Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T16:17:37.792Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ectoparasitic mites exert non-consumptive effects on the larvae of a fruit fly host

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 August 2023

Collin J. Horn
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, 11455 Saskatchewan Drive Edmonton, AB, Canada
Sarah Robinson
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, 11455 Saskatchewan Drive Edmonton, AB, Canada
Holly Tang
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, 11455 Saskatchewan Drive Edmonton, AB, Canada
Lien T. Luong*
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, 11455 Saskatchewan Drive Edmonton, AB, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Lien T. Luong; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

The mere presence of predators or parasites can negatively impact the fitness of prey or hosts. Exposure to predators during an organism's development can have deleterious effects on juvenile survival and the subsequent adult stage. Currently, it is unknown if parasites have analogous impacts on host larval stages and whether these effects carry over into other subsequent life stages. However, parasites may be exerting widespread yet underestimated non-consumptive effects (NCEs). We tested if Drosophila nigrospiracula larvae avoid pupating near mite cues (caged Macrocheles subbadius) in arena experiments, and measured the rate of pupation in arenas with mites and arenas without mites. Larvae disproportionately pupated on the side of arenas that lacked mite cues. Furthermore, fewer larvae successfully pupated in arenas containing mites cues compared to arenas without mite cues. We found that ectoparasitic mites exert NCEs on Drosophila larvae, even though the larval stage is not susceptible to infection. We discuss these results in the context of parasite impacts on host population growth in an infectious world.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Natural selection has driven the evolution of parasite-avoidance behaviours and other anti-parasite defences that reduce the risk of infection and/or limit post-infection proliferation. For example, hosts can reduce infection risk by avoiding infective stages, grooming or altering their habitat use (Hart, Reference Hart1994; Lefevre et al., Reference Lefevre, de Roode, Kacsoh and Schlenke2012; Buck et al., Reference Buck, Weinstein and Young2018; Koprivnikar et al., Reference Koprivnikar, Rochette and Forbes2021). The mere presence of parasites or parasite cues, sans infection, can influence host behaviour (e.g. changes in habitat use, auto-grooming, etc.) and induce physiological stress. In predator-prey ecology, these indirect ‘non-consumptive effects’ (NCEs) can have consequences for prey growth, survival and reproduction (Werner and Peacor, Reference Werner and Peacor2003; Preisser et al., Reference Preisser, Bolnick and Benard2005). By acting on the entire population, NCEs can have greater cumulative impacts than consumption per se on prey populations (Mouritsen and Poulin, Reference Mouritsen and Poulin2005; Clinchy et al., Reference Clinchy, Sheriff and Zanette2013).

Recent work has extended concepts developed for predator-prey systems to disease ecology and parasite-host associations (Raffel et al., Reference Raffel, Martin and Rohr2008; Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Swan, Raffel and Hudson2009; Daversa et al., Reference Daversa, Hechinger, Madin, Fenton, Dell, Ritchie, Rohr, Rudolf, Lafferty and Lafferty2021). Previous reluctance to do so assumed that since parasitic infections do not cause immediate lethality, anti-parasite responses should be relatively weak. However, increasing evidence suggests otherwise (Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Swan, Raffel and Hudson2009; Buck et al., Reference Buck, Weinstein and Young2018). For example, small mammals are quicker to abandon resource patches that are heavily infested with ticks compared to sites with lower infection risk (Fritzsche and Allan, Reference Fritzsche and Allan2012). Likewise, female fruit flies avoid ovipositing in habitats laden with ectoparasitic mites (Mierzejewski et al., Reference Mierzejewski, Horn and Luong2019). Parasite avoidance may be costly, especially if it negatively affects foraging and reproductive activities (Preisser et al., Reference Preisser, Bolnick and Benard2005; Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Sih and Chivers2009; Benoit et al., Reference Benoit, Bose, Bailey and Polak2020). Pea aphid populations declined by 50% in the presence of parasitoid wasps even though the aphids were not a susceptible host, because wasp presence induced escape behaviours and reduced feeding opportunities (Fill et al., Reference Fill, Long and Finke2012). Drosophila nigrospiracula adults in proximity to ectoparasitic mites experienced an energetic cost of parasite exposure, driven by a combination of physiological stress and induction of energetically expensive behavioural responses, and consequently suffered reduced survival and fecundity even though they were never in direct contact with the mites (Luong et al., Reference Luong, Horn and Brophy2017a; Horn and Luong, Reference Horn and Luong2018; Benoit et al., Reference Benoit, Bose, Bailey and Polak2020).

