Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T16:20:19.885Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Trait sensitivity to stress and cognitive bias processes in fish: A brief overview

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2024

Jhon Buenhombre*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Faculty of Agrarian Science, Animal Welfare Program, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia ICB Biological Sciences, Federal University of Pará, Belém, Brazil
Erika Alexandra Daza-Cardona
Affiliation:
Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Faculty of Agrarian Science, Animal Welfare Program, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia
Daniel Mota-Rojas
Affiliation:
Neurophysiology, Behavior and Animal Welfare Assessment, DPAA, Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana, Xochimilco Campus, Mexico City, Mexico
Adriana Domínguez-Oliva
Affiliation:
Neurophysiology, Behavior and Animal Welfare Assessment, DPAA, Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana, Xochimilco Campus, Mexico City, Mexico
Astrid Rivera
Affiliation:
Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Faculty of Agrarian Science, Animal Welfare Program, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia
Catalina Medrano-Galarza
Affiliation:
Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Faculty of Agrarian Science, Animal Welfare Program, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia
Paulo de Tarso
Affiliation:
Centro Universitário Mauricio de Nassau, Sobral, Brazil
María Nelly Cajiao-Pachón
Affiliation:
Especialización en Bienestar Animal y Etología, Fundación Universitaria Agraria de Colombia, Bogotá, Colombia
Francisco Vargas
Affiliation:
Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Faculty of Agrarian Science, Animal Welfare Program, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia
Adriana Pedraza-Toscano
Affiliation:
Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Faculty of Agrarian Science, Animal Welfare Program, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia
Pêssi Sousa
Affiliation:
ICB Biological Sciences, Federal University of Pará, Belém, Brazil
*
Corresponding author: Jhon Jairo Buenhombre Vasquez; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Like other animals, fish have unique personalities that can affect their cognition and responses to environmental stressors. These individual personality differences are often referred to as “behavioural syndromes” or “stress coping styles” and can include personality traits such as boldness, shyness, aggression, exploration, locomotor activity, and sociability. For example, bolder or proactive fish may be more likely to take risks and present lower hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal/interrenal axis reactivity as compared to shy or reactive individuals. Likewise, learning and memory differ between fish personalities. Reactive or shy individuals tend to have faster learning and better association recall with aversive stimuli, while proactive or bold individuals tend to learn more quickly when presented with appetitive incentives. However, the influence of personality on cognitive processes other than cognitive achievement in fish has been scarcely explored. Cognitive bias tests have been employed to investigate the interplay between emotion and cognition in both humans and animals. Fish present cognitive bias processes (CBP) in which fish’s interpretation of stimuli could be influenced by its current emotional state and open to environmental modulation. However, no study in fish has explored whether CBP, like in other species, can be interpreted as long-lasting traits and whether other individual characteristics may explain its variation. We hold the perspective that CBP could serve as a vulnerability factor for the onset, persistence, and recurrence of stress-related disorders. Therefore, studying fish’s CBP as a state or trait and its interactions with individual variations may be valuable in future efforts to enhance our understanding of anxiety and stress neurobiology in animal models and humans.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

The functional homology of neural regions in fish is well conserved in rodents, and their behavior exhibits sufficient complexity to enable translation to both rodents and humans (Khan & Echevarria, Reference Khan, Echevarria, Vonk, Weiss and Kuczaj2017). As such, biological traits that are similar between fish and mammals have been widely utilized in models of anxiety (Egan et al., Reference Egan, Bergner, Hart, Cachat, Canavello, Elegante and Kalueff2009) and stress neurobiology (Collier et al., Reference Collier, Kalueff, Echevarria and Kalueff2017; Song et al., Reference Song, Yang, Wang, Chen, Li, Liu and Kalueff2016, Reference Song, Liu, Zhang, Peng, Wang, Collier and Kalueff2018).

To avoid any confusion about the use of the term “stress” in this article, we provide the following brief definitions: The stress response is an organism’s adaptive reaction to restore homeostasis when encountering a threatening stimulus or event (a stressor) (Chrousos, Reference Chrousos2009), resulting in either adaptive or maladaptive consequences, known as “eustress” and “distress,” respectively (Koolhaas et al., Reference Koolhaas, Bartolomucci, Buwalda, de Boer, Flügge, Korte and Fuchs2011). This response can be modulated by cognitive appraisals, where the organism evaluates the significance of the stimulus based on stored information in memory (Cerqueira et al., Reference Cerqueira, Millot, Castanheira, Félix, Silva, Oliveira and Oliveira2017, Reference Cerqueira, Millot, Felix, Silva, Oliveira, Oliveira and Oliveira2020, Koolhas et al., Reference Koolhaas, Bartolomucci, Buwalda, de Boer, Flügge, Korte and Fuchs2011; as seen in fish). According to this perspective, the stress response involves adaptive assessments and subsequent adjustments, allowing animals to respond effectively to both predictable and unpredictable events – a process known as allostasis (McEwen & Wingfield, Reference McEwen and Wingfield2003; as reviewed in fish by Øverli & Sørensen, Reference Øverli and Sørensen2016). In fish as in other species (Faustino et al., Reference Faustino, Oliveira and Oliveira2015), such as rats (Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Golebiowska, Kregiel, Kubik and Popik2015), dogs (Mendl et al., Reference Mendl, Brooks, Basse, Burman, Paul, Blackwell and Casey2010), lambs (Greiveldinger et al., Reference Greiveldinger, Veissier and Boissy2011), fowl (Zimmerman et al., Reference Zimmerman, Buijs, Bolhuis and Keeling2011), and bees (Bateson et al., Reference Bateson, Desire, Gartside and Wright2011), coping with stress and cognition are closely related processes. For instance, a fish’s appraisal of stimuli, rather than the intrinsic characteristic of the stimuli, can have significant effects on stress responses (Cerqueira et al., Reference Cerqueira, Millot, Felix, Silva, Oliveira, Oliveira and Oliveira2020, Reference Cerqueira, Millot, Silva, Félix, Castanheira, Rey and Oliveira2021) and related emotion-like (Cerqueira et al., Reference Cerqueira, Millot, Castanheira, Félix, Silva, Oliveira and Oliveira2017) or affective states.

This article explores “emotion-like” or “affective” states, encompassing descriptors with valence (indicating positivity or negativity, reward or aversion, pleasure or displeasure, among other attributes), intensity (low or high), and duration/persistence (Paul & Mendl, Reference Paul and Mendl2018). “Affective” is often used interchangeably with “emotion” or “mood” in animal literature (Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Klein Holkenborg, Reimert, Bolhuis and Webb2020) across various species, including mammals (e.g., Mendl & Paul, Reference Mendl and Paul2020), birds (e.g., Košťál et al., Reference Košťál, Skalná and Pichová2020), fish (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa and Gouveia2021; Cerqueira et al., Reference Cerqueira, Millot, Castanheira, Félix, Silva, Oliveira and Oliveira2017), and invertebrates (Perry & Baciadonna, Reference Perry and Baciadonna2017). These states give rise to a multidimensional response that can be objectively assessed through physiological, neurological, behavioral, and cognitive indicators (Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Klein Holkenborg, Reimert, Bolhuis and Webb2020).

Affective states can induce cognitive bias processes (CBP) (Mendl & Paul, Reference Mendl and Paul2020). (We provided a glossary of the terms related to CBP in Box 1 and their types in fish are further discussed in section two). Thus, CBP have been used to study the interplay between cognitive and emotional processes in various animal species, including fish (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa, Gouveia and Cajiao-Pachón2022; Espigares et al., Reference Espigares, Abad-Tortosa, Varela, Oliveira and Ferreira2021; Laubu et al., Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019; Tan et al., Reference Tan, Handasyde, Rault and Mendl2020a). Emotion-like states also seem to affect sensitivity to reward shifts (SRS) (Burman et al., Reference Burman, Parker, Paul and Mendl2008), which is related to a CBP (Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Klein Holkenborg, Reimert, Bolhuis and Webb2020). Two primary sources of variation in CBP have been studied: the living environment and personality traits (Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Bus, Webb, Bokkers, Engel, van der Werf and van Reenen2021).

Box 1. Glossary

Brief definition, and a reference to further reading where appropriate.

Cognitive bias processes (CBP): inclinations to process information in particular ways due to affective states. These cognitive biases include attention, memory, and judgment biases (reviewed by Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Bus, Webb, Bokkers, Engel, van der Werf and van Reenen2021). For example, people in negative states are more likely to make negative (“pessimistic”) judgments about events or stimuli than people in more positive states (Blanchette & Richards, Reference Blanchette and Richards2010; Harding et al., Reference Harding, Paul and Mendl2004).

