant treatment effect for any of the four blocks. When memory recall tasks were used, there were significant effects (higher scores on a spontaneity/self-control scale in the memory/goals treatment) for two blocks (regrets and importance in life), and no significant effects for the other two blocks. Following our pre-registered plan, we used the memory recall tasks in the main survey, which we now describe.

The survey was programmed in oTree (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Schonger and Wickens2016) and conducted online using the Prolific platform. We recruited 240 participants (90 males and 150 females), all of whom were UK residents with English as their first language. The median age was 31. Each participant received £2 for participating. The median duration of the survey was less than 10 minutes. To ensure good quality responses to tasks that were necessarily non-incentivised, the survey was designed to be short, easy to understand, and engaging. The content of the survey, the hypotheses, and the analysis plan were pre-registered.Footnote 7 The three parts of the survey are described next, in the order in which they were faced.

The attention-focusing task

For this task, each participant was randomly assigned to one of two treatments, designed to activate thoughts either about immediate enjoyment or about long-term goals. The randomisation ensured that there were 120 participants in each treatment. In the enjoyment treatment, participants were asked to think about ‘a memorable meal you have had (e.g. at a restaurant, at a friend's house) when you particularly enjoyed the food’. They were then asked to type answers to four questions about the meal: where it took place, who was there, which dish the respondent enjoyed most, and (with the added request to be ‘as detailed as you can’) why they enjoyed it. Finally, they reported how much they had enjoyed the dish, on a 7-point Likert scale. In the goals treatment, participants were asked to think about ‘some effort that you have made (e.g. in relation to exercise or diet) that was good for your health and that you felt satisfied about having done’. They were then asked to type answers to four questions about the effort: what they did, when they did it, why it was good for their health, and (with the added request to be ‘as detailed as you can’) why they felt satisfied about it. Finally, they reported how satisfied they had been, on a 7-point Likert scale.

Although this task might be regarded as a ‘priming’ manipulation, it is significantly different from the types of tasks typically used in psychology to test hypotheses about priming. For example, in a widely cited experiment which built on the early work of Higgins et al. (Reference Higgins, Rholes and Jones1977) and Srull and Wyer (Reference Srull and Wyer1979), Bargh et al. (Reference Bargh, Chen and Burrows1996) used tasks in which subjects were asked to unscramble sentences. The content of these sentences varied; some were related to rudeness, some were related to politeness, and some were neutral. The key finding was that, in a second stage of the experiment that was not overtly linked to the unscrambling task, subjects who had unscrambled sentences about rudeness were more likely than the others to interrupt a conversation. Many subsequent experiments have used scrambled sentences or word search tasks as manipulations.Footnote 8 These experiments test the hypothesis that, as a result of psychological processes below the level of consciousness, incidentally presented words or sentences activate mental constructs that influence subsequent judgements or behaviour. In contrast, our design investigates the effects of conscious processes of memory retrieval, reasoning, and judgement. In our judgement elicitation task, participants need to think about the relative importance of self-control and spontaneity to them. In striking such a balance, it really is relevant to recall previous experiences of conflict between desires and goals, previous feelings of enjoyment from acting contrary to one's goals, and previous feelings of satisfaction from exercising self-control. Our memory recall tasks were intended to direct a participant's attention towards one or other of two sets of relevant considerations, aligned with either self-control or spontaneity. In this respect, they are similar to the manipulations used by Schkade and Kahneman (Reference Schkade and Kahneman1998) to investigate ‘focusing illusions’. Importantly, they activate thoughts about temptations, but do not present actual temptations. To put this another way, our objective was to elicit cool judgements about situations which, if actually experienced, would be liable to activate hot emotions.

The judgement elicitation task

In this part of the study, every participant answered the same 32 questions. In each case, the respondent was shown a statement and asked ‘How well do you recognise yourself in this statement?’ Responses were recorded on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely well’. For the purposes of the design, the questions were pre-classified into four blocks, each of eight questions. However, this structure was not revealed to participants, who simply faced the 32 questions in a single sequence, randomised independently for each participant. The statements are listed below, grouped into four blocks, and numbered for ease of reference. Of these statements, 16 express attitudes favouring self-control; the other 16 (marked with asterisks) express attitudes favouring spontaneity.

