Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T14:31:19.025Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hope, optimism and delusion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rebecca McGuire-Snieckus*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bath Spa University
*
Rebecca McGuire-Snieckus ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Optimism is generally accepted by psychiatrists, psychologists and other caring professionals as a feature of mental health. Interventions typically rely on cognitive–behavioural tools to encourage individuals to ‘stop negative thought cycles’ and to ‘challenge unhelpful thoughts’. However, evidence suggests that most individuals have persistent biases of optimism and that excessive optimism is not conducive to mental health. How helpful is it to facilitate optimism in individuals who are likely to exhibit biases of optimism already? By locating the cause of distress at the individual level and ‘unhelpful’ cognitions, does this minimise wider systemic social and economic influences on mental health?

Type
Editorials
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2014

Optimism is generally accepted by psychiatrists, psychologists and other mental health professionals as a preferred way of being. ‘Hope and optimism about the future’ was identified as one of five processes for recovery in mental illness in a systematic review and narrative synthesis of 97 papers in psychiatry. Reference Leamy, Bird, Le Boutillier, Williams and Slade1 Hope (including optimism) is classified as a ‘character strength’ in the ‘manual of the sanities’ - Character Strengths and Virtues - which was written by positive psychologists in response to the DSM and which identifies key ‘strengths’ that elevate an individual from ‘suboptimal’ to ‘optimal’ functioning. Reference Peterson and Seligman2 Cognitive-behavioural therapy is offered on the National Health Service (NHS) to ‘help you change your negative thought patterns and improve the way you feel’. 3 Indeed, a position of optimism for the NHS system as a whole was called for by the chief executive Mike Farrar at the 2013 NHS Confederation Conference with his claim that ‘If we're not optimistic for the future, then we deserve to fail.’ 4

Early contexts of hope and optimism

Although used as interchangeable concepts by some and as mutually exclusive concepts by others, Reference Bryant and Cvengros5 the term hope precedes optimism. In classical antiquity, hope was the last of the wedding gifts Zeus gave to Epimetheus and his wife, Pandora, really a punishment to the groom and his brother, Prometheus, for giving mortal man fire. When opened by the new bride Pandora, the ills of humanity were unleashed, while hope, blind, fluttered at the lip of the jar. Reference Graves6 Nietzsche interprets hope in this context as ‘the most evil of evils because it prolongs man's torment’. Reference Nietzsche7 In the English language, the word hope found its earliest use in the context of forlorn hope. It derives from the Dutch military expression verloren hoop - referring to lost troops. Hoop was mistaken for hope and ‘the phrase came to mean a body of desperate men who have abandoned all hope for surviving, a desperate enterprise, as in to cherish a forlorn hope’. Reference Liberman8 In Judeo-Christianity, hope is among the ‘greatest of all gifts’, along with faith and charity in 1 Corintheans 13. Hope is defined by some psychologists as ‘the perceived capacity to derive pathways to desired goals, and motivate oneself via agency thinking to use those pathways.’ Reference Snyder9

The term optimism was not used until 1710 by Leibniz in his work Théodicée to mean the greatest good (derived from optimus in Latin) by suggesting that good will ultimately prevail over evil. In his novel Candide, Voltaire was derisive of the shallowness of an optimistic worldview. According to William James, ‘pessimism leads to weakness, optimism to power.’ Reference James10 Some psychologists today regard optimism as a disposition characterised by positive expectation, Reference Carver and Scheier11 whereas others define it as an explanatory style characterised by a tendency to attribute negative outcomes to external causes, specific and temporary causes and positive outcomes to internal, global and permanent causes. Reference Seligman12 The construct as an explanatory style was given popular force by Martin Seligman, credited as the ‘father of positive psychology’, whose research focus turned from the study of ‘learned helplessness’ in dogs as a model for depression in humans (by demonstrating that dogs who were persistently shocked without the opportunity to escape did not later perceive opportunities to escape, as they had ‘learned’ to be ‘helpless’) to ‘learned optimism’. He observed that some dogs never learned to be helpless and began investigating why some people do not give up after being exposed to repeated stressors. Reference Seligman12

