Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T01:36:08.824Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How flexible is the orthographic processing of flankers? Effects for letter order and letter identification

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 March 2023

Miguel Lázaro*
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes and Speech and Language Therapy, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Lorena García
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes and Speech and Language Therapy, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Víctor Illera
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes and Speech and Language Therapy, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study explored the flexibility of the orthographic processing at parafoveal level by manipulating the relationship between flankers and targets in two lexical decision tasks. In the first, we presented the following flankers: (1) the same words as targets (farola farola farola); (2) targets with transposed non-adjacent letters (falora farola falora); (3) the targets with one different letter (fapola farola fapola); and (4) unrelated pseudowords as control stimuli (pilata farola pilata). The results show significant facilitatory effects for all three experimental conditions in comparison to the Unrelated one, as well as differences between the Transposed and One Different Letter when compared to the Identity condition. In the second experiment, the procedure was the same but with the following modifications: the transposed non-adjacent letters were vowels instead of consonants (forala farola forala), and we also presented a condition in which both vowels and consonants were transposed (folara farola folara). The results of the response latencies showed that all the experimental conditions generated facilitatory effects in comparison to the Unrelated condition, with no differences between them, although the analyses of the error rates additionally showed significant differences between the Identity and the Transposed and Vowel and Consonant Transposed conditions. These two experiments are interpreted in terms of a highly flexible orthographic processing of flankers at parafoveal level, both in relation to letter ordering and letter identification.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

A relatively large number of studies have explored orthographic processing by focusing on the transposed letter effect (TL hereafter). This effect refers to the facilitation produced by presenting a masked prime in which two letters in the target are transposed, as in “jugde → judge” (e.g., Forster et al., Reference Forster, Davis, Schoknecht and Carter1987; Perea & Lupker, Reference Perea, Lupker, Kinoshita and Lupker2003; Schoonbaert & Grainger, Reference Schoonbaert and Grainger2004). This finding has been reported not only in adults, but also in children (e.g., Acha & Perea, Reference Acha and Perea2008; Castles et al., Reference Castles, Davis, Cavalot and Forster2007; Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Marcet and Perea2021; Lété & Fayol, Reference Lété and Fayol2013). The TL effect has been interpreted as showing a more flexible coding system for letter ordering than for letter identification, as the results also show that the facilitation is reduced when one letter in the prime is replaced (as in jutge → judge). The term flexible is used here, and throughout this manuscript, to mean that a letter that appears in any position leads to the activation of words containing this letter in different positions.

A number of experimental tasks have been used to provide evidence on the transposed letter effect (i.e., Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Perea and Rayner2007, for a sentence reading task; Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Perea, García-Orza and Barber2012 or Schubert et al., Reference Schubert, Kinoshita and Norris2017, for a same-different task; Perea & Estévez, Reference Perea and Estévez2008, for a naming task; Perea et al., Reference Perea, Palti and Gómez2012, for an associative priming task), although greater emphasis has been placed on the use of the masked priming lexical decision task (e.g. Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Perea and Carreiras2009; Kahraman & Kirciki, Reference Kahraman and Kirciki2021; Schoonbaert & Grainger, Reference Schoonbaert and Grainger2004). The fact that the evidence concerning the TL effect is not circumscribed to just one experimental task is highly positive. This is so because every different task involves specific cognitive demands on readers. Therefore, by considering all results in the context of the different tasks performed, researchers can better interpret and integrate the data collected. The flanker task is, in this sense, an interesting option for two key reasons: i) it allows for the assessment of parallel processing of targets and flankers – unlike priming studies where processing of primes and targets is sequential, and ii) it allows for the study of the parafoveal processing of flankers – unlike tasks in which stimuli are always presented in the fovea. Despite the flanker task having hitherto received little attention, there is solid evidence showing its pertinence for the field of visual word recognition. For example, Dare and Shillcock (Reference Dare and Shillcock2013) used this task to carry out a study in which participants were presented with words and pseudowords flanked by related letter pairs (such as in ro rock ck) or unrelated pairs (such as in an rock ch), with the aim of exploring parallel processing in word recognition. Their results showed facilitation for the related condition with respect to the unrelated condition (see also Dare & Shillcock, Reference Dare and Shillcock2005). This was also true in the related reverse condition, as in ck rock ro. The overall results were interpreted following the assumption that flankers were processed in the parafovea, while targets were fixed and processed in the fovea. In the parafovea, letters presented are processed, with this activation benefiting recognition of the target when it shares the letters with the flankers (related condition). When the letters of the flankers do not match those of the targets, no benefit is observed (unrelated condition). Therefore, shared orthographic activation of flankers and targets explains the benefit at word recognition level. This occurs across both the foveal and parafoveal visual fields, within the duration of just one fixation (Dare, Reference Dare2010).