  • Self-control wishes

    • Q1. I wish I ate more fruit and vegetables.

    • Q2. I ought to drink more mineral or tap water.

    • Q3. I wish I drank less sugary drinks.

    • Q4. I ought to eat less high-fat food.

    • Q5. I wish I took more exercise.

    • Q6. I ought to use the lift less often.

    • Q7. I ought to limit my screen time.

    • Q8. I wish I spent less of my leisure time just sitting around.

  • Spontaneity wishes

    • Q9*. I wish I paid less attention to calorie labels.

    • Q10*. I wish I was more relaxed about eating or drinking what I like.

    • Q11*. I envy people who always eat what they like.

    • Q12*. I wish there was less social pressure to take exercise.

    • Q13*. I wish I felt less bad about lazing around.

    • Q14*. I envy people who don't feel guilty about taking it easy.

    • Q15*. I wish I cared less about being in good shape.

    • Q16*. I envy people who don't worry about their weight.

  • Regrets

    • Q17. If I drink too much alcohol, I feel guilty later, even if I enjoyed it a lot at the time.

    • Q18. After ordering desserts in restaurants, I often feel regret.

    • Q19. After spending hours on my phone or watching TV, I often feel regret.

    • Q20. I feel guilty if I take the lift when I could have walked up the stairs.

    • Q21*. After ordering a healthy dish, I often wish I'd chosen something tastier.

    • Q22*. If I choose to walk because it's healthy, I often regret it along the way.

    • Q23*. I never feel regret after a nice meal, even if I ate too much.

    • Q24*. It doesn't bother me if sometimes I skip my usual exercise.

  • Importance in life

    • Q25. Once I set up an exercise routine, I do all I can to stick to it.

    • Q26. Eating healthily is very important to me, even if it means avoiding some of my favourite foods.

    • Q27. It's important to make long-term plans and stick to them.

    • Q28. In life it's important to be able to resist temptation.

    • Q29.* There's no harm in occasionally taking a break from an exercise routine.

    • Q30.* Having occasional treats is an important source of happiness for me, even if they are bad for my health.

    • Q31.* I like my plans to be flexible and leave plenty of room for spontaneity.

    • Q32.* Tomorrow will look after itself – each day has troubles enough of its own.

The self-control wishes can be paraphrased as ‘I wish I exercised (or I ought to exercise) more self-control.’ They compare the individual's actual degree of self-control with a counterfactual state in which that degree is higher. They are examples of the kind of statement that Thaler and Sunstein (Reference Thaler and Sunstein2008, p. 107) interpret as indicating that the individuals who make them ‘are open to a nudge’ and ‘might even be grateful for one’.

The spontaneity wishes have the opposite content: ‘I wish I exercised less self-control.’ Agreeing with these statements carries the suggestion that being nudged towards self-control might be unwelcome.

The regret statements Q17–Q20 can be paraphrased as ‘After taking enjoyment rather than attempting self-control, I feel regret.’ Such statements are often interpreted as ex-post reports of self-control failure. In a Planner–Doer model, they might be interpreted as expressing a person's identification with their inner Planner. Q21–Q24 have one of two opposite contents: either ‘After exercising self-control rather than taking enjoyment, I feel regret’ (Q21 and Q22) or ‘After taking enjoyment rather than exercising self-control, I do not feel regret’ (Q23 and Q24). They might be interpreted as expressing identification with an inner Doer (either by regretting an earlier decision in favour of self-control or by approving an earlier decision not to exercise self-control). Notice that the regret statements differ from the two types of wish statements in not using the respondent's current lifestyle as a reference point. Thus, a person who believed that her current degree of self-control was about right might disagree with the wish statements while recognising the regret statements as descriptions of her feelings on occasions when she exercised too little or too much self-control.

The importance in life statements are more reflective or philosophical than the others. They express judgements about the overall importance of self-control and spontaneity in the respondent's life. Q25–Q28 have the connotation ‘In life, it's important to exercise self-control’; statements Q29–Q32 have the contrasting connotation ‘In life, it's important to be spontaneous.’ One might say that the first set of statements expresses a Planner's view of life, while the second set expresses a Doer's.

