Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T15:42:07.680Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impulsivity in first-degree relatives at risk of psychosis and mania: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2024

Jess Kerr-Gaffney*
Affiliation:
Psychology, and Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Yahufu Nuerzati
Affiliation:
Psychology, and Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Emma I. Kopra
Affiliation:
Psychology, and Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Allan H. Young
Affiliation:
Psychology, and Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, Bethlem Royal Hospital, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Jess Kerr-Gaffney; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Impulsivity is elevated in psychosis and during mania in bipolar disorder. Studies in unaffected relatives may help establish whether impulsivity is a heritable, state independent endophenotype. The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to examine whether impulsivity is elevated in unaffected relatives of those with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder, compared to controls. Databases were systematically searched up until March 2023 for articles reporting data on a behavioral or self-report measure of impulsivity in first-degree relatives and controls. Nineteen studies were included. Behavioral (10 studies, d = 0.35, p < 0.001) and self-reported impulsivity was significantly elevated in bipolar disorder relatives compared to controls (5 studies, d = 0.46, p < 0.001), with small effect sizes. Relatives of those with schizophrenia did not show significantly elevated impulsivity compared to controls on behavioral measures (6 studies, d = 0.42, p = 0.102). There were not enough studies to conduct a meta-analysis on self-report data in schizophrenia relatives or schizoaffective disorder relatives (self-report or behavioral). Study quality was good, however there was moderate to high heterogeneity in behavioral meta-analyses. Results suggest elevated impulsivity may be an endophenotype for bipolar disorder, present in an attenuated state before and after the illness and in at-risk individuals. This trait, amongst other behavioral and psychological indices, could be used to identify those who are at risk of developing bipolar disorder. Future research should refine measurement across studies and establish which components of impulsivity are affected in those at risk of psychotic and bipolar disorders.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Recently, there has been a shift toward characterizing psychopathology based on neurobiological measures and observable behavior, rather than solely on traditionally defined diagnostic categories (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013). In particular, there is increasing evidence for genetic, biological, and phenomenological similarities between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, with some proposing a continuum model to represent diagnostic variation across these disorders, with schizoaffective disorder lying in between the two (Keshavan et al., Reference Keshavan, Morris, Sweeney, Pearlson, Thaker, Seidman and Tamminga2011). Genetic studies support substantial genetic overlap between these disorders (Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Yip, Björk, Pawitan, Cannon, Sullivan and Hultman2009).

Both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are associated with affective and cognitive impairments, but these can differ in presentation. Schizophrenia is often characterized by emotional blunting, while bipolar disorder is characterized by depressive, mixed, and manic episodes. On the other hand, while delusional beliefs and hallucinations are core symptoms of schizophrenia, they often occur during manic episodes in bipolar disorder. Similarly, while cognitive impairments in executive functioning, attention, and memory are well-characterized in schizophrenia (Bowie & Harvey, Reference Bowie and Harvey2006), there is increasing recognition of similar cognitive difficulties in bipolar disorder, albeit in an attenuated form (Keramatian, Torres, & Yatham, Reference Keramatian, Torres and Yatham2021). Although most pronounced during acute illness, individuals with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia demonstrate persistent, trait-like cognitive deficits in periods of remission (Bora, Yucel, & Pantelis, Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis2009; Nuechterlein, Ventura, Subotnik, & Bartzokis, Reference Nuechterlein, Ventura, Subotnik and Bartzokis2014). There is evidence to suggest that cognitive impairments are predictive of later functional outcomes in both disorders (Ehrminger et al., Reference Ehrminger, Brunet-Gouet, Cannavo, Aouizerate, Cussac, Azorin and Roux2021; Nuechterlein et al., Reference Nuechterlein, Subotnik, Green, Ventura, Asarnow, Gitlin and Mintz2011).

Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct, including both cognitive and behavioral components, defined as a predisposition to react toward stimuli in a rapid and unplanned manner, without regard to the consequences (Lombardo et al., Reference Lombardo, Bearden, Barrett, Brumbaugh, Pittman, Frangou and Glahn2012). Impulsivity is an important clinical feature of several psychiatric disorders, for example, it is a key feature of mania in bipolar disorder, but is also present during euthymia (Ramirez-Martin, Ramos-Martin, Mayoral-Cleries, Moreno-Kustner, & Guzman-Parra, Reference Ramirez-Martin, Ramos-Martin, Mayoral-Cleries, Moreno-Kustner and Guzman-Parra2020; Santana, Kerr-Gaffney, Ancane, & Young, Reference Santana, Kerr-Gaffney, Ancane and Young2022). Evidence suggests that impulsivity in bipolar disorder is associated with a more severe course of illness, higher rates of hospitalization, increased rates of relapse, increased risk of suicide, and reduced quality of life (Baldaçara et al., Reference Baldaçara, Nery-Fernandes, Rocha, Quarantini, Rocha, Guimarães and Jackowski2011; Jimenez et al., Reference Jimenez, Arias, Castellvi, Goikolea, Rosa, Fananas and Benabarre2012; Oquendo et al., Reference Oquendo, Currier, Liu, Hasin, Grant and Blanco2010; Victor, Johnson, & Gotlib, Reference Victor, Johnson and Gotlib2011). Due to its apparent state independence (present during euthymia as well as mania), some have suggested that impulsivity may represent an endophenotype for bipolar disorder (Bora et al., Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis2009).