The data reported by Dare and Shillcock (Reference Dare and Shillcock2013) were replicated in French by Grainger et al. (Reference Grainger, Mathôt and Vitu2014)). These authors also observed a benefit for the related condition in comparison to the unrelated one, but further observed that the benefit was reduced when the position of letters within flankers was reversed (CK ROCK RO vs. KC ROCK OR). This evidence indicates a key role for the order in which the flanker letters appear. The results reported by Dare and Shillcock (Reference Dare and Shillcock2013) and Grainger et al. (Reference Grainger, Mathôt and Vitu2014) were later extended in a study by Grainger et al. (Reference Grainger, Snell and Beyersmann2020), who studied morphological effects through the flanker task. They presented adult readers with flankers and targets with a transparent relationship (lune lunaire lune), with a pseudo-morphological relationship (foule foulard foule), and with a mere orthographic relationship (pot potasse pot). The authors observed that the semantically transparent condition facilitated responses in comparison to pseudo-morphological and form-related conditions, with no differences between the two latter experimental conditions. The authors interpreted that when targets and flankers are presented in different spatial locations, in contrast to what occurs in masked priming experiments, there is no possibility for lexical competition, and, therefore, the semantic transparent condition benefits more than the pseudo-morphological condition. However, more importantly for the present study, the results of Grainger et al. (Reference Grainger, Snell and Beyersmann2020) evidenced that, despite the long chain of characters presented to readers at one time (about 20 characters, considering both flankers, targets and spaces between stimuli), the readers were able to process and activate stems as morphological units. The study, thus, indirectly supported great acuity of parafoveal processing if only one fixation is supposed to occur in the flanker task (Dare, Reference Dare2010).

Contradicting the results of the previous study, Lázaro et al. (Reference Lázaro, García, Illera, García and Acha2022) carried out a replication experiment in Spanish, reporting different results. Indeed, they observed the same facilitatory effects for all three experimental conditions (transparent, pseudo-morphological, and form related conditions) in comparison to the unrelated one, with no statistical differences between them (Experiment 1). These data supported orthographic processing of flankers but not morphological processing. These results were interpreted by assuming that the targets are processed in the fovea, while the flankers are processed through parafoveal processing. As parafoveal processing is known to be less accurate than foveal processing (e.g., Rayner et al., Reference Rayner, Reichle and Pollatsek1998), these authors construed that the ability of parafoveal processing is not sufficiently acute to recognize and activate stems, but only letters from these stems. Therefore, facilitatory effects of experimental conditions emerged from the activation of shared letters in flankers and targets, irrespective of their morphological status. The results of this study led the authors to propose further evaluation of parafoveal processing of flankers. Consequently, the present study was designed and carried out to further explore this issue.

2. Experiment 1

In this first experiment, we explore parafoveal processing of flankers by focusing on the manipulation of both letter order and letter identification. To do so, we transpose non-adjacent letters and replace letters from the flankers with respect to the targets. Our rationale is that if flankers are processed in parafovea, and acuity is limited in comparison to foveal processing, then the same facilitation should appear for the identity and the transposed conditions. This result has been observed in masked priming lexical decision tasks (Forster et al., Reference Forster, Davis, Schoknecht and Carter1987), although the most common finding in the literature is that the TL effect with non-adjacent letters is less powerful than the identical condition (see Rayner et al., Reference Rayner, White, Johnson and Liversedge2006, for a study concerning the costs of reading words with transposed letters). We predict that when a letter is replaced (one letter different condition), the findings will also differ from the results observed in masked priming experiments. In these experiments, this condition generates a reduced benefit in comparison to the transposed. However, we understand that the less accurate orthographic processing at parafoveal level will also imply more flexibility at orthographic identification level and, therefore, we expect the same facilitation for the Identity and the One Letter Different conditions. All in all, we do not expect to replicate previous studies within the paradigm of masked priming but, rather, to obtain similar facilitatory effects for the Identity, the Transposed TL, and the One Letter Different conditions.