The questionnaire

In the final questionnaire, participants reported their age and gender and whether they followed any specific dietary regime. Participants also answered two questions in which they were asked to rate the healthiness of their own habitual diet and exercise: ‘Relative to an average person of your age, would you say that your diet was: much less healthy … much healthier?’ and ‘Relative to an average person of your age, would you say that you take: much less exercise … much more exercise?’ Each of these questions elicited responses on a 7-point Likert scale. Our intention was to use participants’ responses to these questions as control variables in our tests for differences between the enjoyment and goals treatments.

Responses to the memory recall tasks

We reviewed the responses to the memory recall task to check that our manipulation had worked as intended – that participants had taken the task seriously and had recalled specific experiences of enjoyable dishes or of satisfaction from healthy activities. In considering each treatment, we gave particular attention to the question to which participants had been asked to be as detailed as they could.

In both treatments, all participants followed the instructions by describing some specific meal or activity and gave relevant responses to all the memory recall questions. The median length of answers to the ‘details’ question was 18 words in the enjoyment treatment and 19.5 words in the goals treatment (the respective means were 21.4 and 23.3). Some respondents wrote whole paragraphs, but even the short answers gave cogent reasons for feeling enjoyment (e.g., ‘I love lamb cooked over long time’, ‘Love puddings, sweet and tasty’) or satisfaction (‘Lost weight and felt happier’, ‘Felt energised’). Participants’ ratings of remembered enjoyment or satisfaction on the 1–7 Likert scale were very high (the mean ratings were 6.69 for enjoyment and 6.23 for satisfaction).

In answering the ‘details’ question in the enjoyment treatment, most participants wrote (often quite lyrically) about the enjoyable features of the dish. For example, on a meal eaten at home: ‘I enjoyed this meal because the salmon was very moist and succulent. We also had potato and leek gratin and broccoli.’ On smoky BBQ ribs eaten at a restaurant: ‘It has a lot of flavour to it and the meat just falls off the bone. They are big ribs too.’ In the goals treatment, almost all respondents described efforts concerning either diet or exercise. In descriptions of how the effort had been satisfying, two themes were particularly prevalent. One was pleasure in feeling or looking healthier. For example, on a workout every morning: ‘It boosted my mood and made me less tired throughout the day. It also made me feel fitter.’ On going vegan: ‘I got thinner and felt healthier. It was great looking in the mirror and seeing myself as well as feeling less tired.’ The other theme was self-empowerment. For example, on losing weight: ‘Because I felt like I had achieved something by using my willpower.’ On exercise: ‘Because it made me feel better about my self.’

Overall, these responses are coherent and psychologically credible, and clearly unaffected by the lack of direct incentives. It seems clear that the attention-focusing task was engaging and activated real memories of personally significant episodes of enjoyment and self-control.

Tests for context-dependence

Following a pre-registered plan for the analysis of the data, we coded each participant's response to each of the 32 questions from 1 to 7 on a spontaneity/self-control scale, reverse coding the statements favouring spontaneity. So, on this scale, 1 corresponds with ‘not at all’ recognising oneself in a statement favouring self-control or recognising oneself ‘extremely well’ in a statement favouring spontaneity; 7 corresponds with the opposite extremes. The central point of the scale is 4. Our prior expectation was that, for each participant considered separately, there would be a positive correlation between responses (coded as we have described) to any pair of questions, legitimating the use of an individual's mean score for the whole set of 32 questions as an index of their overall judgements about spontaneity and self-control (interpreted as opposites of one another on a single psychological scale). However, we recognised that each of the four blocks of questions tapped into a different aspect of spontaneity/self-control judgements, and hence that individuals’ mean scores for each of the four separate blocks might have additional information content. Our pre-registered tests for context-dependence in spontaneity/self-control judgements use non-parametric and regression methods to compare overall and block-specific scores between the two treatments.

Mean scores, averaging over the 120 participants in each treatment, are shown in Table 1. The final column of the table reports the p-value of a two-tailed Mann–Whitney test for differences between the distributions of responses between the two treatments. Overall, there is no significant difference between the treatments. Only one of the four blocks (regrets) shows a significant difference, with higher scores in the goals treatment; even here, the effect size is small and there is significance only at the 10% level.

Table 1. Tests for context-dependence.

Note: Mean scores of spontaneity/self-control ratings in the relevant questions for the relevant treatments (standard deviations are in parentheses).

