Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:28:05.158Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Faith and Friendship: Religious Bonding and Interfaith Relations in Muslim Countries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2020

Nathanael Gratias Sumaktoyo*
Affiliation:
University of Konstanz
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Nathanael Gratias Sumaktoyo, Universität Konstanz, Universitätsstraße 10, Box 94, D-78464, Konstanz, Germany. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Studies have documented more negative attitudes and a higher level of social hostilities toward religious minorities in Muslim than in non-Muslim countries. I seek to explain what contributes to these poor interfaith relations. Diverging from the mainstream approaches that focus on cultural, institutional, or psychological explanations, I argue that the poorer interfaith relations in Muslim countries are driven by high levels of religious bonding or religiously homogeneous friendships among Muslims in these countries. Analyzing a global survey of more than 17,000 Muslims and a report documenting how religious groups in a country restrict or discriminate against each other, I show that religious bonding is related to more negative attitudes toward religious minorities, that a country's level of religious bonding is positively related to its level of social hostilities, and that religious bonding is indeed higher among Muslims in Muslim countries than among Catholics in Catholic-majority Latin American countries.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Studies have documented more negative attitudes and more social hostilities toward religious minorities in Muslim-majority countries (hereafter, “Muslim countries”) than in non-Muslim majority countries (hereafter, “non-Muslim countries”). At the individual level, compared to non-Muslims, Muslims on average have less positive attitudes toward religiously different others (Gu and Bomhoff Reference Gu and Bomhoff2012; Verkuyten et al. Reference Verkuyten, Maliepaard, Martinovic and Khoudja2014) or toward minorities in general (Inglehart Reference Inglehart2003). At the societal level, Muslim religious groups in Muslim countries are more likely to restrict other religious groups (Pew Research Center 2018).

What, then, contributes to these poor interfaith relations? Diverging from the mainstream approaches that focus on cultural-theological, institutional, and individual-level antecedents, I argue that the more negative interfaith relations in Muslim countries are driven by the religiously homogeneous friendship networks of Muslims in these countries. Drawing from two cross-national surveys, I show that Muslims in Muslim countries have higher levels of religious bonding or ties with co-religionists than non-Muslims in non-Muslim countries do. These homogeneous networks are theorized to affect interfaith relations through reinforcement of group identity and confirmation bias.

The paper is structured as follows. I first review studies on interfaith relations in Muslim countries, highlighting a gap in the quality of interfaith relations within Muslim countries compared to non-Muslim countries. This is followed by a discussion on how other scholars have explained this gap. I then highlight how networks matter and support the argument by presenting three pieces of evidence rooted in the social networks literature.

First, I show that levels of religious bonding among Muslims, i.e., Muslims' ties with fellow Muslims, significantly predict more negative attitudes toward non-Muslims. Second, I show that religious bonding is positively related to the social hostilities index (SHI) at the country level—a measure of ways in which individuals and groups in a society infringe upon religious beliefs and practices (Pew Research Center 2018, 1). Third, I show that Muslims in Muslim-majority countries indeed have more religiously homogeneous friendship networks compared to Catholics in Catholic-majority Latin American countries.

INTERGROUP AND INTERFAITH RELATIONS IN THE MUSLIM WORLD

The current study focuses on interfaith relations broadly defined. At the individual level, I relate interfaith relations to individuals' attitudes toward religiously different others. At the country level, interfaith relations are defined in the context of social hostilities or how the majority religious group persecutes or discriminates against religious minorities. Because the focus is on interfaith relations or social discrimination, the current study is therefore different from studies that focus on state discrimination of religious minorities (Fox Reference Fox2016).

A number of studies have examined interfaith relations in Muslim countries. Some focus on socioeconomic or cultural factors (Ojo and Lateju Reference Ojo and Lateju2010; Menchik Reference Menchik2016; Ventura Reference Ventura2018) whereas others look at how actors or institutions shape the patterns of interfaith relations (Scott Reference Scott2010). Muslim–Christian relations command the most attention, but there are also studies that examine Muslim–Buddhist (Stewart Reference Stewart2014) or Muslim–Hindu relations (Varshney Reference Varshney2001), indicating scholarly and public interests in these topics. Although these studies have contributed valuable insights, most focus on single countries and rely on qualitative case studies. Such an approach often provides a rich picture of interfaith dynamics in the country studied but offers few clues as to how these dynamics vary across countries. Comparative studies of interfaith or intergroup relations are relatively rare.Footnote 1

Studies that engage in a comparative approach have found an intriguing pattern of interfaith relations across countries, documenting less positive intergroup relations in Muslim than in non-Muslim countries. Inglehart (Reference Inglehart2003, 56; also see Spierings Reference Spierings2014) finds that levels of trust and tolerance in Muslim countries are generally lower than ones in more established democracies. Bohnet, Herrmann, and Zeckhauser (Reference Bohnet, Herrmann and Zeckhauser2010) find lower trust between individuals and between groups in the Gulf than in Western countries. Gu and Bomhoff (Reference Gu and Bomhoff2012) arrive at the same conclusion when comparing Muslim-majority to Catholic-majority countries. Muslims are found to be less trustful of people of another religion and of another nationality, and are more likely to believe that ethnic diversity erodes a country's unity.

There is also evidence that these differences between Muslims and non-Muslims hold up outside Muslim countries. Djupe and Calfano (Reference Djupe and Calfano2012) find that Muslims in the United States are more intolerant of people who commit acts against religion. Comparing Muslims and Christians in six Western European countries, Koopmans (Reference Koopmans2015) finds higher hostilities toward homosexuals and Jews among Muslims than among Christians (p. 47). Analyzing the World Values Survey, Milligan, Andersen, and Brym (Reference Milligan, Andersen and Brym2014) find that Muslims have higher objections than non-Muslims do when it comes to having religiously different neighbors. Gu and Bomhoff (Reference Gu and Bomhoff2012) arrive at the same conclusion when comparing individuals in Muslim- and Catholic-majority countries.

The poorer interfaith relations in Muslim countries are also evident when we look at measures of social hostilities (Pew Research Center 2018). Based on Grim and Finke's (Reference Grim and Finke2006) study, the SHI is concerned with religious restrictions imposed by the society (including individuals and social groups) as distinct from the government. Rather than attitudes, the index reflects incidents in which religious groups (predominantly the majority groups) discriminate against other groups (predominantly the minority groups).

Figure 1 compares levels of social hostilities in Muslim and non-Muslim countries between 2007 and 2016. As the figure shows, Muslim countries consistently have higher social hostilities than Christian-majority countries or non-Muslim countries in general. At the same time, it also shows a degree of social hostilities in non-Muslim countries. This suggests that minority groups everywhere continue to face discriminations, albeit to varying degrees. The findings presented here, therefore, must be understood in the context of a comparison as opposed to an argument of the superiority of one culture or religion over the others.

Figure 1. Trends of SHI (Pew Research Center 2018)

EXPLAINING INTERFAITH RELATIONS

Three major perspectives have been widely used to understand interfaith relations. The first two views—cultural-theological and institutional—focus on Muslim societies; whereas the third focuses on individual-level antecedents.