3. Method

3.1. Participants

Fifty-four undergraduate students from the Complutense University of Madrid participated in the study (42 women, mean age 22.1 – range 20–29 years old). All were native Spanish-language speakers and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

3.2. Stimuli

Four counterbalanced lists of 120 words were prepared. All the words were nouns with the exception of 4 adjectives. In each list, 30 words were flanked by the same words (farola farola farola – streetlamp), 30 were flanked by pseudowords containing a transposed letter (falora farola falora), 30 were flanked by pseudowords in which one letter from the targets was different (fapola farola fapola) and a further 30 words were flanked by unrelated pseudowords (pilata farola pilata) (see Appendix A for the stimuli employed). Just one list was presented to the participants, such that no stimulus was repeated. The mean frequency of the words was 1.24 per million (SD 1.04), the mean letter length was (6.20, SD 0.57) and the mean number of neighbors was 0.39 (SD 0.63). The characteristics of the word targets were obtained following the Espal database (Duchon et al., Reference Duchon, Perea, Sebastián-Gallés, Martí and Carreiras2013). A set of 120 pseudowords was created to set up the flanker-lexical decision task. Half of the pseudowords were flanked by identical pseudowords and the other half by unrelated pseudowords.

3.3. Procedure

Participants were tested in a quiet room with subdued lighting. The experiment was programmed using the DMDX software package (Forster & Forster, Reference Forster and Forster2003). Centrally positioned vertical fixation bars, separated from one another by a visual angle of 0.60°, were shown throughout each trial. At 500 ms after the start of every trial, a target stimulus with flankers (separated from the target by one character space) was shown between the fixation bars for 150 ms, after which the participants had a maximum of 2,000 ms to respond. Responses were given by means of a right- or a left-handed button press to indicate “word” or “nonword,” respectively. The display returned to the beginning state, and the next trial then started. The stimuli were presented in a random order.

3.4. Data analysis

Only word targets were considered in the analyses. Errors (26.62% of the data) were eliminated for the analysis of the response latencies. There were 4,755 observations for the response time analysis. These observations are sufficient to assure a good statistical power considering the experimental design implemented (Brysbaert, Reference Brysbaert2019).

We fit linear-mixed effects models with regular maximum-likelihood estimation, as implemented in lme4 package (version 1.1–30, Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) in an R statistical computing environment (R Core Team) with log response latencies as a dependent variable to meet the assumption of normality of the residuals. The model included two fixed effects with four levels each: list (4 lists, sum-coded with list 1 as reference [“2” = c(−1/2, 1/2, 0, 0), “3” = c(−1/2, 0, 1/2, 0), “4” = c(−1/2, 0, 0, 1/2)], and flanker type (4 levels, dummy-coded with Unrelated as reference: identity [0, 1, 0, 0], one letter different [0, 0, 1, 0]), transposed letter [0, 0, 0, 1], and two random effects: subjects and items. As a warning message appeared indicating a singular fit when the maximal random-effects model was fitted (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Levy, Scheepers and Tilly2013), the random-effects structure of the model was simplified step by step until the warning message was no longer shown. The final model included only random intercepts for subjects and items. The emmeans package (version 1.8.0, Length, 2022) was used for pairwise comparisons of the experimental conditions, the results of which are presented below.

4. Results

The results of the response latencies showed clear differences between experimental conditions. The mean latencies were Identity 554 ms, Transposed 570 ms, One Letter Different 568 ms, and Unrelated 592 ms. The 38 ms difference between the Identity and the Unrelated was significant (z = 4.98, p < 0.01); as was the difference of 22 ms between the Transposed and the Unrelated (z = 3.27, p < 0.01) and the 24 ms difference between the One Letter Different and the Unrelated condition (z = 2.53, p = 0.01). Therefore, the three related flanker conditions facilitated the recognition of the targets with respect to the control. There were also significant differences between the Identity and One Letter Different conditions (z = −2.44, p = 0.01), with differences between the Identity and the Transposed conditions also approaching significance (z = −1.72, p = 0.08). The differences between One Letter Different and Transposition were not significant (z = 0.73, p = 0.46) (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Response latencies for Experiment 1. Error bars show standard errors.

The error data were analyzed with a logistic regression model that included only subject and item random intercepts. The differences in the error rates replicated those for the RTs: the three related flanker types facilitated the recognition of the targets in comparison to the unrelated condition: Unrelated versus Identity (z = 8.03, p < 0.01); Unrelated versus One Letter Different (z = 2.53, p = 0.01); Unrelated versus Transposition (z = 3.93, p < 0.01). The analyses also revealed a significant difference between Identity versus One Letter Different (z = −5.53, p < 0.01) and between the Identity versus Transposition conditions (z = −4.17, p < 0.01). However, the differences between the One Letter Different and Transposition conditions were not significant (z = 1.39, p = 0.16) (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Mean error rates for Experiment 1. Error bars show standard errors.

