Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:26:15.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) combined with cognitive training in adolescent boys with ADHD: a double-blind, randomised, sham-controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 July 2021

Samuel J. Westwood*
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Marion Criaud
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Sheut-Ling Lam
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Steve Lukito
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Sophie Wallace-Hanlon
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
Olivia S. Kowalczyk
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK Department of Neuroimaging, King's College London, London, UK
Afroditi Kostara
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Joseph Mathew
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Deborah Agbedjro
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, King's College London, London, UK
Bruce E. Wexler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Roi Cohen Kadosh
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Philip Asherson
Affiliation:
Social Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Katya Rubia
Affiliation:
Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Samuel J. Westwood, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) could be a side-effect-free alternative to psychostimulants in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Although there is limited evidence for clinical and cognitive effects, most studies were small, single-session and stimulated left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). No sham-controlled study has stimulated the right inferior frontal cortex (rIFC), which is the most consistently under-functioning region in ADHD, with multiple anodal-tDCS sessions combined with cognitive training (CT) to enhance effects. Thus, we investigated the clinical and cognitive effects of multi-session anodal-tDCS over rIFC combined with CT in double-blind, randomised, sham-controlled trial (RCT, ISRCTN48265228).

Methods

Fifty boys with ADHD (10–18 years) received 15 weekday sessions of anodal- or sham-tDCS over rIFC combined with CT (20 min, 1 mA). ANCOVA, adjusting for baseline measures, age and medication status, tested group differences in clinical and ADHD-relevant executive functions at posttreatment and after 6 months.

Results

ADHD-Rating Scale, Conners ADHD Index and adverse effects were significantly lower at post-treatment after sham relative to anodal tDCS. No other effects were significant.

Conclusions

This rigorous and largest RCT of tDCS in adolescent boys with ADHD found no evidence of improved ADHD symptoms or cognitive performance following multi-session anodal tDCS over rIFC combined with CT. These findings extend limited meta-analytic evidence of cognitive and clinical effects in ADHD after 1–5 tDCS sessions over mainly left dlPFC. Given that tDCS is commercially and clinically available, the findings are important as they suggest that rIFC stimulation may not be indicated as a neurotherapy for cognitive or clinical remediation for ADHD.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterised by persisting, age-inappropriate and impairing symptoms of inattention and/or impulsivity-hyperactivity (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). ADHD is also associated with deficits in executive functions (EF), most prominently in motor and interference inhibition, sustained attention and vigilance, switching, working memory and timing (Coghill, Toplak, Rhodes, & Adamo, Reference Coghill, Toplak, Rhodes, Adamo, Banaschewski, Coghill and Zuddas2018; Rubia, Reference Rubia2018). Neuroimaging studies indicate delayed structural and functional brain maturation (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Eckstrand, Sharp, Blumenthal, Lerch, Greenstein and Rapoport2007; Sripada, Kessler, & Angstadt, Reference Sripada, Kessler and Angstadt2014), and consistent underactivation in predominantly right inferior frontal (rIFC), dorsolateral prefrontal (dlPFC) and anterior cingulate cortices, as well as striatal, parietal and cerebellar regions during cognitive control, timing and attention tasks (Hart, Radua, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, Reference Hart, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2012, Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Lukito et al., Reference Lukito, Norman, Carlisi, Radua, Hart, Simonoff and Rubia2020; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016; Rubia, Reference Rubia2018).

Psychostimulants are the gold-standard treatment for improving ADHD symptoms, but have possible associated side effects (Cortese, Reference Cortese2020), poor adherence in adolescence (Cortese et al., Reference Cortese, Adamo, Del Giovane, Mohr-Jensen, Hayes, Carucci, Atkinson and Cipriani2018; Cunill, Castells, Tobias, & Capellà, Reference Cunill, Castells, Tobias and Capellà2016) and are not indicated for all individuals with ADHD (Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Asherson, Banaschewski, Biederman, Buitelaar, Ramos-Quiroga and Franke2015). Evidence of longer-term efficacy is also limited (Cortese et al., Reference Cortese, Adamo, Del Giovane, Mohr-Jensen, Hayes, Carucci, Atkinson and Cipriani2018; Swanson et al., Reference Swanson, Arnold, Jensen, Hinshaw, Hechtman, Pelham and Roy2018), possibly due to brain adaptation (Fusar-Poli, Rubia, Rossi, Sartori, & Balottin, Reference Fusar-Poli, Rubia, Rossi, Sartori and Balottin2012). Meta-analyses indicate small to moderate efficacy of behavioural therapies, cognitive training (CT), neurofeedback or dietary interventions on ADHD symptoms (Catalá-López et al., Reference Catalá-López, Hutton, Núñez-Beltrán, Mayhew, Page, Ridao, Tobías and Moher2017). Non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, however, are promising given their potential to stimulate key dysfunctional brain regions associated with ADHD, with potentially longer-term neuroplastic effects that drugs cannot offer (Cinel, Valeriani, & Poli, Reference Cinel, Valeriani and Poli2019; Rubia, Reference Rubia2018; Sierawska et al., Reference Sierawska, Prehn-Kristensen, Moliadze, Krauel, Nowak, Freitag and Buyx2019; STIPED Press Release, 2017; Westwood, Radua, & Rubia, Reference Westwood, Radua and Rubia2020). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is particularly well-suited for paediatric populations as it is user-friendly, well tolerated with minimal side effects (Bikson et al., Reference Bikson, Grossman, Thomas, Zannou, Jiang, Adnan and Woods2016), and is cheaper relative to other techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert, Oberman and Enticott2019).

In tDCS, a weak direct electrical current is delivered through two electrodes placed on the scalp (one anode, one cathode), generating subthreshold, polarity-dependent shifts in resting membrane potentials in underlying brain regions. The resulting net increase (predominantly under the anode) or decrease (predominantly under the cathode) in neuronal excitability leads to modulation of the neuronal network (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Vöröslakos, Kronberg, Henin, Krause, Huang and Buzsáki2018), with neurophysiological effects persisting after stimulation, presumably by potentiating mediators of practice-dependent synaptic plasticity, including GABA, glutamate (Filmer, Ehrhardt, Bollmann, Mattingley, & Dux, Reference Filmer, Ehrhardt, Bollmann, Mattingley and Dux2019; Stagg & Nitsche, Reference Stagg and Nitsche2011), dopamine and noradrenaline (Fonteneau et al., Reference Fonteneau, Redoute, Haesebaert, Le Bars, Costes, Suaud-Chagny and Brunelin2018; Kuo, Paulus, & Nitsche, Reference Kuo, Paulus and Nitsche2008; Monte-Silva, Liebetanz, Grundey, Paulus, & Nitsche, Reference Monte-Silva, Liebetanz, Grundey, Paulus and Nitsche2010).

Evidence of cognitive performance and clinical improvement following tDCS is, however, limited. Two meta-analyses of tDCS studies stimulating mainly the left dlPFC in 1–5 sessions in children and adolescents with ADHD indicate a modest improvement in inhibitory control measures (Salehinejad, Wischnewski, Nejati, Vicario, & Nitsche, Reference Salehinejad, Wischnewski, Nejati, Vicario and Nitsche2019) with the latter one showing non-significant improvement on processing speed and inhibitory measures with no effects on attention measures (Westwood et al., Reference Westwood, Radua and Rubia2020). Only two sham-controlled studies tested ADHD symptoms, reporting improvement in inattentive symptoms, but not impulsivity/hyperactivity, immediately, 1 (Cachoeira et al., Reference Cachoeira, Leffa, Mittelstadt, Mendes, Brunoni, Pinto and Schestatsky2017; Soff, Sotnikova, Christiansen, Becker, & Siniatchkin, Reference Soff, Sotnikova, Christiansen, Becker and Siniatchkin2017) and 2 weeks (Cachoeira et al., Reference Cachoeira, Leffa, Mittelstadt, Mendes, Brunoni, Pinto and Schestatsky2017) after anodal tDCS over left or right dlPFC.