Cultural-Theological

The cultural-theological perspective includes approaches that emphasize the importance of Islamic theology (Huntington Reference Huntington1997) and the effects of modernization on societal values (Inglehart and Welzel Reference Inglehart and Welzel2005). These approaches contend that Muslims' lack of openness to other faiths can be traced back to the absence of certain liberal values in the society. The theological approach looks at Islamic doctrines and argues that the all-encompassing nature of the Qur'an (March Reference March2015, 106) encourages Muslims to be obedient to religious authority. Such a sweeping perception overlooks the reality that Muslims across the world live and understand their religion differently (Sadowski Reference Sadowski2006). A theological approach that attributes poor interfaith relations in Muslim countries to the Qur'an may inadvertently give legitimacy to a certain view in Islam that is intolerant and discredit the more tolerant views as illegitimate.

The modernization theory regards culture as important but adds another explanatory variable: socioeconomic development. Higher socioeconomic development leads to cultural changes, one of them being more positive intergroup attitudes. An obvious limitation of this theory is that it cannot explain the Gulf countries that are less tolerant than one would expect given their levels of development. That is why Inglehart and Welzel (Reference Inglehart and Welzel2005, 19) regard cultural starting points as insurmountable, positing that “The fact that a society was historically Protestant or Orthodox or Islamic or Confucian manifests itself in coherent cultural zones with distinctive value systems that persist even when one controls for the effects of socioeconomic development.” This muddles what we should expect regarding the effects of modernization on interfaith relations in Muslim countries. As Muslim societies become more modernized, should we expect interfaith relations in the countries to be at least as strong as those in non-Muslim countries, or should we expect them to improve only relative to their state in the past?

Institutional

Two institutional factors are of interest in the context of the current study: secularism and political competition. These factors shape interfaith relations through a similar mechanism, namely open competition. The more open a polity is, the greater the opportunity for citizens to encounter diverse viewpoints. This exposure to diversity, along with the learning process individuals experience in responding to that diversity, in turn helps to internalize tolerance in the society (Peffley and Rohrschneider Reference Peffley and Rohrschneider2003).

The level of secularism is concerned with how well separated the state and religious institutions are.Footnote 2 A state's neutrality in religious affairs opens up space for religions to compete in attracting believers (Iannaccone Reference Iannaccone1998). The resulting religious life is more vibrant with believers actively engaging each other. A strict regulation of religion, on the other hand, may privilege certain faiths over others or privilege believers over unbelievers (Grim and Finke Reference Grim and Finke2010). It would be difficult for citizens to tolerate each other when even the government demonstrates through its laws that not all citizens are equal. As such, the poor interfaith relations in Muslim countries are driven by laws that advantage Islam over the other faiths.

The logic is the same with the perspective that emphasizes political competition. The perspective suggests that Islamists become more moderate the more they participate in electoral competition (see Schwedler Reference Schwedler2011, for a review). Electoral incentives induce Islamists to moderate their platforms to attract more votes and political reality forces parties to compromise. According to this perspective, then, the poor interfaith relations in Muslim countries are due to the lack of political competition, which means that parties have few incentives to build inclusive coalitions.

What both perspectives miss, however, is that although free competition can induce moderation, it can also induce religious populism (Tanuwidjaja Reference Tanuwidjaja2010; Lorch Reference Lorch2019). As opposed to moderating themselves, parties may choose to outflank each other. Another limitation of the institutional explanations is that their level of analysis makes them more suited to explain country-level differences than individual-level ones. They are mute when it comes to answering why different individuals in the same country have different interfaith attitudes despite being exposed to the same institutional arrangements. The social relationships-based argument that I propose, on the other hand, is capable of explaining variations in interfaith relations at both the individual and country levels.

Individual-Level Antecedents

In addition to the aforementioned country-level factors, individual-level variables also affect how individuals approach intergroup relations. Three of these variables have been heavily studied (Sullivan and Transue Reference Sullivan and Transue1999). The first relates to cognitive ability and includes factors such as education and political expertise. More politically sophisticated people tend to be more tolerant (Fish Reference Fish2011), presumably because they are exposed to dissonant views more often and have a better internalization of democratic values.

The second variable is threat perception (Marcus et al. Reference Marcus, Sullivan, Theiss-Morse and Wood1995; Muluk, Sumaktoyo, and Ruth Reference Muluk, Sumaktoyo and Ruth2013). Individuals are more likely to have positive attitudes toward an outgroup if they perceive the group as posing little or no threat to them or their way of life. Politicization of social cleavages can affect this threat perception. When certain groups are portrayed as ancient enemies, members of each group would perceive the other group as threatening, making intolerance more likely.

Third, in terms of personality type, negative intergroup attitudes are linked to closed-mindedness and dogmatism (Anderson and Koc Reference Anderson and Koc2015). These predispositions increase the likelihood of individuals conforming to established social norms while rejecting dissenting views. Since religiosity involves a strong adherence to beliefs (Saroglou Reference Saroglou2002), these predispositions can also explain why religious people tend to have more negative interfaith attitudes (Gibson Reference Gibson, Wolfe and Katznelson2010). Thus, Muslim societies' high levels of religiosity might explain their poor interfaith relations.

WHY SOCIAL TIES MATTER

The aforementioned approaches have contributed to our understanding of interfaith relations and intergroup relations in general. However, as discussed above, they have certain limitations in their explanatory powers. They also are incomplete because they overlook an important component of social life: ties among the individuals themselves. Social ties, including religious ties, affect not only the individuals' political behavior (Mckenzie Reference Mckenzie2004; Lewis, MacGregor, and Putnam Reference Lewis, MacGregor and Putnam2013) but also how the society functions (Putnam Reference Putnam1993, Reference Putnam2000; Brehm and Rahn Reference Brehm and Rahn1997; Coleman Reference Coleman1988).

Not all social ties are equal, however. Ties that are inward looking (bonding relationships, i.e., relationships with ingroup members) are more likely to produce undesirable effects on intergroup attitudes compared to ties that are outward looking (bridging relationships or relationships with outgroup members). There are three reasons why bonding relationships may be related to more negative evaluations of an outgroup and, by implication, why religious bonding may be related to poor interfaith relations.

The first relates to the flow of norms and information (Huckfeldt and Sprague Reference Huckfeldt and Sprague1987). Individuals receive political and social cues from their surroundings (Berelson, Lazarsfeld, and McPhee Reference Berelson, Lazarsfeld and McPhee1954; Granovetter Reference Granovetter1973). Psychological research on social learning theory (Bandura Reference Bandura1976) also suggests that one's social environment shapes what are considered appropriate or inappropriate behavior and attitudes. As such, the higher one's level of bonding relationships, the more likely it is for the social network to facilitate the flow of norms and information that are favorable to the ingroup and unfavorable to the outgroup (Huckfeldt, Mendez, and Osborn Reference Huckfeldt, Mendez and Osborn2004).

A second mechanism involves ingroup identity. In an experimental setting Levendusky, Druckman, and McLain (Reference Levendusky, Druckman and McLain2016) show that interactions with politically similar others enhance partisan identity. Coupled with preferential treatments for ingroup members, such an ingroup attachment may lead to rejection of the outgroup (Brewer Reference Brewer1999). In the religious context, religious social identity has been shown to lead to the rejection of religious outgroups (Rhodes Reference Rhodes2012; Ben-Nun Bloom, Arikan, and Courtemanche Reference Ben-Nun Bloom, Arikan and Courtemanche2015).

The third reason why bonding may lead to negative intergroup attitudes concerns bridging relationships. Since maintaining relationships is costly in that one has to devote time to one's friends (Wellman et al. Reference Wellman, Wong, Tindall and Nazer1997, 36) or one's group (Campbell Reference Campbell2004), it is likely that there would be a trade-off between bonding and bridging. The more the resources that one devotes to one's ingroup, the less that one can devote to one's relationships with outgroup members.