Elevated impulsivity has also been reported in those with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder (Nolan, D'Angelo, & Hoptman, Reference Nolan, D'Angelo and Hoptman2011). Impulsivity has been identified as a risk factor for violent behavior in people with psychosis (Witt, van Dorn, & Fazel, Reference Witt, van Dorn and Fazel2013), and, similar to bipolar disorder, increased risk of substance use disorder and suicide (Dervaux et al., Reference Dervaux, Baylé, Laqueille, Bourdel, Le Borgne, Olié and Krebs2001; Meltzer, Reference Meltzer2001; Oquendo et al., Reference Oquendo, Currier, Liu, Hasin, Grant and Blanco2010). However, there is some evidence to suggest different mechanisms behind elevated impulsivity in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nanda et al. (Reference Nanda, Tandon, Mathew, Padmanabhan, Clementz, Pearlson and Keshavan2016) reported that volumes of sub-regions within the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) were significantly inversely associated with impulsivity in both schizoaffective disorder and psychotic bipolar disorder, but not schizophrenia. The OFC may influence ‘top-down’ cognitive control over emotional reactivity and decision making, and deficits in this area may lead to failure of inhibition (Joseph, Liu, Jiang, Lynam, & Kelly, Reference Joseph, Liu, Jiang, Lynam and Kelly2009; Siever, Reference Siever2008). Increased impulsivity may therefore be rooted in different mechanisms in psychotic disorders with and without prominent affective components (Nanda et al., Reference Nanda, Tandon, Mathew, Padmanabhan, Clementz, Pearlson and Keshavan2016).

Although elevated impulsivity has been shown to be stable across illness stages across the psychosis and bipolar spectrum, it may be that enduring subthreshold symptoms, medication, and chronic illness effects contribute to elevated impulsivity during euthymia or recovery. Therefore, studies in unaffected relatives are required to establish whether impulsivity may be a heritable, state independent endophenotype, and not a ‘scar’ of the illness. Alternatively, it may be that elevated impulsivity is shared among affected individuals but not among those at genetic risk, suggesting a phenotypic, rather than endophenotypic overlap. Thus, the aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to examine whether impulsivity is elevated in unaffected relatives of those with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder, compared to HCs.

Methods

Systematic review protocol

This systematic review and meta-analysis were performed in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, Moher, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow and McKenzie2021). The protocol was preregistered on PROSPERO (ID: CRD42023393115). There was one change to the original protocol: RStudio was used for the meta-analysis instead of RevMan.

Information sources and search strategy

Three electronic databases (PubMed, PsycInfo, and Web of Science) were searched for papers up until 12th March 2023. Searches were performed using the following terms: ‘bipolar disorder’ OR schizophrenia OR ‘schizoaffective disorder’ AND proband* OR sibling* OR mother OR father OR parent OR brother OR sister OR FDR OR ‘first degree relative’ AND impulsiv* OR ‘delay* gratification’ OR ‘inattention’ OR ‘response inhibition’. No search limits were applied. Reference lists of relevant review articles and eligible papers were also searched.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria for studies were: (a) case–control, cohort, or cross-sectional studies that include a group of first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder, schizoaffective disorder, or schizophrenia, as well as a group of unaffected healthy controls, (b) report impulsivity data (self-report or behavioral measures) for both groups, (c) include participants that are aged ⩾18, (d) full-text in English is published in a peer-reviewed journal. Papers including participants under the age of 18 were excluded, as were case reports, reviews, or qualitative studies.

Study selection

Screening and selection of articles is displayed in Fig. 1. After removal of duplicates, titles and abstracts of 610 articles were screened by one author (YN). Where titles and abstracts appeared relevant, these were retained and full texts retrieved. 47 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility independently by two authors (YN and JKG). If an article met all eligibility criteria but did not provide data on the impulsivity measure, study authors were contacted for this information. 19 articles met all eligibility criteria and were included in the review.

Figure 1. Systematic review search process.