5. Discussion

Consistent with the hypotheses of the experiment, the results of the response latencies showed that the Identity, the Transposition and the One Letter Different conditions produced facilitatory effects in comparison to the unrelated condition. However, the data also showed that the benefit generated by the Identity condition was significantly larger than that generated by the One Letter Different and Transposition conditions – in the latter case with a marginal significance. These differences were not expected. Concerning the error rates, the data fully replicated the results observed for the response latencies with just one difference, namely, that in the error analyses the comparison between the Identity and the Transposition conditions reached significance. Masked priming studies have repeatedly shown that transposed stimuli generate facilitation of a lesser magnitude than the Identity condition, but of a greater magnitude than when a letter from the target is replaced. Our results also showed a greater benefit for the Identity condition in comparison to the Transposition condition, but failed to show any difference between the Transposition and the One Letter Different conditions. The results of the masked priming experiments have been interpreted by proposing two different orthographic mechanisms; one related to the identification of letters and the other related to the letter ordering. In particular, it has been suggested that the identification process is highly accurate, as any replacement in a letter from the target decreases the facilitation. Letter ordering, however, has been considered more flexible, as transposition of non-adjacent letters generates greater facilitation than in the case of letter replacement. We understand that our results do not necessarily suggest the previous interpretation is inappropriate. Nevertheless, we believe that the same rationale can be applied to explain our results with just one consideration, that is, flankers, unlike primes, are processed parafoveally. As parafoveal processing is known to be less accurate than foveal processing (e.g., Legge et al., Reference Legge, Mansfield and Chung2001; Legge & Mansfield, Reference Legge and Mansfield1997; Rayner et al., Reference Rayner, McConkie and Zola1980, Reference Rayner, Reichle and Pollatsek1998), if we consider that flankers are processed in the parafovea, it makes sense that the processing of letters in flankers is less accurate than the processing of these letters when presented in the fovea (as happens in the masked priming studies). Our results showing significant differences between the Transposition and the One Letter Different conditions with respect to the unrelated one suggest a flexible orthographic processing at parafoveal level in which the transposition of two internal letters or the replacement of one letter do not preclude benefits in target recognition. Moreover, the lack of differences between the One Letter Different and the Transposition conditions further suggests that the flexibility at parafoveal level is not larger with letter order than with letter identification, in contrast to what has been proposed for foveal processing (masked priming experiments). Lastly, the significant differences between the Identity and the Transposition and One Letter Different conditions are very much informative, as they clearly show that parafoveal processing is sufficiently precise to capture orthographic differences between targets and flankers. Therefore, our overall results show, on the one hand, that the parafoveal processing of letters is flexible in terms of both letter order and letter identification and, on the other, that the parafoveal processing of letters is highly precise. The interaction between flexibility and precision in parafoveal processing merits further investigation.

6. Experiment 2

In this second experiment, we further manipulated the orthographic relationship between flankers and targets by creating two new experimental conditions in addition to the Identity (farola farola farola) and Unrelated ones (pilata farola pilata). The first of the new experimental conditions consists of the transposition of vowels instead of consonants (forala farola forala). Previous evidence in masked priming experiments has shown that transposition of vowels does not generate facilitation or that this facilitation is lower than when transposing consonants (see Acha & Perea, Reference Acha and Perea2010; Carreiras et al., Reference Carreiras, Vergara and Perea2007; Lupker et al., Reference Lupker, Perea and Davis2008; Perea & Lupker, Reference Perea and Lupker2004; but see also Colombo et al., Reference Colombo, Spinelli and Lupker2020 and Schubert et al., Reference Schubert, Kinoshita and Norris2017, for conflicting results), so it is of interest to see whether, in the flanker paradigm, this transposition still generates facilitation as we expect. The second condition exacerbates the manipulation of letter ordering as we transpose not only vowels but also consonants, creating pseudowords sharing letters with the targets but with less resemblance (folara farola folara). If this condition generates facilitation in comparison to the unrelated condition, we then have evidence to support highly flexible orthographic processing at parafoveal level.

7. Methods

7.1. Participants

Forty-five undergraduate students from the Complutense University of Madrid participated in the study (37 women, mean age 23.1 – range 21–31 years old). All were native Spanish-language speakers and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

7.2. Stimuli

The same target stimuli as in Experiment 1 were used, except for 20 items, which were omitted. These words were omitted as they had only one vowel, and were thus impossible to transpose (as occurs in words like ozono – ozone). We therefore had four counterbalanced lists of one hundred words. In each list, 25 words were flanked by identical words (farola farola farola), 25 were flanked by pseudowords containing a transposed vowel (forala farola forala), 25 were flanked by pseudowords in which vowels and consonants were transposed (folara farola folara) and a further 25 words were flanked by unrelated pseudowords (pilata farola pilata) (see Appendix B).

