Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T19:19:22.042Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fear of flying, reviewed: An example of evidence-based old age psychiatry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Gianetta Rands*
Affiliation:
Camden and Islington Mental Health NHS Trust, c/o Mental Health Care of Older People, A13, Charterhouse Building, Archway Campus, Archway, London N19 5NF and Honorary Senior Lecturer, Royal Free and UCL Medical School (tel: 020 7530 2306; e-mail: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

‘Doctor, would it be alright to take mum to Cyprus for a family wedding?’ In a cosmopolitan city such as London hardly a month goes by without hearing a similar sort of query. If ‘mum’ has dementia I tend to advise the family against flying. This advice is based on anecdotal observations from past clinical practice. I have witnessed a number of patients experience significant deterioration in cognition following flying. While disorientation in unfamiliar environments may explain some of the difficulties in travelling for a person with dementia, as illustrated by John Bayley in Iris (1998), this may not be the only explanation. On this occasion, I decided to use evidence-based practice to review the situation.

Type
Special Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2002

‘Doctor, would it be alright to take mum to Cyprus for a family wedding?’ In a cosmopolitan city such as London hardly a month goes by without hearing a similar sort of query. If ‘mum’ has dementia I tend to advise the family against flying. This advice is based on anecdotal observations from past clinical practice. I have witnessed a number of patients experience significant deterioration in cognition following flying. While disorientation in unfamiliar environments may explain some of the difficulties in travelling for a person with dementia, as illustrated by John Bayley in Iris (Reference Bayley1998), this may not be the only explanation. On this occasion, I decided to use evidence-based practice to review the situation.

The clinical picture

Mrs P. presented at 67 years of age with a history of intermittently progressive, global cognitive impairment, complicated by aggressive behaviour towards her family. After a full assessment a diagnosis of vascular dementia was made. Over the past 3 years she has attended a day hospital, takes sulpiride and other medications and has a fortnight's respite every 3 months to give the family a break from their caring roles. Her dementia has progressed, she now scores around 15/30 on the Mini-Mental State Examination, and her behaviour has normalised. Because she has not been back to her native Cyprus for 20 years her relatives wanted to take her there for a family wedding.

The stages of evidence-based practice follow.

(1) Formulate answerable clinical questions

  1. (a) In people with dementia does flying affect cognition/behaviour/health/death?

  2. (b) For people aged 65 years and over, do long-haul flights affect their mental or physical health?

(2) Literature search

Electronic library searches included Medline, Pubmed, ClinPsyc and the Australian aviation databases Ozemail and Avmedlink, using key search words of DEMENTIA, MENTAL HEALTH, FLYING and TRAVEL. Media articles obtained from the Daily Mail, The Times, and The Independent on Sunday were used for contacts.

The Alzheimer's Society and Aviation Health Institute (both charities) were contacted directly, as was the Civil Aviation Authority at Gatwick.

A number of international airlines were telephoned for advice on passenger health, and Heathrow Occupational Health Department and Terminal Health Control Units were contacted directly.

(3) Critical appraisal

There were no papers specifically answering these clinical questions. The most recognised health risks of flying are deep vein thrombosis and pulmonary embolus. Over a 3-year period, 11 of the 61 deaths of passengers arriving at Heathrow airport were due to pulmonary emboli. These are associated with prolonged sitting in cramped positions and were first noted to occur in people who had spent many hours in air-raid shelters during World War Two (Reference GiangrandeGiangrande, 1999).

However, ‘changes in environment, altitude and time can increase confusion in the mildly demented passenger, and hypoxia at altitude could conceivably precipitate neurological symptoms in patients with cerebral arterial disease. There is a tendency for patients who become confused at night to do so during flight’ (Reference White and AllenWhite & Allen, 1992).