Data extraction

The following information was extracted from each article: number of participants in each group, mean age, gender, impulsivity measure and outcome variable, mean and standard deviation impulsivity score, and a summary of key findings.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were performed using the metafor package (Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2010) in Rstudio (R Core Team, 2019). Meta-analyses (for each relative subgroup v. HCs, split by self-report or behavioral measurement) were carried out using random-effects models. Cohen's d was used to estimate effect sizes and is reported with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Effect sizes are interpreted using Cohen's (Reference Cohen1988) definitions of small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8). Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group compared to the HC group. Between study heterogeneity was assessed using Cochran's Q test and quantified using I 2, where I 2 < 25% indicates low heterogeneity, 25–75% indicates moderate heterogeneity, and I 2 ⩾ high heterogeneity (Higgins, Reference Higgins2003).

Risk of bias

Risk of publication bias across studies was assessed via visual inspection of funnel plots, and formally using Begg and Mazumdar's (Reference Begg and Mazumdar1994) rank correlation test.

Risk of bias for individual studies was assessed using the Kmet standard quality assessment criteria (Kmet, Lee, & Cook, Reference Kmet, Lee and Cook2004). The Kmet assesses quality of studies based on 14 criteria related study objectives, design, methodology, sample selection, reporting of the results and conclusions. Eleven of the criteria were used in this review, as three of the criteria were only applicable to randomized controlled trials (random allocation and blinding of investigators and participants to interventions). Each criterion was rated as 2 (fully met criteria), 1 (partially meeting criteria), or 0 (failing to meet criteria). A total score was then calculated, then divided by the total possible score (22) to give a summary score ranging from 0 to 1, with higher scores indicating higher quality. Risk of bias assessments were carried out independently by two authors (YN and EK), then any discrepancies resolved by a third author (JKG).

Results

Study characteristics

Study characteristics are shown in Table 1. Fourteen studies included a group of first-degree relatives of bipolar disorder probands, eight included a group of first-degree relatives of schizophrenia probands, and two included a group of first-degree relatives of schizoaffective disorder probands. Most relative groups were siblings only (9 studies) or a mix of siblings, parents, and offspring (7 studies), one study included offspring only, and two studies did not specify the relative composition. Seven studies used a self-report measure of impulsivity (the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale version 11 or 11A in six studies, and the Chapman Impulsive Nonconformity Scale in one study), while 13 used a behavioral measure, with the stop signal task (SST) being most common (5 studies).

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies

s.d., standard deviation; BD, bipolar disorder; HC, healthy control; SZ, schizophrenia; BD-P, bipolar disorder with a history of psychosis; SAD, schizoaffective disorder; SAD-D, schizoaffective disorder, depressed type; SAD-BP, schizoaffective disorder, bipolar disorder type; SSRT, stop signal reaction time; BIS-11, Barrett Impulsiveness Scale-11; BIS-11A, Barrett Impulsiveness Scale-11A; CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.

Synthesis of results

Relatives of bipolar disorder

Ten studies examined impulsivity in bipolar disorder relatives using behavioral measures. The random-effects model with a total sample size of 1344 participants (610 bipolar disorder relatives and 734 HCs) showed that relatives of those with bipolar disorder were significantly more impulsive than HCs, with a small effect size, d = 0.35, 95% CI 0.09–0.61, z = 2.67, p < 0.001 (Fig. 2). There was moderate heterogeneity between studies, Q(9) = 24.60, p = 0.003, I 2 = 72.11%.

Figure 2. Forest plot of standardized mean differences (SMD) between bipolar disorder relatives and healthy controls (HCs) in studies assessing impulsivity with behavioral measures. Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group.

The funnel plot is shown in Fig. 3. There was no evidence of significant publication bias (Begg's test p = 0.484).

Figure 3. Funnel plot of bipolar disorder relatives v. HC behavioral studies included in the meta-analysis.

Five studies examined self-report measures of impulsivity in bipolar relatives. The random-effects model with a total sample size of 504 participants (190 bipolar relatives and 314 healthy controls) showed that relatives of those with bipolar disorder were significantly more impulsive than HCs, with a small effect size, d = 0.46, 95% CI 0.26–0.66, z = 4.54, p < 0.001 (Fig. 4). There was low heterogeneity between studies, Q(4) = 2.60, p = 0.626, I 2 = 0.00%.

Figure 4. Forest plot of standardized mean differences (SMD) between bipolar disorder relatives and healthy controls (HCs) in studies assessing impulsivity with self-report measures. Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group.

The funnel plot is shown in Fig. 5. There was no evidence of significant publication bias (Begg's test p = 0.483).

Figure 5. Funnel plot of bipolar disorder relatives v. HC self-report studies included in the meta-analysis.

