Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T15:12:56.546Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food ordering for children in restaurants: multiple sources of influence on decision making

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 June 2016

Iana A Castro*
Affiliation:
College of Business Administration, San Diego State University, 5500 Campanile Drive, San Diego, CA 92182, USA Institute for Behavioral and Community Health, San Diego State University Research Foundation, San Diego, CA, USA
Christine B Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Hala Madanat
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral and Community Health, San Diego State University Research Foundation, San Diego, CA, USA College of Health and Human Services, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
Julie L Pickrel
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral and Community Health, San Diego State University Research Foundation, San Diego, CA, USA
Hee-Jin Jun
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral and Community Health, San Diego State University Research Foundation, San Diego, CA, USA
Michelle Zive
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Sheila Gahagan
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Guadalupe X Ayala
Affiliation:
Institute for Behavioral and Community Health, San Diego State University Research Foundation, San Diego, CA, USA College of Health and Human Services, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Restaurants are playing an increasingly important role in children’s dietary intake. Interventions to promote healthy ordering in restaurants have primarily targeted adults. Much remains unknown about how to influence ordering for and by children. Using an ecological lens, the present study sought to identify sources of influence on ordering behaviour for and by children in restaurants.

Design

A mixed-methods study was conducted using unobtrusive observations of dining parties with children and post-order interviews. Observational data included: child’s gender, person ordering for the child and server interactions with the dining party. Interview data included: child’s age, restaurant visit frequency, timing of child’s decision making, and factors influencing decision making.

Setting

Ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA participated.

Subjects

Complete observational and interview data were obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years).

Results

Taste preferences, family influences and menus impacted ordering. However, most children knew what they intended to order before arriving at the restaurant, especially if they dined there at least monthly. Furthermore, about one-third of children shared their meals with others and all shared meals were ordered from adult (v. children’s) menus. Parents placed most orders, although parental involvement in ordering was less frequent with older children. Servers interacted frequently with children but generally did not recommend menu items or prompt use of the children’s menu.

Conclusions

Interventions to promote healthy ordering should consider the multiple sources of influence that are operating when ordering for and by children in restaurants.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2016 

Excess energy intake is a risk factor for obesity( Reference Howarth, Murphy and Wilkens 1 ). The restaurant industry plays a central role in dietary intake as consumers eat out more than ever before( 2 4 ). Spending on away-from-home foods has risen to a level near that of food-for-home spending( 2 ), with implications for children’s dietary intake. Although studies have shown that many restaurants have at least one healthful children’s meal( Reference Krukowski and West 5 ) and one children’s fruit and vegetable side option( Reference Anzman-Frasca, Dawes and Sliwa 6 ), children’s menu items lack consistency in meeting the US Department of Agriculture dietary guidelines( Reference Krukowski and West 5 Reference Batada, Bruening and Marchlewicz 7 ). For example, 91 % of children’s meals at the top fifty restaurant chains did not meet the National Restaurant Association’s Kids LiveWell Program nutrition standards and 50 % of children’s meals did not meet the Kids LiveWell Program’s criterion of 2510 kJ (600 kcal) or less( 8 , 9 ). Thus, the potential to improve what is offered to children in restaurants is great.

Prior restaurant-based interventions in the USA and worldwide have focused on menu labelling efforts( Reference Dodds, Wolfenden and Chapman 10 Reference Papies and Veling 14 ), modifications to menu items( Reference Blair, Drass and Stone 15 Reference Richard, O’Loughlin and Masson 18 ), promotional campaigns, including restaurant staff prompts( Reference van Kleef, van den Broek and van Trijp 19 ), and chef trainings( Reference Acharya, Patterson and Hill 20 Reference Nothwehr, Snetselaar and Dawson 22 ). Among child-focused studies, important factors include offering healthy default side dishes (e.g. fruits/vegetables), providing multiple healthy choices and considering the appearance of the food( Reference Krukowski and West 5 , Reference Anzman-Frasca, Dawes and Sliwa 6 , Reference Anzman-Frasca, Mueller and Sliwa 23 Reference Wootan 26 ). Few studies, however, have considered intrapersonal (i.e. child cognitive developmental stage), interpersonal/social environment (i.e. parents, servers) and restaurant/physical environment (i.e. menu) influences on ordering for and by children. Social Cognitive Theory( Reference Bandura 27 ) and the Socio-Ecological Framework( Reference Stokols 28 ) support this focus, as they acknowledge the reciprocal influence of aspects of the person, the behaviour of interest, and the social and physical environments in which behaviours are enacted.