There is evidence that tDCS effects can be enhanced when combined with CT by functionally priming the brain regions that mediate the cognitive function being trained (Au et al., Reference Au, Katz, Buschkuehl, Bunarjo, Senger, Zabel and Jonides2016, Reference Au, Karsten, Buschkuehl and Jaeggi2017; Katz et al., Reference Katz, Au, Buschkuehl, Abagis, Zabel, Jaeggi and Jonides2017; Kronberg, Bridi, Abel, Bikson, & Parra, Reference Kronberg, Bridi, Abel, Bikson and Parra2017). Multi-session anodal tDCS combined with CT has also been shown to elicit longer-term cognitive improvements in healthy controls for up to 1 (Jones, Stephens, Alam, Bikson, & Berryhill, Reference Jones, Stephens, Alam, Bikson and Berryhill2015; Stephens & Berryhill, Reference Stephens and Berryhill2016), nine (Ruf, Fallgatter, & Plewnia, Reference Ruf, Fallgatter and Plewnia2017) or 12 months (Katz et al., Reference Katz, Au, Buschkuehl, Abagis, Zabel, Jaeggi and Jonides2017), and clinical improvements in psychiatric disorders for at least 1 month (Kekic, Boysen, Campbell, & Schmidt, Reference Kekic, Boysen, Campbell and Schmidt2016; Moffa, Brunoni, Nikolin, & Loo, Reference Moffa, Brunoni, Nikolin and Loo2018; Tortella et al., Reference Tortella, Casati, Aparicio, Mantovani, Senço, D'Urso and Brunoni2015), suggesting longer-term neuroplastic effects.

Most studies in ADHD have targeted the left dlPFC. However, one of the most consistent findings of meta-analyses of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies is the underactivation of rIFC during tasks of cognitive control (Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Lukito et al., Reference Lukito, Norman, Carlisi, Radua, Hart, Simonoff and Rubia2020; McCarthy, Skokauskas, & Frodl, Reference McCarthy, Skokauskas and Frodl2014; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016; Rubia, Reference Rubia2018). The rIFC is a cognitive control ‘hub’ region, playing a key role in cognitive control (Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack, Reference Aron, Robbins and Poldrack2014; Langner, Leiberg, Hoffstaedter, & Eickhoff, Reference Langner, Leiberg, Hoffstaedter and Eickhoff2018), sustained attention (Kim, Reference Kim2014; Langner & Eickhoff, Reference Langner and Eickhoff2013; Petersen & Posner, Reference Petersen and Posner2012) and timing networks (Radua, del Pozo, Gómez, Guillen-Grima, & Ortuño, Reference Radua, del Pozo, Gómez, Guillen-Grima and Ortuño2014), mediating key functions of impairment in ADHD (Coghill et al., Reference Coghill, Toplak, Rhodes, Adamo, Banaschewski, Coghill and Zuddas2018; Noreika, Falter, & Rubia, Reference Noreika, Falter and Rubia2013). The rIFC dysfunction has also been shown to be disorder-specific to ADHD compared to several neurodevelopmental disorders in comparative fMRI meta-analyses of cognitive control (Lukito et al., Reference Lukito, Norman, Carlisi, Radua, Hart, Simonoff and Rubia2020; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016; Rubia, Reference Rubia2018). Further, its upregulation is the most consistent effect of single-dose and longer-term psychostimulant medication (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016; Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Alegria, Cubillo, Smith, Brammer and Radua2014).

Only three published studies applied tDCS over rIFC in ADHD, in one or five sessions. One found improvements after anodal relative to sham tDCS in Flanker task errors and intrasubject response variability in 21 adolescents with ADHD (Breitling et al., Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2016), but no effect in 14 children with ADHD on a combined n-back and Stop task with high definition or conventional anodal tDCS (Breitling et al., Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2020). In 20 undiagnosed high-school students with ADHD symptoms, anodal compared to sham tDCS improved Go accuracy in a Go/No-Go (GNG) task but no other inhibitory or Stroop task measures (Soltaninejad, Nejati, & Ekhtiari, Reference Soltaninejad, Nejati and Ekhtiari2015b). To our knowledge, no sham-controlled study so far has measured clinical and cognitive effects of multi-session anodal tDCS over rIFC in combination with CT in patients with ADHD

In this double-blind, sham-controlled, randomised controlled trial (RCT), 50 children and adolescents with ADHD were administered 15 consecutive weekday sessions of anodal or sham tDCS over rIFC combined with CT of EF typically impaired in ADHD. The primary outcome measures were parent-rated clinical symptoms and cognitive performance in an inhibition and a sustained-attention task. Secondary outcome measures were other clinical, safety and EF measures.

Given some evidence of clinical and cognitive improvements with anodal tDCS over the dlPFC (Cachoeira et al., Reference Cachoeira, Leffa, Mittelstadt, Mendes, Brunoni, Pinto and Schestatsky2017; Soff et al., Reference Soff, Sotnikova, Christiansen, Becker and Siniatchkin2017) or rIFC (Breitling et al., Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2016; Soltaninejad, Nejati, & Ekhtiari, Reference Soltaninejad, Nejati and Ekhtiari2015a) and prolonged effects when tDCS is paired with CT, we hypothesised that, compared to sham, at posttreatment, multi-session anodal tDCS of rIFC with multi-EF training would show greater improvement in ADHD symptoms and/or performance on EF tasks mediated by rIFC and targeted by the CT. We also hypothesised that clinical and EF task improvement would persist 6 months after posttreatment. Finally, we hypothesised no side or adverse effects.

Materials and methods

Trial design

This pre-registered (ISRCTN48265228), double-blind, sham-controlled, parallel RCT compared multi-session anodal v. sham tDCS over the rIFC combined with multi-EF training. Randomisation to stimulation group was stratified by age (10–14.5 and 14.5–18 years) and medication status (naïve and non-naïve) using Smith randomisation (Kaiser, Reference Kaiser2012; Smith, Reference Smith1984) conducted by Innosphere Ltd (Haifa, Israel). Experimenters, participants and parents/caregivers were blind to stimulation group. This trial received local research ethics committee approval (RECID: 17/LO/0983) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Hopewell, Schulz, Montori, Gotzsche, Devereaux and Altman2010).

Participants

Fifty male participants (10–18 years) were recruited from South London clinics, social media and parent support groups (February 2018–20). All participants had a clinical DSM-5 diagnosis of ADHD established by a clinician, and validated by the Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL; DSM-IV) (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci and Ryan1997) and the Conners 3rd Edition–Parent (Conners 3-P, cut-off t score > 60) (Conners, Pitkanen, & Rzepa, Reference Conners, Pitkanen, Rzepa, Kreutzer, DeLuca and Caplan2011). Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) was excluded using a combination (both required) of both the parent-rated Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ, cut-off > 17) (Eaves, Wingert, Ho, & Mickelson, Reference Eaves, Wingert, Ho and Mickelson2006) and the pro-social scale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ, cut-off < 5) (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997); for participants falling outside these criteria, the absence of ASD was confirmed by their clinician. Further exclusion criteria were IQ below 80 (Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, WASI-II) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011), history of alcohol or substance abuse, neurological illness, comorbid major psychiatric disorders as established by the K-SADS-PL or clinical diagnoses [except for conduct disorder/oppositional defiant disorder (ODD)], contraindications to tDCS [e.g. metallic implants (except dental braces), previous neurosurgical procedures, history of migraine, diseased/damaged skin on the scalp], treated with drugs that lower seizure thresholds (e.g. antipsychotics, tricyclic antidepressants), and an inability to provide consent from the legal caregiver for children under 16 years or from participants over 16 years (Fig. 1). Participants received £540 plus travel expenses for participating.

Fig. 1. CONSORT flow diagram (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Hopewell, Schulz, Montori, Gotzsche, Devereaux and Altman2010) of this RCT from enrolment, intervention allocation, follow-up and analysis.

Baseline assessment was scheduled at least 2 weeks after medication titration. Thirty-two participants received stable ADHD medication (non-psychostimulants: 2; psychostimulants: 30; between 21 weeks and 10 years). To help avoid medication effects from masking tDCS effects, 14 of the 32 participants followed our optional request to abstain from psychostimulants at least 24 h before each assessment time point (range: 24–72 h), of which eight abstained only 24 h beforehand (sham, 4; anodal tDCS, 4). A further seven participants abstained for trial duration (i.e. baseline to posttreatment). One participant stopped taking medication 3 months before follow-up.

Intervention

Over 15 consecutive weekdays, participants played four 5 min CT games during 20 min of anodal or sham tDCS. CT was composed of four ACTIVATETM (Wexler et al., Reference Wexler, Iseli, Leon, Zaggle, Rush, Goodman and Bo2016) games (Monday, Wednesday and Friday) or a Stop task (Rubia, Smith, & Taylor, Reference Rubia, Smith and Taylor2007) followed by three ACTIVATETM games (Tuesday and Thursday). ACTIVATETM games were played on an iPad Air 2 10.2″, the Stop task on a Dell XPS 15.6″ Touch Laptop (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Schematic overview of the study design. ADHD-RS, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder-Rating Scale; ARI, Affective Reactivity Index; CIS, Columbia Impairment Scale; Conners 3-P, Conners' 3rd Edition Parent Rating; Cognitive battery, Maudsley Attention and Response Suppression task battery, vigilance, Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, visual-spatial working memory, verbal fluency; K-SADS-PL, Kiddie-SADS-Present and Lifetime Version; MEWS, Mind Excessively Wandering Scale; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire (Lifetime), SDQ, Social Difficulties Questionnaire (prosocial scale only); WASI-II, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, 2nd Edition; WREMB-R, Weekly Rating of Evening and Morning Behavior-Revised.