Relatives of schizophrenia

Six studies examined impulsivity in schizophrenia relatives using behavioral measures. The random-effects model with a total sample size of 1241 participants (603 schizophrenia relatives and 638 HCs) showed that relatives of those with schizophrenia did not show significantly elevated impulsivity compared to HCs, d = 0.42, 95% CI −0.08 to 0.92, z = 1.64, p = 0.102 (Fig. 6). There was high heterogeneity between studies, Q(5) = 24.43, p < 0.001, I 2 = 92.33%.

Figure 6. Forest plot of standardized mean differences (SMD) between schizophrenia relatives and healthy controls (HCs) in studies assessing impulsivity with behavioral measures. Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group.

The funnel plot is shown in Fig. 7. There was some evidence of publication bias (Begg's test p = 0.056).

Figure 7. Funnel plot of schizophrenia relatives v. HC behavioral studies included in the meta-analysis.

Two studies examined self-reported impulsivity in relatives of those with schizophrenia, therefore a meta-analysis could not be conducted. Thaker, Moran, Adami, and Cassady (Reference Thaker, Moran, Adami and Cassady1993) administered the Chapman Psychosis Proneness Scales, a group of scales used to identify schizotypal phenotypes in non-psychotic individuals, to relatives of those with schizophrenia, HCs, and relative and non-relative groups with personality disorders. The Impulsive Nonconformity Scale measures unorthodoxy, impulsivity, and insensitivity to the feelings of others. Though scores were slightly higher in the HC group (mean = 10.0, s.d. = 5.3) than the relative group (mean = 7.4, s.d. = 4.7), this difference was not statistically significant. Fekih-Romdhane, Maktouf, and Cheour (Reference Fekih-Romdhane, Maktouf and Cheour2022) administered the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale to patients with first-episode schizophrenia, siblings of those with schizophrenia, and HCs. Siblings showed significantly higher scores on the non-planning subscale compared to HCs but did not differ on attentional or motor subscales.

Relatives of schizoaffective disorder

Only two studies included relatives of people with schizoaffective disorder, both using behavioral measures, therefore a meta-analysis could not be performed. Ethridge et al. (Reference Ethridge, Soilleux, Nakonezny, Reilly, Kristian, Keefe and Carolina2014) examined performance on the SST in relatives of people with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder with a history of psychosis, schizoaffective disorder depressed-type, schizoaffective bipolar-type, and HCs, finding no differences in performance in any relative group compared to HCs when controlling for generalized cognitive impairment. Similarly, Reilly et al. (Reference Reilly, Hill, Gold, Keefe, Clementz, Gershon and Sweeney2017) did not find differences in false alarm rates on the dot probe expectancy task in relatives of those with schizophrenia, psychotic bipolar disorder, or schizoaffective disorder, compared to HCs.

Risk of bias

The full risk of bias rating for all included studies is shown in the online Supplementary material. Generally, study quality was high; summary scores ranged from 0.77 (Henna et al., Reference Henna, Hatch, Nicoletti, Swann, Zunta-Soares and Soares2013) to 1.00 (Fekih-Romdhane et al., Reference Fekih-Romdhane, Maktouf and Cheour2022; Lombardo et al., Reference Lombardo, Bearden, Barrett, Brumbaugh, Pittman, Frangou and Glahn2012; Reilly et al., Reference Reilly, Hill, Gold, Keefe, Clementz, Gershon and Sweeney2017). Objectives, study designs, and outcome measures were appropriate and sufficiently described (likely due in part to our inclusion criteria specifying measures, outcome reporting, and study design), and results and conclusions were supported and reported in detail. Study quality was more variable with regard to recruitment methods, sample size, and control of confounding variables.

Discussion

The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to examine whether impulsivity is elevated in unaffected first-degree relatives of those with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder, representing a putative trait-marker in at-risk populations. Our results suggested that impulsivity is significantly elevated in relatives of those with bipolar disorder compared to HCs, with a small effect size. This was true for both self-reported impulsivity and behavioral measures. Although a similar effect size was found in studies assessing impulsivity with behavioral measures in relatives of those with schizophrenia compared to HCs, this effect was not significant. There was not enough evidence to synthesize findings from studies assessing self-reported impulsivity in schizophrenia relatives or in relatives of patients with schizoaffective disorder (behavioral or self-report); individual studies did not find strong evidence of elevated impulsivity in either population.