Attention bias (AB): refers to the selective allocation of attention to specific stimuli, studied through attention bias tasks that gauge attention allocation (reviewed by Crump et al., Reference Crump, Arnott and Bethell2018)

Memory bias (MB): involves the influence of an individual’s current emotional state on the nature of their recalled memories (Keen et al., Reference Keen, Nelson, Robbins, Evans, Shepherdson and Newberry2014). This bias remains unexplored in fish, and animal studies on this subject have only been conducted in rodents (rats: Burman & Mendl, Reference Burman and Mendl2018; mice: Takatsu-Coleman et al., Reference Takatsu-Coleman, Patti, Zanin, Zager, Carvalho, Borçoi and Frussa-Filho2013).

Judgment bias (JB): also known as the “ambiguous cue interpretation” task (ACI) (Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013), is the propensity to judge ambiguous cues or situations more or less optimistically (reviewed by Lagisz et al., Reference Lagisz, Zidar, Nakagawa, Neville, Sorato, Paul and Løvlie2020 for nonpharmacological studies, reviewed by Neville et al., Reference Neville, Nakagawa, Zidar, Paul, Lagisz, Bateson and Mendl2020 for pharmacological studies).

Sensitivity to reward shift (SRS): also known as “sensitivity to negative and positive feedback” (Noworyta-Sokolowska et al., Reference Noworyta-Sokolowska, Kozub, Jablonska, Rodriguez Parkitna, Drozd and Rygula2019), is an indicator of affect that more or less relies on cognition and may be viewed as a bias in evaluation and involves sensitivity to rewards and losses influenced by emotional states (Burman et al., Reference Burman, Parker, Paul and Mendl2008).

The living environment significantly influences an animal’s affective states, thereby impacting CBP driven by emotions (e.g., Mendl & Paul, Reference Mendl and Paul2020). Interventions aimed at inducing negative affective states (e.g., unpredictable housing, Harding et al., Reference Harding, Paul and Mendl2004; shaking, Bateson et al., Reference Bateson, Desire, Gartside and Wright2011); chronic stress, Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013) increase the likelihood of exhibiting negative cognitive bias (NCB) (Mendl & Paul, Reference Mendl and Paul2020), characterized by a tendency to interpret situations pessimistically (Enkel et al., Reference Enkel, Gholizadeh, Von Bohlen Und Halbach, Sanchis-Segura, Hurlemann, Spanagel and Vollmayr2009). For example, exposure to acute or chronic stressors in fish has been linked to anxiety-related behaviors (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa and Gouveia2021; Collier et al., Reference Collier, Kalueff, Echevarria and Kalueff2017; Golla et al., Reference Golla, Østby and Kermen2020). Additionally, exposure to stressors (Tan, Reference Tan2017) or non-preferred social stimuli (Laubu et al., Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019) has been associated with NCB. Conversely, interventions aimed at inducing positive affective states, such as environmental enrichment or the use of anxiolytic drugs, often result in a more positive or balanced processing bias, referred to as an “optimistic” response (Bateson, Reference Bateson2016). Buenhombre et al. (Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa, Gouveia and Cajiao-Pachón2022) observed this effect in fish subjected to various forms of environmental enrichment. Similarly, Laubu et al. (Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019) found that exposure to preferred social stimuli in fish results in a positive cognitive bias (PCB). These results underscore the influence of the physical and social environment on fish CBP.

In addition to environmental factors, personality traits could also contribute to variations in CBP. For example, calves (Lecorps et al., Reference Lecorps, Weary and von Keyserlingk2018) and parrots (Cussen & Mench, Reference Cussen and Mench2015), characterized as fearful or neurotic, respectively, have exhibited a more pessimistic cognitive bias while housed under the same conditions. Furthermore, housing and personality may interact to affect CBP, as seen in studies with pigs (Asher et al., Reference Asher, Friel, Griffin and Collins2016), cows (Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Bus, Webb, Bokkers, Engel, van der Werf and van Reenen2021; Lecorps et al., Reference Lecorps, Weary and von Keyserlingk2018), and hens (Ross et al., Reference Ross, Garland, Harlander-Matauschek, Kitchenham and Mason2019).

A different approach analyses CBP as stable and enduring behavioral traits. Consequently, consecutive assays measuring CBP have been employed in rats (e.g., Enkel et al., Reference Enkel, Gholizadeh, Von Bohlen Und Halbach, Sanchis-Segura, Hurlemann, Spanagel and Vollmayr2009; Rygula & Popik, Reference Rygula and Popik2016) to categorize individuals into two phenotypic traits: those with a stable PCB, referred to as “optimistic,” and those with a stable NCB, referred to as “pessimistic.” This categorization has played a pivotal role in exploring the idea that CBP could be a trait contributing to the development, persistence, and recurrence of stress-related disorders such as depression and anxiety (Noworyta et al., Reference Noworyta, Cieslik, Rygula, Dziedzicka-Wasylewska and Faron-Górecka2021; Noworyta-Sokolowska et al., Reference Noworyta-Sokolowska, Kozub, Jablonska, Rodriguez Parkitna, Drozd and Rygula2019).

The above suggests that CBP in some species may incorporate aspects of stable personality traits and more transient affective states, similar to CBP in humans (Kluemper et al., Reference Kluemper, Little and Degroot2009; Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013). Moreover, depending on an individual’s personality, specific subpopulations of animals may exhibit varying sensitivity to environmental influences on CBP (e.g., Asher et al., Reference Asher, Friel, Griffin and Collins2016; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Garland, Harlander-Matauschek, Kitchenham and Mason2019). This hints at a potential link between personality traits, CBP, and stress resilience – the ability to manage potential stressors without significant impacts on normal physiology and behavior (Gesto et al., Reference Gesto, Madsen, Andersen and Jokumsen2018). While research indicates that fish exhibit personality traits (e.g., Castanheira et al., Reference Castanheira, Conceição, Millot, Rey, Bégout, Damsgård and Martins2017; Toms & Echevarria, Reference Toms and Echevarria2014), as further discussed in section three, these interactions have not been thoroughly explored in fish. In this context, our review aims to critically analyze and synthesize the current knowledge regarding CBP and personality in fish. We also aim to explore the potential components of CBP and how they might interact with various personality traits, influencing stress resilience or vulnerability in animals. Also, we highlight the relevance of fish studies as models for aspects of human personality.

1.1. CBP and their types in fish

In humans, emotions and moods have been shown to lead to CBP (Mendl & Paul, Reference Mendl and Paul2020). Typically, individuals experiencing negative affective states (e.g., anxiety) tend to exhibit heightened attention toward threatening stimuli (e.g., angry facial expressions), demonstrate a greater tendency to recall negative memories, and manifest negative judgments concerning future events or ambiguous stimuli (“pessimism”) when compared to those in more positive states (Barnard et al., Reference Barnard, Wells, Milligan, Arnott and Hepper2018). Drawing an analogy, similar CBP patterns have been observed in animals, indicating that affective states can also influence attention, memory, and decision-making across various species (Baciadonna & McElligott, Reference Baciadonna and McElligott2015; Bethell, Reference Bethell2015; Raoult et al., Reference Raoult, Trompf, Williamson and Brown2017; Roelofs et al., Reference Roelofs, Boleij, Nordquist and van der Staay2016), including fish (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa, Gouveia and Cajiao-Pachón2022; Espigares et al., Reference Espigares, Abad-Tortosa, Varela, Oliveira and Ferreira2021; Laubu et al., Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019; Tan, Reference Tan2017; Tan et al., Reference Tan, Handasyde, Rault and Mendl2020a).