7.3. Procedure

The same procedure was followed as in Experiment 1.

7.4. Data analysis

Errors were eliminated (22.43%) from the analyses of the response latencies. As in the first experiment, the model included two fixed effects with four levels each: list (4 lists, sum coded), and flanker type (dummy-coded; Unrelated, Identity, Transposed, and Vowel and Consonant Transposition) and two random intercepts for subjects and items. There were 3,568 observations for the RT analysis.

7.5. Results

The results of the response latencies showed the following mean raw latencies: Identity 519 ms, Transposition 519 ms, Vowel and Consonant Transposed 509 ms, and Unrelated 541 ms. As in the first experiment, significant differences between the three related conditions with respect to the unrelated one were found. In particular, we found differences between the Unrelated and Identity conditions (z = 3.84, p < 0.01); between the Unrelated and Transposition conditions (z = 3.56, p < 0.01) and between the Unrelated and Vowel and Consonant Transposed conditions (z = 4.15, p < 0.01). No significant differences were found between the three related flanker conditions: Identity versus Transposition (z = −.24, p = 0.80), Identity versus Vowel and Consonant Transposition (z = 0.35, p = 0.73) and Transposition versus Vowel and Consonant Transposition (z = 0.60, p = 0.55) (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3. Response latencies for Experiment 2. Error bars show standard errors.

In the error analysis, the results also show significant differences between the three related conditions with respect to the unrelated one: Unrelated versus Identity (z = 5.98, p < 0.01); Unrelated versus Transposition (z = 2.94, p < 0.01); Unrelated versus Vowel and Consonant Transposed (z = 2.12, p = 0.03). Furthermore, the results also revealed a significant difference between the Identity and Transposition conditions (z = −3.80, p < 0.01) and between the Identity and Vowel and Consonant Transposition conditions (z = −3.86, p < 0.01). Differences between the Vowel and Consonant Transposition and Transposition conditions were not significant (z = −0.83, p = 0.41) (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4. Mean error rates for Experiment 2. Error bars show standard errors.

8. Discussion

The results of this second experiment showed a significant facilitatory effect for all three related conditions with respect to the unrelated one, with no differences between them in the case of the response latencies, but with significant differences in the error analyses between the Identity and the Vowel and Consonant Transposed and Transposition conditions. The results of the response times were unexpected as previous studies have shown that vowel transposition generates less facilitation that consonant transposition (Lupker et al., Reference Lupker, Perea and Davis2008; Perea & Lupker, Reference Perea and Lupker2004; in line with Colombo et al., Reference Colombo, Spinelli and Lupker2020). Regardless of the inconsistent results in the literature in this regard (see Schubert et al., Reference Schubert, Kinoshita and Norris2017), in light of the first experiment, the most reasonably expected result was that the TL condition would generate less benefit than the Identity condition. The fact that the benefit in this second experiment was the same in these conditions suggests that the orthographic processing at parafoveal level does not code vowel transpositions, that is, changes in the order of vowels at parafoveal level do not alter target recognition at foveal level in the least. The problem with this interpretation is that the Vowel and Consonant Transposed condition also generated the same facilitation as the Identity condition, but incorporated not only a vowel, but also a consonant transposition. In the first experiment, we showed that transpositions of consonants at parafoveal level are detected in such a way that the Transposition condition generated less benefit than the Identity one. The similar facilitation for the Identity and Vowel and Consonant Transposition condition was therefore unexpected and challenging to explain. In our view, the evidence concerning the response times must be considered together with the data on the errors. The analyses of the error rates showed less benefit for the Transposition and Vowel and Consonant Transposition with respect to the Identity condition, which fits better with the results of the first experiment and permits a more parsimonious explanation of the results. Indeed, the facilitation observed for the Vowel and Consonant Transposition, despite the reduced resemblance between flankers and targets, suggests highly flexible processing in letter ordering at parafoveal level; the mere concurrence of the same letters in flankers and targets is sufficient to generate facilitation because of the shared activation of letters units.