Bridging relationships, on the other hand, have been linked to tolerance and acceptance of outgroups (Allport Reference Allport1954; Cigler and Joslyn Reference Cigler and Joslyn2002; Mutz Reference Mutz2006; Ikeda and Richey Reference Ikeda and Richey2009; Harell Reference Harell2010; Putnam and Campbell Reference Putnam and Campbell2010; Rapp and Freitag Reference Rapp and Freitag2015). Positive interactions with people from a different group reduce anxiety about the group and increase the ability of individuals to empathize with and understand the other group's viewpoints (Pettigrew and Tropp Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2008). That high levels of bonding may take up the time and resources needed to develop bridging relationships might, in turn, hinder the developments of these positive effects.

Based on the preceding review, I argue that one of the drivers for poor interfaith relations in Muslim countries is the countries' high levels of religious bonding. Supporting this argument requires three pieces of evidence. First, given the review above about how social ties shape attitudes, I need to show that religious bonding is related to more negative interfaith attitudes.

Hypothesis 1 (Negative Attitudes): At the individual level, greater religious bonding is associated with more negative attitudes toward members of religious outgroups.

Second, I need to show that religious bonding is also positively related to a more behavioral measure of interfaith relations, namely a country's level of social hostilities. Studies have shown how attitudes should shape behavior especially if they are salient and involve one-sided information (Glasman and Albarracín Reference Glasman and Albarracín2006). As bonding keeps religious identity salient and influences the type of information individuals are exposed to, it is plausible that religious bonding is related not only to more negative interfaith attitudes but also to higher levels of social hostilities.

Hypothesis 2 (Social Hostilities): At the country level, greater religious bonding is associated with higher levels of social hostilities.

Finally, I need to show that religious bonding in Muslim countries is indeed higher than in non-Muslim countries. This is a plausible hypothesis considering the central roles of religious organizations and mosque networks in shaping the political dynamics of Muslim countries (Sadowski Reference Sadowski2006). These organizations arguably are more conducive to bonding than bridging. Furthermore, the idea of a global ummah (Roy Reference Roy2006) that envisions Muslims around the world as one big family may also lead to higher bonding among Muslims.

Hypothesis 3 (Higher Bonding): Religious bonding is higher in Muslim than in non-Muslim countries.

DATA

I analyze data from two surveys. The first is a study of “The World's Muslims” (TWM; Pew Research Center 2013), which was conducted between October 2011 and November 2012 and surveyed 32,604 Muslims in 26 countries. I excluded Bosnia-Herzegovina, Russia, and Thailand from the analysis because they are not Muslim majority (Pew Research Center 2014b).

The second dataset is a survey of “Religion in Latin America” (RILA; Pew Research Center 2014a) that covers 30,326 respondents in 19 Latin American countries and was conducted between October 2013 and February 2014. This dataset is primarily used to enable a comparison between Muslims and non-Muslims necessary for testing the Higher Bonding hypothesis.

I focus on 17 countries that are Catholic majority and on respondents who were Catholics—Catholics in Catholic-majority Latin American countries. This criterion is justified in light of my goal to find a comparison for Muslims in Muslim-majority countries. “Catholics” is relatively well defined compared to “Protestants” or “Evangelicals” that encompass different denominations (Steensland et al. Reference Steensland, Robinson, Wilcox, Park, Regnerus and Woodberry2000). There is also a practical reason in that the TWM and RILA surveys are the only comparative surveys that include a question on social ties. Both surveys employed face-to-face interviews and were designed to be nationally representative. Wording for all the questions below is available in the online Appendix, along with sample size from each country.

A NOTE ON CAUSATION

This study analyzes observational data, which means that some limitations commonly found in an analysis of such data also apply here. The first concerns causal ordering. It is possible that a relationship between bonding and interfaith attitudes is driven by self-selection or people with negative interfaith attitudes being more likely to befriend religiously similar others in the first place. Second, unlike in an experimental study where randomization voids the effects of confounding factors, both observed and unobserved, an analysis of observational data relies on the observables. Only those that are measured can be controlled.

I employ three strategies to address these concerns. First, I rely on insights from previous studies that have demonstrated the causal effect of social relationships on attitudes and behavior (Van Laar et al. Reference Van Laar, Levin, Sinclair and Sidanius2005; Lazer et al. Reference Lazer, Rubineau, Chetkovich, Katz and Neblo2010; Putnam and Campbell Reference Putnam and Campbell2010). These insights suggest that self-selection, although possible, does not constitute the whole story. Even if people build their networks based on attitudinal similarities, these networks would still have an effect on their attitudes. The homogeneous friendship networks of Muslims in Muslim countries, in turn, should lead to more negative interfaith attitudes, even if these homogeneous networks are in part a product of negative interfaith attitudes. Second, I leverage the temporal ordering of the variables in the Social Hostilities hypothesis. Here, the dependent variable was measured 1 year after the independent variable. This correct temporal ordering provides us with a stronger causal identification. Finally, I take alternative explanations into account, as far as the data allow, by including an extensive set of control variables in all of the analyses.

I nonetheless acknowledge that without experimental data it is impossible to completely rule out the possibilities of reverse causation and spurious correlations. As such, I position the current study as a pioneering examination of a relationship between bonding and interfaith relations in Muslim countries. By proposing a theory grounded on the literature on social networks and political behavior, and by testing this theory using the best available data, I aspire to make a case for the plausibility of the theory as well as encourage further research on the topic.

NEGATIVE ATTITUDES HYPOTHESIS

Dependent Variable

To test the Negative Attitudes hypothesis, I relied on three questions from the TWM survey. The first two questions tap into interfaith marriage and asked respondents “How comfortable would you be if a son (daughter) of yours someday married a Christian?” I recoded the responses into a 5-point scale ranging from “very comfortable” to “not at all comfortable.” The third question taps into perception of similarity and asked respondents “From what you know, do you think that the Muslim religion and the Christian religion have a lot in common, or do you think that the Muslim religion and the Christian religion are very different?” Possible responses were “have a lot in common” (coded as “0”) and “are very different” (coded as “1”).

A factor analysis based on the variables' polychoric correlation matrix reveals that these questions have a unidimensional structure that explains 64% of the variance. The eigenvalues are 1.93, 0.90, and 0.17; and the loadings for son intermarriage, daughter intermarriage, and perception of similarity are 0.94, 0.93, and 0.43, respectively. The correlation between the two intermarriage variables is r = 0.83, and the correlations between son and daughter intermarriage on the one hand and perception of similarity on the other are r = 0.23 and r = 0.19, respectively. Given this evidence for unidimensionality, I use as the dependent variable the factor score generated from these loadings by the regression method. The method calculates each respondent's factor score by multiplying their responses on the three interfaith items with the corresponding items' factor loadings and then taking the sum of the resulting values.Footnote 3

As the reader may have noticed, the interfaith variables specify Christians as the target group. Does the relationship apply to other minority groups? Although this is an understandable concern, specifying Christians as the target group should not harm the theory's generalizability for two reasons. First, Gibson (Reference Gibson1992) finds that an a priori specification of the target group affects studies that focus on descriptive statistics (e.g., how many people have positive attitudes toward a particular group) but not studies that examine antecedents of intergroup attitudes. Second, although religious conflicts are rarely purely religious, several conflicts between Muslims and Christians carry with them strong religious sentiments (Ayoub Reference Ayoub1989), making Muslims–Christians relations an important topic to study.Footnote 4 The importance of this topic becomes even more obvious once we consider how the war on terror is sometimes framed as a Christian West's war on Islam.