Among cognitive measures of affective states in fish, various tasks assessing CBP, attention bias (AB), judgment bias (JB), and sensitivity to reward shift (SRS) (definitions provided in Box 1) have been employed as follows. In AB tasks, animal attention is typically assessed by tracking looking times or recording reaction times in response to specific cues. For instance, in sheep (Lee, C. et al., Reference Lee, Verbeek, Doyle and Bateson2016; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Doyle, Colditz, Belson, Cronin and Lee2018) and cattle (Lee, C. et al., Reference Lee, Cafe, Robinson, Doyle, Lea, Small and Colditz2018), anxiogenic drug administration increased looking time at a hatch (opened to reveal a threatening dog), while anxiolytics decreased it. Although eye movements can be tracked in larvae and adult fish (e.g., Dehmelt et al., Reference Dehmelt, Von Daranyi, Leyden and Arrenberg2018), there have been no formal AB studies using looking time tasks in fish. Reaction times can reveal how emotional information distracts individuals during a neutral cognitive task, potentially uncovering attentional biases, such as slower responses to negative stimuli, especially in anxious populations (Cisler & Koster, Reference Cisler and Koster2010). However, studies of this nature are scarce in animals (Crump et al., Reference Crump, Arnott and Bethell2018) and remain unexplored in fish. To our knowledge, only one study has examined AB in fish, measuring avoidance responses toward a threatening stimulus as an indicator of animal attention (Tan, Reference Tan2017). In this experiment, a rotating black strip positioned along the edge of a circular aquarium served as the threatening stimulus. Stressed fish exhibited a higher tendency to position themselves in the inner half of the tank, farther away from the threatening stimulus, in contrast to the control fish. This suggests that fish may demonstrate heightened attention toward novel visual stimuli when exposed to stress-like conditions (Tan, Reference Tan2017).

JB tasks have been adapted from rodents (Harding et al., Reference Harding, Paul and Mendl2004) to various species (see Lagisz et al., Reference Lagisz, Zidar, Nakagawa, Neville, Sorato, Paul and Løvlie2020 for a review), including fish. This experimental approach assesses the expectations of favorable or unfavorable outcomes based on previously acquired cues. During training, subjects learn to respond positively (e.g., approach a location) to a positive stimulus (e.g., position and/or color A) to receive a positive outcome (e.g., food) and negatively (e.g., avoid a location) to a different stimulus (e.g., position and/or color B) to avoid relatively negative outcomes, such as receiving no food (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa, Gouveia and Cajiao-Pachón2022; Laubu et al., Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019) or being chased with a net (Espigares et al., Reference Espigares, Abad-Tortosa, Varela, Oliveira and Ferreira2021) as observed in fish. Subsequently, ambiguous cues are occasionally introduced to evaluate their anticipation of positive or negative outcomes. The hypothesis is that, similar to humans, negative affective states lead animals to respond to ambiguous cues as if they predict a negative event, and vice versa (for a review and meta-analysis, see Lagisz et al., Reference Lagisz, Zidar, Nakagawa, Neville, Sorato, Paul and Løvlie2020).

So far, both physical and social conditions have been shown to affect JB in fish. Female convict cichlids assigned to non-preferred partners, which were predicted to elicit negative emotions, exhibited a pessimistic bias (Laubu et al., Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019). In contrast, manipulations predicted to elicit positive emotions, such as constant environmental enrichment in zebrafish (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa, Gouveia and Cajiao-Pachón2022) and staying with a preferred partner in cichlids (Laubu et al., Reference Laubu, Louâpre and Dechaume-Moncharmont2019), generated positive JB. Additionally, there is evidence suggesting the involvement of certain key genes in JB. For instance, Espigares et al. (Reference Espigares, Abad-Tortosa, Varela, Oliveira and Ferreira2021) found that telomerase-deficient fish exhibited a more pessimistic response toward ambiguity compared to their wild-type conspecifics.

When it comes to SRS, humans, for instance, tend to exhibit greater sensitivity to potential losses than gains (e.g., Dreher, Reference Dreher2007). Moreover, individuals in a negative affective state often show enhanced sensitivity to loss or failure (e.g., Taylor Tavares et al., Reference Taylor Tavares, Clark, Furey, Williams, Sahakian and Drevets2008). The assessment of animals’ SRS tasks can be conducted through operant conditioning studies designed to investigate successive negative or positive contrast effects, as comprehensively reviewed by Rygula et al. (Reference Rygula, Noworyta-Sokolowska, Drozd and Kozub2018). For example, fish can be trained to swim down a channel to obtain either high or lower value food rewards, with reward values being unexpectedly switched, and the effect of this switch on the time taken to complete the action recorded (Tan et al., Reference Tan, Handasyde, Rault and Mendl2020b). Sensitivity to reward loss has been demonstrated in various mammals, such as rats housed in unenriched conditions, which typically exhibit indicators of a more negative affective state compared to those in enriched housing (e.g., Burman et al., Reference Burman, Parker, Paul and Mendl2008). Similarly, pharmacological manipulations in rats that boost serotonin neurotransmission have been shown to decrease sensitivity to loss and increase reward sensitivity (Bari et al., Reference Bari, Theobald, Caprioli, Mar, Aidoo-Micah, Dalley and Robbins2010). However, in the case of fish, goldfish have been observed to display a downshift in performance with reduced rewards but did not perform worse than controls, indicating no sensitivity to reward loss (e.g., Couvillon & Bitterman, Reference Couvillon and Bitterman1985). Similarly, in zebrafish, individuals conditioned to high-value rewards did not change their swimming speed when rewards were downshifted, suggesting no sensitivity to reward loss. Housing type did not affect swim time either (Tan et al., Reference Tan, Handasyde, Rault and Mendl2020a).

1.2. Personality in fish

The concept of personality in humans encompasses enduring behavioral, emotional, and cognitive traits that persist over time and across different situations. However, the definition and measurement of personality can vary depending on the research approach employed. In the context of studying personality in nonhuman species, a trait approach is commonly adopted (Khan & Echevarria, Reference Khan, Echevarria, Vonk, Weiss and Kuczaj2017). Research indicates that fish exhibit personality traits (known as consistency in behavior and physiology across time and context and which is characteristic of a certain group of individuals) (Castanheira et al., Reference Castanheira, Herrera, Costas, Conceição and Martins2013a; Toms & Echevarria, Reference Toms and Echevarria2014). Fish researchers frequently employ a cluster of overlapping terms, including “personality traits,” “coping styles,” “behavioural syndromes,” “phenotypic expression,” “behavioural plasticity,” and “individual differences” (e.g., Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa and Gouveia2021; Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Demin et al., Reference Demin, Lakstygal, Alekseeva, Sysoev, de Abreu, Alpyshov and Kalueff2019). Currently, studies in fishes have identified personality traits such as boldness, shyness (e.g., Thorbjørnsen et al., Reference Thorbjørnsen, Moland, Villegas-Ríos, Bleeker, Knutsen and Olsen2021), exploration, avoidance, aggressiveness, locomotor activity, and sociability (e.g., Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Khan & Echevarria, Reference Khan, Echevarria, Vonk, Weiss and Kuczaj2017; Szopa-Comley et al., Reference Szopa-Comley, Duffield, Ramnarine and Ioannou2020).

Fish personality traits, akin to those in humans, are conceptualized as latent axes of variation that underlie observed behaviors, and these traits are often quantified using mathematical models (for a detailed explanation, see Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Houslay and Wilson2022; Toms et al., Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010). Specific assays, such as the open-field test, the novel tank test, the emergence test, and the Y-maze, among others (e.g., Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa and Gouveia2021), are employed to position fish along a continuous dimension defined by two or more axes of interest, such as boldness versus shyness. While some studies use only one of these assays to categorize fish, most employ multiple assays and assess various behaviors over time (e.g., Colchen et al., Reference Colchen, Faux, Teletchea and Pasquet2017; Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Toms et al., Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010). This approach involves using correlational and multivariate analyses to establish behavioral clusters representing the underlying behavioral axes. For example, if a fish population exhibits variation in aggressiveness, it implies that certain individuals tend to be more aggressive, leading them to display behaviors such as attacking a mirror stimulus, engaging in increased rivalry displays, or frequently chasing tank mates (Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Houslay and Wilson2022). However, recent findings indicate that there are distinct hormonal and genomic responses in fish when they engage in combat with real conspecific opponents compared to when they confront their own mirror images (Balzarini et al., Reference Balzarini, Taborsky, Wanner, Koch and Frommen2014; Oliveira et al., Reference Oliveira, Simes, Teles, Oliveira, Becker and Lopes2016; Teles & Oliveira, Reference Teles and Oliveira2016).