CT games

ACTIVATETM trains multiple EF with engaging video games that increase in complexity as participants improve. Five of the six games were selected to target ADHD-relevant EF, including selective and multiple simultaneous attention, inhibition and cognitive flexibility (i.e. Magic Lens, Peter's Printer Panic, Treasure Trunk) and visual-spatial working memory (i.e. Grub Ahoy, Monkey Trouble). Before each game, participants chose a game to play from three options; the ACTIVATETM programme tracked choices and constrained future options so that multiple EF were twice as likely to be trained as working memory (Wexler et al., Reference Wexler, Iseli, Leon, Zaggle, Rush, Goodman and Bo2016).

The training tracking Stop task trained motor response inhibition by requiring participants to cancel a prepotent motor response to a go signal that is followed shortly by an unexpected and rare (30% of trials) stop signal (Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Smith and Taylor2007). The delay between go and stop signals is dynamically adjusted to the participant performance, with better inhibition resulting in longer stop signal delays thus increasing the difficulty to withhold a response. Participants were encouraged to wait for the stop signal before responding to the go signal, thereby training inhibition and waiting behaviour/response delay (see online Supplementary material).

tDCS

Direct current was delivered by NovoStim (InnoSphere, Haifa, Israel) via a pair of 25 cm2 brush electrodesFootnote Footnote 1 dipped in saline solution (0.45 mol). Using the EEG 10–20, the anode was placed over rIFC (F8) (Okamoto et al., Reference Okamoto, Dan, Sakamoto, Takeo, Shimizu, Kohno and Dan2004; Okamoto & Dan, Reference Okamoto and Dan2005), the cathode over the contralateral supraorbital area (Fp1), and held in place by a 10–20 EEG cap. For anodal tDCS, 1 mA was administered for 20 min (current density: 0.04 cm2; total charge: 0.8 C/cm2) with a 30 s fade-in/fade-out. For sham stimulation, only the 30 s fade-in/fade-out was administered (Palm et al., Reference Palm, Reisinger, Keeser, Kuo, Pogarell, Leicht and Padberg2013). After each week, participants, parents and tDCS administrators were asked to guess if participants had received anodal or sham tDCS.

Primary outcomes

Cognitive

The adult version of the Maudsley Attention and Response Suppression (MARS) task battery (Penadés et al., Reference Penadés, Catalán, Rubia, Andrés, Salamero and Gastó2007; Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Smith and Taylor2007) measured motor response inhibition [GNG; dependent variable (DV): probability of inhibition (PI)] and sustained attention [Continuous Performance Task (CPT); DV: omission and commission errors] (see online Supplementary material).

ADHD symptoms

Parent-rated ADHD Rating Scale-IV (ADHD-RS) Home Version (DV: Total Scores) (DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos, & Reid, Reference DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid1998), a standard measure of treatment effects in DSM-IV ADHD symptoms.

Secondary outcomes

Cognitive measures

MARS tasks (Penadés et al., Reference Penadés, Catalán, Rubia, Andrés, Salamero and Gastó2007; Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Smith and Taylor2007) measured interference inhibition (Simon Task; DV: Simon reaction time effect) or time discrimination (Time Discrimination Task; DV: percentage correct). The Mackworth Clock Task (Lichstein, Riedel, & Richman, Reference Lichstein, Riedel and Richman2000; Mackworth, Reference Mackworth1948; PsyTookit, 2017) measured vigilance (DV: percentage omissions and commission errors), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (PsyToolkit, 2018) (WCST; DV: total and preservative errors) measured cognitive flexibility, and the C8 Sciences version of the NIH List Sorting Working Memory (WM) Task (DV: total score) (Tulsky et al., Reference Tulsky, Carlozzi, Chiaravalloti, Beaumont, Kisala and Gershon2014) measured visuo-spatial working memory. Phonetic and semantic fluency (DV: percentage of correct responses) (Troyer, Reference Troyer2000) were measured to account for potential downregulation of lIFC mediated language production functions via interhemispheric inhibition.

Generic EF

Three intercorrelated generic, task-independent EF were measured, which included: (1) general speed of information processing [i.e. mean reaction times (MRT) for Go (GNG), Congruent (Simon) and Target (CPT) trials, weighted by number of trials] (2); intrasubject response variability [i.e. Coefficient of Variance (CV; s.d. of MRT divided by MRT) for Go (GNG), Congruent (Simon) and Target (CPT) trials]; and (3) prematurity (i.e. premature responses to all trials in GNG, Simon and CPT) (online Supplementary Tables S1–S5 and Figs S1–S3).

ADHD symptoms and related impairments

Caregiver-rated Conners 3-P ADHD Index (Conners et al., Reference Conners, Pitkanen, Rzepa, Kreutzer, DeLuca and Caplan2011) measured ADHD symptoms, Weekly Parent Ratings of Evening and Morning Behaviour-Revised scale (WREMB-R) (Wehmeier, Dittmann, Schacht, Helsberg, & Lehmkuhl, Reference Wehmeier, Dittmann, Schacht, Helsberg and Lehmkuhl2009) and the Columbia Impairment Scale-Parent version (CIS) (Bird et al., Reference Bird, Shaffer, Fisher and Gould1993) measured ADHD-related difficulties and functional impairments, the child- and caregiver-rated Affective Reactivity Index (ARI) measured irritability (Stringaris et al., Reference Stringaris, Goodman, Ferdinando, Razdan, Muhrer, Leibenluft and Brotman2012), and the child-rated Mind Excessively Wandering Scale (MEWS) measured mind-wandering (Mowlem et al., Reference Mowlem, Skirrow, Reid, Maltezos, Nijjar, Merwood and Asherson2019).

Safety, feasibility and tolerability measures

Safety was measured with parent-rated questionnaires on side effects (Hill & Taylor, Reference Hill and Taylor2001) and adverse events (Döpfner, Lehmkuhl, & Steinhausen, Reference Döpfner, Lehmkuhl and Steinhausen2006). Participants rated their mood and wakefulness via a visual analogue scale (Alegria et al., Reference Alegria, Wulff, Brinson, Barker, Norman, Brandeis and Rubia2017; Maurizio et al., Reference Maurizio, Liechti, Heinrich, Jaencke, Steinhausen, Walitza and Drechsler2014) and tDCS tolerability (Antal et al., Reference Antal, Alekseichuk, Bikson, Brockmöller, Brunoni, Chen and Paulus2017). Parents and participants rated the overall impression of tDCS and CT using a rating scale from 0 (never) to 4 (always) designed for this study.

EEG

EEG was measured at rest and during the GNG task at each assessment timepoint but will be reported in a separate publication.

Training outcome measures

The ACTIVATETM DV was the highest game level reached each week averaged across three games that loaded on multiple EF (i.e. Treasure Trunk, Magic Lens, Printer Panic). The Stop task DV was PI on average for each week.

Assessment time points

Primary and secondary outcome measures were collected at baseline, posttreatment and 6-month follow-up. Adverse events and overall impression of tDCS and CT were measured at posttreatment. Ratings of mood, wakefulness and the tolerability of tDCS were measured in each stimulation session. Baseline and posttreatment sessions were scheduled up to 3 weeks before the first and after the last stimulation session, respectively.

Statistical methods

Confirmatory analysis

Repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) tested group differences across posttreatment and follow-up, covarying for age in months, medication status (naïve, on- or off-medication) and baseline value of outcomes where applicable. Sensitivity analyses removed statistical outliers on cognitive tasks, participants who changed medication between posttreatment and follow-up, or participants whose treatment spanned over 4 not 3 weeks (online Supplementary Tables S6–S8). The α level was 0.05, and corrected for multiple testing with False Discovery Rate (FDR) with Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). Only results with adjusted p values are reported (for unadjusted, see Tables 23). Analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics 26 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Medication status

Medication status was coded as a categorical covariate with three groups: medication-naïve, on-medication and off-medication. Participants who abstained for the treatment trial period were coded as off-medication, participants who abstained at assessment time points only were coded as off-medication for cognitive outcomes and on-medication for clinical outcomes (which can cover 3 weeks).

Missing data and statistical outliers

In primary and secondary outcomes, only posttreatment WM task data for one participant were missing. Missing stop signal task data (1.33%) were random and replaced by group averages.

Power analysis

An a priori power analysis conducted prior to trial registration indicated that a minimum of 40 participants would be adequate to detect a meaningful effect on clinical and cognitive measures (Ditye, Jacobson, Walsh, & Lavidor, Reference Ditye, Jacobson, Walsh and Lavidor2012; Jacobson, Ezra, Berger, & Lavidor, Reference Jacobson, Ezra, Berger and Lavidor2012).