***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

Table 2 reports OLS regression results.Footnote 9 In each regression equation, the dependent variable is a participant's mean score on the spontaneity/self-control scale, either for all 32 questions, or for one of the blocks of eight questions. The treatment variable is a dummy that takes the value of 1 in the goals treatment. There are controls for age (in years),Footnote 10 gender (with female as the baseline), and for responses to the diet and exercise questions in the final part of the survey, with the least (most) healthy diet and least (most) exercise coded as 1 (7).

Table 2. Regression results.

Note: Standard errors are in parentheses.

***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

Consistently with the Mann–Whitney tests, there are no significant treatment effects. Diet and exercise have significantly negative effects on scores for self-control wishes and generally positive effects on scores for the other blocks. We conjecture that this pattern is the result of two mechanisms that work in opposite directions. Positive attitudes to self-control are likely to be associated with following healthy regimes of diet and exercise. (Causation could go in either direction. An inclination to exercise self-control might be a cause of healthy lifestyles. Alternatively, people with healthy lifestyles might attribute those lifestyles to their powers of self-control, and this might induce self-serving approval of self-control.) However, people with healthy lifestyles may believe they already exert high levels of self-control and might approve of this without wishing to exert more self-control than they do. If such people do not identify with Q1–Q8, they may give relatively low-scoring responses.Footnote 11 We found no significant interaction between treatment and either of these lifestyle variables.

Two of the regression equations show a significant gender effect: men express more favourable attitudes to self-control than do women. Following up on this finding, we investigated whether our attention manipulation had different effects for male and female participants. Table 3 breaks down the data in Table 1 by gender. For male participants, spontaneity/self-control scores were significantly higher in the goals treatment than in the enjoyment treatment for the 32 questions overall, for the importance in life block, and (at the 10% significance level) for the regrets block. For female participants, there was no significant treatment effect overall; for the importance in life block, the goals treatment induced significantly lower scores. We do not want to make too much of this evidence as the survey was not planned with any intention of investigating gender differences.

Table 3. Gender breakdown of context-dependence effects.

Note: Standard deviations are in parentheses.

***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1

As we have explained, one of the objectives of our study was to investigate whether judgements about the relative importance of spontaneity and self-control are differentially influenced by cues that direct attention to immediate enjoyment or to long-term goals. We found no firm evidence that attention-directing cues have systematic effects on such judgements. Given that this was a study with pre-registered hypothesis tests, it is appropriate to interpret its results in Popperian terms.Footnote 12 The null hypothesis was that judgements about spontaneity and self-control are context-independent. Our design tested that hypothesis in a situation in which there was prior reason to expect it to be falsified, but in fact it was not. This result does not establish the null hypothesis as true, but (according to Popperian methodology) one can be more confident in a hypothesis, the more successfully it withstands attempts to falsify it.

As explained in the ‘The attention-focusing task’ section, our memory recall tasks were not priming manipulations expected to work through subconscious channels. They were intended to activate, and did indeed activate, conscious processes of relevant memory retrieval, reasoning, and judgement. Thus, our results should not be interpreted merely as corroboration of recent scepticism about the replicability of priming experiments (e.g., Kahneman, Reference Kahneman2012). Our experimental design was based on the premise that, if individuals’ judgements about the relative importance of spontaneity and self-control were characterised by a considerable degree of context-dependence, our memory recall tasks would exert measurable influences.Footnote 13 It should be remembered that our aim was to investigate the effects of attention-focusing cues on individuals’ cool judgements. Context-independence of such judgements may coexist with significant shifts of preference between hot and cold emotional states. Of course, there are severe constraints on the kinds of manipulations that can be used in online or laboratory experiments. As suggested by an anonymous referee, it could be that the phrasing of the judgement elicitation questions encouraged participants to think of their attitudes to self-control and spontaneity as fixed traits.

The relative importance of spontaneity and self-control

As explained in the ‘Tests for context-dependence’ section, we designed the judgement elicitation questions in the hope that spontaneity/self-control scores for the 32 individual questions could be combined into a reliable scale for measuring individuals’ judgements about the importance of exercising self-control. Failing that, we hoped to define such scales for the separate categories corresponding with our four blocks of questions, or for combinations of those. In this section, we discuss how far we succeeded and what can be learned by organising individuals’ responses in this way. Because we did not find differences between the two treatments, for the purposes of this analysis we will pool all the data.