Independent Variable

As the independent variable, I use a question that asked respondents the level of religious bonding in their friendship networks.Footnote 5 The question read, “How many of your close friends are Muslim?” Possible responses were “none of them,” “hardly any of them,” “some of them,” “most of them,” or “all of them.” Higher scores indicate higher levels of religious bonding.

Two concerns regarding the variable are worth discussing. The first relates to the variable not capturing network density or how well the friends themselves know each other (Everton Reference Everton2018). This means that I cannot examine whether the effects of bonding on interfaith attitudes are constrained to dense religious networks or are more generalizable across levels of network density. On the one hand, it is possible that dense social networks more strongly reinforce group identity than sparse networks do through the development of strong ties. On the other hand, it is possible that sparse networks more effectively facilitate the flow of information that can advantage the ingroup or put the outgroup in negative light (Granovetter Reference Granovetter1973). Future studies will benefit from exploring the plausibility of such interactions by collecting and analyzing whole network data.

The second concern relates to whether the independent variable itself is a measure of interfaith attitudes like the dependent variables. If that was the case, then a relationship between bonding and interfaith attitudes would be given. There are three reasons to argue why this is not the case. First, from the wording alone, we can see that the dependent variables measure perceptions or attitudes and the independent variable measures network composition. Specifically, the bonding question is an example of a global estimate measure of social network (Eveland, Hutchens, and Morey Reference Eveland, Hutchens and Morey2013).

Second, a connection between social relationships and attitudes is not given and subject to various conditions (Pettigrew Reference Pettigrew1998). This means that the question of whether or not religious bonding would lead to more negative interfaith attitudes is also an empirical one. This is particularly true among Muslims as some scholars have argued that religion would be the primary driver of Muslim political behavior (Huntington Reference Huntington1997).

Finally, even if the concern was plausible at the individual level, it would be less likely so at the country level where the dependent variable is the level of social hostilities. As mentioned above, the SHI captures actual incidents of social discriminations and is based on a coding of various human rights reports and news sources. As such, it is not an attitudinal measure and is even more conceptually different from religious bonding than the interfaith attitudes measures are.

Control Variables

To account for alternative explanations, I included in all individual-level models nine individual-level and four country-level control variables. The country-level models include only country-level covariates. The individual-level covariates include age, gender, whether living in a rural or urban area, education, political efficacy, personal economic condition, support for democracy, frequency of prayer, and believing that Islam is the one true faith. Education is intended to capture cognitive sophistication and is coded as less than high school, at least high school, or at least college degree. Political efficacy is intended to capture political interest and involvement, which should be related to more positive attitudes toward diversity (Galston Reference Galston2001). Personal economic condition serves as an indicator for existential security (Norris and Inglehart Reference Norris and Inglehart2012), which should be positively related to outgroup attitudes. Support for democracy was measured with a binary variable that asked respondents whether they thought that the people should rely on a democratic government or a strong leader to solve the country's problems. Finally, frequency of prayer and believing that one's own religion is the one true faith tap into different types of religiosity. The first relates to piety, whereas the latter is more about orthodoxy and theological exclusivity. The two may have different consequences when it comes to how individuals translate their faiths into actions (Muluk, Sumaktoyo, and Ruth Reference Muluk, Sumaktoyo and Ruth2013). Specifically, Merino (Reference Merino2010) finds that theological exclusivity among Americans predicts less acceptance of Muslims and Hindus.

The four country-level covariates include logged GDP per capita; religious diversity index (RDI; Pew Research Center 2014a); government restrictions on religion (GRI; Pew Research Center 2018)Footnote 6; and Polity IV score. All variables except for the RDI are from 1 year before the survey (i.e., the 2010 values for the Muslim countries and the 2012 values for the Catholic countries). The RDI is from 2010 as this is the only data available.

GDP per capita is used as an indicator of modernization corresponding to the modernization explanation outlined above. The RDI, based on the Herfindahl–Hirschman formula, measures the relative sizes of religious traditions in a country. It takes into account not only the size of the majority but also the number of minority groups and their sizes. The index is coded so that higher values represent more diversity. Finally, the GRI and Polity IV scores capture the effects of institutional factors. Interfaith relations in a country should be negatively related to how tightly the government regulates religion and positively to its level of democracy.

Analysis and Results

I employed a multilevel linear regression to test the hypothesis, which is appropriate because respondents are nested within countries and respondents within the same country are likely more similar to each other than they are to respondents from other countries.Footnote 7 I standardized all non-binary variables to have mean zero and standard deviation one.

The regression coefficients are presented in Table 1. The table supports the prediction that higher levels of religious bonding are related to more negative interfaith attitudes. The coefficient of religious bonding is also one of the largest among the individual-level predictors, comparable to how often one prays and only weaker than believing that Islam is the one true faith.

Table 1. Multilevel linear regression of interfaith attitudes

Non-binary predictors have been standardized to have mean zero and standard deviation one.

a Albania, Algeria, Azerbaijan, Bangladesh, Egypt, Indonesia, Iraq, Jordan, Kazakhstan, Kosovo, Kyrgyzstan, Lebanon, Malaysia, Niger, Pakistan, Tajikistan, Tunisia, and Turkey.

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; +p < 0.10.

Higher education and better personal economic condition are related to less negative interfaith attitudes. This is consistent with earlier observations about how education is positively related to tolerance (Sullivan and Transue Reference Sullivan and Transue1999). Religiosity and belief orthodoxy, on the other hand, are related to more negative interfaith attitudes, affirming previous findings on the importance of religiosity and theological exclusivity in shaping negative interfaith attitudes (Gibson Reference Gibson, Wolfe and Katznelson2010).

The positive relationship between political efficacy and negative interfaith attitudes is likely related to the high levels of government restriction of religion in Muslim countries. Those with high efficacy are more likely to be interested in politics, immersing themselves in a system that generally favors Islam at the expense of other religions. As opposed to inoculating in them a respect for diversity, this activism does the opposite. This highlights the complex relationships between political system, political participation, and political attitudes (Jamal Reference Jamal2007). In a secular democracy, political activism might lead to more positive interfaith attitudes. Activism in a less democratic, less secular societies, on the other hand, might inoculate just the opposite attitudes.

In terms of country-level covariates, government restriction on religion is the only statistically significant variable. The higher the level of government restriction in a country, the more negative the interfaith attitudes of respondents in the country are. This highlights the importance of institutional settings in shaping individuals' attitudes (Peffley and Rohrschneider Reference Peffley and Rohrschneider2003; Dunn and Singh Reference Dunn and Singh2014).Footnote 8

THE SOCIAL HOSTILITIES HYPOTHESIS

Variables

The Social Hostilities hypothesis extends the Negative Attitudes hypothesis by showing that the relationship between religious bonding and negative interfaith relations holds up at the country level. The dependent variable in this analysis is the SHI score published by the Pew Research Center (2018). I use the score from the year after the survey ended (i.e., the 2014 score for the Muslim countries and the 2015 score for the Catholic-majority Latin American countries). The independent variable is the country-level score of religious bonding, calculated as the average of the levels of religious bonding of respondents in the country, accounting for sampling weight. This operationalization provides a stronger causal identification by establishing the correct temporal order in which the independent variable (country-level bonding score) was measured before the dependent variable (the country's social hostilities score 1 year after the survey). As controls, I use the same country-level covariates as in the Negative Attitudes hypothesis.