Results

Baseline demographics

Compared to sham tDCS, the anodal tDCS group was significantly younger; had fewer years in education; higher ADHD-RS Total Scores and ODD symptoms; worse performance on the Macworth Clock, Time Discrimination and list sort working memory tasks; and spent more time playing Peter's Printer Panic (Table 1).

Table 1. Baseline demographic, clinical, cognitive measures and medication status; the number of tDCS and CT sessions; and the time spent playing each CT game in the sham and anodal tDCS groups

ADHD-RS, Caregiver-rated ADHD Rating Scale; ARI, Affective Reactivity Index; CIS, Columbia Impairment Scale-Parent; MEWS, Mind Excessively Wandering Scale; s.d., standard deviation; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; SDQ, Social Difficulties Questionnaire; WASI-II, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WREMB-R, Weekly Parent Ratings of Evening and Morning Behaviour-Revised. Significance group differences are highlighted in bold.

a One participant could not attend three stimulation sessions due to extreme weather conditions.

Primary outcome measures

Cognitive

There were no significant effects on primary cognitive outcome measures after adjusting for multiple testing (Table 2; online Supplementary Fig. S4).

Table 2. Summary of adjusted average performance on primary cognitive and clinical outcome measures after sham and anodal tDCS combined with CT

ADHD-RS, ADHD Rating Scale; PI, probability of inhibition; s.d., standard deviation.

Benjamini–Hochberg adjusted p values given in parentheses.

a Adjusted values as predicted by the repeated-measures ANCOVA testing group differences at posttreatment and follow-up, adjusting for baseline, age at entry and medication status (naïve, off-medication, on-medication).

b Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment was applied to p values for time, group and time by group interaction effect separately and was applied separately to primary cognitive, secondary cognitive and secondary clinical outcome measures separately.

c Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment was not applied to these measures.

ADHD symptoms

There was only a significant time by group interaction (F 1,44 = 10.58, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.19); simple effects analysis showed that the anodal v. sham tDCS group had higher scores at posttreatment but not at follow-up [posttreatment: p = 0.011 (95% CI 1.65–12.21); follow-up: p = 0.20 (95% CI −9.30 to 1.97)]. To determine what drove this effect, exploratory simple effects analysis of subscales showed that the anodal v. sham tDCS group had higher scores at posttreatment on Inattention (p = 0.03) and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity subscales (p = 0.06), with the latter being lower for active v. sham tDCS at follow-up (p = 0.07) (Table 2; online Supplementary Fig. S5).

Secondary outcomes measures

Cognitive

The were no significant effects on secondary cognitive outcome measures after adjusting for multiple testing (Table 3).

Table 3. Summary of adjusted average performance on secondary cognitive and clinical outcome measures after sham and anodal tDCS combined with CT

ARI, Affective Reactivity Index; CIS, Columbia Impairment Scale-Parent; Corr, Correct; ICV, intrasubject coefficient of variance; MEWS, Mind Excessively Wandering Scale; MRT, mean reaction times; Resp, responses; s.d., standard deviation; WREMB-R, Weekly Parent Ratings of Evening and Morning Behaviour-Revised.

Benjamini–Hochberg adjusted p values given in parentheses.

a Adjusted values as predicted by the repeated-measures ANCOVA testing group differences at posttreatment and follow-up, adjusting for baseline, age at entry and medication status (naïve, off-medication, on-medication).

b Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment was applied to p values for time, group and time by group interaction effects separately and was applied separately to primary cognitive, secondary cognitive and secondary clinical outcome measures separately.

c Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment was not applied to these measures.

ADHD symptoms and related impairments

There was only a significant time by group interaction for Conners 3-P ADHD Index (F 1,44 = 13.726, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.238). Simple effects analysis showed significantly higher scores following anodal v. sham tDCS at posttreatment only [p = 0.001 (95% CI 1.92 to −7.11); follow-up: p = 0.73 (95% CI −3.39 to 2.41)] (Table 3).

Exploratory analyses

Separate repeated-measures ANOVAs showed an effect of time (baseline v. posttreatment or follow-up) across groups. In cognitive measures, both groups improved in GNG, Simon, Letter Fluency tasks (baseline v. posttreatment or follow-up); Speed of Processing (baseline v. posttreatment only); and CPT % Omissions and Commissions, Mackworth Clock % Omissions, WM, Response Variability, and Prematurity (baseline v. follow-up only) (online Supplementary Table S9). In clinical measures, both groups improved in ADHD-RS and Conners 3-P Index (baseline v. posttreatment or follow-up); and in ARI Parent and Child, WREMB-R, and CIS (baseline v. posttreatment only).

Group differences in CT performance across the 3 weeks were explored with repeated-measures ANCOVAs covarying for baseline, age, medication status and total time spent playing each game. There were no significant effects after adjusting for multiple testing (online Supplementary Table S10).

We also explored if outcome changes that showed a significant time effect across both groups from baseline to posttreatment or follow-up were correlated with changes in CT performance scores (week 3 minus week 1). No correlations were significant (online Supplementary Table S11).

Given that age was not matched between groups, we conducted a post-hoc moderation analysis (Hayes, Reference Hayes2013), predicting a change in ADHD-RS (baseline minus posttreatment) in a regression analysis of stimulation group, age and a stimulation by age interaction. While the stimulation by age interaction was not significant [β = 0.2, s.e. = 0.12, t(46) = 1.7, p = 0.096], the simple effects showed a significantly reduced improvement for anodal tDCS v. sham in older participants [1s.d. above mean age; β = 9.53, s.e. = 4.26, t(46) = 2.24, p = 0.03], and an opposite but not significant pattern in younger children [1s.d. below mean age; β = −0.6, s.e. = 4.11, t(46) = −0.15, p = 0.88], indicating that older participants benefitted clinically less from anodal v. sham tDCS.

Safety, feasibility, tolerability and blinding integrity.

There were no significant group differences in ratings of mood, wakefulness, overall impression of tDCS and CT (online Supplementary Tables S12 and S13), and side effects (Table 3). Adverse effects were significantly higher after anodal v. sham tDCS at posttreatment (F 1,44 = 4.09, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.08), driven mainly by higher parent-ratings for ‘he seems more grumpy and irritable’, ‘has little appetite’ and ‘has more problems falling asleep’ (Table 3). Stimulation was well tolerated, with only significantly higher reports of burning sensation during anodal than sham tDCS (online Supplementary Table S14). Group assignment guesses were not above chance for experimenters [χ2(1) = 3.9, p = 0.28], participants [χ2(1) = 1.85, p = 0.17] and borderline for parent guesses for anodal tDCS [χ2(1) = 3.57, p = 0.06], thus blinding was overall successful.

Discussion

This double-blind, sham-controlled RCT administered 15 sessions of anodal or sham tDCS over rIFC combined with CT in 50 boys with ADHD, and found no improvement in ADHD symptoms or cognitive performance. Although both groups improved in clinical and cognitive measures over time, anodal relative to sham tDCS was associated with higher primary (ADHD-RS) and secondary (Conners 3-P ADHD Index) clinical outcome measures. Side effects did not differ, but at posttreatment, adverse effects relating to mood, sleep and appetite were higher following anodal than sham tDCS.

The lack of an observable clinical or cognitive effect extend previous meta-analytic evidence of no significant cognitive effects and limited evidence of clinical effects in ADHD with 1–5 anodal tDCS sessions over predominantly left dlPFC (Westwood et al., Reference Westwood, Radua and Rubia2020). These findings are unexpected given that rIFC underactivation is consistently associated with poor cognitive control, attention and clinical symptoms in ADHD (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Lukito et al., Reference Lukito, Norman, Carlisi, Radua, Hart, Simonoff and Rubia2020; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016; Rubia, Reference Rubia2018). While the findings of no clinical effect of tDCS of rIFC are novel, these extend evidence of no or moderate effects from prior 1–5 session sham-controlled tDCS studies stimulating rIFC in ADHD (see introduction) (Breitling et al., Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2016, Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2020; Soltaninejad et al., Reference Soltaninejad, Nejati and Ekhtiari2015a).