Previous meta-analyses have reported elevated impulsivity during euthymia in those with bipolar disorder (with medium to large effect sizes), suggesting impulsivity may be a trait feature of the disorder, present before and after illness onset (Santana et al., Reference Santana, Kerr-Gaffney, Ancane and Young2022). Our results extend these findings, showing elevated impulsivity, albeit, in an attenuated form, in at-risk populations. Together, these results indicate that impulsivity may be a heritable, state independent endophenotype for bipolar disorder. Indeed, impulsivity has a genetic basis, and similar to other complex traits, a large number of genetic variants seem to influence it's expression, each with small individual effects (Bezdjian, Baker, & Tuvblad, Reference Bezdjian, Baker and Tuvblad2011; Khadka et al., Reference Khadka, Narayanan, Meda, Gelernter, Han, Sawyer and Pearlson2014). In particular, some studies have found that genetic dopaminergic variation is associated with differences in impulsivity in the general population (Colzato, van den Wildenberg, Van der Does, & Hommel, Reference Colzato, van den Wildenberg, Van der Does and Hommel2010; Congdon, Lesch, & Canli, Reference Congdon, Lesch and Canli2008). Others have reported associations between BDNF and serotonin transporter (5-HTT) protein polymorphisms and impulsivity in bipolar disorder and HCs (Boscutti et al., Reference Boscutti, Pigoni, Delvecchio, Lazzaretti, Mandolini, Girardi and Brambilla2022), but no interactions between diagnosis and polymorphism status, suggesting comparable effects of these genes among those with and without bipolar disorder.

Although we did not find a significant difference in impulsivity assessed via behavioral measures between relatives of patients with schizophrenia and controls, we cannot rule out the possibility that impulsivity is elevated in this population, given the relatively small number of studies included. The effect size (d = 0.42) was very similar to that found in bipolar disorder relatives (d = 0.35), but there was greater variability in results across studies. More studies in relatives of those with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder are required to establish whether impulsivity may represent an endophenotype for these disorders, as in bipolar disorder. Importantly, consistency of measurement across studies is required, as currently studies use a wide array of measures to assess different aspects of impulsivity, leading to variability in results. Unfortunately, it was not possible to perform a moderator analysis with each different measure as a factor level, due to the small number of included studies and large number of measures used. Establishing which measures are related to functional impairment, or detrimental behaviors (e.g. substance use, self-harm) will also be an important avenue for future research.

Nonetheless, it may be that elevated impulsivity in those with schizophrenia is, at least partially, a consequence of the acute illness, or that different mechanisms underlie impulsivity in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Indeed, impulsivity appears to be less pronounced in younger patients with early onset, first episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Jepsen et al., Reference Jepsen, Rydkjaer, Fagerlund, Pagsberg, Jespersen, Glenthøj and Oranje2018). Some have suggested that impulsive responding in behavioral tasks (such as response inhibition) seen in those with schizophrenia is due to generalized cognitive deficits, rather than impulsivity per se (Christodoulou, Messinis, Papathanasopoulos, & Frangou, Reference Christodoulou, Messinis, Papathanasopoulos and Frangou2012). Response inhibition tends to be particularly affected in bipolar disorder, more so than schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, and appears to represent a specific cognitive deficit rather than a generalized one, as in the psychotic disorders (Ethridge et al., Reference Ethridge, Soilleux, Nakonezny, Reilly, Kristian, Keefe and Carolina2014). Furthermore, in a large sample of patients with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder and their relatives, Ethridge et al. (Reference Ethridge, Soilleux, Nakonezny, Reilly, Kristian, Keefe and Carolina2014) reported that SST performance was associated with social adjustment and self-reported impulsivity in bipolar disorder only, suggesting that reduced inhibitory control may contribute to behavioral impulsivity and social difficulties in this population. In sum, although first-degree relatives and patients across diagnoses may show elevated impulsivity (with strongest evidence in bipolar disorder and their relatives), the mechanisms underlying these similarities are likely to differ across diagnoses.

To our knowledge, this is the first review to synthesize studies assessing impulsivity in relatives of those with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and schizoaffective disorder. Studies in first-degree relatives are useful in distinguishing trait v. state features, as relatives are generally medication-free, and have not experienced repeated episodes of the illness in question, which may have lasting effects on behavior and cognition. However, several limitations should be noted. Firstly, the small number of studies in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder prevented us from making firm conclusions regarding impulsivity as an endophenotype in these disorders. As previously mentioned, studies included a wide array of measures, many of which assessed different aspects of impulsivity (e.g. response inhibition, inattention). The small number of studies included prevented separate sub-analyses of these aspects, as has been done in previous reviews in probands (Ramirez-Martin et al., Reference Ramirez-Martin, Ramos-Martin, Mayoral-Cleries, Moreno-Kustner and Guzman-Parra2020; Santana et al., Reference Santana, Kerr-Gaffney, Ancane and Young2022). Finally, included studies sometimes did not control for confounding variables or did not match relative and control groups on key demographic variables, which may have introduced bias to our results.