Research also suggests that fish exhibit “behavioural syndromes,” essentially personality traits that are correlated with each other (e.g., Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Torgerson-White & Sánchez-Suárez, Reference Torgerson-White and Sánchez-Suárez2022). For instance, in zebrafish (e.g., Ariyomo & Watt, Reference Ariyomo and Watt2012; Martins & Bhat, Reference Martins and Bhat2014), stickleback (e.g., Bell & Sih, Reference Bell and Sih2007), and guppies (e.g., Smith & Blumstein, Reference Smith and Blumstein2010), a recurring “bold-aggression syndrome” has been observed. This relationship may arise from shared physiological and genetic mechanisms, as well as environmental effects (e.g., Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Houslay and Wilson2022). However, it is crucial to recognize that not all studies have established a direct link between boldness and aggression (Way et al., Reference Way, Kiesel, Ruhl, Snekser and McRobert2015). Additionally, the various approaches to defining and measuring boldness (Toms et al., Reference Toms, Echevarria and Jouandot2010), along with the context-dependent nature of aggressiveness (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz and Sih2011; Dahlbom et al., Reference Dahlbom, Backström, Lundstedt-Enkel and Winberg2012; Zabegalov et al., Reference Zabegalov, Kolesnikova, Khatsko, Volgin, Yakovlev, Amstislavskaya and Kalueff2018), can introduce complexities in interpreting this association. Moreover, besides boldness, aggression is often associated with other traits such as activity and dominance, contributing to a broader “aggression” + behavioral syndrome (Zabegalov et al., Reference Zabegalov, Kolesnikova, Khatsko, Volgin, Yakovlev, Amstislavskaya and Kalueff2018).

Furthermore, in fish, the shy-bold dimension is associated with individual variations in both behavioral and physiological responses to stressful stimuli, often referred to as “stress coping styles” (e.g., Castanheira et al., Reference Castanheira, Conceição, Millot, Rey, Bégout, Damsgård and Martins2017; Thörnqvist et al., Reference Thörnqvist, McCarrick, Ericsson, Roman and Winberg2019; Torgerson-White & Sánchez-Suárez, Reference Torgerson-White and Sánchez-Suárez2022). These trait variations frequently cluster into two contrasting styles, representing the extremes of a continuous axis. Fish can be characterized as either proactive (engaging in active coping or bold behaviors like “fight-flight”) or reactive (exhibiting passive coping or shy behaviors, often labeled as “non-aggressive”) (e.g., Castanheira et al., Reference Castanheira, Herrera, Costas, Conceição and Martins2013b; Saraiva et al., Reference Saraiva, Castanheira, Arechavala-López, Volstorf, Studer, Saraiva and Studer2018). For example, lines of rainbow trout selected for stress-induced plasma cortisol levels exhibited correlated changes in social, feeding, and locomotor behavior (as reviewed by (Øverli et al., Reference Øverli, Winberg and Pottinger2005). Similarly, wild-type guppies demonstrated evidence of genetic correlation structures between stress-related behavioral traits (such as thigmotaxis and freezing) expressed in open-field trials (OFTs) and the levels of free circulating cortisol produced in response to an isolation and confinement stressor (Houslay et al., Reference Houslay, Earley, White, Lammers, Grimmer, Travers and Wilson2022).

However, the stress coping style model presents certain challenges. Traits vary along two independent axes: a qualitative coping style axis and a quantitative stress reactivity axis (Koolhaas et al., Reference Koolhaas, de Boer, Coppens and Buwalda2010), which can make it challenging and subjective at times to determine how observed data align with these axes (Houslay et al., Reference Houslay, Earley, White, Lammers, Grimmer, Travers and Wilson2022). Furthermore, only a limited number of studies in fish have incorporated repeated observations of both endocrine and behavioral stress response traits (Boulton et al., Reference Boulton, Couto, Grimmer, Earley, Canario, Wilson and Walling2015; Thörnqvist et al., Reference Thörnqvist, McCarrick, Ericsson, Roman and Winberg2019), and some of these studies present inconsistent (Boulton et al., Reference Boulton, Couto, Grimmer, Earley, Canario, Wilson and Walling2015) and context-dependent findings (Alfonso et al., Reference Alfonso, Sadoul, Gesto, Joassard, Chatain, Geffroy and Bégout2019; Thomson et al., Reference Thomson, Watts, Pottinger and Sneddon2012), complicating the interpretation somewhat (Prentice et al., Reference Prentice, Houslay and Wilson2022). Additionally, a recent study suggests that a single divergent stress coping style may not fully capture the diverse range of behavioral clusters beyond the original bimodal reactive–proactive characterization (Rajput et al., Reference Rajput, Parikh and Kenney2022). Furthermore, behavioral clusters can be influenced by factors such as social context (Magnhagen & Bunnefeld, Reference Magnhagen and Bunnefeld2009), strain, and sex (Rajput et al., Reference Rajput, Parikh and Kenney2022; Wong et al., Reference Wong, French and Russ2019).

1.3. Trait sensitivity to stress and cognitive bias processes

From fish through humans, some individuals are likely better at coping with adverse conditions than others (Sørensen et al., Reference Sørensen, Johansen and Øverli2013). Experience (e.g., habitat complexity and rearing conditions) (Lee, C. J. et al., Reference Lee, Cafe, Robinson, Doyle, Lea, Small and Colditz2018) and genetic factors (e.g., fibroblast growth factor receptor 1a simultaneously increase aggression, boldness, and exploration in adult zebrafish) (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Stumpenhorst, Faus-Kessler, Folchert, Rohner, Harris and Bally-Cuif2011) may control this among individual differences in fish’s stress responses (Sørensen et al., Reference Sørensen, Johansen and Øverli2013). These consistent differences among fishes modulate the way they perceive and react to their environment, which in turn affects their robustness (Vindas et al., Reference Vindas, Magnhagen, Brännäs, Øverli, Winberg, Nilsson and Backström2017) and resilience (Buenhombre et al., Reference Buenhombre, Daza-Cardona, Sousa and Gouveia2021) to challenges. For instance, proactive individuals create routines, are explorative and risk-taking (Sih et al., Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004), and seem to have a high level of active avoidance, locomotor activity, and low flexibility in behavioral responses when faced with challenges, while reactive individuals behave with the opposite patterns (Ruiz-Gomez et al., Reference Ruiz-Gomez, Huntingford, Øverli, Thörnqvist and Höglund2011; Sih et al., Reference Sih, Bell, Johnson and Ziemba2004). In addition, proactive individuals exhibit typical physiological and neuroendocrine characteristics such as higher expression of dopamine and opioid receptors (Thörnqvist et al., Reference Thörnqvist, McCarrick, Ericsson, Roman and Winberg2019), lower levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) and baseline ratio of 5-HIAA/serotonin (5-HT) (Øverli et al., Reference Øverli, Pottinger, Carrick, Øverli and Winberg2001; Winberg & Thörnqvist, Reference Winberg and Thörnqvist2016), lower hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal/interrenal activity (Øverli et al., Reference Øverli, Sørensen, Pulman, Pottinger, Korzan, Summers and Nilsson2007; Silva et al., Reference Silva, Martins, Engrola, Marino, Øverli and Conceição2010) as compared to reactive individuals.

Regarding individual differences in cognition, these have rarely been addressed in fish compared with humans and rodents (Lucon-Xiccato & Bisazza, Reference Lucon-Xiccato and Bisazza2017). Some studies suggest that fish’s personality traits can exert different influences on cognitive performance, depending on the specific task. Faster learning rates to avoid experiencing an unpleasant stimulus (an aversion learning paradigm that requires avoidance or reduced levels of activity) have been observed in risk-averse reactive individuals (Baker & Wong, Reference Baker and Wong2019; Budaev & Zhuikov, Reference Budaev and Zhuikov1998), and it has been hypothesized that reactive individuals may perceive stressors as more threatening, which could facilitate faster encoding of aversive experiences (Baker & Wong, Reference Baker and Wong2019). The expression of two neural plasticity and neurotransmission-related genes (npas4a and gabbr1a) may be involved in fear learning differences among stress coping styles (Baker & Wong, Reference Baker and Wong2021). On the contrary, the more risk-prone proactive individuals tend to show faster acquisition of memories that require higher levels of activity or paradigms with positive and rewarding valence (Baker & Wong, Reference Baker and Wong2019; Lucon-Xiccato & Bisazza, Reference Lucon-Xiccato and Bisazza2017). Likewise, Ferrari et al. (Reference Ferrari, Wisenden and Chivers2010) found that shy rainbow trout had better memory for a predator odor 8 days after conditioning it with alarm cues from conspecific skin. The latency of the fish to emerge from an opaque chamber placed in a novel tank after a 20-min habituation was used to categorize the trout. The longer the latency to emerge, the shier the individual was. However, it is worth noting that not all studies have established these associations (Kareklas et al., Reference Kareklas, Elwood and Holland2018; Vital & Martins, Reference Vital and Martins2013).