In predator-prey systems NCEs can carry over from one developmental stage to another, and potentially across generations. Fly larvae can sense and avoid predators, and larval stages of D. melanogaster exposed to spiders have lower adult masses, potentially mediated by accelerated development (Krams et al., Reference Krams, Inwood, Trakimas, Krams, Burghardt, Butler, Luoto and Krama2016). Furthermore, exposure to predator cues reduced the pupation success rate (i.e. survival to adulthood) of dragonfly larvae (McCauley et al., Reference McCauley, Rowe and Fortin2011). There is currently no evidence that parasites have NCEs on larvae, and if this impacts larval survival and/or carry over to the adult stage (Ellrich et al., Reference Ellrich, Scrosati, Bertolini and Molis2016; Krams et al., Reference Krams, Inwood, Trakimas, Krams, Burghardt, Butler, Luoto and Krama2016). However, larvae could benefit from sensing and responding to parasites both by (1) directly avoiding infection and (2) pupating in less infectious environments, but these responses may be costly, requiring time and energy.

Here we test the hypothesis that exposure to parasites has effects on larval behaviour (mite-cue avoidance) and development (measured as pupation success). Drosophila larvae possess an olfactory system and can learn olfactory cues (Python and Stocker, Reference Python and Stocker2002; Scherer et al., Reference Scherer, Stocker and Gerber2003). Hence, it is reasonable to assume that fruit fly larvae have the potential to detect cues from ectoparasitic mites without direct contact. We tested our hypotheses using the cactiphilic fly D. nigrospiracula and a natural hemolymph-feeding ectoparasite Macrocheles subbadius (caged to avoid contact). We predict that (1) D. nigrospiracula larvae will preferentially pupate away from mites in a choice-test and (2) exhibit a higher rate of pupation in the total absence of mites. We also consider if exposure during the larval stage carries over into the adult stage in the form of reduced body size.

Materials and methods

Study system

D. nigrospiracula and M. subbadius cultures were founded from wild-caught organisms from necrotic Carnegiae gigantea in the Sonoran Desert (Arizona, USA). Cultures are fully described in Horn and Luong (Reference Horn and Luong2021).

Mite attachment on larval stages

This experiment was designed to test whether mites attach to larval D. nigrospiracula, which have 3 instar stages (1–3). Larvae were transferred from a culture bottle to a 50 mL cup of 20% sucrose. After 20 min, larvae were transferred from the surface of the solution via a sterilized spoon to a Petri dish lined with wet filter paper. Individual 1st/2nd (N = 50) or 3rd (N = 40) instar larvae (L1/L2s and L3s respectively) were placed into a ventilated 0.5 mL Eppendorf microtube lined with moist filter paper. Three mites previously starved for 3–4 days were then added to the tube. The position of mites was observed every 30 min for 2 hours (the majority of M. subbadius that attach to adult flies do so within 1–2 hours, Luong et al., Reference Luong, Brophy, Stolz and Chan2017b). The number of mites on/adjacent larvae was modelled with observation time (since experiment start) and larval stage (L3 or L1/L2) using a glm (family = Poisson). Attachment was deemed successful if a mite attached and remained attached for an additional hour. Larvae survival was checked an additional 2 hours after the exposure period.

Pupation site and success

This experiment tested whether larvae avoided pupating near mites, and if mite exposure, sans contact, had knock-on effects on larval pupation success. Pupation arenas consisted of a 60 mm aerated Petri dishes lined with Drosophila media and ~1 g autoclaved cactus (Fig. 1). Cotton dental rolls placed on opposite sides of the Petri dish provided pupation substrates. A mite cage (cropped translucent, yellow pipet tip with mesh ends, ~2 cm long × 0.5 cm diameter) was placed adjacent to each dental roll. Mite cages allowed mite cues to exit the cage but prevented mites from contacting the larvae. Treatment arenas (n = 45) had an empty cage on one side and a cage containing 5 mites on the opposite side. In control arenas (n = 9) both cages were empty (i.e. mite-free). Arenas were replicated over a 4-day period; for a given day sixty 3rd instar fly larvae were taken from a single culture bottle and randomly added to the centre of each arena. Petri dish arenas were sealed with parafilm and placed in an incubator (25°C, 70% relative humidity, 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle) for 5 days after which we recorded the number of pupae present, and if they occurred on the mite-side or the non-mite side in the treatment groups, after which mite cages were removed.