9. General discussion

In this study, we explored the orthographic processing of flankers by transposing non-adjacent consonants (falora farola falora) and replacing letters (fapola farola fapola) (Experiment 1) and transposing non-adjacent vowels (forala farola forala) and consonants and vowels (folara farola folara) (Experiment 2). These conditions were compared to an unrelated one (pilata farola pilata) and to an identity condition (farola farola farola). The results of Experiment 1 showed facilitation for all three related conditions with respect to the unrelated one (Andrews, Reference Andrews1996), suggesting flexible processing at parafoveal level in terms of both letter order and letter identification. The results of this first experiment also showed that the facilitatory effect generated by the Transposition and One Letter Different conditions was of the same magnitude. This finding differs from the data observed in masked priming studies where the effect generated by the transposed stimuli is typically larger than the effect generated by the One Letter Different ones (e.g., Perea & Lupker, Reference Perea, Lupker, Kinoshita and Lupker2003; Schoonbaert & Grainger, Reference Schoonbaert and Grainger2004). The results of masked priming studies have typically been explained by proposing that the letter position is encoded flexibly during word processing, while letter identification is more accurately processed (e.g., Perea & Estévez, Reference Perea and Estévez2008; Peressotti & Grainger, Reference Peressotti and Grainger1999; Rayner et al., Reference Rayner, White, Johnson and Liversedge2006). Following this rationale, in our study, the lack of differences for the Transposed and One Letter Different conditions suggests that, in the case of parafoveal processing, letter order and letter identification are both processed with similar flexibility. Lastly, with respect to Experiment 1, the results showed a larger facilitatory effect for the Identity than for the Transposed and One Letter Different conditions. This finding suggests that the orthographic processing at parafoveal level is sufficiently precise to benefit more from the identical condition than from other conditions in which the order or the identity of the flankers do not perfectly match the targets. Therefore, this last finding advance on previous ones showing flexibility of the orthographic processing at parafoveal level, because it indicates that the system is not just flexible, but also highly precise.

The results of the second experiment showed facilitation for the three related conditions with respect to the unrelated one. This result clearly supports the view by which the parafoveal processing of letters is highly flexible regarding letter ordering. However, one difficulty in explaining the results of the second experiment is that the data of the response latencies revealed no differences in the magnitude of the benefit for the three related conditions, while the results for the error rates indicated greater benefit for the Identity condition than for the Transposition and Vowel and Consonant Transposed ones. In our view, the fact that the Vowel and Consonant Transposed generated similar facilitation to the Identity in the response latencies is challenging because we showed in the first experiment, with similar items, that consonant transposition reduced the magnitude of the benefit. We believe that the results for the error rates fit better with previous findings but, more importantly, are compatible with the results of the response latencies in one key aspect, namely, that the orthographic processing at parafoveal level is highly flexible regarding letter ordering.

It is worth nothing that the discussion of the results assumes that the flankers are processed in the parafovea. Two arguments support this assumption. The first is the visual acuity hypothesis proposed by Bertram and Hyönä (Reference Bertram and Hyönä2003). These authors explored word recognition of long and short compounds with an eye tracking system, finding different effects in word recognition depending on the letter length of the compound’s first constituent. They concluded that “the system seems to be tuned in such a way that it starts to analyze the string of letters that is under foveal inspection. If this string [the compound] is around 8 letters (or less) it starts to analyze the whole word, whereas if the letter string is longer, it will start to analyze the beginning of the word to acquire access to a meaningful substring” (p. 630). In other words, these authors argued that the letter length of the target to be recognized affects how it is processed (see Lázaro et al., Reference Lázaro, Pérez and Martínez2020, for similar rationale). The reason for this is that the fovea was unable to fixate and extract information from the entire compounds because they had more letters than the fovea is able to capture within a single fixation. Bertram and Hyönä (Reference Bertram and Hyönä2003) observed that differences in their compounds emerged as a length of 8 letters, and so these authors considered that the capacity of the fovea is limited to 8 characters. In our study, the targets had a mean of 6.2 letters, such that they could be fixated and processed at foveal level. However, flankers, together with targets and spaces between them, involved a mean length of 20 characters, clearly outside the span of the fovea. Another factor that supports the parafoveal processing of flankers is that participants were asked to respond to whether the target – not the flankers, were real words or not. Therefore, readers had no need to focus on flankers to correctly perform the task. We believe that participants focused on targets because this is precisely what is needed to appropriately perform the task (see Veldre et al., Reference Veldre, Reichle, Yu and Andrews2022; for similar results with and without eye tracking when assessing the role of the retinal eccentricity on word identification – experiments 1a and 1b). This is our belief even in the extreme case that the left flanker would have been neglected because of the right-hand bias of the foveal processing. In this unlikely case, the target, plus the right flanker, would still have been far beyond the span of the foveal processing (a mean of 13.4 letters considering the space between target and flanker), and, therefore, the right flanker would have been parafoveally processed.

