1 Introduction
Understanding donor motives can help charity organisations and governments in designing fund-raising campaigns and supporting programmes. The two most prominent motivations discussed in the literature are warm glow and altruism. Whereas pure altruists derive utility from total donations (i.e., their donations and the donations of others are perfect substitutes); pure warm-glow givers only derive utility from their own donation; warm-glow givers are thought to derive utility from the joy of giving (Andreoni, Reference Andreoni1989, Reference Andreoni1990; Null, Reference Null2011). What precisely motivates donors is, however, not easy to identify empirically.
Several techniques have been developed to identify charitable motivations, including Crumpler and Grossman (Reference Crumpler and Grossman2008), Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018, Reference Gangadharan, Grossman and Xue2023), Fielding et al. (Reference Fielding, Knowles and Peeters2022), Ottoni-Wilhelm et al. (Reference Ottoni-Wilhelm, Vesterlund and Xie2017), and Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021). Donors are typically classified into two extreme categories: warm-glow givers and pure altruists. However, many experimental participants do not fit either strictly defined category, and are best classified as impure altruists. Critically, impure altruists exhibit great heterogeneity in donation behaviour, which suggests a need for further refinement of this category. Crumpler and Grossman (Reference Crumpler and Grossman2008) measure the strength of warm-glow preferences, while holding constant altruistic preferences. In contrast, Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman and Xue2023) measure the strength of altruistic preferences, while holding constant warm-glow preferences.
We suggest an alternative approach. Whereas Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018) sort participants into four strict categories (non-donor, warm-glow giver, pure altruist, and impure altruist), we instead identify the relative strength of warm glow and altruistic motives using their same two-stage decision task. To accomplish this, we follow Ottoni-Wilhelm et al. (Reference Ottoni-Wilhelm, Vesterlund and Xie2017) in assuming participants maximise a Cobb–Douglas impure altruism utility function, allowing us to identify structural parameters for giving motives. We thus obtain a novel fluid measure of an individual's strength of altruistic motive, and we validate our measure via a comparison to the survey measure of Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021).
2 Experimental design
We use the two-stage decision task of Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018), as implemented by Gandullia et al. (Reference Gandullia, Lezzi and Parciasepe2020). Participants are given 100 points and are asked first to choose an amount, , to donate to their preferred charity.Footnote 1 They are informed that we (the experimenters) will donate points to the charity, such that the charity organisation is guaranteed to receive a total of 100 points, regardless of their donation decision. After making this decision, the participant has points left. They are then informed that they have a second (unforeseen) opportunity to donate an amount, , to the charity, knowing that this time no complementary donation is made by us. Thus, the charity organisation will receive in total points, while the participant will keep points.Footnote 2
After completing this giving-type task, participants are asked to respond to the question “Think about the last time you gave to charity before today. What was most important to you?” with possible answers being (i) “the total amount given by everyone”, (ii) “the amount that you personally gave”, and (iii) “some other aspect of giving”.Footnote 3 This question was suggested by Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021) to elicit giving motivations and was validated using the Crumpler and Grossman (Reference Crumpler and Grossman2008) method.
In total, 357 participants completed the experiment/survey on 14 April 2023. These participants (aged 18–65 and fluent in English) were recruited via Prolific and redirected to our Qualtrics survey. Our subject pool includes 213 male and 140 female participants; 3 participants are gender diverse and 1 preferred not to answer. Two participants preferred not to reveal their age; the average age of the participants who revealed their age is 27.26. The median completion time was 3 min and 6 s, and participants earned on average 1.27 GBP including the fixed participation fee of 1.00 GBP.Footnote 4 Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Otago Human Ethics Committee (reference code D23/082).
3 Strength of altruistic motive
Following Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018), a participant is classified as a non-donor if and , a warm-glow giver if and , a pure altruist if and , and as an impure altruist if and .Footnote 5 Regarding the question of Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021), participants who gave the first response are categorised as pure altruists and those who gave the second one as high warm-glow altruists. In the following, we construct a more fluid method to measure the extent to which an individual's charitable behaviour is motivated by altruism.Footnote 6
Consider an individual with the Cobb–Douglas impure altruism utility function
where x is private consumption, g is the amount given by the individual, and G is total funds received by the charity. In the first decision of the Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018) giving-type task, the individual who seeks to maximise this utility (with , and ) will choose
In the second decision, the individual (using , , and ) will choose
Thus, choices and yield the following unique values of and :
Due to the design of the giving task, is at least . We normalise the parameters to ensure that they lie within [0, 1] using and . Furthermore, because of the two-stage design of the giving task, we can estimate the parameters directly on the donation choices without the use of statistics.
We note that this estimation procedure aligns well with the derivations laid out by Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018) for distinguishing different giving motives (see Appendix D, especially equations D7 and D8). An alternative approach might sequentially estimate the and parameters. However, under such an approach, the individual's preferences change from the first to the second stage, which is inconsistent with the wider literature. Moreover, separating the and means that the individual never makes a direct trade-off between warm-glow and altruism, which is what we seek to capture. We also note that the rescaling of and described above is simply a normalisation and does not materially affect the analysis in any way. These monotonic transformations preserve the ordering of parameter values across individuals.