Analysis and Results

I employed ordinary least squares (OLS) regression models to test the hypothesis, standardizing all variables to have mean zero and standard deviation one. Table 2 presents two models that test the hypothesis. The first model regresses the SHI at the country-level covariates. The second model uses the same specification but adds the SHI score from the year before the survey as another control variable. If religious bonding still has a significant effect on the SHI even after controlling for the previous SHI score, it would suggest that the variable has a unique explanatory power on the SHI that is unaccounted for by either the stability of social hostilities or other variables controlled in the model.

Table 2. Country-level OLS regression of social hostilities on religious bonding

The dependent variable and the predictors have been standardized to have mean zero and standard deviation one.

a TWM survey: Albania, Algeria, Azerbaijan, Bangladesh, Egypt, Indonesia, Iraq, Jordan, Kazakhstan, Kosovo, Kyrgyzstan, Lebanon, Malaysia, Morocco, Niger, Pakistan, Tajikistan, Tunisia, Turkey, and Uzbekistan. RILA survey: Argentina, Bolivia, Brazil, Chile, Colombia, Costa Rica, Dominican Republic, Ecuador, El Salvador, Guatemala, Mexico, Nicaragua, Panama, Paraguay, Peru, and Venezuela.

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; +p < 0.10.

In model 1, we see that a country's level of religious bonding has a positive relationship with social hostilities (η 2 = 0.211). The more religiously homogeneous the friendship networks of respondents in a country, the higher the country's level of social hostilities is 1 year after the survey. The importance of government restriction on religion is again evident from its relationship with social hostilities (η 2 = 0.146). Countries with more religious restrictions have higher social hostilities. Finally, the model also suggests a positive relationship between level of democracy and social hostilities (η 2 = 0.301). As the countries' levels of democracy were relatively stable in the years of the study, this relationship is unlikely to be driven by time-specific dynamics and might reflect the more open and less repressive nature of more democratic governments. This greater openness provides more space for dissents, differences, and to an extent conflicts to be more overtly expressed.

Model 2 shows that including the lagged score of SHI weakens the explanatory powers of all the variables. However, the model also shows that religious bonding is the only predictor in the model that continues to maintain its statistical significance, albeit only marginally (p = 0.057). This suggests that although religious bonding's relationship with social hostilities is in part due to its shared variance with the lagged social hostilities (just like the other variables' relationships are), it has sufficiently unique variance that is not due to the previous level of social hostilities. In fact, religious bonding's partial eta-squared (η 2 = 0.120), an indicator of a predictor's unique shared variance with the dependent variable, is second only to that of the lagged SHI (online Appendix S8).

This unique variance of religious bonding is at the core of this study. The current study does not argue against the possibility of a reverse causation. Rather, what the current study contends is that, just like previous studies have shown, reverse causation is unlikely to fully account for the effect of religious bonding. Religious bonding would still have a unique effect on interfaith attitudes even if interfaith attitudes also shape the composition of one's friendship network. This unique effect, in turn, should contribute to the gap in the quality of interfaith relations between Muslim and non-Muslim societies.

HIGHER BONDING HYPOTHESIS

Variables

Having shown how religious bonding predicts more negative interfaith attitudes at the individual level and more negative interfaith relations at the country level, the next exercise is to test whether Muslim countries indeed have higher bonding than non-Muslim countries, which in this case are represented by Catholic-majority Latin American countries. In this hypothesis, the dependent variables are the individual- and the country-level religious bonding scores whereas a dummy variable that indicates whether a country is Muslim- or Catholic-majority becomes the independent variable. I include the same individual and country level covariates as in the previous two analyses.

Regression Models of Religious Bonding

Figure 2 presents a comparison of religious bonding in 21 Muslim- and 17 Catholic-majority countries. The x-axis represents the level of religious diversity and the y-axis represents the level of religious bonding. The figure shows that Muslim countries almost consistently have higher bonding than Catholic-majority countries with comparable shares of Catholics.

Figure 2. Religious bonding in Muslim- and Catholic-majority countries (see online Appendix 10 for actual values).

I run a multilevel ordinal regression and an OLS model to more formally test the hypothesis at the individual and the country levels, respectively. Table 3 presents results from the models. Models 1 and 3 include all countries that have the required variables, whereas Models 2 and 4 exclude Muslim countries that are less religiously diverse than the least religiously diverse Catholic country. This exercise tests whether the Higher Bonding hypothesis is simply driven by extremely religiously homogeneous Muslim countries.

Table 3. Regression of religious bonding in Muslim- and Catholic-majority countries

The dependent variable and all non-binary predictors have been standardized to have mean zero and standard deviation one.

a Multilevel ordinal logistic regression.

b Ordinary least squares regression.

c TWM survey: Albania, Algeria, Azerbaijan, Bangladesh, Egypt, Indonesia, Iraq, Jordan, Kazakhstan, Kosovo, Kyrgyzstan, Lebanon, Malaysia, Niger, Pakistan, Tajikistan, Tunisia, and Turkey. RILA survey: Argentina, Bolivia, Brazil, Chile, Colombia, Costa Rica, Dominican Republic, Ecuador, El Salvador, Guatemala, Mexico, Nicaragua, Panama, Paraguay, Peru, and Venezuela.

d Countries in footnote c minus Algeria, Iraq, Jordan, Niger, Tunisia, and Turkey.

e Countries in footnote c plus Morocco and Uzbekistan.

f Countries in footnote e minus Algeria, Iraq, Jordan, Morocco, Niger, Tunisia, and Turkey.

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; +p < 0.10.

As Table 3 shows, the Muslim dummy is statistically significant in all the models. Muslim countries indeed have higher levels of religious bonding than the Catholic-majority Latin American countries. In fact, whether or not a country is Muslim majority is the strongest predictor for the levels of religious bonding among its respondents, stronger even than the country's level of religious diversity (see online Appendix S9 for effect size).

Addressing Compatibility Concerns

Notwithstanding that the RILA survey is the only available data to use as a comparison for the Muslim countries, the curious reader might raise concerns over three issues: sectarian divide among Muslims, theological distance between the religious traditions, and characteristics of the countries studied.

The concern about sectarian divide is related to the notion that Muslims may not be a homogeneous group but rather belong to different traditions with the two largest being Sunni and Shia. This divide may influence how the respondent answered the bonding question. This is an interesting concern but it should not harm the conclusion of the current study for two reasons. First, I find only little evidence of sectarian divide among the Muslim respondents (online Appendix S5). A vast majority of respondents who identified as Sunnis or Shias still regarded followers of the other tradition as Muslim. Specifically, 64% of the Sunni respondents believed that Shias are Muslim and 92.76% of the Shia respondents believed Sunnis are Muslim.

Second, the notion of sectarian divide actually works in favor of the current study as it makes regression models in Table 3 a conservative test of the Higher Bonding hypothesis. This is because a sectarian divide should increase the levels of religious diversity in the Muslim countries, making bridging more likely. That the current study does not take into account the possibility of a sectarian divide, therefore, means that it offers only a low-end estimate of the gap in the levels of bonding between the Muslim- and the Catholic-majority countries.

The concern about theological distance relates to religious compositions in the countries studied. Broadly speaking, the second largest religious tradition in the Muslim countries is Christianity, whereas in the Catholic countries it is not Islam but either Protestantism or Evangelicalism, which are other traditions of Christianity. Maybe it was easier for the Catholic respondents to befriend non-Catholics because it would mean befriending other Christians.