The right-hand bias of the system i.e., the fact that the size of the optimal perceptual span within an eye fixation for parafoveal processing is larger to the right of the fixation than to the left (e.g., Ducrot & Grainger, Reference Ducrot and Grainger2007; Rayner et al., Reference Rayner, Castelhano and Yang2009) has already been explored in a flanker task. This was addressed by Snell and Grainger (Reference Snell and Grainger2018), who carried out a flanker task in which readers were presented with several conditions, among which were the following: both flankers (rock rock rock), just the right flanker (____ rock rock), and just the left flanker (rock rock ___). The authors observed that the leftward repetition flanker generated longer response latencies than the rightward repetition flanker condition. They thus concluded that the recognition process is facilitated by orthographic information on the right, while information on the left provides no benefit, and may even interfere with recognition. Based on the study by Snell et al. (Reference Snell, Mathôt, Mirault and Grainger2018), in which it was observed that the spatial orthographic integration effects are driven by attention, the authors concluded, in parallel to what occurs in normal reading sentences (see Rayner, Reference Rayner1998), that attention in a flanker task is biased toward the right (see Scaltritti et al., Reference Scaltritti, Grainger and Dufau2021; for conflicting results). For our study, we understand that the right bias of attention and parafoveal processing has no impact on the present results, even in the most extreme case, given that the length of targets and right flankers exceeds the span of the fovea.

Another important issue to consider in explaining our results is that of the number of overlapped letters between targets and flankers. The first letters to the right of the visual field (especially the first three ones) have been shown to be particularly important for letter coding (Rayner et al., Reference Rayner, Well, Pollatsek and Bertera1982; see also Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Perea and Rayner2007). In our experiment, the Identity condition shared these first three letters between flankers and targets, but the Transposition and the One Letter Different conditions (first experiment) shared two non-consecutive ones (e.g., abedul abedul abedul vs. adebul abedul adebul vs. apedul abedul apedul). This raises the question of whether the greater benefit observed for the Identity condition emerged from the one more overlapped letter among the three first ones. We do not believe this has played a significant role in the results despite the paper by Snell and Grainger (Reference Snell and Grainger2018) showing a right bias in a flanker task. The evidence for Spanish in this very same task showed no additional benefit for the right flanker in comparison to the left one (Lázaro et al., Reference Lázaro, García, Illera, García and Acha2022) and, moreover, the potential right-hand bias does not necessarily rule out the left flanker playing a role in word recognition. In the previous example (abedul abedul abedul vs. apedul abedul apedul), the last three letters of the left flanker are identical for the Identity and One Letter Different conditions. In sum, we do not believe that the one more overlapped letter in the Identity than in the other conditions can explain the results, although it is reasonable to propose further research on this highly specific issue.

All in all, we understand that the results of our study show highly flexible orthographic processing at parafoveal level, such that relatively extreme manipulations in the orthographic resemblance between flankers and targets does not prevent a facilitatory effect appearing. Evidently, even more extreme manipulations can be carried out to identify a limit in the orthographic resemblance needed to generate facilitation (e.g., by presenting the letters in a random order or by manipulating the proportion and location of shared letters). However, our data clearly show highly flexible orthographic processing at parafoveal level. This flexibility, however, needs to be considered in the light of the results showing a precise letter processing of flankers – the greatest orthographic effects for the Identity condition. Additional research needs to be conducted in order to further characterize the orthographic abilities at parafoveal level.

Data availability statement

Raw data as well as R code can be downloaded here: https://osf.io/gke6v/.