Despite several studies exploring numeracy, spatial cognition, social cognition (as reviewed by Lucon-Xiccato & Bisazza, Reference Lucon-Xiccato and Bisazza2017; Salena et al., Reference Salena, Turko, Singh, Pathak, Hughes, Brown and Balshine2021), and more recently, some studies exploring CBP in fish, the influence of fish’s personality traits on cognitive processes other than cognitive achievement has not been explored yet. Nonetheless, fish personality traits could likely be intertwined with CBP, as observed in pigs (Asher et al., Reference Asher, Friel, Griffin and Collins2016), cows (Kremer et al., Reference Kremer, Bus, Webb, Bokkers, Engel, van der Werf and van Reenen2021), hens (Ross et al., Reference Ross, Garland, Harlander-Matauschek, Kitchenham and Mason2019), and dogs (Barnard et al., Reference Barnard, Wells, Milligan, Arnott and Hepper2018).

CBP in animals is often considered a transient state influenced by the animal’s mood (Mendl et al., Reference Mendl, Burman, Parker and Paul2009). However, an alternative perspective has emerged, suggesting that CBP could also be seen as enduring traits (Faustino et al., Reference Faustino, Oliveira and Oliveira2015). Evidence from studies in rodents (e.g., Noworyta et al., Reference Noworyta, Cieslik, Rygula, Dziedzicka-Wasylewska and Faron-Górecka2021; Noworyta-Sokolowska et al., Reference Noworyta-Sokolowska, Kozub, Jablonska, Rodriguez Parkitna, Drozd and Rygula2019; Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013) and calves (Lecorps et al., Reference Lecorps, Weary and von Keyserlingk2018) supports this idea, demonstrating that these animals exhibit stable individual differences in their levels of pessimism or optimism within CBP. Furthermore, Rygula et al. (Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013) found that rodents categorized as optimistic or pessimistic after chronic stress exposure consistently made pessimistic judgments about ambiguous stimuli. This suggests that CBP in animals, similar to humans, may encompass characteristics of both a trait and a transient state (Kluemper et al., Reference Kluemper, Little and Degroot2009; Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013).

CBP has also been considered a vulnerability factor for the etiology, maintenance, and recurrence of stress-related disorders (e.g., Clark & Beck, Reference Clark and Beck2010). In humans, patients with these disorders often exhibit NCB (e.g., Disner et al., Reference Disner, Beevers, Haigh and Beck2011; Robinson, Reference Robinson, Baune and Harmer2019). Similarly, in rodents, environmental, pharmacological, and genetic manipulations that induce stress-like states (reviewed by Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Guo and Homberg2020) have been found to cause NCB. Furthermore, rats classified as pessimistic tend to show higher vulnerability to stress-induced anhedonia (Rygula et al., Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013), increased sensitivity to reward losses (Rygula & Popik, Reference Rygula and Popik2016), and an inflammatory immune profile compared to optimistic animals (Curzytek et al., Reference Curzytek, Kubera, Trojan, Wójcik, Basta-Kaim, Detka and Rygula2018).

In fish, Espigares et al. (Reference Espigares, Abad-Tortosa, Varela, Oliveira and Ferreira2021) observed NCB in a mutant strain of zebrafish with shorter telomeres and in aging fish with age-related telomere shortening. These mutant zebrafish exhibit early phenotypic alterations, including increased inflammation, which are common in aged organisms and may contribute to the NCB observed in the telomerase-deficient mutants. Additionally, Espigares et al. (Reference Espigares, Alvarado, Faísca, Abad-Tortosa and Oliveira2022) found that fish categorized as pessimistic increase their reproductive investment after chronic stress, leading to increased vitellogenesis. These findings suggest that in animals, including fish, optimistic and pessimistic traits and states may confer different levels of resilience to individuals in stressful situations (Faustino et al., Reference Faustino, Oliveira and Oliveira2015), indicating that pessimistic traits/states may be less resilient to stress, and vice versa.

2. Conclusions

Clearly, zebrafish models of CBP are still in the early stages of development, and numerous unanswered questions persist (Table 1). For instance, although zebrafish do exhibit CBP, these responses may be influenced by individual differences, such as age, sex, personality, and strain, owing to environmental and genetic variations, along with their interplay (Volgin et al., Reference Volgin, Yakovlev, Demin, de Abreu, Alekseeva, Friend and Kalueff2019). Consequently, CBP research in fish must meticulously consider, distinguish, and control for these factors and their interactions, with particular attention to personality traits. The intricate nature of these variables can potentially complicate the interpretation of CBP results, as we have discussed in this review across various species. Moreover, CBP may encompass both transient affective states and more enduring personality traits (e.g., Faustino et al., Reference Faustino, Oliveira and Oliveira2015; Kluemper et al., Reference Kluemper, Little and Degroot2009; Rygula & Popik, Reference Rygula and Popik2016). Consequently, CBP in fish could serve as a valuable model for disentangling which of these components is being assessed and how negative or positive states/traits influence an animal’s evaluation of ambiguous stimuli. This understanding can aid in identifying individuals who may exhibit higher stress resilience. For example, rodents in negative states have been shown to be less resilient to aversive events, as demonstrated by Rygula et al. (Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013), and pessimistic fish display different reproductive outcomes after experiencing stress, as observed by Espigares et al. (Reference Espigares, Alvarado, Faísca, Abad-Tortosa and Oliveira2022). Additional research is warranted to establish the validity and reproducibility of CBP as suitable measures of affective states or traits in fish. Finally, insights gained from fish research on CBP may contribute to cognitive models suggesting that stress-related disorders in humans are linked to biases in cognitive processing (Beck, Reference Beck2008).

Table 1. Selected outstanding questions in fish CBP research

Author contributions

Conceptualization: J.B, EADC, DMR, ADO, AR, CMG, PT, MNCP, FV, APT, and PS; Validation: J.B, EADC, DMR, ADO, and AR; Searching, screening, and articles extraction: J.B, EADC, DMR, and ADO; Writing – original draft: J.B, EADC, DMR, ADO, AR, CMG, PT, and APT; Visualization: J.B, EADC, DMR, ADO, and AR; Writing – review and editing: J.B, EADC, DMR, ADO, AR, and APT; Approbation: J.B, EADC, DMR, ADO, AR, CMG, PT, MNCP, FV, APT, and PS.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

The authors report no declarations of interest.

Footnotes

This is part of the special issue on animal personality.