To examine this possibility we can look at the variable that asked respondents whether they thought that “the Muslim religion and the Christian religion” (the TWM survey) or “the Catholic religion and the Protestant/Evangelical religion” (the RILA survey) have a lot in common. Only 33% of the Muslim respondents thought that Islam and Christianity share similarities. Similarly, only 36% of the Catholic respondents thought that Catholicism and Protestantism/Evangelicalism have a lot in common. Thus, the theological distance argument is not supported by the data.

Finally, the concern over country characteristics is related to whether it is possible to compare the Muslim- and the Catholic-majority countries given how different these countries are. To this concern, I would point out that an empirical comparison between countries is not uncommon and they often spark interesting debates in the literature (Fox and Sandler Reference Fox and Sandler2005; Rizzo, Abdel-Latif, and Meyer Reference Rizzo, Abdel-Latif and Meyer2007; Gu and Bomhoff Reference Gu and Bomhoff2012). I also already acknowledge this concern by including in the regression models individual- and country-level covariates. As the preceding subsection has shown, even after these factors are taken into account, whether or not a country is Muslim majority is the strongest predictor for its level of religious bonding.

DISCUSSION

I argue that one of the main reasons why Muslim countries have poor interfaith relations is because of the religiously homogeneous friendship networks of Muslims in these countries. At the policy level, I aspire that this study would motivate policymakers, civil society organizers, and other concerned stakeholders to look beyond essentialist arguments that blame Muslim countries' poorer interfaith relations on Islamic theology.

Tolerance and interfaith attitudes, as the current study has shown, are shaped by social interactions, which in turn can be shaped through policies and activities that foster cooperation between religious groups. These policies and activities can take many forms, such as diversity-oriented curriculums (Rockenbach et al. Reference Rockenbach2015) or grassroots-level community meetings. Muwahidah (Reference Muwahidah2008), for example, finds interfaith cooperation can be fostered through secular community meetings that are originally intended to solve land-dispute problems.

At the scholarly level, I hope that this study motivates scholars of the Muslim world to pay more attention to social relationships in explaining Muslims' political behavior. A significant improvement in this direction would be for major comparative surveys to include questions on social relationships in their questionnaires. As more data become available we will be able to gain more insights into the mechanisms and contexts that may amplify or mitigate the effects of religious bonding or bridging.

Increased data availability will also enable us to inquire about the antecedents of religious bonding. What explains Muslim countries' high levels of bonding? Or, more generally, what shapes the levels of bonding and bridging relationships in a society? To be clear, bonding is generally the norm when it comes to social relationships. Humans are more likely to form relationships with similar than dissimilar others (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook Reference McPherson, Smith-Lovin and Cook2001). There is little reason, however, to argue that something unique about Islam's theology makes its followers more predisposed to religious bonding than followers of other religions.

Two hypotheses, briefly outlined above in the presentation of the Higher Bonding hypothesis, are particularly fruitful to explore. The first is political. Political Islam is driven more by social movements as opposed to political parties competing in an institutionalized system (Sadowski Reference Sadowski2006, 226). Informal networks such as volunteer groups and mosques are important in enabling these movements to survive repression from the authoritarian state, the type of state many Muslims still live under today (Wickham Reference Wickham2002). Mosques, in particular, are more than a place of worship as they also serve as a center of political activities. Although a church can also be a nexus of political activities (Djupe and Gilbert Reference Djupe and Gilbert2008), these activities are different in that they revolve around advocacy on social issues and are not about changing the regime.

How social movements shape bonding or bridging therefore is related to what these movements are organized for. Movements that place themselves in opposition to the regime probably promote bonding more than bridging. That religious movements in Muslim countries are more political than religious movements in non-Muslim countries may contribute to the countries' higher levels of bonding.

The second hypothesis is social-psychological. Studies have discussed how some Muslims adhere to the idea of a global ummah (Roy Reference Roy2006). The notion that all Muslims are one big family is unique in that it goes beyond the traditional concepts of nations, countries, and borders. Although claimed to be based on the Qur'an, the idea is largely political, driven by transnational Islamic organizations adhering to pan-Islamism and opposed to the West-dominated globalization (Roy Reference Roy2006; Saunders Reference Saunders2008).

It is different from Catholics' relationship with the Pope in that a global ummah concerns both cultural and political matters whereas the papacy is largely a cultural symbol with no jurisdiction over political matters in Catholic countries. It is also different from Judaism in that Judaism is more confined to a specific geographic area, compared to an ummah that transcends borders and continents. A perception that all Muslims are family is arguably more conducive to bonding than bridging. This effect should be more pronounced if the individuals believe that the family is under attack either militarily through “the war on terror” or culturally through globalization.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755048320000589.

Data

Data and code necessary to replicate the findings in the manuscript is available from the author's website (http://www.nathanael.id).

Acknowledgements

The author thanks the editors and the three anonymous reviewers, as well as Michael Coppedge, Tom Pepinsky, Geoff Layman, Dave Campbell, David Nickerson, participants of the 2018 AALIMS Conference at Princeton, and members of the Comparative Politics Workshop at the University of Notre Dame for valuable feedback and comments. All errors are the author's.

Footnotes

1. Exceptions would be religious freedom datasets by Fox (Reference Fox2008) and by the Pew Research Center (2018) derived from Grim and Finke (Reference Grim and Finke2010).

2. Secularism can also be understood from a behavioral-attitudinal perspective, namely how individuals view the relationship between state and religion (e.g., Fish Reference Fish2011). Although interesting, this perspective is beyond the purview of the current study which focuses instead on secularism as an institutional variable.

3. Predicting the three outcomes separately does not change the findings (see online Appendix). The Appendix also shows a model that predicts the Catholic respondents' perception of whether or not Catholicism and Protestantism/Evangelicalism are very different. The model shows that, among the Catholic respondents, higher bonding is related to more negative interfaith attitudes and that the effect is slightly weaker than among the Muslim respondents. This difference in the effect magnitude, though unanticipated and beyond the purview of the current study, points to potential research avenues. Future studies, for example, can explore how institutions affect how powerfully social ties shape intergroup attitudes either at national or subnational level.

4. I do not deny the importance of examining the Sunni-Shia divide, but space constraints force me to focus on Muslims and Christians. The TWM survey also reveals that Muslims in many countries did not identify themselves in a sectarian way and that even those who identified as Sunni or Shia still considered the other tradition as Muslim (online Appendix).

5. I conceptualize religious bonding as a secular social construct. As such, even though participation in religious services may be considered a form of bonding, it is not the form of bonding that is the interest of the current study.

6. I use the Pew's report rather than the Religion and State dataset (Fox Reference Fox2016) because the Pew dataset is the only one with data from 2015. The data are critical to establish the correct temporal ordering in the Social Hostilities hypothesis.

7. The critical reader might be concerned about whether employing a multilevel modeling with four country-level covariates and 18 countries (second-level group) is justifiable. Maas and Hox (Reference Maas and Hox2005) show that the estimates of regression coefficients are unbiased and the estimates of standard errors of regression coefficients are acceptable even with 10 level-two groups of size five (p. 90). To acknowledge this concern I also run fixed effects models, which yield substantively similar findings (see online Appendix).

8. There might be concerns related to the statistical power to detect effects of the country-level variables. Following Selya et al. (Reference Selya2012), I calculated the effect sizes of the non-statistically significant country-level variables in Table 1 and found that their effects are indeed weak (f 2 < 0.02), suggesting that the multilevel model might be underpowered to estimate these variables. Although keeping this in mind, it is worth remembering that the focus of the analysis is on the individual-level bonding variable and the other variables are there purely as controls to partial out potential confounders. The purpose is to show that religious bonding matters even after the traditional predictors are taken into account, not that the traditional predictors do not matter.