The relevant data are summarised in Table 4. The ‘mean score’ column of this table shows the mean and standard deviation of responses to each of the 32 questions. The next column reports the item-rest correlations generated by treating all 32 responses as items on a single scale. It is immediately clear that each of the items in the self-control wishes, spontaneity wishes, and (with the exception of Q24) regrets blocks is strongly and positively correlated with the other items in the overall scale. In contrast, the importance in life items have generally low (and sometimes negative) item-rest correlations. This pattern suggests that our aim of expressing all elicited judgements on a single scale was too ambitious.

Table 4. Responses to individual judgement questions.

Notes: Standard deviations are in parentheses. Questions favouring spontaneity (marked with asterisks) are reverse-coded.

The ‘Q1–Q24’ column of Table 4 uses the responses from the first 24 questions to create a single scale. Notice that this scale preserves equality between the number of items that favour spontaneity and the number that favour self-control.Footnote 14 The consistently high and positive item-rest correlations and the Cronbach's α value of 0.837 give us confidence that this is a reliable scale for measuring attitudes that people express in relation to concrete activities, events, and feelings that recur in everyday life. We will refer to this as a scale of everyday spontaneity/self-control.

In contrast, we suggest that Q25–Q32 are picking up more ‘philosophical’ judgements about spontaneity or self-control, considered in general. The final column of Table 4 uses the responses to Q25–Q32 to create a spontaneity/self-control scale that is related to such judgements. We will call this the importance in life scale. The item-rest correlations are all positive, but generally lower than those for the everyday scale, and the Cronbach's α value of 0.527 is rather low. Our tentative conclusion is that Q25–Q32 may not be picking up a single attitude. There is no significant correlation between individual scores on the ‘everyday’ and ‘importance in life’ scales (Spearman's ρ = 0.074, p = 0.255).

Because our everyday spontaneity/self-control scale combines responses to equal numbers of items favouring spontaneity and items favouring self-control, it is a reasonable starting point for analysis to interpret the mid-point of the scale as expressing an equal degree of approval of spontaneity and self-control. For example, consider a participant who recognises herself equally well in the statements ‘After ordering desserts in restaurants, I often feel regret’ (Q18) and ‘After ordering a healthy dish, I often wish I'd chosen something tastier’ (Q21). The first statement expresses regret about having chosen enjoyment when the alternative was self-control; the second expresses regret about having exercised self-control when the alternative was enjoyment. It is natural to interpret these statements as equal and opposite. Of course, many of the statements we use cannot be paired in such an obvious way, and even Q18 and Q21 are not exact opposites. This is an unavoidable property of a survey instrument that presents respondents with informal and engaging statements about everyday life. We can only say that we did our best to ensure the balance between statements in favour of spontaneity and statements in favour of self-control.

Figure 1 shows the distributions of the scores for each of the four blocks and for the everyday spontaneity/self-control scale obtained by aggregating across the first three blocks. For each participant, we averaged their responses to the questions belonging to the relevant block. The histograms report the frequencies of the corresponding mean scores. The left-hand side of each diagram (score < 4) indicates that, on average, the participants lean towards spontaneity; the right-hand side (score > 4) indicates that they lean towards self-control.

Figure 1. Distributions of Spontaneity/Self-Control Scores.

Averaging over Q1–Q24, the mean response was 4.17, not far above the mid-point of the scale. Average responses for self-control wishes (4.29) and spontaneity wishes (4.35) were close to one another and slightly above the mid-point; average responses for regrets (3.88) were slightly below it. Some statements consistently elicited responses at the high end of the scale – for example, support for ‘I wish I took more exercise’ (Q5, average score of 5.16), or lack of support for ‘If I choose to walk because it's healthy, I often regret it along the way’ (Q22, average reverse-coded score of 5.67). Other statements consistently elicited responses at the opposite end of the scale – for example, lack of support for ‘I wish I drank less sugary drinks’ (Q3, average score of 3.03) and ‘After ordering desserts in restaurants, I often feel regret’ (Q18, average score of 2.71). Overall, the evidence from the ‘everyday’ questions suggests that our participants were about as likely to identify with statements favouring spontaneity as with statements favouring self-control.