The role of children as consumers and their ability to influence decisions is related to their cognitive developmental stage( Reference Valkenburg and Cantor 29 , Reference John 30 ). As children develop cognitively, they are more capable of processing information and making choices( Reference Piaget and Inhelder 31 ). As such, we hypothesized that parents would be more involved in ordering for younger children than for older children. Relatedly, while parents are an important source of influence and accurately predict what their children will eat( Reference Mata, Scheibehenne and Todd 32 ), less is known about the influence of other family members and servers( Reference van Kleef, van den Broek and van Trijp 19 ) and of the restaurant environment( Reference Libotte, Siegrist and Bucher 33 , Reference Ohri-Vachaspati, Isgor and Rimkus 34 ). Thus, to inform actionable steps towards improving what is ordered for and by children in restaurants, the present study identified sources of influence on ordering decisions.

Methods

Study design

The current paper reports on a mixed-methods observational study to understand restaurant-ordering behaviour for and by children. It represents the formative research study of the Kids’ Choice Restaurant Program, a cluster randomized controlled trial designed to test the introduction of healthy children’s menu items in eight pair-matched independent restaurants( Reference Ayala, Castro and Pickrel 35 ). Outcome and impact evaluation activities will determine whether the intervention results in sales of healthy children’s menu items through changes in ordering. In the present study, dining parties were observed unobtrusively while ordering. Interviews were conducted both after orders were placed and after meals were consumed (the latter not reported here). San Diego State University’s Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures.

Setting and restaurant recruitment

Eligible restaurants met the following criteria: (i) categorized as independent; (ii) prepared the majority of food in-house; (iii) provided table service; (iv) offered dinner at least five nights/week; (v) imposed no age restrictions (e.g. over 21 only); (vi) 33 % of dinner parties/week included children; and (vii) provided high chairs/booster seats. Using the San Diego County Department of Environmental Health food-service permit holders list (February 2014), a list of 1589 potential outlets was compiled after removing clearly ineligible outlets (e.g. food stores, gas stations, chain and fast-food outlets) and restaurants located in zip codes with less than 29 % Latino population and further than 16 km (10 miles) from project offices. Eligibility was verified using online sources and/or telephone calls. Next, study staff visited eligible restaurants with recruitment flyers. They approached the managers/owners to discuss study requirements, which included allowing research staff to shadow interactions between servers and customers and interview parent and child customers. Study incentives included a Certificate of Appreciation and positive Yelp review for the restaurant, $US 10 server compensation for each shift observed and two pre-paid $US 5 restaurant vouchers for participating dining parties.

Of forty-nine potentially eligible restaurants identified, twenty-seven were actively recruited and thirteen consented to participate. Three restaurants were dropped due to lack of observations and ten participated in the study.

Procedures and data collection

Restaurant audits assessed characteristics of the ten participating restaurants at the beginning of the study. Two study staff members (observer and interviewer) were deployed to each restaurant on days and times the restaurant indicated dining parties with children would be present. The observer and interviewer interacted at the restaurant only via text message to identify participating dining parties; they did not interact with each other in front of customers.

Observational component

The observational protocol was developed for the present study based on other protocols implemented in grocery stores( Reference Ayala, Mueller and Lopez-Madurga 36 ), schools( Reference McKenzie, Marshall and Sallis 37 ) and communities( Reference McKenzie, Cohen and Sehgal 38 ). Two important characteristics of observed data were employed: (i) we limited the number of coded answer choices; and (ii) we did not ask observers to make attributions for behaviours. Prior to the study, the protocol was refined during study staff training and tested with twenty-two dining parties at three pilot restaurants (different from the restaurants recruited into the study).