Our findings suggest elevated impulsivity may be a putative endophenotype for bipolar disorder, present in an attenuated state before and after the illness and in at-risk individuals. This trait, amongst other behavioral and psychological indices, could be used to identify those who are at risk of developing bipolar disorder. Indeed, higher impulsivity in those at risk of bipolar disorder has been shown to predict bipolar disorder diagnosis 13 years later (Kwapil et al., Reference Kwapil, Miller, Zinser, Chapman, Chapman and Eckblad2000). In turn, early interventions could be targeted to high-risk groups, for example, cognitive interventions that target aspects of impulsivity may be beneficial in improving outcomes (Tsapekos, Strawbridge, Cella, Young, & Wykes, Reference Tsapekos, Strawbridge, Cella, Young and Wykes2023). There was not enough evidence to conclude whether impulsivity is also elevated in relatives of those with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Future research should refine measurement across studies and establish which components of impulsivity are affected in those at risk of psychotic and bipolar disorders.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001752

Acknowledgments

None.

Funding statement

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. AHY's independent research is funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Competing interests

AHY is deputy editor of BJPsych Open, and has given paid lectures and advisory boards for Flow Neuroscience, Novartis, Roche, Janssen, Takeda, Noema pharma, Compass Pathways, Astrazenaca, Boehringer Ingelheim, Eli Lilly, LivaNova, Lundbeck, Sunovion, Servier, Livanova, Janssen, Allegan, Bionomics, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sage, and Neurocentrx, He is principal investigator on studies funded by Janssen, LivaNova, Novartis, and Compass Pathways. He has received grant funding from NIMH (USA), CIHR (Canada), NARSAD (USA), Stanley Medical Research Institute (USA), MRC (UK), Wellcome Trust (UK), Royal College of Physicians (Edin), BMA (UK), UBC-VGH Foundation (Canada), WEDC (Canada), CCS Depression Research Fund (Canada), MSFHR (Canada), NIHR (UK), Janssen (UK) and EU Horizon 2020.

Footnotes

*

Joint first authors.