References

Allport, Gordon W. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Anderson, Joel, and Koc, Yasin. 2015. “Exploring Patterns of Explicit and Implicit Anti-Gay Attitudes in Muslims and Atheists.” European Journal of Social Psychology 45 (6): 687701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayoub, Mahmoud M. 1989. “Roots of Muslim-Christian Conflict.” The Muslim World 79 (1): 2545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, Albert. 1976. Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Ben-Nun Bloom, Pazit, Arikan, Gizem, and Courtemanche, Marie. 2015. “Religious Social Identity, Religious Belief, and Anti-Immigration Sentiment.” American Political Science Review 109 (2): 203221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berelson, Bernard R., Lazarsfeld, Paul F., and McPhee, William N.. 1954. Voting: A Study of Opinion Formation in a Presidential Campaign. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bohnet, Iris, Herrmann, Benedikt, and Zeckhauser, Richard. 2010. “Trust and the Reference Points for Trustworthiness in Gulf and Western Countries.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 125 (2): 811828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehm, John, and Rahn, Wendy. 1997. “Individual-Level Evidence for the Causes and Consequences of Social Capital.” American Journal of Political Science 41 (3): 9991023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, Marilynn B. 1999. “The Psychology of Prejudice: Ingroup Love and Outgroup Hate?Journal of Social Issues 55 (3): 429444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, David E. 2004. “Acts of Faith: Churches and Political Engagement.” Political Behavior 26 (2): 155180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cigler, Allan, and Joslyn, Mark R.. 2002. “The Extensiveness of Group Membership and Social Capital: The Impact on Political Tolerance Attitudes.” Political Research Quarterly 55 (1): 7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, James S. 1988. “Social Capital in the Creation of Human Capital.” American Journal of Sociology 4:S95S120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Djupe, Paul A., and Calfano, Brian R.. 2012. “American Muslim Investment in Civil Society Political Discussion, Disagreement, and Tolerance.” Political Research Quarterly 65 (3): 516528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Djupe, Paul A., and Gilbert, Christopher P.. 2008. The Political Influence of Churches. 1st ed. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, Kris, and Singh, Shane P.. 2014. “Pluralistic Conditioning: Social Tolerance and Effective Democracy.” Democratization 21 (1): 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eveland, William P., Hutchens, Myiah J., and Morey, Alyssa C.. 2013. “Political Network Size and Its Antecedents and Consequences.” Political Communication 30 (3): 371394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everton, Sean F. 2018. Networks and Religion: Ties That Bind, Loose, Build-up, and Tear Down. 1st ed. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fish, M. Steven. 2011. Are Muslims Distinctive?: A Look at the Evidence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Jonathan. 2008. A World Survey of Religion and the State. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Jonathan. 2016. The Unfree Exercise of Religion: A World Survey of Discrimination against Religious Minorities. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Jonathan, and Sandler, Shmuel. 2005. “Separation of Religion and State in the Twenty-First Century: Comparing the Middle East and Western Democracies.” Comparative Politics 37 (3): 317335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galston, William A. 2001. “Political Knowledge, Political Engagement, and Civic Education.” Annual Review of Political Science 4 (1): 217234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. 1992. “Alternative Measures of Political Tolerance: Must Tolerance Be ‘Least-Liked’?American Journal of Political Science 36 (2): 560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. 2010. “The Political Consequences of Religiosity: Does Religion Always Cause Political Intolerance?” In Religion and Democracy in the United States: Danger or Opportunity?, eds. Wolfe, Alan and Katznelson, Ira, New Jersey, NJ: Princeton University Press, 147175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glasman, Laura R., and Albarracín, Dolores. 2006. “Forming Attitudes That Predict Future Behavior: A Meta-Analysis of the Attitude-Behavior Relation.” Psychological Bulletin 132 (5): 778822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granovetter, Mark S. 1973. “The Strength of Weak Ties.” American Journal of Sociology 78(6):13601380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grim, Brian J., and Finke, Roger. 2006. “International Religion Indexes: Government Regulation, Government Favoritism, and Social Regulation of Religion.” Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion 2: 140.Google Scholar
Grim, Brian J., and Finke, Roger. 2010. The Price of Freedom Denied: Religious Persecution and Conflict in the Twenty-First Century. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gu, Man-Li, and Bomhoff, Eduard J.. 2012. “Religion and Support for Democracy: A Comparative Study for Catholic and Muslim Countries.” Politics and Religion 5 (2): 280316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harell, Allison. 2010. “Political Tolerance, Racist Speech, and the Influence of Social Networks.” Social Science Quarterly 91 (3): 724740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huckfeldt, Robert, Mendez, Jeanette Morehouse, and Osborn, Tracy. 2004. “Disagreement, Ambivalence, and Engagement: The Political Consequences of Heterogeneous Networks.” Political Psychology 25 (1): 6595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huckfeldt, Robert, and Sprague, John. 1987. “Networks in Context: The Social Flow of Political Information.” The American Political Science Review 81 (4): 1197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huntington, Samuel P. 1997. The Clash of Civilizations and the Remaking of World Order. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Iannaccone, Laurence R. 1998. “Introduction to the Economics of Religion.” Journal of Economic Literature 36 (3): 14651495.Google Scholar
Ikeda, Ken'ichi, and Richey, Sean. 2009. “The Impact of Diversity in Informal Social Networks on Tolerance in Japan.” British Journal of Political Science 39 (3): 655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald. 2003. “How Solid Is Mass Support for Democracy—and How Can We Measure It?Political Science and Politics 36 (1): 5157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald, and Welzel, Christian. 2005. Modernization, Cultural Change, and Democracy: The Human Development Sequence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jamal, Amaney. 2007. Barriers to Democracy: The Other Side of Social Capital in Palestine and the Arab World. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Koopmans, Ruud. 2015. “Religious Fundamentalism and Hostility against Out-Groups: A Comparison of Muslims and Christians in Western Europe.” Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 41 (1): 3357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazer, David, Rubineau, Brian, Chetkovich, Carol, Katz, Nancy, and Neblo, Michael 2010. “The Coevolution of Networks and Political Attitudes.” Political Communication 27 (3): 248274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levendusky, Matthew S., Druckman, James N., and McLain, Audrey. 2016. “How Group Discussions Create Strong Attitudes and Strong Partisans.” Research & Politics 3 (2):16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, Valerie A., MacGregor, Carol Ann, and Putnam, Robert D.. 2013. “Religion, Networks, and Neighborliness: The Impact of Religious Social Networks on Civic Engagement.” Social Science Research 42 (2): 331346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorch, Jasmin. 2019. “Islamization by Secular Ruling Parties: The Case of Bangladesh.” Politics and Religion 12 (2): 257282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maas, Cora J, and Hox, M. 2005. “Sufficient Sample Sizes for Multilevel Modeling.” Methodology 1(3):8692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