What can we learn from the importance in life questions? The mean response to Q25–Q32 was 3.77, slightly below the mid-point of the scale. Four of these questions are particularly interesting because they elicit responses towards the two ends of the scale. There was strong support for ‘It's important to make long-term plans and stick to them’ (Q27, average score of 4.90) and ‘In life it's important to be able to resist temptation’ (Q28, average score of 4.79). These are direct statements of the value of self-control as a general principle or life strategy. But there was even stronger support for ‘There's no harm in occasionally taking a break from an exercise routine’ (Q29, average reverse-coded score of 2.61) and ‘Having occasional treats is an important source of happiness for me, even if they are bad for my health’ (Q30, average reverse-coded score of 2.13). It seems that a typical person's philosophical attitude to self-control can be expressed in proverbial (or fortune cookie) form as: There's a time for keeping resolutions, and a time for breaking them.

Figure 1 also reveals a considerable degree of heterogeneity in people's judgements. In all four blocks, we see that there are people with a strong preference for self-control, as well as people with a strong preference for spontaneity. Obviously, because each subscale is obtained by averaging (at least) eight responses, intermediate values are more common than extreme ones. This is also why the ‘everyday’ scale (which averages over 24 as opposed to 8 responses) has a less spread out distribution. It is notable that some scores (e.g., spontaneity wishes) show a higher degree of individual-level heterogeneity than others (e.g., regrets). Even for our most reliable ‘everyday’ scale, the degree of heterogeneity is substantial. Although a sizeable majority of respondents lean towards self-control, a significant minority displays an overall preference for spontaneity. This suggests that policies based on a one-size-fits-all approach that assumes a universal desire for self-control may not do justice to the preferences of many.

Conclusion

Sunstein and Thaler's New Year's Resolution Test is an informal expression of an idea that is widely used in discussions of behavioural public policy. This idea is that, in many everyday decisions about such matters as diet and exercise, individuals experience self-control failures – they face conflicts between desires for immediate enjoyment and commitments to long-term goals, try to resist the temptation to act on those desires, but fail in the attempt. By investigating individuals’ self-acknowledged goals, it is suggested, behavioural economists can identify self-control failures and design policy interventions to counter them – interventions that can be justified as enactments of individuals’ own judgements.

Our findings are consistent with a more nuanced understanding of the psychology of self-control. Conflicts between desires and goals do indeed appear to be a fundamental feature of human life, but it would be a mistake to assume that people's considered judgements always favour their self-acknowledged goals over conflicting desires. Just as people may value the exercise of self-control in achieving their goals, so also they may value spontaneity in responding to their desires. Our findings suggest that spontaneity and self-control can be interpreted as opposite directions along a single scale. We have found considerable heterogeneity in individuals’ judgements about the relative importance of spontaneity and self-control, with a large minority of our respondents leaning more towards spontaneity. We have not found evidence that such judgements, when elicited from individuals in cool emotional states, are context-dependent.

As we have acknowledged from the outset, our findings are those of an exploratory study. We see them as opening up an agenda of under-researched questions about attitudes to spontaneity. One obvious question concerns the robustness of our finding that those attitudes are not subject to significant attention-focusing effects. To answer that question, it would be useful to investigate wider ranges of long-term goals and attention-focusing cues. It would also be useful to investigate the possibility, tentatively suggested in the ‘Tests for context-dependence’ section, of gender differences in attitudes to spontaneity and self-control: men's attitudes may be more favourable to self-control, and more susceptible to attention-focusing cues, than women's. A further possibility is that some kinds of deviation from long-term goals are viewed as more spontaneity-affirming than others. For example, recall the contrast between our respondents’ spontaneity-favouring attitudes to sugary drinks and restaurant desserts and their self-control-favouring attitudes to exercise. Breaking a health-oriented resolution by ordering a crème brûlée is perhaps a more positive way of expressing spontaneity than not taking one's daily run on a wet day.