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental set up. Arenas (60 mm Petri dishes) contained 2 mite cages (cropped translucent pipette tips closed with mesh) as well as Drosophila medium and dental rolls as a pupation substrate. Treatment arenas (left) had a side with mites in the cage and a mite-free side, the position of the cages with and without mites was alternated between dishes. Control arenas (right) had 2 empty mite cages.

The proportion of pupae on the mite-side of dishes with mites (pupae on mite side/total number of pupae) was calculated for each dish, then a 1 group t-test (H0: u = 0.5) was used to test if larvae disproportionately pupated on the mite-free or mite-containing side. A generalized linear model (family = beta distribution, betareg function, betareg package) was used to test if the overall pupation rate was a significant predictor of the proportion of pupation on the mite side. A glm (glm.nb function, family = negative binomial, link = log, MASS package) was used to test if mite exposure affected the number of D. nigrospiracula pupae between dishes with and without mites.

Long term storage of arenas inevitably led to mould – in both treatment and control groups – causing adult emergence to be inconsistent. Thus, we did not record the proportion of flies that eclosed as adults. However, arenas were monitored, and adults were collected and immediately frozen so that they could be weighed (Mettler Toledo XPE105 balance, OH USA); the date of emergence but not the specific arena each fly came from was recorded. The body masses of flies from the treatment arenas were compared to flies from control arenas. Mixed effect models (lmer function) were used to test if larval mite exposure, fly sex and the exposure-sex interaction predicted the adult mass of flies, along with the random effect collection date.

Results

Mite attachment on larval stages

The majority of fly larvae (including 100% of L3 larvae) exposed to mites were alive 2 hours after the exposure period; 18 1st/2nd instar larvae died towards the middle and later part of this period for reasons unrelated to mites (desiccation). At the outset, mites were more likely to be on or attached to (we could not confirm if mites had attached without disturbing the assay) L1/L2s than L3s (df = 5, Δdeviance = 7.37, P = 0.007, 95% CI: −1.73 to −0.26). However, mites were much less likely to be observed on a larva (L1/L2 and L3) the longer the trial continued (df = 5, Δdeviance = 15.82, P < 0.001, 95% CI: −0.034 to −0.011). The interaction between stage and time was not a significant predictor of if mites were near larvae (df = 4, Δdeviance = 1.51, P = 0.22, 95% CI: −0.009 to 0.040). Although some mites initially approached the fly larvae, none remained on or attached to the larva for more than 30 min, suggesting the initial approach was likely exploratory behaviour. We did not find evidence that mites infect larvae at ecologically relevant rates or time spans.

Pupation site and success

In total, 44 of 45 of the treatment dishes (containing mite cages) were successful (1 dish was discarded due to mould). Larvae were disproportionately more likely to pupate on the side of the dish without mites (t = −2.3, df = 43, P = 0.028). The mean proportion of pupae on the side with mites was 0.45 (95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.41–0.49). Thus, larvae had a moderate, but statistically significant, preference for pupating away from mites (Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Number of larvae of the cactiphilic fly Drosphila nigrospiracula that pupated on the mite-free side vs the side of the arena with caged mites. Each arena contained sixty 3rd instar larvae able to move freely throughout the arena. Upper and lower box edges represent the first and third quartiles, while the middle line indicates the median.

The rate of pupation also differed between dishes containing mites (treatment arenas) and those that did not contain mites (control arenas). In the dishes containing mites there were 25.4 ± 1.3 (mean ± standard error of the mean) pupae, whereas in mite-free dishes there were 47.7 ± 1.7 pupae. Mite presence in the arena was a statistically significant predictor of the number of pupae (df = 51, Δdeviance = 40.1, P < 0.001, 95% CI of the regression coefficient: 0.43–0.83) (Fig. 3).

Figure 3. Number of Drosphila nigrospiracula that successfully formed pupae in treatment arenas containing caged mites (n = 44) or control arenas with no mites (n = 9). 60 larvae were placed in mite-free or mite-containing Petri dish arenas. Upper and lower box edges represent the first and third quartiles, while the middle line indicates the median.