Dining parties of two to six people with at least one child aged 3–14 years and one adult aged 18+ years were targeted for observation. The observer accompanied a server to tables with children as he/she went about normal activities. To minimize bias, the observer was simply identified as shadowing the server. A structured observation instrument with a primarily closed-ended coding scheme was used. Variables recorded during the observation included: child’s gender, who ordered for the child and server interactions with the dining party. The observer joined the server for each table interaction until every customer at the table received his/her order, at which point the observation ended. Observations were not conducted on days when children ate for free, as this could influence ordering behaviour.

Interview components

Once the interviewer received a text message from the observer, he/she approached the observed dining party. The interviewer obtained verbal consent by handing an informational flyer to one of the adults and reading an approved recruitment script. Eligible and consenting dining parties were interviewed post-order. For each child at the table, the interviewer asked who ordered for the child. If the child ordered, he/she was asked to answer a few questions. If the child chose not to answer, an adult was asked the questions. If an adult ordered for the child, he/she was asked the questions.

A series of closed-ended questions was asked: restaurant visit frequency, child’s age, whether or not the child knew what to order when he/she arrived at the restaurant, and if so, if that was ordered, menu ordered from and whether the child shared the item ordered (captured only if the respondent volunteered that the child was sharing and with whom). Respondents who indicated they did not know what they were going to order before arriving, or those who knew what they were going to order but ordered something else, were asked how they decided what to order (open-ended). The open-ended responses were grouped thematically consistent with the Socio-Ecological Framework( Reference Stokols 28 ) by two study co-authors: intrapersonal, interpersonal/social environment, restaurant/physical environment or multiple influences.

Respondents who knew what they wanted before arriving and ordered as planned were asked what might have persuaded them to try a new children’s menu item instead (open-ended; grouped thematically using a similar approach). Then, they were given a list of items that may influence whether they would try a new children’s menu item and asked to select all of the responses that applied. The list contained response options related to sources at each level of influence: intrapersonal (i.e. ability to taste items), interpersonal/social environment (i.e. server suggestions) and restaurant/physical environment (e.g. menu items, descriptions and pictures, nutritional information, price/value and marketing materials). When the dining party finished eating, the interviewer approached the table for a post-meal interview (results not reported here) and, upon completion of the interview, provided the table with two $US 5 restaurant vouchers.

Statistical analyses

Data were entered and analysed using the statistical software package IBM SPSS Statistics Version 23.0. Children were placed into one of three cognitive developmental stages based on age( Reference John 30 , Reference Piaget and Inhelder 31 ): 3–6 years old (45·3 %), 7–11 years old (43·3 %) and 12–14 years old (11·3 %). Descriptive statistics, cross-tabulations and χ 2 tests identified sources of influence on ordering behaviours for and by children in restaurants.

Results

Restaurants and dining parties

Data were collected at ten full-service restaurants between November 2014 and February 2015. Restaurants ranged in size from twenty to forty-six tables and included the following cuisines: American, Italian, Latin and Mediterranean. None of the restaurants had nutritional information available and half had a children’s menu.

Of 138 dining parties with children seated during observations, 120 (87 %) were observed. Of the observed dining parties, 102 (85 %) completed the post-order interview. Therefore, observational data for 102 dining parties, ranging in size from two to six individuals (mean=4) with one to three children (mean=1·5), were used. Complete interview data were obtained from/for 150 children aged 3–14 years (mean age=7 years; 51 % girls).

Sources of influence on ordering behaviour

Most children (60 %) knew what they wanted to order when they arrived at the restaurant and 92 % ordered as planned (Table 1). Children who visited the restaurant monthly were more likely to know what they intended to order before arriving at the restaurant compared with those who visited less frequently (Fig. 1). Parents placed most food orders (62 %) and parental involvement in ordering for children decreased with child age (χ 2=20·17, P<0·001; Fig. 2). Children shared their meal 34 % of the time, most often with other children (Table 1). One hundred per cent of those who shared ordered from an adult (v. children’s) menu compared with 69 % of those who did not share (χ 2=19·55, P<0·001).

