Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:13:37.293Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grey matter or social matters? Causal attributions in the era of biological psychiatry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Peter Brugger*
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Zurich, Switzerland
Ira Kurthen
Affiliation:
Developmental Psychology: Infancy and Childhood, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Switzerland
Neda Rashidi-Ranjbar
Affiliation:
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, University of Toronto, Canada
Bigna Lenggenhager
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (P. Brugger).

Abstract

Type
Viewpoint
Copyright
Copyright © European Psychiatric Association 2018

Health policy makers predict that, ultimately, new diagnostics will likely redefine any “mental disorder” as a “brain circuit disorder” [Reference Insel and Cuthbert1,p.499]. Such exclusive reductionism not only neglects the sociocultural side of neuropsychiatric disease [Reference Schwartz, Lilienfeld, Meca and Sauvigné2], but is also based on unjustified causal attribution. In view of the correlative nature of most neuroimaging studies, such unidirectional interpretation might be grounded in the “neuroseductive” appeal of brain images [Reference Schwartz, Lilienfeld, Meca and Sauvigné2]. This might have important consequences as it is likely to facilitate the transition of a lifestyle condition to a disease once some neural correlate has been uncovered. Here, we used the example of xenomelia to investigate and illustrate the perception and interpretation of correlations in the field of biological psychiatry.

Xenomelia (from Greek xeno = foreign and melos = limb) designates the pervasive and persistent feeling that one or more of one’s limbs does not belong to one’s bodily self. Xenomelia is accompanied by the conviction that only an amputation would bring about relief. On grounds of its frequent association with an individual's erotic life, it had long been considered a paraphilia, but was re-interpreted as an identity disorder (BIID, for “body integrity identity disorder” [Reference First3]). As such it would comprise, apart from the desire for amputation, the more general longing for a physical disability. Only more recently, xenomelia has attracted the spotlight of neuroscientific enquiry [Reference Brang and McGeoch4; Reference Brugger, Christen, Jellestad and Hänggi5 for review] and ever since studies aiming to determine biological substrates have been on the increase.

We were specifically interested in investigating the causal attribution from correlations in individuals who received third-level education. They should be sensitized to such logical fallacies, since the distinction between correlation and causality is being addressed across the whole range of academic subjects. But when it comes to gut decisions about a potentially stigmatizing condition, how much do we judge the brain to be responsible for the particular behaviour?

In order to gain insight in persons’ causal inferences in the face of correlational evidence, we set out to investigate European academics’ interpretation of a visually depicted correlation from the first structural neuroimaging study on xenomelia. The correlation shows an inverse relation between the strength of a xenomelic individual’s amputation desire as measured by a questionnaire and the size of a circumscribed surface area of parietal cortex (n = 13 men with xenomelia; [Reference Hilti, Hänggi, Vitacco, Kraemer, Palla and Luechinger6], see Fig. 1A and B). We were interested in the balance of any causal inferences from this correlation, i.e. how plausible the primacy of brain-over-behaviour (amputation desires caused by altered neural structure) would be judged compared to that from behaviour-to-brain (structural brain alterations caused by amputation desire). We further explored whether the preference for one primacy type over the other depended on a participant’s academic training, with respect to a trainee's potential role in a medical or neuroscience-related occupation.

Fig 1. Correlation between the strength of an individual’s amputation desire and the surface area of a circumscribed region in the inferior parietal lobe (depicted in B). C: Box-and-whisker plots show distributions of plausibility ratings for two types of causality arguably implied by this correlation, i.e. neural primacy (dark bars) or behavioural primacy (light bars). A and B reprinted, (with permission), from ref. [Reference Hilti, Hänggi, Vitacco, Kraemer, Palla and Luechinger6].