Findings are furthermore unexpected given evidence of a synergistic effect of combined CT and tDCS on improving cognition (Allenby et al., Reference Allenby, Falcone, Bernardo, Wileyto, Rostain, Ramsay and Loughead2018; Au et al., Reference Au, Katz, Buschkuehl, Bunarjo, Senger, Zabel and Jonides2016; Katz et al., Reference Katz, Au, Buschkuehl, Abagis, Zabel, Jaeggi and Jonides2017). Although we covaried for age, one possible explanation for the negative findings on clinical symptoms and cognition is that the anodal tDCS group were significantly younger with larger baseline clinical and cognitive impairments compared to sham, both of which could have impaired learning (Loe & Feldman, Reference Loe and Feldman2007). This is supported by evidence that ADHD children with worse neurocognitive skills at baseline show less CT gains (Minder, Zuberer, Brandeis, & Drechsler, Reference Minder, Zuberer, Brandeis and Drechsler2019) and neurofeedback learning (Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Halder, Blankertz, Sannelli, Dickhaus, Kleih and Kübler2012; Lam et al., Reference Lam, Criaud, Alegria, Barker, Giampietro and Rubia2020; Zilverstand et al., Reference Zilverstand, Sorger, Slaats-Willemse, Kan, Goebel and Buitelaar2017), while healthy controls with poorer cognitive performance or lower education achievement at baseline can nullify or lead to detrimental tDCS effects (Hsu, Juan, & Tseng, Reference Hsu, Juan and Tseng2016; Jones & Berryhill, Reference Jones and Berryhill2012).

Alternatively, given the stronger electric field strengths in children v. adults (Minhas, Bikson, Woods, Rosen, & Kessler, Reference Minhas, Bikson, Woods, Rosen and Kessler2012; Salehinejad et al., Reference Salehinejad, Nejati, Mosayebi-Samani, Mohammadi, Wischnewski, Kuo and Nitsche2020), multiple tDCS sessions may have triggered a homeostatic plasticity response – i.e. the amount and direction of plasticity was attenuated in response to excessive increases in neuronal excitability – thereby temporarily disrupting the excitability of rIFC (Fricke et al., Reference Fricke, Seeber, Thirugnanasambandam, Paulus, Nitsche and Rothwell2011; Hoy & Fitzgerald, Reference Hoy and Fitzgerald2015; Karabanov et al., Reference Karabanov, Ziemann, Hamada, George, Quartarone, Classen and Siebner2015; Wefelmeyer & Burrone, Reference Wefelmeyer and Burrone2015). This is in line with our post-hoc moderation analysis that revealed older but not younger participants improved less in the ADHD-RS Total Scores in the anodal v. sham tDCS group at posttreatment. Future studies should verify if tDCS has differential effects depending on current strength and age of participants with ADHD.

Another possibility is that rIFC stimulation downregulated neighbouring dorsal prefrontal or parietal regions part of the dorsal attention network (Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor, & Rubia, Reference Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor and Rubia2012; Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013), or left hemispheric prefrontal regions that mediate positive emotions (Gainotti, Reference Gainotti2019; Groenewold, Opmeer, de Jonge, Aleman, & Costafreda, Reference Groenewold, Opmeer, de Jonge, Aleman and Costafreda2013).

Interestingly, however, impulsiveness/hyperactivity symptoms, which are most closely associated with rIFC activation (Rubia, Reference Rubia2018; Rubia, Smith, Brammer, Toone, & Taylor, Reference Rubia, Smith, Brammer, Toone and Taylor2005), were lower at follow-up after anodal relative to sham tDCS. This finding – that needs replication – could suggest longer-term neuroplastic consolidation effects as have been shown in other neurotherapies, such as neurofeedback (Alegria et al., Reference Alegria, Wulff, Brinson, Barker, Norman, Brandeis and Rubia2017; Enriquez-Geppert, Smit, Pimenta, & Arns, Reference Enriquez-Geppert, Smit, Pimenta and Arns2019).

Both groups improved in symptoms and cognitive performance from baseline to posttreatment or follow-up, which could suggest gains due to CT (Wexler et al., Reference Wexler, Iseli, Leon, Zaggle, Rush, Goodman and Bo2016); however, given the lack of correlation with CT performance, placebo effects cannot be ruled out.

The negative findings from this trial are crucial given that tDCS is being increasingly incorporated into clinical practice, is considered an acceptable alternative to medication by parents, and is already commercially available (Buchanan, D'Angiulli, Samson, Maisonneuve, & Robaey, Reference Buchanan, D'Angiulli, Samson, Maisonneuve and Robaey2020; Sierawska et al., Reference Sierawska, Prehn-Kristensen, Moliadze, Krauel, Nowak, Freitag and Buyx2019). Particularly alarming is that parent-rated ADHD symptoms and adverse effects were higher at posttreatment after anodal tDCS relative to sham.

Findings are not encouraging for the efficacy of multi-session tDCS of rIFC combined with CT in ADHD. However, there are limitations. Although our sample of 50 participants is the largest sample of any tDCS study in paediatric ADHD, recent meta-analyses report relatively small cognitive effects of tDCS in ADHD (Westwood et al., Reference Westwood, Radua and Rubia2020), suggesting that larger samples may be required to detect tDCS effects. Unfortunately, despite randomisation, groups were not balanced in several baseline measures which could have confounded the tDCS effects. However, measures that differed (i.e. clinical scores, performance, age and medication status) were covaried, minimising any confounding effects. Also, the anodal group had higher ODD symptoms at baseline, which could have affected emotional lability and confounded tDCS effects. However, no group differences were observed in changes of emotional dysregulation as measured on the ARI or in mood ratings. The findings in this study are limited to male adolescents with ADHD and the adopted protocol, and cannot be generalised to other populations (e.g. females; adults) or other protocols. Although we applied the largest number of sessions with the view of boosting consolidation of tDCS effects, the optimal number of sessions is not known in ADHD, leaving the possibility that fewer or even more sessions would optimise the tDCS effect (Au et al., Reference Au, Katz, Buschkuehl, Bunarjo, Senger, Zabel and Jonides2016; Katz et al., Reference Katz, Au, Buschkuehl, Abagis, Zabel, Jaeggi and Jonides2017). Computational current flow models suggest higher stimulation intensities might be required to modulate clinical symptoms and cognitive functions mediated by rIFC given this is a deeper region compared to the commonly stimulated dlPFC (Salehinejad et al., Reference Salehinejad, Nejati, Mosayebi-Samani, Mohammadi, Wischnewski, Kuo and Nitsche2020). Further, this study stimulated ‘F8’ in line with other studies (Breitling et al., Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2016, Reference Breitling, Zaehle, Dannhauer, Tegelbeckers, Flechtner and Krauel2020; Campanella et al., Reference Campanella, Schroder, Monnart, Vanderhasselt, Duprat, Rabijns and Baeken2016, Reference Campanella, Schroder, Vanderhasselt, Baeken, Kornreich, Verbanck and Burle2018; Dambacher et al., Reference Dambacher, Schuhmann, Lobbestael, Arntz, Brugman and Sack2015; Soltaninejad et al., Reference Soltaninejad, Nejati and Ekhtiari2015a); however, improved performance on inhibitory control tasks in healthy adults has been reported when stimulating T4-Fz and F8-Cz intersection (Cunillera, Brignani, Cucurell, Fuentemilla, & Miniussi, Reference Cunillera, Brignani, Cucurell, Fuentemilla and Miniussi2016; Jacobson, Javitt, & Lavidor, Reference Jacobson, Javitt and Lavidor2011, Reference Jacobson, Ezra, Berger and Lavidor2012; Stramaccia et al., Reference Stramaccia, Penolazzi, Sartori, Braga, Mondini and Galfano2015) or F6 (Cai et al., Reference Cai, Li, Liu, Li, Feng, Wang and Xue2016; Gómez-Ariza, Martín, & Morales, Reference Gómez-Ariza, Martín and Morales2017; Hogeveen et al., Reference Hogeveen, Grafman, Aboseria, David, Bikson and Hauner2016; Sallard, Mouthon, De Pretto, & Spierer, Reference Sallard, Mouthon, De Pretto and Spierer2018), which cover the rIFC along with areas closer to the surface implicated in motor inhibition (e.g. superior and middle frontal cortex, and the supplementary motor area) (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; McCarthy et al., Reference McCarthy, Skokauskas and Frodl2014; Zhang, Geng, & Lee, Reference Zhang, Geng and Lee2017) and attention (e.g. right dlPFC, part of the dorsal attention network and typically underactivated in ADHD) (Cubillo et al., Reference Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor and Rubia2012; Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013). Another limitation is that we could not test for weekly dose effects as ADHD symptoms were only measured at baseline, posttreatment and follow-up; yet weekly changes in CT performance did not show dose effects.

Larger, double-blind, randomised-controlled trials should systematically investigate optimal and ideally individualised stimulation protocols (e.g. different stimulation sites, intensity, duration, number of sessions, etc.) measuring clinical, cognitive and possible non-targeted cognitive outcomes. Stimulating T4-Fz and F8-Cz intersection and F6 could potentially be more effective for improving inhibitory control and attention functions in ADHD.