Most long-haul flights cruise at around 500 miles per hour at altitudes of 30 000-40 000 feet above sea level. At sea level, air pressure is approximately 14 pounds per square inch (PSI) and at 40 000 feet it is approximately 2.7 PSI — this is incompatible with life. The cabin environment is artificially controlled and pressurised at 10.8-12.2 PSI, equivalent to 6 000-8 000 feet above sea level. At this controlled altitude there is a reduction of 20-26% in the amount of oxygen available, resulting in haemoxyhaemoglobin saturations of 83-85% of normal. This can cause tachycardia, tachypnoea, headaches, dizziness, impaired coordination, fatigue and confusion (Reference Ernsting, Nicholson and RainfordErnsting et al, 1999).

Reduced air pressure can also cause peripheral oedema and increase in the volume of any air filled spaces, for example sinuses, stomach and guts, and any post-operative or plaster cast air pockets. According to Boyle's law, air spaces will increase by 25-35% at altitudes of 6 000-8 000 feet (Reference Ernsting, Nicholson and RainfordErnsting et al, 1999).

Humidity is also reduced at high altitudes. This can cause dehydration, hypovolaemia and reduced peripheral circulation. The humidity ‘comfort zone’ is 50-65%. A centrally heated room has about 25% humidity. Air cabin humidity is 1-20% depending on the density of passengers, each giving out moisture.

The cabin environment is controlled by air conditioning units that draw air from the plane's jet engines. This is cooled and supplied to the cabin from the front to the rear so the quality of air is best to the flight deck and first class passengers and poorest to the economy passengers at the rear of the plane. Sometimes one of the three air conditioning units is turned off to save fuel, and all units can become clogged by nicotine, reducing their efficiency.

There are no internationally agreed standards for cabin air that may have high levels of carbon dioxide, ozone, nicotine and microbes unacceptable in other artificially controlled environments such as office blocks.

Flying at high altitudes increases exposure to natural radiation. This cannot be artifically controlled and exposure in air crew is carefully monitored.

There is no international organisation with a remit to monitor health and well-being of aircraft passengers and in-flight or post-flight deaths. The International Air Transport Association carried out an 8-year survey of these deaths, but only 39% of the 120 members participated. On a major airline body bags were used once or twice per month (Kahn, 1993).

There is some evidence to suggest that time zone changes may precipitate affective disorders, particularly depression after flying east to west (Reference Jauhur and WellerJauhar & Weller, 1982).

This summarises the findings of the literature search. Critical appraisal pertaining to the clinical questions is not possible given the lack of data.

(4) Implications

There is no good evidence answering the clinical questions, but the information available supports anecdotal observations that flying may pose extra risks for people with dementia. Mrs P.'s family decided that as flying to Cyprus may be difficult and possibly harmful, it was better to leave her in respite while they enjoyed their holiday.

The number of older people is increasing and many of them are fit, rich and have friends and relatives abroad. There seems to be sufficient evidence to suggest that flights with current cabin environments may challenge mental and physical health. It may be that the costs of increasing oxygen concentrations, humidity and leg room are small compared with litigation or losing passengers when they become better informed. Despite the Warsaw Convention (1929) stating that airlines are not responsible for their customer's health (passengers are responsible for their own health), there are sufficient concerns to warrant comprehensive research and monitoring of the welfare of aircraft passengers.

References

Bayley, J. (1998) Iris. A Memoir of Iris Murdoch. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
Ernsting, J., Nicholson, A. N. & Rainford, D. J. (1999) Aviation Medicine (3rd edn). Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.Google Scholar
Giangrande, P. L. F. (1999) Aircare Worldwide – Thrombosis and Air Travel. Oxford: Aviation Health Institute.Google Scholar
Jauhur, P. & Weller, M. P. I. (1982) Psychiatric morbidity and time zone changes: a study of patients from HeathrowAirport. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 231235.Google Scholar
Khan, F. S. (1993) Airwise, the Essential Guide for Frequent Fliers. C. W. Daniel CoLtd: Saffron Walden.Google Scholar
White, A & Allen, S. (1992) How to ensure happy landings for elderly travellers. Geriatric Medicine, March, 2324.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.