References

Baldaçara, L., Nery-Fernandes, F., Rocha, M., Quarantini, L. C., Rocha, G. G. L., Guimarães, J. L., … Jackowski, A. (2011). Is cerebellar volume related to bipolar disorder? Journal of Affective Disorders, 135(1–3), 305309. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, I. E., Wu, M. J., Frazier, T. W., Mwangi, B., Spiker, D., Zunta-Soares, G. B., & Soares, J. C. (2016). Neurocognitive functioning in individuals with bipolar disorder and their healthy siblings: A preliminary study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 201, 5156. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50(4), 10881101. doi: 10.2307/2533446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bezdjian, S., Baker, L. A., & Tuvblad, C. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on impulsivity: A meta-analysis of twin, family and adoption studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(7), 12091223. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., Vahip, S., Akdeniz, F., Ilerisoy, H., Aldemir, E., & Alkan, M. (2008). Executive and verbal working memory dysfunction in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 161(3), 318324. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. Journal of Affective Disorders, 113(1–2), 120. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boscutti, A., Pigoni, A., Delvecchio, G., Lazzaretti, M., Mandolini, G. M., Girardi, P., … Brambilla, P. (2022). The influence of 5-HTTLPR, BDNF Rs6265 and COMT Rs4680 polymorphisms on impulsivity in bipolar disorder: The role of gender. Genes, 13(3), 482. doi: 10.3390/genes13030482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowie, C. R., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). Cognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2(4), 531536. doi: 10.2147/nedt.2006.2.4.531CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christodoulou, T., Messinis, L., Papathanasopoulos, P., & Frangou, S. (2012). Dissociable and common deficits in inhibitory control in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 262(2), 125130. doi: 10.1007/s00406-011-0213-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Van der Does, A. J. W., & Hommel, B. (2010). Genetic markers of striatal dopamine predict individual differences in dysfunctional, but not functional impulsivity. Neuroscience, 170(3), 782788. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.07.050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Congdon, E., Lesch, K. P., & Canli, T. (2008). Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: Implications for impulsivity. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B(1), 2732. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30557CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 11, 126. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dervaux, A., Baylé, F. J., Laqueille, X., Bourdel, M.-C., Le Borgne, M.-H., Olié, J.-P., & Krebs, M.-O. (2001). Is substance abuse in schizophrenia related to impulsivity, sensation seeking, or anhedonia? American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(3), 492494. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.3.492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrminger, M., Brunet-Gouet, E., Cannavo, A.-S., Aouizerate, B., Cussac, I., Azorin, J.-M., … Roux, P. (2021). Longitudinal relationships between cognition and functioning over 2 years in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder: A cross-lagged panel model approach with the FACE-BD cohort. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 218(2), 8087. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ethridge, L. E., Soilleux, M., Nakonezny, P. A., Reilly, J. L., Kristian, S., Keefe, R. S. E., … Carolina, N. (2014). Behavioral response inhibition in psychotic disorders: Diagnostic specificity, familiality and relation to generalized cognitive deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 159(0), 491498. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.08.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fekih-Romdhane, F., Maktouf, H., & Cheour, M. (2022). Aggressive behaviour in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia patients, their unaffected siblings and healthy controls. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 17(3), 299310. doi: 10.1111/eip.13329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrier, I. N., Chowdhury, R., Thompson, J. M., Watson, S., & Young, A. H. (2004). Neurocognitive function in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder: A preliminary report. Bipolar Disorders, 6(4), 319322. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00122.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelstein, J. R. J., Cannon, T. D., Gur, R. E., Gur, R. C., & Moberg, P. (1997). Attentional dysfunctions in neuroleptic-naive and neuroleptic-withdrawn schizophrenic patients and their siblings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(2), 203212. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.2.203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frangou, S., Haldane, M., Roddy, D., & Kumari, V. (2005). Evidence for deficit in tasks of ventral, but not dorsal, prefrontal executive function as an endophenotypic marker for bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 58(10), 838839. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henna, E., Hatch, J. P., Nicoletti, M., Swann, A. C., Zunta-Soares, G., & Soares, J. C. (2013). Is impulsivity a common trait in bipolar and unipolar disorders? Bipolar Disorders, 15(2), 223227. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hidiroglu, C., Esen, O. D., Tunca, Z., Yalcin, S. N. G., Lombardo, L., Glahn, D. C., & Ozerdem, A. (2013). Can risk-taking be an endophenotype for bipolar disorder? A study on patients with bipolar disorder type I and their first-degree relatives. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(4), 474482. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hidiroğlu, C., Torres, I. J., Er, A., Işik, G., Yalin, N., Yatham, L. N., … Özerdem, A. (2015). Response inhibition and interference control in patients with bipolar I disorder and first-degree relatives. Bipolar Disorders, 17(7), 781794. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jepsen, J. R. M., Rydkjaer, J., Fagerlund, B., Pagsberg, A. K., Jespersen, R. A. F., Glenthøj, B. Y., & Oranje, B. (2018). Overlapping and disease specific trait, response, and reflection impulsivity in adolescents with first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 48(4), 604616. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001921CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jimenez, E., Arias, B., Castellvi, P., Goikolea, J. M., Rosa, A. R., Fananas, L., … Benabarre, A. (2012). Impulsivity and functional impairment in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 136(3), 491497. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, J. E., Liu, X., Jiang, Y., Lynam, D., & Kelly, T. H. (2009). Neural correlates of emotional reactivity in sensation seeking. Psychological Science, 20(2), 215223. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02283.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keramatian, K., Torres, I. J., & Yatham, L. N. (2021). Neurocognitive functioning in bipolar disorder: What we know and what we don't. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 23(1), 2938. doi: 10.1080/19585969.2022.2042164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keshavan, M. S., Morris, D. W., Sweeney, J. A., Pearlson, G., Thaker, G., Seidman, L. J., … Tamminga, C. (2011). A dimensional approach to the psychosis spectrum between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: The Schizo-Bipolar Scale. Schizophrenia Research, 133(1–3), 250254. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khadka, S., Narayanan, B., Meda, S. A., Gelernter, J., Han, S., Sawyer, B., … Pearlson, G. D. (2014). Genetic association of impulsivity in young adults: A multivariate study. Translational Psychiatry, 4(9), e451e451. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kmet, L. M., Lee, R. C., & Cook, L. S. (2004). Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. Edmonton: Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research.Google Scholar