Conclusion

This rigorously conducted double-blind, randomised, sham-controlled trial of 15-weekday sessions of anodal tDCS over rIFC combined with CT in 50 boys with ADHD showed no clinical or cognitive improvement. Findings suggest that rIFC stimulation may not be indicated as a neurotherapy for cognitive or clinical remediation for ADHD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001859

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the families and children who took part in this study, and the South London and Maudsley NHS Trust and local parent groups for their support and participation in this study. We would like to thank Yuanyuan Yang for her help in recruiting participants and data collection.

Author contributions

Conceptualisation: Samuel J. Westwood, Marion Criaud, Sheut-Ling Lam, Steve Lukito, Bruce Wexler, Roi Cohen-Kadosh, Philip Asherson and Katya Rubia. Data curation: Samuel J. Westwood, Sophie Wallace-Hanlon and Afroditi Kostara. Formal analysis: Samuel J. Westwood, Marion Criaud, Sheut-Ling Lam, Steve Lukito, Deborah Agbedjro, Roi Cohen-Kadosh and Katya Rubia. Funding acquisition: Bruce Wexler, Roi Cohen-Kadosh, Philip Asherson and Katya Rubia. Investigation: Samuel J. Westwood, Marion Criaud, Sheut-Ling Lam, Steve Lukito, Sophie Wallace-Hanlon, Olivia S. Kowalczyk, Afroditi Kostara and Joseph Mathew. Methodology: Samuel J. Westwood, Marion Criaud, Sheut-Ling Lam, Steve Lukito, Olivia S. Kowalczyk, Bruce Wexler, Roi Cohen-Kadosh, Philip Asherson and Katya Rubia. Project administration: Samuel J. Westwood and Katya Rubia. Resources: Bruce Wexler, Roi Cohen-Kadosh, Philip Asherson and Katya Rubia. Software: Marion Criaud, Bruce Wexler and Katya Rubia. Supervision: Samuel J. Westwood and Katya Rubia. Validation: Samuel J. Westwood and Katya Rubia. Visualisation: Samuel J. Westwood and Katya Rubia. Writing – original draft preparation: Samuel J. Westwood and Katya Rubia. Writing – review and editing: Samuel J. Westwood, Marion Criaud, Sheut-Ling Lam, Steve Lukito, Sophie Wallace-Hanlon, Olivia S. Kowalczyk, Deborah Agbedjro, Bruce Wexler, Roi Cohen-Kadosh and Katya Rubia.

Financial support

This work was supported by grants from Action Medical Research (GN2426), the Garfield Weston Foundation, and National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and the Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, and King's College London to KR. KR has received additional research support for other projects from the Medical Research Council (MR/P012647/1), and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and the Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. The funders were not involved in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflict of interest

Bruce E. Wexler is Chief Scientist and an equity holder in the Yale Start Up company C8 Sciences that sells the cognitive training program evaluated in this study. RCK serves on the scientific advisory boards for Neuroelectrics and Innosphere. PA reports honoraria for consultancy to Shire/Takeda, Eli Lilly and Novartis; educational and research awards from Shire, Lilly, Novartis, Vifor Pharma, GW Pharma and QbTech; and speaking at sponsored events for Shire, Lilly, Flynn Pharma and Novartis. KR has received funding from Takeda pharmaceuticals for another project.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 The saline solution was held in tufts by capillary forces between strands, which prevented it from spreading/dripping.