An online survey was programmed and hosted on the LimeSurvey Software Version 2.0+ (Germany). The link to the survey was sent to members of European academic communities representing different fields for further distribution. After providing informed consent, participants read a brief text describing the clinical picture of xenomelia. This text was accompanied by a scattergram depicting the correlation mentioned above (Fig. 1A) alongside the display of an inflated right hemisphere showing the location of the cortical area involved (Fig. 1B). Participants were asked to rate, on the basis of the described finding, the plausibility of each of two statements by placing a slider on a line between 0 (maximally implausible) and 100 (maximally plausible). The statements read “Changes in the brain may produce changes in the acceptance of one’s limbs” and, respectively, “Changes in the acceptance of one’s limbs may produce changes in the brain”. Presentation order of the two statements was randomized across participants.

In total, 769 participants from a European country, who were at least 18 years old, answered the survey. As we were interested in causal attributions of individuals with third-level education, we only included the 631 participants who either held a university degree (n = 381, mean age = 38.89, 200 women) or were studying to obtain one (n = 250, mean age = 26.46, 175 women). Fig. 1C contrasts the plausibility ratings of two groups with a different academic background (medicine, psychology, or neuroscience; n = 380, mean age = 32.0 years, 258 women, or arts, law, economy, or natural sciences other than neuroscience, n = 251, mean age = 37.0 years, 117 women). We refer to the participants of group 1 as “potential medical specialists” and the participants of group 2 as “unlikely medical specialists”. Nonparametric tests were used as the plausibility ratings were not normally distributed. A Wilcoxon signed rank test revealed that, over both groups, neural primacy (NP) was rated more plausible than behavioural primacy (BP; Z = −12.17, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.24). Two separate Mann-Whitney tests showed that NP was comparable between the two groups (Z = −1.14, p = 0.28; η2 = 0.002), whereas the plausibility of BP was rated higher by the potential medical specialists (Z = −4.42, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.03). Neither gender nor age influenced the plausibility ratings significantly (correlation gender and NP: r = 0.068, p = 0.085; gender and BP: r = −0.01, p = 0.794, age and NP: r = −0.035, p = 0.377, age and BP: r = −0.019, p = 0.617).

Together, these findings confirm that, in making causal inferences about brain-behaviour correlations, “the vector of causality is unidirectional: from brain to mind” [Reference Eisenberg7,p. 502], even in academia. The inference of causality regarding the symptoms in conditions such as xenomelia has wide-ranging societal implications [Reference Corrigan and Watson8]. Access to and nature of care will heavily depend on the emerging definitions of such conditions. Fortunately, those whose training prepares them to work in the medical or neuroscientific sector seem slightly less inclined to put grey matter before social matters. There is thus hope that the social and cultural components of xenomelia as well as other conditions in a similar situation will not be underestimated [Reference Brugger, Christen, Jellestad and Hänggi5].

Acknowledgment

Parts of this work was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation, Grant # 320030_127480 to PB, Grant # 170511 to BL.

References

Insel, TRCuthbert, BNBrain disorders? Precisely. Science 2015; 348:4995–00.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, SJLilienfeld, SOMeca, ASauvigné, KCThe role of neuroscience within psychology: a call for inclusiveness over exclusiveness. Am Psychol 2016; 71:5270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
First, MBDesire for amputation of a limb: paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity disorder. Psychol Med 2005; 35:9199–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brang, DMcGeoch, PDRamachandran VS: apotemnophilia: a neurological disorder. NeuroReport 2008; 19:13051306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugger, PChristen, MJellestad, LHänggi, JLimb amputation and other disability desires as a medical condition. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3:11761186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilti, LMHänggi, JVitacco, DAKraemer, BPalla, ALuechinger, Ret al.The desire for healthy limb amputation: structural brain correlates and clinical features of xenomelia. Brain 2013; 136:3183–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, LMindlessness and brainlessness in psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry 1986; 148:4975–08.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrigan, PWWatson, ACAt issue: stop the stigma: call mental illness a brain disease. Schizophr Bull 2004;30(3):4774–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig 1. Correlation between the strength of an individual’s amputation desire and the surface area of a circumscribed region in the inferior parietal lobe (depicted in B). C: Box-and-whisker plots show distributions of plausibility ratings for two types of causality arguably implied by this correlation, i.e. neural primacy (dark bars) or behavioural primacy (light bars). A and B reprinted, (with permission), from ref. [6].

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.