Optimism and individualism

Optimism is viewed by many as an indication of mental health, associated with higher levels of subjective well-being, better physical health and more success. Reference Forgeard and Seligman13 Some propose optimists provide ‘models of living for others to learn from’, citing findings that link optimism to better subjective well-being, better physical health, persistent educational efforts, higher income and better relationships. Reference Carver, Scheier and Segerstrom14 As much of the evidence is based on correlational research which cannot infer causation, it could be equally argued that better health, education, income and good relationships could be predictive of an optimistic worldview, and not the reverse. Indeed, the same authors concede that ‘a poor childhood socioeconomic circumstance breeds pessimism later in life’. Reference Carver, Scheier and Segerstrom14

It could be argued that much discourse around optimism may be a function of individualism. By identifying distress as a ‘psychological matter’ rather than locating it in ‘the social and material world with which we are intimately interconnected’, Reference Cromby, Diamond, Kelly, Moloney, Priest and Smail15 key proponents ‘promote positive thinking and to systematically dispel the negative thoughts that affect us all’ Reference Layard16 despite epidemiological evidence that fewer economic and social resources predict higher mental and physical health problems throughout life. Reference Sayer17 It has been further argued that ‘the current official preoccupation with happiness’ may be at best ‘a naïve attempt to improve the world through wishful thinking, and at worst a form of insidious social control, where people are encouraged to look inwards for the sources of their troubles, and in the end to implicitly blame themselves for these ills’. Reference Cromby, Diamond, Kelly, Moloney, Priest and Smail15

Interventions that improve optimism

Interventions are used to increase optimism in both clinical and non-clinical populations. Cognitive-behavioural therapy is offered on the NHS to stop ‘negative thought cycles’ based on the presupposition that ‘there are helpful and unhelpful ways of reacting to a situation, often determined by how you think about them’. 3 Such ‘cognitive restructuring tools’ which encourage individuals to ‘challenge unhelpful thoughts’ that threaten self-esteem and therefore mental health are used to prevent mental health problems, to enhance performance and to reduce work-related stress. Reference Palmer and Gyllensten18 Some interventions emphasise the importance of ‘increasing the frequency of positive cognitions and self-statements that foster optimism’ to combat ‘everyday malady’; Reference Riskind, Sarampote and Mercier19 whereas others encourage clients to adopt an ‘as if’ approach to life, to ask clients to ‘pretend that their lives are improved in some way, however small, and to experience their lives anew in the face of these positive changes’. Reference O'Hanlon and Bertolino20 Blackwell et al propose that ‘boosting positive future imagery’ to promote optimism could provide ‘implications for mental health and even physical well-being’. Reference Blackwell, Rius-Ottenheim, Schulte-van Maaren, Carlier, Middelkoop and Zitman21 Further interventions to improve optimism encourage individuals to make external attributions for negative outcomes and internal attributions for positive ones to improve success. Reference Schulman22

Cognitive biases

However, social psychological research reveals that most individuals already tend to exhibit persistent cognitive biases characterised by biases in probabilistic reasoning and attribution. Reference Taylor, Brown and Baumeister23 The ‘optimism bias’ shows that when asked to make comparative judgements about future life events, individuals consistently expect that positive events are more likely to happen to them (i.e. having a gifted child) and that negative events (i.e. divorced after a few years) are less likely. Reference Weinstein24 The ‘better than average effect’ demonstrates that individuals tend to evaluate themselves more favourably compared with others. Reference Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher, Yurak and Vredenburg25 A variety of biases of attribution have also been identified, from the self-serving bias (the tendency to deny responsibility for failure and take credit for success) and the self-centred bias (taking more credit for a jointly produced outcome) to the false consensus effect (the tendency to see one's own behaviour, thoughts and feelings as typical). Reference Fiske and Taylor26 Other self-favouring biases have been identified such as spatial biases (things are better here than there), environmental comparative optimism (things are environmentally safer here than elsewhere) and temporal biases (discounting the importance of a problem the farther away in the future that it seems). Reference Gifford, Scannell, Kormos, Smolova, Biel and Boncu27

Delusions

Delusions are characterised by biases in attribution and probabilistic reasoning. Reference Kemp, Chua, McKenna and David28-Reference LaRocco and Warman30 Karl Jaspers viewed delusions as deeply-held beliefs that are impervious to logic. Reference Jaspers, Hoenig and Hamilton31 If not slightly deluded, the average individual does appear to be consistently biased. Perhaps, as Festinger et al suggested, we are not rational, but ‘rationalising’ animals. Reference Festinger, Riecken and Schachter32