References

Alfonso, S., Sadoul, B., Gesto, M., Joassard, L., Chatain, B., Geffroy, B., & Bégout, M. L. (2019). Coping styles in European sea bass: The link between boldness, stress response and neurogenesis. Physiology & Behavior, 207, 7685. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PHYSBEH.2019.04.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ariyomo, T. O., & Watt, P. J. (2012). The effect of variation in boldness and aggressiveness on the reproductive success of zebrafish. Animal Behaviour, 83, 4146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asher, L., Friel, M., Griffin, K., & Collins, L. M. (2016). Mood and personality interact to determine cognitive biases in pigs. Biology Letters, 12, 20160402. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2016.0402 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baciadonna, L., & McElligott, A. G. (2015). The use of judgement bias to assess welfare in farm livestock. Animal Welfare, 24, 8191. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.24.1.081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, M. R., & Wong, R. Y. (2019). Contextual fear learning and memory differ between stress coping styles in zebrafish. Scientific Reports, 9, 9935. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-46319-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, M. R., & Wong, R. Y. (2021). Npas4a expression in the teleost forebrain is associated with stress coping style differences in fear learning. Scientific Reports, 11, 12074. https://doi.org/10.1038/S41598-021-91495-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balzarini, V., Taborsky, M., Wanner, S., Koch, F., & Frommen, J. G. (2014). Mirror, mirror on the wall: The predictive value of mirror tests for measuring aggression in fish. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 68, 871878. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00265-014-1698-7/FIGURES/3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bari, A., Theobald, D. E., Caprioli, D., Mar, A. C., Aidoo-Micah, A., Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2010). Serotonin modulates sensitivity to reward and negative feedback in a probabilistic reversal learning task in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 12901301. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.233 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnard, S., Wells, D. L., Milligan, A. D. S., Arnott, G., & Hepper, P. G. (2018). Personality traits affecting judgement bias task performance in dogs (Canis familiaris). Scientific Reports, 8, 6660. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25224-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, M. (2016). Optimistic and pessimistic biases: A primer for behavioural ecologists. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 12, 115121. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2016.09.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, M., Desire, S., Gartside, S. E., & Wright, G. A. (2011). Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Current Biology, 21, 1070. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2011.05.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 969977. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.2008.08050721 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A. M., & Sih, A. (2007). Exposure to predation generates personality in threespined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Ecology Letters, 10, 828834. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1461-0248.2007.01081.X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bethell, E. J. (2015). A “how-to” guide for designing judgment bias studies to assess captive animal welfare. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 18(Suppl 1), S18S42. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1075833 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 561595. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903132496 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boulton, K., Couto, E., Grimmer, A. J., Earley, R. L., Canario, A. V. M., Wilson, A. J., & Walling, C. A. (2015). How integrated are behavioral and endocrine stress response traits? A repeated measures approach to testing the stress-coping style model. Ecology and Evolution, 5, 618633. https://doi.org/10.1002/ECE3.1395 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budaev, S. V., & Zhuikov, A. Y. (1998). Avoidance learning and “personality” in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 9294. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.112.1.92 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buenhombre, J., Daza-Cardona, E. A., Sousa, P., & Gouveia, A. (2021). Different influences of anxiety models, environmental enrichment, standard conditions and intraspecies variation (sex, personality and strain) on stress and quality of life in adult and juvenile zebrafish: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 131, 765791. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2021.09.047 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buenhombre, J., Daza-Cardona, E. A., Sousa, P., Gouveia, A., & Cajiao-Pachón, M. N. (2022). Structural environmental enrichment and the way it is offered influence cognitive judgement bias and anxiety-like behaviours in zebrafish. Animal Cognition, 26, 563577. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10071-022-01700-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burman, O. H. P., & Mendl, M. T. (2018). A novel task to assess mood congruent memory bias in non-human animals. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 308, 269275. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JNEUMETH.2018.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burman, O. H. P., Parker, R. M. A., Paul, E. S., & Mendl, M. (2008). Sensitivity to reward loss as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare. Biology Letters, 4, 330333. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSBL.2008.0113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanheira, M. F., Conceição, L. E. C., Millot, S., Rey, S., Bégout, M.-L., Damsgård, B., … Martins, C. I. M. (2017). Coping styles in farmed fish: Consequences for aquaculture. Reviews in Aquaculture, 9, 2341. https://doi.org/10.1111/raq.12100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanheira, M. F., Herrera, M., Costas, B., Conceição, L. E. C., & Martins, C. I. M. (2013a). Can we predict personality in fish? Searching for consistency over time and across contexts. PLoS ONE, 8, e62037. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castanheira, M. F., Herrera, M., Costas, B., Conceição, L. E. C., & Martins, C. I. M. (2013b). Linking cortisol responsiveness and aggressive behaviour in gilthead seabream Sparus aurata: Indication of divergent coping styles. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 143, 7581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.11.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cerqueira, M., Millot, S., Castanheira, M. F., Félix, A. S., Silva, T., Oliveira, G. A., … Oliveira, R. F. (2017). Cognitive appraisal of environmental stimuli induces emotion-like states in fish. Scientific Reports, 7, 13181. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13173-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerqueira, M., Millot, S., Felix, A., Silva, T., Oliveira, G. A., Oliveira, C. C. V., … Oliveira, R. (2020). Cognitive appraisal in fish: Stressor predictability modulates the physiological and neurobehavioural stress response in sea bass. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287, 20192922. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2922 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerqueira, M., Millot, S., Silva, T., Félix, A. S., Castanheira, M. F., Rey, S., … Oliveira, R. F. (2021). Stressor controllability modulates the stress response in fish. BMC Neuroscience, 22, 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/S12868-021-00653-0/FIGURES/4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chrousos, G. P. (2009). Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews Endocrinology, 5, 374381. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrendo.2009.106 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in the anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203216. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2009.11.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression: Convergence with neurobiological findings. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 418424. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2010.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colchen, T., Faux, E., Teletchea, F., & Pasquet, A. (2017). Is personality of young fish consistent through different behavioural tests? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 194, 127134. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2017.05.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collier, A. D., Kalueff, A. V., & Echevarria, D. J. (2017). Zebrafish models of anxiety-like behaviors. In Kalueff, A. V. (Ed.), The rights and wrongs of zebrafish: Behavioral phenotyping of zebrafish (pp. 4572), Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33774-6_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, J. L., Weinersmith, K. L., Brodin, T., Saltz, J. B., & Sih, A. (2011). Behavioural syndromes in fishes: A review with implications for ecology and fisheries management. Journal of Fish Biology, 78, 395435. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1095-8649.2010.02874.X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couvillon, P. A., & Bitterman, M. E. (1985). Effect of experience with a preferred food on consummatory responding for a less preferred food in goldfish. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 433438. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208020/METRICS CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crump, A., Arnott, G., & Bethell, E. J. (2018). Affect-driven attention biases as animal welfare indicators: Review and methods. Animals, 8, 136. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8080136 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curzytek, K., Kubera, M., Trojan, E., Wójcik, K., Basta-Kaim, A., Detka, J., … Rygula, R. (2018). The effects of pessimism on cell-mediated immunity in rats. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 80, 295303. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PNPBP.2017.04.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cussen, V. A., & Mench, J. A. (2015). The relationship between personality dimensions and resiliency to environmental stress in orange-winged amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica), as indicated by the development of abnormal behaviors. PLOS ONE, 10, e0126170. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlbom, S. J., Backström, T., Lundstedt-Enkel, K., & Winberg, S. (2012). Aggression and monoamines: Effects of sex and social rank in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behavioural Brain Research, 228, 333338. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2011.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehmelt, F. A., Von Daranyi, A., Leyden, C., & Arrenberg, A. B. (2018). Evoking and tracking zebrafish eye movement in multiple larvae with ZebEyeTrack. Nature Protocols, 13, 15391568. https://doi.org/10.1038/S41596-018-0002-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demin, K. A., Lakstygal, A. M., Alekseeva, P. A., Sysoev, M., de Abreu, M. S., Alpyshov, E. T., … Kalueff, A. V. (2019). The role of intraspecies variation in fish neurobehavioral and neuropharmacological phenotypes in aquatic models. Aquatic Toxicology, 210, 4455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2019.02.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A. P., & Beck, A. T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 12, 467477. https://doi.org/10.1038/NRN3027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dreher, J. C. (2007). Sensitivity of the brain to loss aversion during risky gambles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 270272. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2007.05.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, R. J., Bergner, C. L., Hart, P. C., Cachat, J. M., Canavello, P. R., Elegante, M. F., … Kalueff, A. V. (2009). Understanding behavioral and physiological phenotypes of stress and anxiety in zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research, 205, 3844. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.06.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enkel, T., Gholizadeh, D., Von Bohlen Und Halbach, O., Sanchis-Segura, C., Hurlemann, R., Spanagel, R., … Vollmayr, B. (2009). Ambiguous-cue interpretation is biased under stress- and depression-like states in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 10081015. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.204 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espigares, F., Abad-Tortosa, D., Varela, S., Oliveira, R., & Ferreira, M. G. (2021). Short telomeres drive pessimistic judgement bias in zebrafish. Biology Letters, 17, 20200745. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2020.0745ï CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espigares, F., Alvarado, M. V., Faísca, P., Abad-Tortosa, D., & Oliveira, R. F. (2022). Pessimistic cognitive bias is associated with enhanced reproductive investment in female zebrafish. Biology Letters, 18, 20220232. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSBL.2022.0232 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faustino, A. I., Oliveira, G. A., & Oliveira, R. F. (2015). Linking appraisal to behavioral flexibility in animals: Implications for stress research. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 104. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, M. C. O., Wisenden, B. D., & Chivers, D. P. (2010). Chemical ecology of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: A review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 88, 698724. https://doi.org/10.1139/Z10-029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gesto, M., Madsen, L., Andersen, N. R., & Jokumsen, A. (2018). Differences in stress and disease resilience related to emergence time for first feeding in farmed rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). The Journal of Experimental Biology, 221, jeb174623. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.174623 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golla, A., Østby, H., & Kermen, F. (2020). Chronic unpredictable stress induces anxiety-like behaviors in young zebrafish. Scientific Reports, 10, 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67182-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greiveldinger, L., Veissier, I., & Boissy, A. (2011). The ability of lambs to form expectations and the emotional consequences of a discrepancy from their expectations. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 806815. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2010.11.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, E. J., Paul, E. S., & Mendl, M. (2004). Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature, 427, 312. https://doi.org/10.1038/427312a CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houslay, T. M., Earley, R. L., White, S. J., Lammers, W., Grimmer, A. J., Travers, L. M., … Wilson, A. (2022). Genetic integration of behavioural and endocrine components of the stress response. ELife, 11, e67126. https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.67126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kareklas, K., Elwood, R. W., & Holland, R. A. (2018). Fish learn collectively, but groups with differing personalities are slower to decide and more likely to split. Biology Open, 7, bio033613. https://doi.org/10.1242/bio.033613 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keen, H. A., Nelson, O. L., Robbins, C. T., Evans, M., Shepherdson, D. J., & Newberry, R. C. (2014). Validation of a novel cognitive bias task based on difference in quantity of reinforcement for assessing environmental enrichment. Animal Cognition, 17, 529541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0684-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, K. M., & Echevarria, D. J. (2017). Feeling fishy: Trait differences in zebrafish (Danio rerio). In Vonk, J., Weiss, A., & Kuczaj, S. A. (Eds.), Personality in nonhuman animals (pp. 111127), Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-59300-5_6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kluemper, D. H., Little, L. M., & Degroot, T. (2009). State or trait: Effects of state optimism on job-related outcomes. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 209231. https://doi.org/10.1002/JOB.591 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koolhaas, J. M., Bartolomucci, A., Buwalda, B., de Boer, S. F., Flügge, G., Korte, S. M., … Fuchs, E. (2011). Stress revisited: A critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 2911301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koolhaas, J. M., de Boer, S. F., Coppens, C. M., & Buwalda, B. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: Towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 31, 307321. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.YFRNE.2010.04.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Košťál, Ľ., Skalná, Z., & Pichová, K. (2020). Use of cognitive bias as a welfare tool in poultry. Journal of Animal Science, 98, S63S79. https://doi.org/10.1093/jas/skaa039 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremer, L., Bus, J. D., Webb, L. E., Bokkers, E. A. M., Engel, B., van der Werf, J. T. N., … van Reenen, C. G. (2021). Housing and personality effects on judgement and attention biases in dairy cows. Scientific Reports, 11, 118. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-01843-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremer, L., Klein Holkenborg, S. E. J., Reimert, I., Bolhuis, J. E., & Webb, L. E. (2020). The nuts and bolts of animal emotion. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 113, 273286. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2020.01.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lagisz, M., Zidar, J., Nakagawa, S., Neville, V., Sorato, E., Paul, E. S., … Løvlie, H. (2020). Optimism, pessimism and judgement bias in animals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 317. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2020.07.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laubu, C., Louâpre, P., & Dechaume-Moncharmont, F. X. (2019). Pair-bonding influences affective state in a monogamous fish species. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286, 20190760. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0760 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecorps, B., Weary, D. M., & von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. G. (2018). Pessimism and fearfulness in dairy calves. Scientific Reports, 8, 1421. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17214-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C., Cafe, L. M., Robinson, S. L., Doyle, R. E., Lea, J. M., Small, A. H., & Colditz, I. G. (2018). Anxiety influences attention bias but not flight speed and crush score in beef cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 205, 210215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.11.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. J., Paull, G. C., & Tyler, C. R. (2018). Effects of environmental enrichment on survivorship, growth, sex ratio and behaviour in laboratory maintained zebrafish Danio rerio . Journal of Fish Biology, 94, 8695. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfb.13865 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C., Verbeek, E., Doyle, R., & Bateson, M. (2016). Attention bias to threat indicates anxiety differences in sheep. Biology Letters, 12, 20150977. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0977 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucon-Xiccato, T., & Bisazza, A. (2017). Individual differences in cognition among teleost fishes. Behavioural Processes, 141, 184195. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BEPROC.2017.01.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnhagen, C., & Bunnefeld, N. (2009). Express your personality or go along with the group: What determines the behaviour of shoaling perch? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276, 33693375. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2009.0851 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, E. P., & Bhat, A. (2014). Population-level personalities in zebrafish: Aggression-boldness across but not within populations. Behavioral Ecology, 25, 368373. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, B. S., & Wingfield, J. C. (2003). The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 215. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00024-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M., Brooks, J., Basse, C., Burman, O., Paul, E., Blackwell, E., & Casey, R. (2010). Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias. Current Biology, 20, R839R840. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2010.08.030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M., Burman, O. H. P., Parker, R. M. A., & Paul, E. S. (2009). Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 118, 161181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M., & Paul, E. S. (2020). Animal affect and decision-making. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 112, 144163. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2020.01.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monk, J. E., Doyle, R. E., Colditz, I. G., Belson, S., Cronin, G. M., & Lee, C. (2018). Towards a more practical attention bias test to assess affective state in sheep. PLoS ONE, 13, e0190404. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, V., Nakagawa, S., Zidar, J., Paul, E. S., Lagisz, M., Bateson, M., … Mendl, M. (2020). Pharmacological manipulations of judgement bias: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 269286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, H. A. T., Guo, C., & Homberg, J. R. (2020). Cognitive bias under adverse and rewarding conditions: A systematic review of rodent studies. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, W. H. J., Stumpenhorst, K., Faus-Kessler, T., Folchert, A., Rohner, N., Harris, M. P., … Bally-Cuif, L. (2011). Modulation of Fgfr1a signaling in zebrafish reveals a genetic basis for the aggression–boldness syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1379613807. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2892-11.2011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noworyta, K., Cieslik, A., Rygula, R., Dziedzicka-Wasylewska, M., & Faron-Górecka, A. (2021). Neuromolecular underpinnings of negative cognitive bias in depression. Cells, 10, 3157. https://doi.org/10.3390/CELLS10113157 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noworyta-Sokolowska, K., Kozub, A., Jablonska, J., Rodriguez Parkitna, J., Drozd, R., & Rygula, R. (2019). Sensitivity to negative and positive feedback as a stable and enduring behavioural trait in rats. Psychopharmacology, 236, 23892403. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-019-05333-W/FIGURES/7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveira, R. F., Simes, J. M., Teles, M. C., Oliveira, C. R., Becker, J. D., & Lopes, J. S. (2016). Assessment of fight outcome is needed to activate socially driven transcriptional changes in the zebrafish brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, E654E661. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1514292113/SUPPL_FILE/PNAS.1514292113.SD01.XLS Google ScholarPubMed