Adult flies were harvested the day of eclosion following mite exposure (or control unexposed flies) to assess differences in mass as a result of exposure (i.e. parasitic NCEs) as larvae. A total of 93 flies (56 male and 37 females) were retrieved on 5 collection dates. A male-biased sex ratio was expected from previous literature (Polak and Markow, Reference Polak and Markow1995). There was no significant interaction between mite exposure and fly sex on mass (df = 89 [from lmertest], F = 0.072, P = 0.79, 95% CI: −0.22 to 0.21). Fly sex was a highly significant predictor of mass, aligning with previously observed sexual dimorphism (df = 89, F = 100.1, P < 0.0001, 95% CI: 0.42–0.62). Mite exposure as larvae was only a marginal predictor of adult mass (df = 51, F = 3.1, P = 0.087, 95% CI: −0.25 to 0.013). Unexposed females were on average 2.43 ± 0.06 mg, and unexposed males were on average 1.90 ± 0.04 mg. Among flies exposed to mites as larvae, female flies were on average 2.24 ± 0.06 mg and male flies were 1.75 ± 0.04 mg.

Discussion

We tested for NCEs of ectoparasitic mites on fly larvae. Even though mites do not attach to the larval stage, we observed avoidance behaviours and reduced pupation success. We found that larvae responded to the presence of mites and preferentially pupated on the mite-free side of the treatment arenas. Additionally, mites exerted NCEs on the larvae by reducing pupation success.

Drosophila larvae can actively avoid predators and parasitoids (Ebrahim et al., Reference Ebrahim, Dweck, Stoekl, Hofferberth, Trona, Weniger, Rybak, Seki, Stensmyr, Sachse, Hansson and Knaden2015; Krams et al., Reference Krams, Inwood, Trakimas, Krams, Burghardt, Butler, Luoto and Krama2016); our results extend this observation to ectoparasites. The ability to avoid mites may be directly beneficial to larvae, but our observations indicate that M. subbadius do not attach to the L3 larvae and largely ignored L1/L2 larvae during the observation period. It's not clear if the larval stages are resistant to infection and/or the mites actively avoid attaching to larvae because of the low potential for dispersal. Female mites (usually gravid) attach to adults flies for nutrients and dispersal between ephemeral habitats. Most likely, larvae benefit in the long-term by pupating in less infectious environments because mite infection is highly deleterious to the survival and reproductive success of adult flies (Polak, Reference Polak1996). This may be particularly important for newly eclosed adults which are not fully sclerotized and cannot immediately take flight to escape mites – wings must unfurl and dry before flight. Remarkably, the presence of mites elicited avoidance behaviour (pupation site selection) by the larval stage even though it is not susceptible to infection by the mites.

Furthermore, the pupation success (i.e. survival during metamorphosis) was adversely affected even though mites do not infect larvae or cause direct larval mortality. It is not clear how larvae sensed mites, however the visual system of larvae is simpler than that of adult flies (Keene and Sprecher, Reference Keene and Sprecher2012). Fly larvae may be able to sense mites by olfactory/chemical cues, and/or vibrations when mites contact the mesh barrier. The artificial nature of the Petri dishes may have confined or concentrated the cues, which would otherwise be more diffuse in the wild. However, the flies and mites typically interact within small pockets of necrotic tissue, so the size of the observational arena is biologically relevant. The presence of mites may negatively affect feeding among the larvae with knock-on effects on pupation success. Pea aphid exposed to parasitoids exhibited increased escape behaviours and consequently reduced feeding (Fill et al., Reference Fill, Long and Finke2012). Future research is needed to examine the NCEs of parasite exposure on host foraging behaviour in the fly-mite system. When dragonfly larvae were exposed to predator cues, pupation success rate but not adult body size was reduced (McCauley et al., Reference McCauley, Rowe and Fortin2011). Similarly, we only detected a marginal effect of parasite exposure on adult body size. This may be because food was provided ad libitum, which may have mitigated the deleterious effects of parasite exposure on the surviving larvae/pupae. Host responses to parasites may also trigger a stress response in larvae that is not necessarily adaptive. Stress responses may cause a cascade of deleterious physiological responses that potentially increase the risk of mortality (i.e. failure to pupate), as has been seen in predator-prey relationships (Preisser, Reference Preisser2009; Sheriff et al., Reference Sheriff, Krebs and Boonstra2009; McCauley et al., Reference McCauley, Rowe and Fortin2011). Such stress-induced NCEs could have significant implications for the ecology of fear and host-parasite dynamics.