Depressive realism

Biases of optimism are said to apply to most people, but generally not to individuals with depression (termed depressive realism). Although some have found no support for the notion of depressive realism but rather suggest that patients with depression ‘distort their judgements in a characteristically negative fashion’, Reference Carson, Hollon and Shelton33 there appears to be reliable evidence that even individuals with depression exhibit persistent cognitive biases of optimism. Reference Moore and Fresco34 A meta-analytic review of 75 studies representing 7305 individuals indicated a small overall effect of depressive realism (Cohen's d = −0.07) and that both individuals with depression (d = 0.14) and individuals without depression (d = 0.29) showed a substantial positive bias. Reference Moore and Fresco34

Excessive optimism

Although optimism may serve the function to motivate individuals in the present in the service of future goals, excessive optimism may blind individuals to perceive the inherent risk in their present actions, resulting in consequences that individuals might better permit themselves to anticipate. Optimism is credited with predicting a catalogue of negative outcomes from risk-taking in unprotected sex, Reference Anderson and Galinsky35 underestimating risks in driving, Reference Harre and Sibley36 continued gambling after losses, Reference Gibson and Sanbonmatsu37 engagement in music piracy, Reference Nandedkar and Midha38 to minimising the health consequences of smoking. Reference Williams and Clarke39 Moreover, optimism can create the potential for unmet expectations and heightened negative reactions when such expectations are not realised, including increased physical and psychological symptoms and reduced mental health. Reference Britton, Sliter and Jex40,Reference Chang and Sanna41

More clarity is needed regarding this concept. What is the purpose of facilitating biases of optimism in a non-clinical population who is likely to have persistent esteem-enhancing biases of attribution and optimism already? If excessive optimism can lead to a catalogue of ills, how are the proposed techniques designed to facilitate optimism conducive to mental health in a non-clinical population? How helpful are such interventions even among individuals with depression who appear to also exhibit persistent cognitive biases of optimism? Reference Moore and Fresco34 By locating the cause of optimal or suboptimal functioning at the individual level, does this minimise wider systemic social and economic influences on mental health?