To our knowledge, the idea that spontaneity might be valued does not appear explicitly in Sunstein and Thaler's writings, or in other work in behavioural economics that seeks to remedy self-control problems. However, as a footnote to a discussion of diet and obesity, Sunstein and Thaler (Reference Sunstein and Thaler2003, p. 1167, note 19) discuss the related value of autonomy – that ‘people are entitled to make their own choices even if they err’. Recognising that idea as a possible objection to their proposals for nudging, they say: ‘We do not disagree with the view that autonomy has claims of its own, but we believe that it would be fanatical, in the settings we discuss, to treat autonomy, in the form of freedom of choice, as a kind of trump, not to be overridden on consequentialist grounds.’ An advocate of libertarian paternalism might make a similar argument about spontaneity – that spontaneity has moral claims of its own, but that it would be fanatical to claim that, if the pleasures of spontaneity are taken into account, most obese people are choosing the best means of promoting their well-being. If the criterion on which public policies are justified is the maximisation of overall well-being, the truth or falsity of such claims is clearly important. But our article is about the idea that public policies can be justified on the grounds that they help individuals to overcome self-acknowledged self-control problems. If that idea is to be used as a guiding principle, we need to be assured that those individuals want to be helped.

Financial support

This research was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (award ES/P008976/1) and by the European Research Council under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement 670103). The funders had no involvement in the actual research. We are grateful to the editor and two anonymous referees for their useful comments.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare none.

Footnotes

1 We will use the term goal to refer to preferences that apply to extended sequences of decisions, or to the outcomes of such sequences – for example, a preference for taking regular exercise or for not being overweight or obese.

2 The idea that paternalism should be defined in terms of the intentions of the policy-maker is defended by Le Grand and New (Reference Le Grand and New2015).

3 This way of modelling human agency as an interaction between the preferences and judgements of an ‘inner rational agent’ and ‘errors’ and ‘biases’ that are induced by psychological mechanisms is examined and critiqued by Infante et al. (Reference Infante, Lecouteux and Sugden2016).

4 DellaVigna and Malmendier reject the explanation, sometimes suggested by behavioural economists (e.g., Ho et al., Reference Ho, Lim and Camerer2006), that ‘paying not to go to the gym’ is a by-product of a sophisticated self-control strategy. Their primary explanation is that, when buying gym memberships, people fail to anticipate future failures of self-control.

5 Hofmann et al. do not report the breakdown of actively resisted desires according to whether they were or were not problematic. In Delaney and Lades's study, 23% of non-problematic desires were resisted. If one assumed the same rate of non-problematic resistance in Hofmann et al.'s data, the proportion of problematic desires that were resisted would be 71%.

6 The pilot study was pre-registered at AsPredicted.org as ‘Normative economics: context-dependence of well-being judgements – pilot’ (aspredicted.org, #45444). It was appended to, and formed the final part of, an online experiment which investigated an entirely different topic.

7 See AsPredicted.org (#47116): ‘Normative economics: context-dependence of well-being judgements’.

8 Weingarten et al. (Reference Weingarten, Chen, McAdams, Helper, Yi and Albarracín2016) provide a meta-analysis of 133 such studies.

9 Similar results are found in Tobit models which take account of the censored nature of responses.

10 Because of a software problem that was specific to the demographic questions in the questionnaire, our econometric analysis uses age and gender data supplied later by Prolific. The survey data were collected on September 7 and 8, 2020, but participants’ ages are coded as at March 23, 2021. Our conclusions are unaffected.

11 In principle, a mirror-image mechanism is also possible: people with unhealthy lifestyles might approve of their own spontaneity without wishing to be more spontaneous, and thus give relatively high-scoring responses to Q9–Q16. However, we find no evidence of this effect.

12 We do not want to claim that experiments should always be set up as tests of pre-registered hypotheses.

13 This methodological strategy, of looking for replicable ‘exhibits’ or ‘bottled phenomena’, is explained and defended by Bardsley et al. (Reference Bardsley, Cubitt, Loomes, Moffatt, Starmer and Sugden2010, pp. 156–69).

14 For this reason, we include Q24 as an item in the scale. However, responses to Q24 have stronger correlations with the importance in life items than with the Q1–Q23 items.