Øverli, Ø., Pottinger, T. G., Carrick, T. R., Øverli, E., & Winberg, S. (2001). Brain monoaminergic activity in rainbow trout selected for high and low stress responsiveness. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 57, 214224. https://doi.org/10.1159/000047238 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Øverli, Ø., & Sørensen, C. (2016). On the role of neurogenesis and neural plasticity in the evolution of animal personalities and stress coping styles. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 87, 167174. https://doi.org/10.1159/000447085 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Øverli, Ø., Sørensen, C., Pulman, K. G. T., Pottinger, T. G., Korzan, W., Summers, C. H., & Nilsson, G. E. (2007). Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 396412. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2006.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Øverli, Ø., Winberg, S., & Pottinger, T. G. (2005). Behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates of selection for stress responsiveness in rainbow trout—a review. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 45, 463474. https://doi.org/10.1093/ICB/45.3.463 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, E. S., & Mendl, M. T. (2018). Animal emotion: Descriptive and prescriptive definitions and their implications for a comparative perspective. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 205, 202209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, C. J., & Baciadonna, L. (2017). Studying emotion in invertebrates: What has been done, what can be measured and what they can provide. Journal of Experimental Biology, 220, 38563868. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.151308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prentice, P. M., Houslay, T. M., & Wilson, A. J. (2022). Exploiting animal personality to reduce chronic stress in captive fish populations. Frontiers in Veterinary Science, 9, 1929. https://doi.org/10.3389/FVETS.2022.1046205/BIBTEX CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajput, N., Parikh, K., & Kenney, J. W. (2022). Beyond bold versus shy: Zebrafish exploratory behavior falls into several behavioral clusters and is influenced by strain and sex. Biology Open, 11. https://doi.org/10.1242/bio.059443 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raoult, V., Trompf, L., Williamson, J. E., & Brown, C. (2017). Stress profile influences learning approach in a marine fish. PeerJ, 5, e3445. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3445 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, O. J. (2019). The neural circuitry of negative bias, oversensitivity to negative feedback, and hyposensitivity to reward in major depressive disorder. In Baune, B. T. & Harmer, C. (Eds.), Cognitive dimensions of major depressive disorder (pp. 115128), Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/MED/9780198810940.003.0010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, S., Boleij, H., Nordquist, R. E., & van der Staay, F. J. (2016). Making decisions under ambiguity: Judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 119. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, M., Garland, A., Harlander-Matauschek, A., Kitchenham, L., & Mason, G. (2019). Welfare-improving enrichments greatly reduce hens’ startle responses, despite little change in judgment bias. Scientific Reports, 9, 11881. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-48351-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruiz-Gomez, M. de L., Huntingford, F. A., Øverli, Ø., Thörnqvist, P. O., & Höglund, E. (2011). Response to environmental change in rainbow trout selected for divergent stress coping styles. Physiology & Behavior, 102, 317322. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PHYSBEH.2010.11.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rygula, R., Golebiowska, J., Kregiel, J., Kubik, J., & Popik, P. (2015). Effects of optimism on motivation in rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 32. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rygula, R., Noworyta-Sokolowska, K., Drozd, R., & Kozub, A. (2018). Using rodents to model abnormal sensitivity to feedback in depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 95, 336346. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2018.10.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rygula, R., Papciak, J., & Popik, P. (2013). Trait pessimism predicts vulnerability to stress-induced anhedonia in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38, 21882196. https://doi.org/10.1038/NPP.2013.116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rygula, R., & Popik, P. (2016). Trait “pessimism” is associated with increased sensitivity to negative feedback in rats. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 516526. https://doi.org/10.3758/S13415-016-0410-Y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salena, M. G., Turko, A. J., Singh, A., Pathak, A., Hughes, E., Brown, C., & Balshine, S. (2021). Understanding fish cognition: A review and appraisal of current practices. Animal Cognition, 24, 395406. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10071-021-01488-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saraiva, J. L., Castanheira, M. F., Arechavala-López, P., Volstorf, J., Studer, B. H., Saraiva, J. L., … Studer, B. H. (2018). Domestication and welfare in farmed fish. Animal Domestication, 46, 2018. https://doi.org/10.5772/INTECHOPEN.77251 Google Scholar
Sih, A., Bell, A. M., Johnson, J. C., & Ziemba, R. E. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: An integrative overview. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 79, 241277. https://doi.org/10.1086/422893 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, P. I. M., Martins, C. I. M., Engrola, S., Marino, G., Øverli, Ø., & Conceição, L. E. C. (2010). Individual differences in cortisol levels and behaviour of Senegalese sole (Solea senegalensis) juveniles: Evidence for coping styles. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 124, 7581. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2010.01.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. R., & Blumstein, D. T. (2010). Behavioral types as predictors of survival in Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behavioral Ecology, 21, 919926. https://doi.org/10.1093/BEHECO/ARQ084 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, C., Liu, B.-P. P., Zhang, Y.-P. P., Peng, Z., Wang, J. J., Collier, A. D., … Kalueff, A. V. (2018). Modeling consequences of prolonged strong unpredictable stress in zebrafish: Complex effects on behavior and physiology. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 81, 384394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.08.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, C., Yang, L., Wang, J., Chen, P., Li, S., Liu, Y., … Kalueff, A. V. (2016). Building neurophenomics in zebrafish: Effects of prior testing stress and test batteries. Behavioural Brain Research, 311, 2430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, C., Johansen, I. B., & Øverli, Ø. (2013). Neural plasticity and stress coping in teleost fishes. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 181, 2534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.12.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szopa-Comley, A. W., Duffield, C., Ramnarine, I. W., & Ioannou, C. C. (2020). Predatory behaviour as a personality trait in a wild fish population. Animal Behaviour, 170, 5164. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2020.10.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takatsu-Coleman, A. L., Patti, C. L., Zanin, K. A., Zager, A., Carvalho, R. C., Borçoi, A. R., … Frussa-Filho, R. (2013). Short-term social isolation induces depressive-like behaviour and reinstates the retrieval of an aversive task: Mood-congruent memory in male mice? Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 38, 259268. https://doi.org/10.1503/JPN.120050 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, S. L. T. (2017). Cognitive bias as an indicator of emotional state and welfare in captive zebrafish. Thesis, The University of Melbourne. http://hdl.handle.net/11343/192860 Google Scholar
Tan, S. L. T., Handasyde, K. A., Rault, J. L., & Mendl, M. (2020a). Insensitivity to reward shifts in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and implications for assessing affective states. Animal Cognition, 23, 87100. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10071-019-01318-6/FIGURES/7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, S. L. T., Handasyde, K. A., Rault, J. L., & Mendl, M. (2020b). Insensitivity to reward shifts in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and implications for assessing affective states. Animal Cognition, 23, 87100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-019-01318-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor Tavares, J. V., Clark, L., Furey, M. L., Williams, G. B., Sahakian, B. J., & Drevets, W. C. (2008). Neural basis of abnormal response to negative feedback in unmedicated mood disorders. NeuroImage, 42, 1118. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2008.05.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teles, M. C., & Oliveira, R. F. (2016). Androgen response to social competition in a shoaling fish. Hormones and Behavior, 78, 812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, J. S., Watts, P. C., Pottinger, T. G., & Sneddon, L. U. (2012). Plasticity of boldness in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss: Do hunger and predation influence risk-taking behaviour? Hormones and Behavior, 61, 750757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.03.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorbjørnsen, S. H., Moland, E., Villegas-Ríos, D., Bleeker, K., Knutsen, H., & Olsen, E. M. (2021). Selection on fish personality differs between a no-take marine reserve and fished areas. Evolutionary Applications, 14, 18071815. https://doi.org/10.1111/EVA.13242 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thörnqvist, P. O., McCarrick, S., Ericsson, M., Roman, E., & Winberg, S. (2019). Bold zebrafish (Danio rerio) express higher levels of delta opioid and dopamine D2 receptors in the brain compared to shy fish. Behavioural Brain Research, 359, 927934. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2018.06.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toms, C. N., & Echevarria, D. J. (2014). Back to basics: Searching for a comprehensive framework for exploring individual differences in zebrafish (Danio Rerio) behavior. Zebrafish, 11, 325340. https://doi.org/10.1089/zeb.2013.0952 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toms, C. N., Echevarria, D. J., & Jouandot, D. J. (2010). A methodological review of personality-related studies in fish: Focus on the shy-bold axis of behavior. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 125. https://doi.org/10.46867/IJCP.2010.23.01.08 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgerson-White, L., & Sánchez-Suárez, W. (2022). Looking beyond the shoal: Fish welfare as an individual attribute. Animals, 12, 2592. https://doi.org/10.3390/ANI12192592 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vindas, M. A., Magnhagen, C., Brännäs, E., Øverli, Ø., Winberg, S., Nilsson, J., & Backström, T. (2017). Brain cortisol receptor expression differs in Arctic charr displaying opposite coping styles. Physiology & Behavior, 177, 161168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.04.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vital, C., & Martins, E. P. (2013). Socially-central zebrafish influence group behavior more than those on the social periphery. PLoS ONE, 8, e55503. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055503 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volgin, A. D., Yakovlev, O. A., Demin, K. A., de Abreu, M. S., Alekseeva, P. A., Friend, A. J., … Kalueff, A. V. (2019). Zebrafish models for personalized psychiatry: Insights from individual, strain and sex differences, and modeling gene x environment interactions. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 97, 402413. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Way, G. P., Kiesel, A. L., Ruhl, N., Snekser, J. L., & McRobert, S. P. (2015). Sex differences in a shoaling-boldness behavioral syndrome, but no link with aggression. Behavioural Processes, 113, 712. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BEPROC.2014.12.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winberg, S., & Thörnqvist, P. O. (2016). Role of brain serotonin in modulating fish behavior. Current Zoology, 62, 317323. https://doi.org/10.1093/CZ/ZOW037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, R. Y., French, J., & Russ, J. B. (2019). Differences in stress reactivity between zebrafish with alternative stress coping styles. Royal Society Open Science, 6, 181797. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.181797 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabegalov, K. N., Kolesnikova, T. O., Khatsko, S. L., Volgin, A. D., Yakovlev, O. A., Amstislavskaya, T. G., … Kalueff, A. V. (2018). Understanding zebrafish aggressive behavior. Behavioural Processes, 158, 200210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.11.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, P. H., Buijs, S. A. F., Bolhuis, J. E., & Keeling, L. J. (2011). Behaviour of domestic fowl in anticipation of positive and negative stimuli. Animal Behaviour, 81, 569577. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2010.11.028 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Selected outstanding questions in fish CBP research