Computer models of fly-mite populations found that the earlier the impacts of NCEs are in the fly lifespan, the larger the impacts of mites are on host population growth (Horn et al., Reference Horn, Visscher and Luong2022). Theoretical work suggests that, if natural enemies are not the only source of mortality, attacks earlier in the lifespan will have larger impacts on the population growth of the prey/host species (Godfray and Waage, Reference Godfray and Waage1991; Murdoch and Briggs, Reference Murdoch and Briggs1996). However, until now there was no evidence parasite NCEs affected host development and survival to adulthood. Our results show that larval exposure to parasites impacts pupation site selection and induces carryover effects (metamorphosis). Thus, previous studies (Horn et al., Reference Horn, Visscher and Luong2022) may have underestimated the non-consumptive impacts of mites on the growth rate of fly populations by only accounting for impacts of mites on adult flies.

Parasitologists and ecologists increasingly recognize that parasites have diverse ecological effects beyond infection. NCEs are a major avenue by which parasites may exert influence outside of infection. This study provides evidence that an ectoparasite has NCEs on the behaviour and survival of host larvae to the pupal stage. Impacts of natural enemies on pre-reproductive stages can have outsized impacts on host population growth. Investigating the NCEs of parasites on larval stages may reveal widespread yet underestimated impacts of parasites.

Data availability

Raw data and R code can be accessed online (OSF doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/CVU28).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank C. MacLeod for maintaining the fly culture and C. Liang for maintaining the mite culture.

Authors’ contributions

C. J. H. analysed the data and co-wrote the article, S. R. performed the larval exposure experiments, H. T. conducted the infection trials, L. T. L. co-wrote the article.

Financial support

Funding was provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

Not applicable.

Footnotes

*

Present address: Dalhousie University (Current), Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, 1355 Oxford Street, Halifax, NS, Canada.