Kwapil, T. R., Miller, M. B., Zinser, M. C., Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J., & Eckblad, M. (2000). A longitudinal study of high scorers on the Hypomanic Personality Scale. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 222226. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Yip, B. H., Björk, C., Pawitan, Y., Cannon, T. D., Sullivan, P. F., & Hultman, C. M. (2009). Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: A population-based study. The Lancet, 373(9659), 234239. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60072-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindberg, P. G., Térémetz, M., Charron, S., Kebir, O., Saby, A., Bendjemaa, N., … Amado, I. (2016). Altered cortical processing of motor inhibition in schizophrenia. Cortex, 85, 112. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.09.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombardo, L. E., Bearden, C. E., Barrett, J., Brumbaugh, M. S., Pittman, B., Frangou, S., & Glahn, D. C. (2012). Trait impulsivity as an endophenotype for bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 14(5), 565570. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01035.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathias De Almeida, K., Nery, F. G., Moreno, R. A., Gorenstein, C., & Lafer, B. (2013). A sib-pair analysis of impulsivity in bipolar disorder type I. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(8), 11481152. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.05.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzer, H. Y. (2001). Treatment of suicidality in schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 932(1), 4460. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05797.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nanda, P., Tandon, N., Mathew, I. T., Padmanabhan, J. L., Clementz, B. A., Pearlson, G. D., … Keshavan, M. S. (2016). Impulsivity across the psychosis spectrum: Correlates of cortical volume, suicidal history, and social and global function. Schizophrenia Research, 170(1), 8086. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, K. A., D'Angelo, D., & Hoptman, M. J. (2011). Self-report and laboratory measures of impulsivity in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and healthy controls. Psychiatry Research, 187(1–2), 301303. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.10.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuechterlein, K. H., Subotnik, K. L., Green, M. F., Ventura, J., Asarnow, R. F., Gitlin, M. J., … Mintz, J. (2011). Neurocognitive predictors of work outcome in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(Suppl. 2), S33S40. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuechterlein, K. H., Ventura, J., Subotnik, K. L., & Bartzokis, G. (2014). The early longitudinal course of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 75(Suppl. 2), 2529. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13065su1.06CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oquendo, M. A., Currier, D., Liu, S.-M., Hasin, D. S., Grant, B. F., & Blanco, C. (2010). Increased risk for suicidal behavior in comorbid bipolar disorder and alcohol use disorders. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(07), 902909. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05198gryCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, M. J., Moher, D., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., … McKenzie, J. E. (2021). PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 370, n160. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Ramirez-Martin, A., Ramos-Martin, J., Mayoral-Cleries, F., Moreno-Kustner, B., & Guzman-Parra, J. (2020). Impulsivity, decision-making and risk-taking behaviour in bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 50(13), 21412153. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720003086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, J. L., Hill, S. K., Gold, J. M., Keefe, R. S. E., Clementz, B. A., Gershon, E., … Sweeney, J. A. (2017). Impaired context processing is attributable to global neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(2), 397406. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw081Google ScholarPubMed
Santana, R. P., Kerr-Gaffney, J., Ancane, A., & Young, A. H. (2022). Impulsivity in bipolar disorder: State or trait? Brain Sciences, 12(10), 118. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12101351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siever, L. J. (2008). Neurobiology of aggression and violence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(4), 429442. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thaker, G., Moran, M., Adami, H., & Cassady, S. (1993). Psychosis proneness scales in schizophrenia spectrum personality disorders: Familial vs. nonfamilial samples. Psychiatry Research, 46(1), 4757. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(93)90007-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivedi, J. K., Goel, D., Dhyani, M., Sharma, S., Singh, A. P., Sinha, P. K., & Tandon, R. (2008). Neurocognition in first-degree healthy relatives (siblings) of bipolar affective disorder patients. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62(2), 190196. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01754.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsapekos, D., Strawbridge, R., Cella, M., Young, A. H., & Wykes, T. (2023). Does cognitive improvement translate into functional changes? Exploring the transfer mechanisms of cognitive remediation therapy for euthymic people with bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine, 53(3), 936944. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721002336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victor, S. E., Johnson, S. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Quality of life and impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 13(3), 303309. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00919.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wessa, M., Kollmann, B., Linke, J., Schonfelder, S., & Kanske, P. (2015). Increased impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for bipolar disorder: Evidence from self-report and experimental measures in two high-risk populations. Journal of Affective Disorders, 178, 1824. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.02.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witt, K., van Dorn, R., & Fazel, S. (2013). Risk factors for violence in psychosis: Systematic review and meta-regression analysis of 110 studies. PLoS ONE, 8(2), e55942. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055942CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandbelt, B. B., Van Buuren, M., Kahn, R. S., & Vink, M. (2011). Reduced proactive inhibition in schizophrenia is related to corticostriatal dysfunction and poor working memory. Biological Psychiatry, 70(12), 11511158. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Systematic review search process.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies

Figure 2

Figure 2. Forest plot of standardized mean differences (SMD) between bipolar disorder relatives and healthy controls (HCs) in studies assessing impulsivity with behavioral measures. Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Funnel plot of bipolar disorder relatives v. HC behavioral studies included in the meta-analysis.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Forest plot of standardized mean differences (SMD) between bipolar disorder relatives and healthy controls (HCs) in studies assessing impulsivity with self-report measures. Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Funnel plot of bipolar disorder relatives v. HC self-report studies included in the meta-analysis.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Forest plot of standardized mean differences (SMD) between schizophrenia relatives and healthy controls (HCs) in studies assessing impulsivity with behavioral measures. Positive effect sizes indicate higher impulsivity in the relative group.

Figure 7

Figure 7. Funnel plot of schizophrenia relatives v. HC behavioral studies included in the meta-analysis.

Supplementary material: File

Kerr-Gaffney et al. supplementary material

Kerr-Gaffney et al. supplementary material
Download Kerr-Gaffney et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.4 KB