Fig. 1 Percentage of children who knew what they intended to order before arriving at the restaurant by restaurant visit frequency; interview data obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years) dining at ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA, November 2014–February 2015. Never v. monthly: χ 2=6·48, P=0·011; less than once monthly v. monthly: χ 2=6·12, P=0·013; more than once monthly v. monthly: χ 2=3·19, P=0·074. The Bonferroni correction was done and the critical P value used for these tests was ~0·02

Fig. 2 Who placed the order (, child; , both parent and child; , parent) by child cognitive developmental stage; observational and interview data obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years) dining at ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA, November 2014–February 2015

Table 1 Food ordering behaviour for and by children in restaurants; observational and interview data obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years) dining at ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA, November 2014–February 2015

* n 103; six missing cases, forty-one children did not order food (including children that another person shared with).

n 149; one missing case.

Servers interacted with children in 53 % of dining parties (e.g. directly speaking with the child). In most cases, servers did not prompt child menu usage or choices (73 %) or make food or beverage recommendations (80 %). Not surprisingly, they did not engage in dietary/nutritional health talk (97 %).

When asked how they decided what to order, 13 % of respondents stated intrapersonal sources of influence (e.g. past experience, taste preference, dietary needs), 33 % stated interpersonal/social environment sources (e.g. family, parent, server), 33 % stated restaurant/physical environment sources (e.g. menu) and 21 % stated two sources of influence (i.e. intrapersonal and restaurant/physical environment; intrapersonal and interpersonal/social environment).

When asked what would encourage them to try new children’s menu items, 57 % of responses related to menu items and design. Availability of specific foods and health-related requests (i.e. healthier alternatives, vegetarian options, fruits/vegetables) were the most common. Modified portion sizes (11 %), taste (8 %) and price and promotion (6 %) were also reported. The rest (8 %) would not change their order or did not know (10 %). When asked to select all items on a list that could influence whether they would try a new children’s menu item, children’s menu pictures (44 %), children’s menu descriptions (25 %), server suggestions (22 %), nutritional information (19 %) and price/value (15 %) were selected. The remaining options were selected by less than 15 % of respondents.

Discussion

Interventions to modify what is ordered for and by children in restaurants are needed given the impact of away-from-home foods on obesity risk( Reference Howarth, Murphy and Wilkens 1 , Reference Poti and Popkin 3 , Reference Batada, Bruening and Marchlewicz 7 , 8 ). Research suggests that targeting restaurant menu changes is a potentially relevant environmental strategy for improving dietary intake( Reference Anzman-Frasca, Dawes and Sliwa 6 , Reference Anzman-Frasca, Mueller and Sliwa 23 ). However, improving what is offered on a menu without a better understanding of what influences ordering may have limited impact. Thus, the present study used a mixed-methods approach (observations and interviews) with children and accompanying adults to identify sources of influence on what is ordered for and by children in restaurants. The rigorous methods used minimized social desirability bias compared with studies relying solely on self-report. Findings identified intrapersonal influences such as food/taste preferences, interpersonal/social environment influences such as other dining party members and restaurant/physical environment influences such as features of the menu that influenced ordering.

The innovative nature of studying restaurant-ordering behaviour addresses an important gap in the literature on away-from-home food consumption. Prior studies have used environmental strategies within restaurants to improve dietary intake( Reference Anzman-Frasca, Dawes and Sliwa 6 , Reference Anzman-Frasca, Mueller and Sliwa 23 , Reference Libotte, Siegrist and Bucher 33 , Reference Ohri-Vachaspati, Isgor and Rimkus 34 ). However, our results suggest that efforts to influence what is ordered for and by children in restaurants may need to begin before the dining party arrives at the restaurant. Focusing solely on factors within the restaurant environment may be too late to impact these decisions. Furthermore, as indicated by the relationship between child age and independence in ordering, healthy children’s menu interventions may benefit from strategies that target parents and children equally. Finally, while ordering decisions can be influenced by server suggestions( Reference van Kleef, van den Broek and van Trijp 19 , Reference Nothwehr, Snetselaar and Dawson 22 ), the present study highlights the untapped potential for servers to promote healthy ordering specifically to children.