March, Andrew F. 2015. “Political Islam: Theory.” Annual Review of Political Science 18 (1): 103123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, George E., Sullivan, John L., Theiss-Morse, Elizabeth, and Wood, Sandra L.. 1995. With Malice toward Some: How People Make Civil Liberties Judgments. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mckenzie, Brian D. 2004. “Religious Social Networks, Indirect Mobilization, and African-American Political Participation.” Political Research Quarterly 57 (4): 621632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, Miller, Smith-Lovin, Lynn, and Cook, James M.. 2001. “Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks.” Annual Review of Sociology 27: 415444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menchik, Jeremy. 2016. Islam and Democracy in Indonesia: Tolerance without Liberalism. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merino, Stephen M. 2010. “Religious Diversity in a ‘Christian Nation’: The Effects of Theological Exclusivity and Interreligious Contact on the Acceptance of Religious Diversity.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 49 (2): 231246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milligan, Scott, Andersen, Robert, and Brym, Robert. 2014. “Assessing Variation in Tolerance in 23 Muslim-Majority and Western Countries.” Canadian Review of Sociology/Revue canadienne de sociologie 51 (3): 239261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muluk, Hamdi, Sumaktoyo, Nathanael G., and Ruth, Dhyah Madya. 2013. “Jihad as Justification.” Asian Journal of Social Psychology 16 (2): 101111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, Diana C. 2006. Hearing the Other Side: Deliberative versus Participatory Democracy. 1st ed. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muwahidah, Siti. 2008. “Interfaith Dialogue at the Grassroots Level: A Case Study of an Interfaith Empowerment Program in East Java, Indonesia.” Political Theology 9 (1): 7992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, Pippa, and Inglehart, Ronald. 2012. Sacred and Secular: Religion and Politics Worldwide. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ojo, Matthews A., and Lateju, Folaranmi T.. 2010. “Christian-Muslim Conflicts and Interfaith Bridge-Building Efforts in Nigeria.” The Review of Faith & International Affairs 8 (1): 3138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peffley, Mark, and Rohrschneider, Robert. 2003. “Democratization and Political Tolerance in Seventeen Countries: A Multi-Level Model of Democratic Learning.” Political Research Quarterly 56 (3): 243257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, Thomas F. 1998. “Intergroup Contact Theory.” Annual Review of Psychology 49 (1): 6585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, Thomas F., and Tropp, Linda R.. 2008. “How Does Intergroup Contact Reduce Prejudice? Meta-Analytic Tests of Three Mediators.” European Journal of Social Psychology 38 (6): 922934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center. 2013. “The World's Muslims: Religion, Politics and Society.” http://www.pewforum.org/2013/04/30/the-worlds-muslims-2013-2/ (December 22, 2017).Google Scholar
Pew Research Center. 2014a. “Religion in Latin America.” http://www.pewforum.org/2014/11/13/religion-in-latin-america/ (December 22, 2017).Google Scholar
Pew Research Center. 2014b. “Religious Diversity Index Scores by Country.” http://www.pewforum.org/2014/04/04/religious-diversity-index-scores-by-country/.Google Scholar
Pew Research Center. 2018. “Global Restrictions on Religion 2007–2016.” http://www.pewforum.org/dataset/global-restrictions-on-religion-2007-2016/ (December 22, 2017).Google Scholar
Putnam, Robert D. 1993. Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in Modern Italy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, Robert D. 2000. Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Putnam, Robert D., and Campbell, David E.. 2010. American Grace: How Religion Divides and Unites Us. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Rapp, Carolin, and Freitag, Markus. 2015. “Teaching Tolerance? Associational Diversity and Tolerance Formation.” Political Studies 63:10311051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhodes, Jeremy. 2012. “The Ties That Divide: Bonding Social Capital, Religious Friendship Networks, and Political Tolerance among Evangelicals.” Sociological Inquiry 82 (2): 163186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzo, Helen, Abdel-Latif, Abdel-Hamid, and Meyer, Katherine. 2007. “The Relationship between Gender Equality and Democracy: A Comparison of Arab versus Non-Arab Muslim Societies.” Sociology 41 (6): 11511170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rockenbach, Alyssa N., et al. 2015. “Fostering the Pluralism Orientation of College Students through Interfaith Co-Curricular Engagement.” The Review of Higher Education 39 (1): 2558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roy, Olivier. 2006. Globalized Islam: The Search for a New Ummah. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Sadowski, Yahya. 2006. “Political Islam: Asking the Wrong Questions?Annual Review of Political Science 9 (1): 215240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saroglou, Vassilis. 2002. “Religion and the Five Factors of Personality: A Meta-Analytic Review.” Personality and Individual Differences 32 (1): 1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, Robert A. 2008. “The Ummah as Nation: A Reappraisal in the Wake of the ‘Cartoons Affair’.” Nations and Nationalism 14 (2): 303321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwedler, Jillian. 2011. “Can Islamists Become Moderates? Rethinking the Inclusion-Moderation Hypothesis.” World Politics 63 (2): 347376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, Rachel M. 2010. The Challenge of Political Islam: Non-Muslims and the Egyptian State. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selya, Arielle S., Jennifer Rose, Lisa Dierker, Donald Hedeker, and Robin Mermelstein. 2012. “A Practical Guide to Calculating Cohen's F2, a Measure of Local Effect Size, from PROC MIXED.” Frontiers in Psychology 3:111116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spierings, Niels. 2014. “The Influence of Islamic Orientations on Democratic Support and Tolerance in Five Arab Countries.” Politics and Religion 7 (4): 706733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steensland, Brian, Robinson, Lynn, Wilcox, W. Bradford, Park, Jerry, Regnerus, Mark, and Woodberry, Robert 2000. “The Measure of American Religion: Toward Improving the State of the Art.” Social Forces 79 (1): 291318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, James John. 2014. “Muslim–Buddhist Conflict in Contemporary Sri Lanka.” South Asia Research 34 (3): 241260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, John L., and Transue, John E.. 1999. “The Psychological Underpinnings of Democracy: A Selective Review of Research on Political Tolerance, Interpersonal Trust, and Social Capital.” Annual Review of Psychology 50 (1): 625650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanuwidjaja, Sunny. 2010. “Political Islam and Islamic Parties in Indonesia: Critically Assessing the Evidence of Islam's Political Decline.” Contemporary Southeast Asia 32 (1): 2949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Laar, Colette, Levin, Shana, Sinclair, Stacey, and Sidanius, Jim. 2005. “The Effect of University Roommate Contact on Ethnic Attitudes and Behavior.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41 (4): 329345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varshney, Ashutosh. 2001. “Ethnic Conflict and Civil Society: India and Beyond.” World Politics 53 (3): 362398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ventura, Lorella. 2018. “‘Thank God We Are in Syria!’ Modernization, Interfaith Relations and Women's Rights in Syria Before the ‘Arab Spring’ (2000–2010).” Islam and Christian–Muslim Relations 29 (3): 349369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verkuyten, Maykel, Maliepaard, Mieke, Martinovic, Borja, and Khoudja, Yassine. 2014. “Political Tolerance among Muslim Minorities in Western Europe: The Role of Denomination and Religious and Host National Identification.” Politics and Religion 7 (2): 265286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, Barry, Wong, Renita Yuk-lin, Tindall, David, and Nazer, Nancy. 1997. “A Decade of Network Change: Turnover, Persistence and Stability in Personal Communities.” Social Networks 19 (1): 2750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, Carrie Rosefsky. 2002. Mobilizing Islam: Religion, Activism, and Political Change in Egypt. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Trends of SHI (Pew Research Center 2018)

Figure 1

Table 1. Multilevel linear regression of interfaith attitudes

Figure 2

Table 2. Country-level OLS regression of social hostilities on religious bonding

Figure 3

Figure 2. Religious bonding in Muslim- and Catholic-majority countries (see online Appendix 10 for actual values).

Figure 4

Table 3. Regression of religious bonding in Muslim- and Catholic-majority countries

Supplementary material: File

Sumaktoyo Supplementary Materials

Sumaktoyo Supplementary Materials

Download Sumaktoyo Supplementary Materials(File)
File 55.1 KB