In the spirit of Footnote 9 of Ottoni-Wilhelm et al. (Reference Ottoni-Wilhelm, Vesterlund and Xie2017), we construct a fluid measure of altruism as . It captures the amount of generosity that is attributable to the altruistic motive relative to the warm-glow motive. For participants who donated their full endowment during the giving-type task ( ), values for and cannot be properly specified, since their donation decisions meet the constraint and do not follow from the first-order conditions of the maximisation problems. As a consequence, cannot be properly specified either. Likewise, for non-donors ( ), we find , so cannot be properly specified for these participants.
4 Results
The left plot in Fig. 1 illustrates participants’ choices in the giving-type task. The size of each bubble is proportional to the number of participants making that choice. Classifying according to Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018), we have 6% non-donors, 36% warm-glow givers, 9% pure altruists, and 48% impure altruists. Classifying according to Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021), we have 37% pure altruists and 23% high warm-glow altruists; the residual 40% indicated having another giving motive.
The right plot shows a scatter plot of estimated and parameters for each individual. The sizes of the bubbles are proportional to the number of participants for which a pair of parameter values applies. The plot excludes the 164 participants (64%) for whom the constraint was binding (i.e., ). According to the classification of Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018), 83 (51%) of these participants are warm-glow givers, 12 (7%) of these pure altruists, and 69 (42%) of these impure altruists.
The value is visible via the angle of the ray from the origin. Circles on rays closer to the x-axis represent individuals for whom warm glow is a relatively more important motive for their generosity; circles on rays closer to the y-axis indicate that altruism is the more dominant motive. As noted above, there were 12 pure altruists (according to the Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018) classification) in the excluded group of 164 participants; for these participants, could be set to 1, since their observed generosity cannot be attributed to warm glow. Given that for the 22 (6%) non-donors, the value of cannot be specified, they are, in addition to the earlier 164 participants, excluded from further analysis. The results we present in the next paragraph are robust to including the 12 pure altruists on the budget constraint with .
According to the classification of Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021), of the 171 included participants, 69 (40%) are pure altruists, 40 (23%) are high warm-glow altruists, and 62 (36%) are in the residual group. Figure 2 presents three cumulative distributions over , one for each of the three answers to Carpenter's question. Values of are larger for those who responded with the altruistic answer; differences are statistically significant compared to those who gave the third answer ( ) or the second or third answer ( ).Footnote 7 The observed differences may be mainly explained by the extremes. In this regard, we find that 15 of the 21 participants with responded with the altruistic answer to Carpenter's question. Yet, we can conclude that there is consistency between responses given to the question of Carpenter and the strength of altruism we constructed using choices in the Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018) giving-type task.
We see no major differences in the distributions of males ( ) and females ( ) over the four giving types or over the three answers to Carpenter's question. We also do not find a significant difference in their estimated values of ( ). We do, however, find differences in the level of generosity. Over the full sample, the average total donation in the giving-type task is 68 points for males and 81 for females ( ). However, this difference is not significant ( ) when restricting attention to males and females for whom could be specified, with averages of 57 points donated for males and 58 for females. This endogenous selection bias is caused by 59% of females donating the full 100 points, while only 38% of males donated the full amount, due to which could only be identified for 39% of females and 54% of males. Both of these differences in proportions are significant (Chi-square test: , ).
5 Conclusion
We introduce a novel way to measure individuals’ strength of altruist motivation for charitable giving. Participants in an online experiment whose answer to the survey question of Carpenter (Reference Carpenter2021) indicates a more altruistic giving motive show a larger strength of altruist motivation according to our measure. This adds validity to our constructed measure.
A clear advantage of our method relative to Crumpler and Grossman (Reference Crumpler and Grossman2008) and Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman and Xue2023) is that in their methods, respectively, altruism and warm glow are kept constant, whereas our method allows both motives to operate simultaneously. This is important given the large share of impure altruists that is typically observed when using the method of Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018). Another notable advantage of our method relative to the one by Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman and Xue2023) is that the first stage of their elicitation task involves a binary decision, and with only a 10% chance, the donation will be forwarded to the charity, which leads to many individuals not being recognised as being somewhat generous.Footnote 8
As our method for developing a fluid measure of altruism draws on Ottoni-Wilhelm et al. (Reference Ottoni-Wilhelm, Vesterlund and Xie2017), it is interesting to compare our results to those reported in that paper. In Fig. 2, which corresponds to the right-hand-plot of Fig. 1, most observations are close to the axes, with the majority of observations having an equivalent of our equal to 0 or 1 (12% and 47%, respectively), and could be captured via the pure altruist and pure warm-glow giving-type categories. Our approach, in contrast, produces a much more even distribution of values of with fewer values at the boundary (27% at 0 and 12% at 1). Yet, we acknowledge that a perfect comparison is not possible given that there are several mechanical differences between the two approaches.Footnote 9
Finally, in our study, could not be specified for 52% of the sample. In Chan et al. (forthcoming) and Chan et al. (Reference Chan, Knowles, Peeters and Wolk2023), we found this fraction to be 43% and 31%, respectively. Although the fraction was lower in those studies, it still constitutes a substantial number of participants for whom cannot be specified. This, together with the potential bias we reported on with regard to gender differences, suggests that our measure is best used in a complementary manner to Gangadharan et al. (Reference Gangadharan, Grossman, Jones and Leister2018)'s categorical giving-type classification.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions.
Data availability
The replication material for the study is available at http://osf.io/uh7e8/. More specifically, it provides (1) screenshots of the experiment/survey, (2) deidentified raw data, and (3) Matlab code used for the analysis of the data.
Declarations
Conflict of interest
None of the four authors has relevant or material financial interests that relate to the research described in this paper.