References

Alegria, A. A., Wulff, M., Brinson, H., Barker, G. J., Norman, L. J., Brandeis, D., … Rubia, K. (2017). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 38(6), 31903209. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23584CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allenby, C., Falcone, M., Bernardo, L., Wileyto, E. P., Rostain, A., Ramsay, J. R., … Loughead, J. (2018). Transcranial direct current brain stimulation decreases impulsivity in ADHD. Brain Stimulation, 11(5), 974981. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2018.04.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Antal, A., Alekseichuk, I., Bikson, M., Brockmöller, J., Brunoni, A. R., Chen, R., … Paulus, W. (2017). Low intensity transcranial electric stimulation: Safety, ethical, legal regulatory and application guidelines. Clinical Neurophysiology, 128(9), 17741809. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 177185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, J., Karsten, C., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2017). Optimizing transcranial direct current stimulation protocols to promote long-term learning. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1(1), 6572. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0007-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Au, J., Katz, B., Buschkuehl, M., Bunarjo, K., Senger, T., Zabel, C., … Jonides, J. (2016). Enhancing working memory training with transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(9), 14191432. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00979CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300, JSTOR.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bikson, M., Grossman, P., Thomas, C., Zannou, A. L., Jiang, J., Adnan, T., … Woods, A. J. (2016). Safety of transcranial direct current stimulation: Evidence based update 2016. Brain Stimulation, 9(5), 641661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.06.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, H. R., Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., & Gould, M. S. (1993). The Columbia Impairment Scale (CIS): Pilot findings on a measure of global impairment for children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 3(3), 167176.Google Scholar
Breitling, C., Zaehle, T., Dannhauer, M., Bonath, B., Tegelbeckers, J., Flechtner, H.-H., … Krauel, K. (2016). Improving interference control in ADHD patients with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 10(72), 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2016.00072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breitling, C., Zaehle, T., Dannhauer, M., Tegelbeckers, J., Flechtner, H.-H., & Krauel, K. (2020). Comparison between conventional and HD-tDCS of the right inferior frontal gyrus in children and adolescents with ADHD. Clinical Neurophysiology, 131, 11461154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2019.12.412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, D. M., D'Angiulli, A., Samson, A., Maisonneuve, A. R., & Robaey, P. (2020). Acceptability of transcranial direct current stimulation in children and adolescents with ADHD: The point of view of parents. Journal of Health Psychology, 111. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105320937059.Google ScholarPubMed
Cachoeira, C. T., Leffa, D. T., Mittelstadt, S. D., Mendes, L. S. T., Brunoni, A. R., Pinto, J. V., … Schestatsky, P. (2017). Positive effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder A pilot randomized controlled study. Psychiatry Research, 247, 2832. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, Y., Li, S., Liu, J., Li, D., Feng, Z., Wang, Q., … Xue, G. (2016). The role of the frontal and parietal cortex in proactive and reactive inhibitory control: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(1), 177186. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00888CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campanella, S., Schroder, E., Monnart, A., Vanderhasselt, M.-A., Duprat, R., Rabijns, M., … Baeken, C. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation over the right frontal inferior cortex decreases neural activity needed to achieve inhibition: A double-blind ERP study in a male population. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 48(3), 176188. https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059416645977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campanella, S., Schroder, E., Vanderhasselt, M.-A., Baeken, C., Kornreich, C., Verbanck, P., & Burle, B. (2018). Short-term impact of tDCS over the right inferior frontal cortex on impulsive responses in a go/no-go task. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 49(6), 398406. https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059418777404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catalá-López, F., Hutton, B., Núñez-Beltrán, A., Mayhew, A. D., Page, M. J., Ridao, M., Tobías, A., … Moher, D. (2017). The pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLOS ONE, 12(7), e0180355. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-015-0005-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cinel, C., Valeriani, D., & Poli, R. (2019). Neurotechnologies for human cognitive augmentation: Current state of the art and future prospects. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 1-24, 124. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00013.Google ScholarPubMed
Coghill, D., Toplak, M., Rhodes, S., & Adamo, N. (2018). Cognitive functioning in ADHD: Inhibition, memory, temporal discounting, decision-making, timing and reaction time variability. In Banaschewski, T., Coghill, D., & Zuddas, A. (Eds.), Oxford textbook of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (pp. 94102). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Available at https://oxfordmedicine.com/view/10.1093/med/9780198739258.001.0001/med-9780198739258-chapter-10.Google Scholar
Conners, C. K., Pitkanen, J., & Rzepa, S. R. (2011). Conners 3rd edition (Conners 3; Conners 2008). In Kreutzer, J. S., DeLuca, J. & Caplan, B. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 675678). New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortese, S. (2020). Pharmacologic treatment of attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder. New England Journal of Medicine, 383(11), 10501056. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra1917069CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cortese, S., Adamo, N., Del Giovane, C., Mohr-Jensen, C., Hayes, A. J., Carucci, S., Atkinson, L. Z., … Cipriani, A. (2018). Comparative efficacy and tolerability of medications for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children, adolescents, and adults: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(9), 727738. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30269-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Smith, A., Taylor, E., & Rubia, K. (2012). A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 48(2), 194215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunill, R., Castells, X., Tobias, A., & Capellà, D. (2016). Efficacy, safety and variability in pharmacotherapy for adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis and meta-regression in over 9000 patients. Psychopharmacology, 233(2), 187197. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-015-4099-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunillera, T., Brignani, D., Cucurell, D., Fuentemilla, L., & Miniussi, C. (2016). The right inferior frontal cortex in response inhibition: A tDCS–ERP co-registration study. NeuroImage, 140, 6675. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dambacher, F., Schuhmann, T., Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Brugman, S., & Sack, A. T. (2015). No effects of bilateral tDCS over inferior frontal gyrus on response inhibition and aggression. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0132170. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ditye, T., Jacobson, L., Walsh, V., & Lavidor, M. (2012). Modulating behavioral inhibition by tDCS combined with cognitive training. Experimental Brain Research, 219(3), 363368. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3098-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Döpfner, M., Lehmkuhl, G., & Steinhausen, H. (2006). Kinder-Diagnostik-System (KIDS), Band 1: Aufmerksamkeitsdefizitund Hyperaktivitätsstörungen (ADHS). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Eaves, L. C., Wingert, H. D., Ho, H. H., & Mickelson, E. C. R. (2006). Screening for autism spectrum disorders with the social communication questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP, 27(2 Suppl), S95S103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enriquez-Geppert, S., Smit, D., Pimenta, M. G., & Arns, M. (2019). Neurofeedback as a treatment intervention in ADHD: Current evidence and practice. Current Psychiatry Reports, 21(46). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-1021-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Biederman, J., Buitelaar, J. K., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., … Franke, B. (2015). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 1(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrdp.2015.20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filmer, H. L., Ehrhardt, S. E., Bollmann, S., Mattingley, J. B., & Dux, P. E. (2019). Accounting for individual differences in the response to tDCS with baseline levels of neurochemical excitability. Cortex, 115, 324334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonteneau, C., Redoute, J., Haesebaert, F., Le Bars, D., Costes, N., Suaud-Chagny, M.-F., & Brunelin, J. (2018). Frontal transcranial direct current stimulation induces dopamine release in the ventral striatum in human. Cerebral Cortex, 28(7), 26362646. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fricke, K., Seeber, A. A., Thirugnanasambandam, N., Paulus, W., Nitsche, M. A., & Rothwell, J. C. (2011). Time course of the induction of homeostatic plasticity generated by repeated transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 105(3), 11411149. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00608.2009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Rubia, K., Rossi, G., Sartori, G., & Balottin, U. (2012). Striatal dopamine transporter alterations in ADHD: Pathophysiology or adaptation to psychostimulants? A meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(3), 264272. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11060940CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G. (2019). Emotions and the right hemisphere: Can new data clarify old models? The Neuroscientist: A Review Journal Bringing Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry, 25(3), 258270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858418785342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, D. L. (2019). Chapter 6 – transcranial magnetic stimulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In Oberman, L. M. & Enticott, P. G. (Eds.), Neurotechnology and brain stimulation in pediatric psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 115146). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-812777-3.00006-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez-Ariza, C. J., Martín, M. C., & Morales, J. (2017). Tempering proactive cognitive control by transcranial direct current stimulation of the right (but not the left) lateral prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groenewold, N. A., Opmeer, E. M., de Jonge, P., Aleman, A., & Costafreda, S. G. (2013). Emotional valence modulates brain functional abnormalities in depression: Evidence from a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(2), 152163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, E. M., Halder, S., Blankertz, B., Sannelli, C., Dickhaus, T., Kleih, S., … Kübler, A. (2012). Psychological predictors of SMR-BCI performance. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 8086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, H., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2012). Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of timing in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 22482256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2013). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(2), 185198. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional analysis. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hill, P., & Taylor, E. (2001). An auditable protocol for treating attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 84(5), 404409. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.84.5.404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogeveen, J., Grafman, J., Aboseria, M., David, A., Bikson, M., & Hauner, K. K. (2016). Effects of high-definition and conventional tDCS on response inhibition. Brain Stimulation, 9(5), 720729. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoy, K. E., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2015). From bench to clinic to community: The far reaching implications of basic research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 112(42), E5658. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1516590112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, T.-Y., Juan, C.-H., & Tseng, P. (2016). Individual differences and state-dependent responses in transcranial direct current stimulation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 643. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00643CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, L., Ezra, A., Berger, U., & Lavidor, M. (2012). Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(5), 979984. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, L., Javitt, D. C., & Lavidor, M. (2011). Activation of inhibition: Diminishing impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 33803387. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, K. T., & Berryhill, M. (2012). Parietal contributions to visual working memory depend on task difficulty. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 3, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, K. T., Stephens, J. A., Alam, M., Bikson, M., & Berryhill, M. E. (2015). Longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS ONE, 10(4), e0121904. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121904CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, L. D. (2012). Dynamic randomization and a randomization model for clinical trials data. Statistics in Medicine, 31(29), 38583873. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.5448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karabanov, A., Ziemann, U., Hamada, M., George, M. S., Quartarone, A., Classen, J., … Siebner, H. R. (2015). Consensus paper: Probing homeostatic plasticity of human cortex with non-invasive transcranial brain stimulation. Brain Stimulation, 8(3), 442454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, B., Au, J., Buschkuehl, M., Abagis, T., Zabel, C., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J. (2017). Individual differences and long-term consequences of tDCS-augmented cognitive training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(9), 14981508. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kekic, M., Boysen, E., Campbell, I. C., & Schmidt, U. (2016). A systematic review of the clinical efficacy of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in psychiatric disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 74, 7086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.12.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. (2014). Involvement of the dorsal and ventral attention networks in oddball stimulus processing: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 35(5), 22652284. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22326CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronberg, G., Bridi, M., Abel, T., Bikson, M., & Parra, L. C. (2017). Direct current stimulation modulates LTP and LTD: Activity dependence and dendritic effects. Brain Stimulation, 10(1), 5158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, M.-F., Paulus, W., & Nitsche, M. A. (2008). Boosting focally-induced brain plasticity by dopamine. Cerebral Cortex, 18(3), 648651. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, S.-L., Criaud, M., Alegria, A., Barker, G. J., Giampietro, V., & Rubia, K. (2020). Neurofunctional and behavioural measures associated with fMRI-neurofeedback learning in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 27, 102291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 870900. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030694CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langner, R., Leiberg, S., Hoffstaedter, F., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Towards a human self-regulation system: Common and distinct neural signatures of emotional and behavioural control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 400410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichstein, K. L., Riedel, B. W., & Richman, S. L. (2000). The Mackworth Clock Test: A computerized version. The Journal of Psychology, 134(2), 153161. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980009600858CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, A., Vöröslakos, M., Kronberg, G., Henin, S., Krause, M. R., Huang, Y., … Buzsáki, G. (2018). Immediate neurophysiological effects of transcranial electrical stimulation. Nature Communications, 9(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-07233-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(6), 643654. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsl054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukito, S., Norman, L., Carlisi, C., Radua, J., Hart, H., Simonoff, E., & Rubia, K. (2020). Comparative meta-analyses of brain structural and functional abnormalities during cognitive control in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Medicine, 50(6), 894919. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 621. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470214808416738CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maurizio, S., Liechti, M. D., Heinrich, H., Jaencke, L., Steinhausen, H.-C., Walitza, S., … Drechsler, R. (2014). Comparing tomographic EEG neurofeedback and EMG biofeedback in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychology, 95(SI), 3144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, H., Skokauskas, N., & Frodl, T. (2014). Identifying a consistent pattern of neural function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(4), 869880. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713001037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minder, F., Zuberer, A., Brandeis, D., & Drechsler, R. (2019). Specific effects of individualized cognitive training in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The role of pre-training cognitive impairment and individual training performance. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 0(0), 115. https://doi.org/10.1080/17518423.2019.1600064Google Scholar