Given the sparse literature on ordering for and by children in restaurants, future research should explore the complexities of the ordering process and how parent feeding styles, for example, are associated with ordering behaviour. Additionally, a better understanding of the family decision-making process that occurs prior to arrival at the restaurant, and at the restaurant related to meal sharing, is needed. Future collaborations between restaurants and public health have the potential to improve children’s dietary intake.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors wish to thank Shih-Fan Lin, project staff and all participating restaurants. Financial support: This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD; G.X.A., grant number R21HD071324; I.A.C., grant number R21HD071324-S1). NICHD had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: G.X.A. is the principal investigator of the study and secured the funding from NICHD. G.X.A., I.A.C., H.M., J.L.P., H.J.J., M.Z., S.G. and C.B.W. had input into study design and methods. J.L.P. was responsible for implementing the study, including restaurant recruitment and supervising data collection. All authors had input related to the interpretation of the results and contributed to writing this paper. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Institutional Review Board at San Diego State University. Verbal informed consent was obtained from all subjects. Verbal consent was witnessed and formally recorded.

References

1. Howarth, NC, Murphy, SP, Wilkens, LR et al. (2006) Dietary energy density is associated with overweight status among 5 ethnic groups in the multiethnic cohort study. J Nutr 136, 22432248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Food Marketing Institute (2014) US Grocery Shopper Trends 2014. http://www.fmi.org/research-resources/grocerytrends2014 (accessed August 2015).Google Scholar
3. Poti, JM & Popkin, BM (2011) Trends in energy intake among US children by eating location and food source, 1977–2006. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 11561164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. White House Task Force on Childhood Obesity (2010) Report to the President. Solving the Problem of Childhood Obesity within a Generation. http://www.letsmove.gov/white-house-task-force-childhood-obesity-report-president (accessed April 2016).Google Scholar
5. Krukowski, RA & West, D (2013) No financial disincentive for choosing more healthful entrées on children’s menus in full-service restaurants. Prev Chronic Dis 10, E94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Anzman-Frasca, S, Dawes, F, Sliwa, S et al. (2014) Healthier side dishes at restaurants: an analysis of children’s perspectives, menu content, and energy impacts. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 11, 81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Batada, A, Bruening, M, Marchlewicz, EH et al. (2012) Poor nutrition on the menu: children’s meals at America’s top chain restaurants. Child Obes 8, 251254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Center for Science in the Public Interest (2013) Kids’ meals II: Obesity and poor nutrition on the menu. http://cspinet.org/new/pdf/cspi-kids-meals-2013.pdf (accessed April 2016).Google Scholar
9. National Restaurant Association (n.d.) Kids LiveWell Criteria. http://www.restaurant.org/Industry-Impact/Food-Healthy-Living/Kids-LiveWell/About (accessed April 2016).Google Scholar
10. Dodds, P, Wolfenden, L, Chapman, K et al. (2014) The effect of energy and traffic light labeling on parent and child fast food selection: a randomised controlled trial. Appetite 73, 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Elbel, B, Gyamfi, J, Kersh, R et al. (2011) Child and adolescent fast-food choice and the influence of calorie labeling: a natural experiment. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, 493500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Harnack, LJ & French, SA (2008) Effect of point-of-purchase calorie labeling on restaurant and cafeteria food choices: a review of the literature. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 5, 51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Forster-Coull, L & Gillis, D (1988) A nutrition education program for restaurant patrons. J Nutr Educ 20, 22B23B.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Papies, EK & Veling, H (2013) Healthy dining. Subtle diet reminders at the point of purchase increase low-calorie food choices among both chronic and current dieters. Appetite 61, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Blair, AM, Drass, JA, Stone, M et al. (2011) Restaurant challenge offers healthful meal options and builds diabetes awareness. Diabetes Educ 37, 581588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Hanni, KD, Garcia, E, Ellemberg, C et al. (2009) Targeting the taqueria: implementing healthy food options at Mexican American restaurants. Health Promot Pract 10, 2 Suppl., 91S99S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Fitzpatrick, PM, Chapman, GE & Barr, SI (1997) Lower-fat menu items in restaurants satisfy customers. J Am Diet Assoc 97, 510514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Richard, L, O’Loughlin, J, Masson, P et al. (1999) Healthy menu intervention in restaurants in low-income neighbourhoods: a field experience. J Nutr Educ 31, 5459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19. van Kleef, E, van den Broek, O & van Trijp, HC (2015) Exploiting the spur of the moment to enhance healthy consumption: verbal prompting to increase fruit choices in a self-service restaurant. Appl Psychol Health Well Being 7, 149166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20. Acharya, RN, Patterson, PM, Hill, EP et al. (2006) An evaluation of the ‘TrEAT Yourself Well’ restaurant nutrition campaign. Health Educ Behav 33, 309324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. Fitzgerald, CM, Kannan, S, Sheldon, S et al. (2004) Effect of a promotional campaign on heart-healthy menu choices in community restaurants. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 429432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Nothwehr, FK, Snetselaar, L, Dawson, J et al. (2013) Promoting healthy choices in non-chain restaurants: effects of a simple cue to customers. Health Promot Pract 14, 132138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Anzman-Frasca, S, Mueller, M, Sliwa, S et al. (2015) Changes in children’s meal orders following healthy menu modifications at a regional US restaurant chain. Obesity (Silver Spring) 23, 10551062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24. Bernhardt, AM, Wilking, C, Gottlieb, M et al. (2014) Children’s reaction to depictions of healthy foods in fast-food television advertisements. JAMA Pediatr 168, 422426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Wansink, B & Hanks, AS (2014) Calorie reductions and within-meal calorie compensation in children’s meal combos. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, 630632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Wootan, M (2012) Children’s meals in restaurants: families need more help to make healthy choices. Child Obes 8, 3133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Bandura, A (1986) Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
28. Stokols, D (1996) Translating social ecological theory into guidelines for community health promotion. Am J Health Promot 10, 282298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Valkenburg, PM & Cantor, J (2001) The development of a child into a consumer. J Appl Dev Psychol 22, 6172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30. John, DR (1999) Consumer socialization of children: a retrospective look at 25 years of research. J Consum Res 26, 183213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31. Piaget, J & Inhelder, B (1969) The Psychology of the Child. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
32. Mata, J, Scheibehenne, B & Todd, PM (2008) Predicting children’s meal preferences: how much do parents know? Appetite 50, 367375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Libotte, E, Siegrist, M & Bucher, T (2014) The influence of plate size on meal composition: literature review and experiment. Appetite 82, 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Ohri-Vachaspati, P, Isgor, Z, Rimkus, L et al. (2015) Child-directed marketing inside and on the exterior of fast food restaurants. Am J Prev Med 48, 2230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. Ayala, GX, Castro, IA, Pickrel, JL et al. (2016) A restaurant-based intervention to promote sales of healthy children's menu items: the Kids’ Choice Restaurant Program cluster randomized trial. BMC Public Health 16, 250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Ayala, GX, Mueller, K, Lopez-Madurga, E et al. (2005) Restaurant and food shopping selections among Latino women in Southern California. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 3845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. McKenzie, T, Marshall, S, Sallis, J et al. (2000) Leisure-time physical activity in school environments: an observational study using SOPLAY. Prev Med 30, 7077.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. McKenzie, TL, Cohen, DA, Sehgal, A et al. (2006) System for Observing Play and Recreation in Communities (SOPARC): reliability and feasibility measures. J Phys Act Health 3, Suppl. 1, S208S222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Percentage of children who knew what they intended to order before arriving at the restaurant by restaurant visit frequency; interview data obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years) dining at ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA, November 2014–February 2015. Never v. monthly: χ2=6·48, P=0·011; less than once monthly v. monthly: χ2=6·12, P=0·013; more than once monthly v. monthly: χ2=3·19, P=0·074. The Bonferroni correction was done and the critical P value used for these tests was ~0·02

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Who placed the order (, child; , both parent and child; , parent) by child cognitive developmental stage; observational and interview data obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years) dining at ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA, November 2014–February 2015

Figure 2

Table 1 Food ordering behaviour for and by children in restaurants; observational and interview data obtained from 102 dining parties with 150 children (aged 3–14 years) dining at ten independent, table-service restaurants in San Diego, CA, USA, November 2014–February 2015