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Leamy, M, Bird, V, Le Boutillier, C, Williams, J, Slade, M. Conceptual framework for personal recovery in mental health: systematic review and narrative synthesis. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199: 445–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Peterson, R, Seligman, M. Character Strengths and Virtues: A Handbook and Classification. Oxford University Press, 2004.Google Scholar
3 NHS Choices. Cognitive behavioural therapy – how it works. NHS Choices, 2012.Google Scholar
4 National Health Executive. ‘Optimism will carry us through’ – Farrar. NHE, 2013.Google Scholar
5 Bryant, FB, Cvengros, JA. Distinguishing hope and optimism: two sides of a coin, or two separate coins? J Soc Clin Psychol 2004; 23: 273302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6 Graves, R. Greek Myths. Penguin, 1991.Google Scholar
7 Nietzsche, F. Human, All Too Human: A Book for Free Spirits. Cambridge University Press, 1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8 Liberman, A. Word Origins. Oxford University Press, 2005.Google Scholar
9 Snyder, C. Hope theory: rainbows in the mind. Psychol Inq 2002; 13: 249–75.Google Scholar
10 James, W. The Variety of Religious Experiences. Library of America, 1902.Google Scholar
11 Carver, C, Scheier, M. Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behaviour. Springer-Verlag, 1981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12 Seligman, M. Learned Optimism. Pocket Books, 1990.Google Scholar
13 Forgeard, M, Seligman, M. Seeing the glass half full: a review of causes and consequences of optimism. Pratiques Psychologiques 2012; 18: 107–20.Google Scholar
14 Carver, C, Scheier, M, Segerstrom, S. Optimism. Clin Psychol Rev 2010; 30: 879–89.Google Scholar
15 Cromby, J, Diamond, B, Kelly, P, Moloney, P, Priest, P, Smail, D. Questioning the science and politics of happiness. Psychologist 1997; 20: 422–5.Google Scholar
16 Layard, R. Income and Happiness: Rethinking Economic Policy. London School of Economics and Political Science, 2003.Google Scholar
17 Sayer, A. The Moral Significance of Class. Cambridge University Press, 2005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18 Palmer, S, Gyllensten, K. How cognitive behavioral, rational emotive behavioral, and multimodal coaching could prevent mental health problems, enhance performance and reduce work related stress. J Ration Emot Cogn Behav Ther 2008; 26: 3852.Google Scholar
19 Riskind, J, Sarampote, C, Mercier, M. For every malady a sovereign cure: optimism training. J Cogn Psychother 1996; 10: 105–17.Google Scholar
20 O'Hanlon, B, Bertolino, B. The Therapist's Notebook on Positive Psychology. Taylor & Francis, 2012.Google Scholar
21 Blackwell, SE, Rius-Ottenheim, N, Schulte-van Maaren, YW, Carlier, IV, Middelkoop, VD, Zitman, FG, et al Optimism and mental imagery: a possible cognitive marker to promote well-being? Psychiatry Res 2013; 206: 5661.Google Scholar
22 Schulman, P. Applying learned optimism to increase sales productivity. J Pers Selling Sales Manage 1999; 19: 31–7.Google Scholar
23 Taylor, SE, Brown, JD. Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. In The Self in Social Psychology (ed. Baumeister, RF): 4366. Taylor & Francis, 1998.Google Scholar
24 Weinstein, N. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J Pers Soc Psychol 1980; 39: 806–20.Google Scholar
25 Alicke, M, Klotz, M, Breitenbecher, D, Yurak, T, Vredenburg, D. Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1995; 68: 804–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26 Fiske, S, Taylor, S. Social Cognition. McGraw-Hill, 1999.Google Scholar
27 Gifford, R, Scannell, L, Kormos, C, Smolova, L, Biel, A, Boncu, S, et al Temporal pessimism and spatial optimism in environmental assessments: an 18-nation study. J Environ Psychol 2008; 29: 112.Google Scholar
28 Kemp, R, Chua, S, McKenna, P, David, A. Reasoning and delusions. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 170: 398405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29 Garety, P, Freeman, D, Jolley, S, Dunn, G, Bebbington, P, Fowler, D. Reasoning, emotions and delusional conviction in psychosis. J Abnorm Psychol 2005; 114: 373–84.Google Scholar
30 LaRocco, V, Warman, D. Probability estimations and delusion-proneness. Pers Individ Dif 2009; 47: 197202.Google Scholar
31 Jaspers, K. General Psychopathology (trans. Hoenig, J, Hamilton, MW). Johns Hopkins University Press, 1991.Google Scholar
32 Festinger, L, Riecken, H, Schachter, S. When Prophecy Fails. Pober Publishing, 1956.Google Scholar
33 Carson, R, Hollon, S, Shelton, R. Depressive realism and clinical depression. Behav Res Ther 2010; 48: 257–65.Google Scholar
34 Moore, M, Fresco, D. Depressive realism: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2011; 32: 496509.Google Scholar
35 Anderson, C, Galinsky, A. Power, optimism and risk taking. Eur J Soc Psychol 2006; 36: 511–36.Google Scholar
36 Harre, N, Sibley, C. Explicit and implicit self-enhancement biases in drivers and their relationship to driving violations and crash-risk optimism. Accid Anal Prev 2007; 39: 1155–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Gibson, B, Sanbonmatsu, D. Optimism, pessimism and gambling: the downside of optimism. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2004; 20: 149–60.Google Scholar
38 Nandedkar, A, Midha, V. It won't happen to me: An assessment of optimism bias in music piracy. Comput Hum Behav 2012; 28: 41–8.Google Scholar
39 Williams, T, Clarke, V. Optimistic bias in beliefs about smoking. Aust J Psychology 1997; 49: 106–12.Google Scholar
40 Britton, A, Sliter, M, Jex, S. Is the glass really half-full? The reverse-buffering effect of optimism on undermining behaviour. Pers Individ Dif 2012; 52: 712–7.Google Scholar
41 Chang, E, Sanna, L. Optimism, accumulated life stress and psychological and physical adjustment: is it always adaptive to expect the best? J Soc Clin Psychol 2003; 22: 97115.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.