References

Andreoni, J., Rao, J. and Trachtman, H. (2017), ‘Avoiding the ask: A field experiment on altruism, empathy, and charitable giving’, Journal of Political Economy, 125: 625–53.10.1086/691703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardsley, N., Cubitt, R., Loomes, G., Moffatt, P., Starmer, C. and Sugden, R. (2010), Experimental economics: Rethinking the rules. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400831432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J., Chen, M. and Burrows, L. (1996), ‘Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 230–44.10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C., Issacharoff, S., Loewenstein, G., O'Donoghue, T. and Rabin, M. (2003), ‘Regulation for conservatives: Behavioral economics and the case for “asymmetric paternalism”’, University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 151: 1211–54.10.2307/3312889CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, D. L., Schonger, M. and Wickens, C. (2016), ‘oTree – An open-source platform for laboratory, online and field experiments’, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 9: 8897.10.1016/j.jbef.2015.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delaney, L. and Lades, L. (2017), ‘Present bias and everyday self-control failures: A day reconstruction study’, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30: 1157–67.10.1002/bdm.2031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DellaVigna, S. and Malmendier, U. (2006), ‘Paying not to go to the gym’, American Economic Review, 96: 694719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fudenberg, D. and Levine, D. (2006), ‘A dual-self model of impulse control’, American Economic Review, 96: 1449–76.10.1257/aer.96.5.1449CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. and Jones, C. (1977), ‘Category accessibility and impression formation’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13: 141–54.10.1016/S0022-1031(77)80007-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, T., Lim, N. and Camerer, C. (2006), ‘Modeling the psychology of consumer and firm behavior with behavioral economics’, Journal of Marketing Research, 43: 307–31.10.1509/jmkr.43.3.307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R., Förster, G. and Vohs, K. (2012a), ‘Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102: 1318–35.10.1037/a0026545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Vohs, K. and Baumeister, R. (2012b), ‘What people desire, feel conflicted about, and try to resist in everyday life’, Psychological Science, 23: 582–8.10.1177/0956797612437426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Infante, G., Lecouteux, G. and Sugden, R. (2016), ‘Preference purification and the inner rational agent: A critique of the conventional wisdom of behavioural welfare economics’, Journal of Economic Methodology, 23: 125.10.1080/1350178X.2015.1070527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2003), ‘A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality’, American Psychologist, 58: 697720.10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011), Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2012), A proposal to deal with questions about priming effects. Open email. Retrieved from: https://www.nature.com/articles/nature.2012.11535.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Frederick, S. (2002), ‘Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment’, in Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., and Kahneman, D. (eds), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 4981.10.1017/CBO9780511808098.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Sunstein, C. (2006), ‘Cognitive psychology of moral intuitions’, in Changeux, J.-P., Damasio, A., Singer, W., and Christen, Y. (eds), Neurobiology of Human Values, Berlin: Springer Science and Business Media, 91105.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N. and Stone, A. (2004), ‘A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method’, Science, 306(5702): 1776–80.10.1126/science.1103572CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Grand, J. and New, B. (2015), Government paternalism: Nanny state or helpful friend? Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
O'Donoghue, T. and Rabin, M. (2003), ‘Studying optimal paternalism, illustrated by a model of sin taxes’, American Economic Review, 93: 186–91.10.1257/000282803321947029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schkade, D. and Kahneman, D. (1998), ‘Does living in California make people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction’, Psychological Science, 9: 340–6.10.1111/1467-9280.00066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Srull, T. and Wyer, R. (1979), ‘The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37: 1660–72.10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugden, R. (2017), ‘Do people really want to be nudged towards healthy lifestyles?’, International Review of Economics, 64: 113123.10.1007/s12232-016-0264-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunstein, C. and Thaler, R. (2003), ‘Libertarian paternalism is not an oxymoron’, University of Chicago Law Review, 70: 1159–202.10.2307/1600573CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R. and Sunstein, C. (2008), Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Wason, P. and Evans, J. (1975), ‘Dual processes in reasoning?’, Cognition, 3: 141–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weingarten, E., Chen, Q., McAdams, M., Helper, J., Yi, J. and Albarracín, D. (2016), ‘From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words’, Psychological Bulletin, 142: 472–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Tests for context-dependence.

Figure 1

Table 2. Regression results.

Figure 2

Table 3. Gender breakdown of context-dependence effects.

Figure 3

Table 4. Responses to individual judgement questions.

Figure 4

Figure 1. Distributions of Spontaneity/Self-Control Scores.