References

Benoit, J, Bose, J, Bailey, S and Polak, M (2020) Interactions with ectoparasitic mites induce host metabolic and immune responses in flies at the expense of reproduction-associated factors. Parasitology 147, 11961205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buck, JC, Weinstein, SB and Young, HS (2018) Ecological and evolutionary consequences of parasite avoidance. Trends Ecology & Evolution 33, 619632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clinchy, M, Sheriff, MJ and Zanette, LY (2013) Predator-induced stress and the ecology of fear. Functional Ecology 27, 5665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daversa, DR, Hechinger, RF, Madin, E, Fenton, A, Dell, AI, Ritchie, EG, Rohr, J, Rudolf, VHW, Lafferty, VHW and Lafferty, KD (2021) Broadening the ecology of fear: non-lethal effects arise from diverse responses to predation and parasitism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 288, 20202966. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.2966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebrahim, SAM, Dweck, HKM, Stoekl, J, Hofferberth, JE, Trona, F, Weniger, K, Rybak, J, Seki, Y, Stensmyr, MC, Sachse, S, Hansson, BS and Knaden, M (2015) Drosophila avoids parasitoids by sensing their semiochemicals via a dedicated olfactory circuit. Plos Biology 13, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellrich, JA, Scrosati, RA, Bertolini, C and Molis, M (2016) A predator has nonconsumptive effects on different life-history stages of a prey. Marine Biology 163, 8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, MCO, Sih, A and Chivers, DP (2009) The paradox of risk allocation: a review and prospectus. Animal Behavior 78, 579585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fill, A, Long, EY and Finke, DL (2012) Non-consumptive effects of a natural enemy on a non-prey herbivore population. Ecological Entomology 37, 4350.10.1111/j.1365-2311.2011.01333.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fritzsche, A and Allan, BF (2012) The ecology of fear: host foraging behavior varies with the spatio-temporal abundance of a dominant ectoparasite. Ecohealth 9, 7074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godfray, HCJ and Waage, JK (1991) Predictive modeling in biological control – the mango mealy bug (Rastrococcus invadens) and its parasitoids. Journal of Applied Ecology 28, 434453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, BL (1994) Behavioral defense against parasites: interaction with parasite invasiveness. Parasitology 109, S139S151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, CJ and Luong, LT (2018) Proximity to parasites reduces host fitness independent of infection in a Drosophila-Macrocheles system. Parasitology 145, 15641569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, CJ and Luong, LT (2021) Trade-offs between reproduction and behavioural resistance against ectoparasite infection. Physiology and Behavior 239, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, CJ, Visscher, DR and Luong, LT (2022) Relative contributions of parasite consumptive and non-consumptive effects to host population suppression in simulated fly-mite populations. Oecologia 200, 339347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keene, AC and Sprecher, SG (2012) Seeing the light: photobehavior in fruit fly larvae. Trends in Neuroscience 35, 104110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koprivnikar, J, Rochette, A and Forbes, M (2021) Risk-induced trait responses and non-consumptive effects in plants and animals in response to their invertebrate herbivore and parasite natural enemies. Frontier in Ecology and Evolution 9, 667030. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.667030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krams, I, Inwood, SE, Trakimas, G, Krams, R, Burghardt, GM, Butler, DM, Luoto, S and Krama, T (2016) Short-term exposure to predation affects body elemental composition, climbing speed and survival ability in Drosophila melanogaster. Peerj 4, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lefevre, T, de Roode, J, Kacsoh, B and Schlenke, T (2012) Defence strategies against a parasitoid wasp in Drosophila: fight or flight? Biology Letters 8, 230233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luong, LT, Horn, CJ and Brophy, T (2017a) Mitey costly: energetic costs of parasite avoidance and infection. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 90, 471477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luong, LT, Brophy, T, Stolz, E and Chan, SJ (2017b) State-dependent parasitism by a facultative parasite of fruit flies. Parasitology 144, 14681475.10.1017/S0031182017000890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCauley, SJ, Rowe, L and Fortin, MJ (2011) The deadly effects of “nonlethal” predators. Ecology 92, 20432048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mierzejewski, MK, Horn, CJ and Luong, LT (2019) Ecology of fear: environment-dependent parasite avoidance among ovipositing Drosophila. Parasitology 146, 15641570.10.1017/S0031182019000854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mouritsen, KN and Poulin, R (2005) Parasites boosts biodiversity and changes animal community structure by trait-mediated indirect effects. Oikos 108, 344350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdoch, WW and Briggs, CJ (1996) Theory for biological control: recent developments. Ecology 77, 20012013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polak, M (1996) Ectoparasitic effects on host survival and reproduction: the Drosophila-Macrocheles association. Ecology 77, 13791389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polak, M and Markow, TA (1995) Effect of ectoparasitic mites on sexual selection in a sonoran desert fruit-fly. Evolution 49, 660669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preisser, EL (2009) The physiology of predator stress in free- ranging prey. Journal of Animal Ecology 78, 11031105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preisser, EL, Bolnick, DI and Benard, MF (2005) Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86, 501509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Python, F and Stocker, R (2002) Adult-like complexity of the larval antennal lobe of D. melanogaster despite markedly low numbers of odorant receptor neurons. Journal of Comparative Neurology 445, 374387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffel, TR, Martin, LB and Rohr, JR (2008) Parasites as predators: unifying natural enemy ecology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23, 610618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohr, JR, Swan, A, Raffel, TR and Hudson, PJ (2009) Parasites, info-disruption, and the ecology of fear. Oecologia 159, 447454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, S, Stocker, R and Gerber, B (2003) Olfactory learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae. Learning and Memory 10, 217225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheriff, MJ, Krebs, CJ and Boonstra, R (2009) The sensitive hare: sublethal effects of predator stress on reproduction in snowshoe hares. Journal of Animal Ecology 78, 12491258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, EE and Peacor, SD (2003) A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84, 10831100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental set up. Arenas (60 mm Petri dishes) contained 2 mite cages (cropped translucent pipette tips closed with mesh) as well as Drosophila medium and dental rolls as a pupation substrate. Treatment arenas (left) had a side with mites in the cage and a mite-free side, the position of the cages with and without mites was alternated between dishes. Control arenas (right) had 2 empty mite cages.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Number of larvae of the cactiphilic fly Drosphila nigrospiracula that pupated on the mite-free side vs the side of the arena with caged mites. Each arena contained sixty 3rd instar larvae able to move freely throughout the arena. Upper and lower box edges represent the first and third quartiles, while the middle line indicates the median.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Number of Drosphila nigrospiracula that successfully formed pupae in treatment arenas containing caged mites (n = 44) or control arenas with no mites (n = 9). 60 larvae were placed in mite-free or mite-containing Petri dish arenas. Upper and lower box edges represent the first and third quartiles, while the middle line indicates the median.