Minhas, P., Bikson, M., Woods, A. J., Rosen, A. R., & Kessler, S. K. (2012). Transcranial direct current stimulation in pediatric brain: A computational modeling study. Conference Proceedings :…Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Conference, 2012, 859862. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2012.6346067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffa, A. H., Brunoni, A. R., Nikolin, S., & Loo, C. K. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation in psychiatric disorders: A comprehensive review. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 41(3), 447463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2018.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Hopewell, S., Schulz, K. F., Montori, V., Gotzsche, P. C., Devereaux, P. J., … Altman, D. G. (2010). CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ, 340(mar23 1), c869. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.c869CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monte-Silva, K., Liebetanz, D., Grundey, J., Paulus, W., & Nitsche, M. A. (2010). Dosage-dependent non-linear effect of l-dopa on human motor cortex plasticity. The Journal of Physiology, 588(18), 34153424. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2010.190181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowlem, F. D., Skirrow, C., Reid, P., Maltezos, S., Nijjar, S. K., Merwood, A., … Asherson, P. (2019). Validation of the mind excessively wandering scale and the relationship of mind wandering to impairment in adult ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(6), 624634. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054716651927CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noreika, V., Falter, C. M., & Rubia, K. (2013). Timing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 235266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, L. J., Carlisi, C., Lukito, S., Hart, H., Mataix-Cols, D., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2016). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: A comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(8), 815825. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okamoto, M., & Dan, I. (2005). Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping. NeuroImage, 26(1), 1828. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Sakamoto, K., Takeo, K., Shimizu, K., Kohno, S., … Dan, I. (2004). Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. NeuroImage, 21(1), 99111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palm, U., Reisinger, E., Keeser, D., Kuo, M.-F., Pogarell, O., Leicht, G., … Padberg, F. (2013). Evaluation of sham transcranial direct current stimulation for randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trials. Brain Stimulation, 6(4), 690695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penadés, R., Catalán, R., Rubia, K., Andrés, S., Salamero, M., & Gastó, C. (2007). Impaired response inhibition in obsessive compulsive disorder. European Psychiatry, 22(6), 404410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 7389. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PsyTookit. (2017). Mackworth Clock Task. Available at https://www.psytoolkit.org/experiment-library/mackworth.htmlGoogle Scholar
PsyToolkit. (2018). Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST). Available at https://www.psytoolkit.org/experiment-library/wcst.htmlGoogle Scholar
Radua, J., del Pozo, N. O., Gómez, J., Guillen-Grima, F., & Ortuño, F. (2014). Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies indicates that an increase of cognitive difficulty during executive tasks engages brain regions associated with time perception. Neuropsychologia, 58, 1422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K. (2018). Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and its clinical translation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Alegria, A. A., Cubillo, A. I., Smith, A. B., Brammer, M. J., & Radua, J. (2014). Effects of stimulants on brain function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 76(8), 616628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.10.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Brammer, M. J., Toone, B., & Taylor, E. (2005). Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 9, 10671075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubia, K., Smith, A., & Taylor, E. (2007). Performance of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on a test battery of impulsiveness. Child Neuropsychology, 13(3), 276304. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040600770761CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruf, S. P., Fallgatter, A. J., & Plewnia, C. (2017). Augmentation of working memory training by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Scientific Reports, 7(1), 876. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-01055-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salehinejad, M. A., Nejati, V., Mosayebi-Samani, M., Mohammadi, A., Wischnewski, M., Kuo, M.-F., … Nitsche, M. A. (2020). Transcranial direct current stimulation in ADHD: A systematic review of efficacy, safety, and protocol-induced electrical field modeling results. Neuroscience Bulletin, 36, 11911212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-020-00501-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salehinejad, M. A., Wischnewski, M., Nejati, V., Vicario, C. M., & Nitsche, M. A. (2019). Correction: Transcranial direct current stimulation in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits. PLoS ONE, 14(8), e0221613. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sallard, E., Mouthon, M., De Pretto, M., & Spierer, L. (2018). Modulation of inhibitory control by prefrontal anodal tDCS: A crossover double-blind sham-controlled fMRI study. PLoS ONE, 13(3), e0194936. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194936CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D., … Rapoport, J. L. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(49), 19649. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707741104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sierawska, A., Prehn-Kristensen, A., Moliadze, V., Krauel, K., Nowak, R., Freitag, C. M., … Buyx, A. (2019). Unmet needs in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-can transcranial direct current stimulation fill the gap? Promises and ethical challenges. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 334. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00334CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, R. L. (1984). Sequential treatment allocation using biased coin designs. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 46(3), 519543. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1984.tb01323.xGoogle Scholar
Soff, C., Sotnikova, A., Christiansen, H., Becker, K., & Siniatchkin, M. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation improves clinical symptoms in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 124(1), 133144. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-016-1646-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soltaninejad, Z., Nejati, V., & Ekhtiari, H. (2015a). Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on remediation of inhibitory control on right inferio frontal gyrus in attention deficit and hyperactivity symptoms. Rehabilitation Medicine, 3(4), 19. https://doi.org/10.22037/r.m.v3i4.7837.Google Scholar
Soltaninejad, Z., Nejati, V., & Ekhtiari, H. (2015b). Effect of anodal and cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation on DLPFC on modulation of inhibitory control in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(4), 325332. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715618792CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sripada, C. S., Kessler, D., & Angstadt, M. (2014). Lag in maturation of the brain's intrinsic functional architecture in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(39), 14259. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1407787111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stagg, C. J., & Nitsche, M. A. (2011). Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. The Neuroscientist, 17(1), 3753. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858410386614CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, J. A., & Berryhill, M. E. (2016). Older adults improve on everyday tasks after working memory training and neurostimulation. Brain Stimulation, 9(4), 553559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
STIPED Press Release. (2017). New therapy for ADHD and autism: EU funds international project on brain stimulation. Available at https://www.stiped.eu/fileadmin/websites/stiped/Documents/STIPED_press_release_EN.pdfGoogle Scholar
Stramaccia, D. F., Penolazzi, B., Sartori, G., Braga, M., Mondini, S., & Galfano, G. (2015). Assessing the effects of tDCS over a delayed response inhibition task by targeting the right inferior frontal gyrus and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 233(8), 22832290. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4297-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stringaris, A., Goodman, R., Ferdinando, S., Razdan, V., Muhrer, E., Leibenluft, E., & Brotman, M. A. (2012). The Affective Reactivity Index: A concise irritability scale for clinical and research settings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53(11), 11091117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02561.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. M., Arnold, L. E., Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Hechtman, L. T., Pelham, W. E., … Roy, A. (2018). Long-term outcomes in the Multimodal Treatment study of Children with ADHD (the MTA): From beginning to end. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Available at https://oxfordmedicine.com/view/10.1093/med/9780198739258.001.0001/med-9780198739258-chapter-34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tortella, G., Casati, R., Aparicio, L. V. M., Mantovani, A., Senço, N., D'Urso, G., … Brunoni, A. R. (2015). Transcranial direct current stimulation in psychiatric disorders. World Journal of Psychiatry, 5(1), 88102. https://doi.org/10.5498/wjp.v5.i1.88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(3), 370378. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-V;FT370CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulsky, D. S., Carlozzi, N., Chiaravalloti, N. D., Beaumont, J. L., Kisala, P. A., … Gershon, R. (2014). NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB): List sorting test to measure working memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 599610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence, Second Editions (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
Wefelmeyer, W., & Burrone, J. (2015). Reply to Hoy and Fitzgerald: Considering homeostatic mechanisms in long-term treatments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(42), E5659E5659. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1516941112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wehmeier, P. M., Dittmann, R. W., Schacht, A., Helsberg, K., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2009). Morning and evening behavior in children and adolescents treated with atomoxetine once daily for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Findings from two 24-week, open-label studies. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 3, 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-3-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westwood, S. J., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2020). Non-invasive brain stimulation in children and adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 46(1), 1433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, B. E., Iseli, M., Leon, S., Zaggle, W., Rush, C., Goodman, A., … Bo, E. (2016). Cognitive priming and cognitive training: Immediate and far transfer to academic skills in children. Scientific Reports, 6, 32859. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep32859CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, R., Geng, X., & Lee, T. M. C. (2017). Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: An fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Structure & Function, 222(9), 39733990. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1443-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilverstand, A., Sorger, B., Slaats-Willemse, D., Kan, C. C., Goebel, R., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2017). fMRI neurofeedback training for increasing anterior cingulate cortex activation in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. An exploratory randomized, single-blinded study. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0170795. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. CONSORT flow diagram (Moher et al., 2010) of this RCT from enrolment, intervention allocation, follow-up and analysis.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Schematic overview of the study design. ADHD-RS, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder-Rating Scale; ARI, Affective Reactivity Index; CIS, Columbia Impairment Scale; Conners 3-P, Conners' 3rd Edition Parent Rating; Cognitive battery, Maudsley Attention and Response Suppression task battery, vigilance, Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, visual-spatial working memory, verbal fluency; K-SADS-PL, Kiddie-SADS-Present and Lifetime Version; MEWS, Mind Excessively Wandering Scale; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire (Lifetime), SDQ, Social Difficulties Questionnaire (prosocial scale only); WASI-II, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, 2nd Edition; WREMB-R, Weekly Rating of Evening and Morning Behavior-Revised.

Figure 2

Table 1. Baseline demographic, clinical, cognitive measures and medication status; the number of tDCS and CT sessions; and the time spent playing each CT game in the sham and anodal tDCS groups

Figure 3

Table 2. Summary of adjusted average performance on primary cognitive and clinical outcome measures after sham and anodal tDCS combined with CT

Figure 4

Table 3. Summary of adjusted average performance on secondary cognitive and clinical outcome measures after sham and anodal tDCS combined with CT

Supplementary material: File

Westwood et al. supplementary material

Westwood et al. supplementary material

Download Westwood et al. supplementary material(File)
File 520.3 KB