Appendix A. Stimuli of the experiment 1

Appendix B. Stimuli of the experiment 2

References

Acha, J., & Perea, M. (2008). The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in lexical decision: evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 245264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acha, J., & Perea, M. (2010). On the role of consonants and vowels in visual-word processing: Evidence with a letter search paradigm. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(3), 423438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S. (1996). Lexical retrieval and selection processes: Effects of transposed-letter confusability. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 775800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tilly, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2003). The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3), 615634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. Journal of Cognition, 19, 2. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.72Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., & Perea, M. (2007). ERP correlates of transposed-letter similarity effects: Are consonants processed differently from vowels? Neuroscience Letters, 419, 219224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K. I. (2007). Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked form priming and transposed-letter effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 165182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, L., Spinelli, G., & Lupker, S. J. (2020). The impact of consonant–vowel transpositions on masked priming effects in Italian and English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(2), 183198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dare, N. (2010). Out of this word: the effect of parafoveal orthographic information on central word processing. Doctoral dissertation, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Dare, N., & Shillcock, R. (2005). The rock-rock-rock paradigm: Simulating word skipping in text reading. In Poster presented at the 11th architectures and mechanisms for language processing (AMLaP) conference, Ghent, Belgium. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Dare, N., & Shillcock, R. (2013). Serial and parallel processing in reading: Investigating the effects of parafoveal orthographic information on nonisolated word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(3), 487504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, A., & Carreiras, M. (2013). EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 12461258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ducrot, S., & Grainger, J. (2007). Deployment of spatial attention to words in central and peripheral vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(4), 578590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2009). There is no clam with coats in the calm coast: Delimiting the transposed-letter priming effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 19301947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, K., & Forster, J. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, K. I., Davis, C., Schoknecht, C., & Carter, R. (1987). Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 211251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, P., Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2021). Are better young readers more likely to confuse their mother with their mother? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(9), 15421552. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211012960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J., Mathôt, S., & Vitu, F. (2014). Tests of a model of multi-word reading: Effects of parafoveal flanking letters on foveal word recognition. Acta Psychologica, 146, 3540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., Snell, J., & Beyersmann, E. (2020). Morphological processing in the flankers task. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 36, 288295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, R. L., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2007). Transposed-letter effects in reading: evidence from eye movements and parafoveal preview. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(1), 209229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.209Google ScholarPubMed
Kahraman, H., & Kirciki, B. (2021). Letter transpositions and morphemic boundaries in the second language processing of derived words: An exploratory study of individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42, 417446. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716420000673CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lázaro, M., García, A., Illera, V., García, L., & Acha, J. (2022). The effect of semantic transparency in a flanker task: Different roles for stems and derivational suffixes. Experimental Psychology, 69, 132145. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lázaro, M., Pérez, E., & Martínez, R. (2020). Perceptual salience of derivational suffixes in visual word recognition. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 61, 348360. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12617CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legge, G. E., & Mansfield, J. S. (1997). The visual span for reading decreases in peripheral vision. Investigative Ophthalmology and Vision Science, 38, 223.Google Scholar
Legge, G.E., Mansfield, J.S., & Chung, S.T. (2001). Psychophysics of reading. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 4, 725743. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00295-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lété, B., & Fayol, M. (2013). Substituted-letter and transposed-letter effects in a masked priming paradigm with French developing readers and dyslexics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(1) 4762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupker, S. J., Perea, M., & Davis, C. J. (2008). Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1) 93116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñoz, S., Perea, M., García-Orza, J., & Barber, H. A. (2012). Electrophysiological signatures of masked transposition priming in a same-different task: Evidence with strings of letters vs. pseudoletters. Neuroscience Letters, 515, 7176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Estévez, A. (2008). Transposed-letter similarity effects in naming pseudowords: Evidence from children and adults. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(1), 3346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2003). Transposed-letter confusability effects in masked form priming. In Kinoshita, S. & Lupker, S. J. (Eds.), Masked priming: State of the art (pp. 97120). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2004). Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 231246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., Palti, D., & Gómez, P. (2012). Associative priming effects with visible, transposed-letter nonwords: JUGDE facilitates COURT. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 481488. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0271-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peressotti, F., & Grainger, J. (1999). The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(4), 691706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K., Castelhano, M. S., & Yang, J. (2009). Eye movements and the perceptual span in older and younger readers. Psychology and Aging, 24(3), 755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K., McConkie, G. W., & Zola, D. (1980). Integrating information across eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 206226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K., Reichle, E.D., & Pollatsek, A. (1998). Eye movement control in reading: An overview and model. In Eye Guidance in Reading and Scene Perception (pp. 243268). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Well, A. D., Pollatsek, A., & Bertera, J. H. (1982). The availability of useful information to the right of fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 537550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., White, S. J., Johnson, R. L., & Liversedge, S. P. (2006). Raeding wrods with jubmled lettres: There is a cost. Psychological Science, 17, 192193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaltritti, M., Grainger, J., & Dufau, S. (2021). Letter and word identification in the fovea and parafovea. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 83, 20712082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J. (2004). Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(3), 333367. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schubert, T., Kinoshita, S., & Norris, D. (2017). What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(3), 642656.Google Scholar
Snell, J., & Grainger, J. (2018). Parallel word processing in the flanker paradigm has a rightward bias. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 15121519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snell, J., Mathôt, S., Mirault, J., & Grainger, J. (2018). Parallel graded attention in reading: A pupillometric study. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veldre, A., Reichle, E. D., Yu, L., & Andrews, S. (2022). Understanding the visual constraints on lexical processing: New empirical and simulation results. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001295Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Response latencies for Experiment 1. Error bars show standard errors.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Mean error rates for Experiment 1. Error bars show standard errors.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Response latencies for Experiment 2. Error bars show standard errors.

Figure 3

Fig. 4. Mean error rates for Experiment 2. Error bars show standard errors.