Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T03:22:49.122Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ageing and taste

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2012

Lisa Methven*
Affiliation:
Department of Food and Nutritional Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6AP, UK
Victoria J. Allen
Affiliation:
Clinical Health Sciences, University of Reading, London Road, Reading RG1 5AQ, UK The Royal Berkshire NHS Foundation Trust, Reading RG1 5AN, UK
Caroline A. Withers
Affiliation:
Department of Food and Nutritional Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6AP, UK
Margot A. Gosney
Affiliation:
Clinical Health Sciences, University of Reading, London Road, Reading RG1 5AQ, UK The Royal Berkshire NHS Foundation Trust, Reading RG1 5AN, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Lisa Methven, fax +44 1183 787708, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Taste perception has been studied frequently in young and older adult groups. This paper systematically reviews these studies to determine the effect of ageing on taste perception and establish the reported extent of sensory decline. Five databases were searched from 1900 to April 2012. Articles relating to healthy ageing in human subjects were included, reviewed and rated (Downs and Black scoring system). Sixty-nine studies investigated the effect of ageing on taste perception; forty examined detection thresholds of which twenty-three provided sufficient data for meta-analysis, eighteen reported identification thresholds and twenty-five considered supra-threshold intensity perception. Researchers investigating detection thresholds considered between one and thirteen taste compounds per paper. Overall, the consensus was that taste detection thresholds increased with age (Hedges’ g = 0·91, P < 0·001), across all taste modalities. Identification thresholds were reported to be higher for older adults in seventeen out of eighteen studies. Sixteen out of twenty-five studies reported perception of taste intensity at supra-threshold levels to be significantly lower for older adults. However, six out of nine studies concerning sucrose found perceived intensity of sweet taste not to diminish with age. The findings of this systematic review suggest taste perception declines during the healthy ageing process, although the extent of decline varies between studies. Overall, the studies reviewed had low Downs and Black scores (mean 16 (SD 2)) highlighting the need for more robust large scale and longitudinal studies monitoring the impact of ageing on the sensory system, and how this influences the perception of foods and beverages.

Type
Conference on ‘Malnutrition matters’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Abbreviation:
AFC

alternative forced choice

Older adults are at risk of under nutrition due to a multitude of physiological, psychological and socio-economic factors. Physiological factors are diverse, such as malabsorption of nutrients, infection, dysphagia, as well as loss of appetite and sensory decline. Older people frequently complain of blandness of foods or sensory changes that may influence their liking and subsequent consumption of food, further impacting on their risk of malnutrition( Reference de Jong, Mulder and de Graaf 1 ). Previous researchers have used taste enhancement, aiming to increase liking and consumption of meals by older adults, with conflicting results( Reference Schiffman 2 , Reference Essed, Kleikers and van Staveren 3 ). Therefore, in order to develop foods leading to improved liking and consumption by older adults, analysis of age-related changes in taste perception is essential. This paper systematically reviews the evidence for deterioration of taste perception within healthy ageing and discusses the extent of change.

Methods: search strategy, selection, scoring and data extraction

Medical databases Medline, EMBASE and CINAHL as well as Science Direct and Web of Science databases were searched from 1900 to April 2012 for relevant articles. Search terms were: ‘taste, tastant, gustation, threshold, identification, perception, intensity, acuity.’ Within the medical databases the search included age category limits of over 65, over 80 years and human subjects’ studies. In the latter two databases, search terms ‘age, elder, old and geriatric’ were also included. Articles were excluded by either the abstract or the full paper if they did not fall within the inclusion criteria; the papers had to investigate both younger adults and older adults (over 65 years), be related to healthy ageing and not a disease state. Aroma, olfaction and smell were also excluded. Review articles found in searches were hand searched for relevant articles within reference lists, with resulting studies assessed for relevance and where suitable included in this review. Accepted articles were reviewed by two researchers independently and appraised using the Downs and Black scoring system( Reference Downs and Black 4 ). The checklist comprised twenty-six questions to evaluate the reporting, external and internal validity (bias and confounding). The final question regarding statistical power was removed as most studies were not intervention studies and hence did not provide a power calculation; however, the total number of participants in each study was reviewed. Disagreements in ratings were discussed and final consensus scores were given for each study. The data extracted included whether the study investigated taste detection or identification thresholds or supra-threshold intensities, as well as authors, publication year, sensory methodology, participant information and key findings. Meta-analysis was carried out on the data extracted from articles which investigated taste-detection thresholds using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (Version 2).

Results

Sixty-nine relevant articles were included in this review, from the initial search acquisition of 3959 articles of which 127 non-English articles were excluded. Participant numbers varied greatly depending on the study type and size, from twelve to 761 respondents; however, the study sizes were small with sixty participants as the median size. Taste detection thresholds were studied in forty papers of which twenty-three provided sufficient data to be included in a meta-analysis, either as independent group means with standard deviation or as correlation coefficients of threshold against age. Identification thresholds were reported in eighteen papers and taste intensity perception was considered in twenty-five papers. The taste modalities considered included sweet, salty, sour, bitter and umami. Papers ranged in their consideration from one to all modalities; the number of tastant compounds considered within each modality varied from one to thirteen.

Effects of ageing on taste detection

Fig. 1 summarises the meta-analysis output across all taste modalities reported. The effect size (reported as Hedges’ g), the sample size and the significance (P value) of each study can be seen in Fig. 1. Where the bar is located to the right side of the plot it indicates that a study found higher detection thresholds in older adults, the bar is on the left where thresholds were higher in younger adults; centred bars indicate no difference in thresholds between either group. However, only the tastants typically tested have been included in the plot, for example, sucrose for sweet taste and NaCl for salt taste. Other tastants within each modality, which were included in a limited number of studies, are discussed later separately. The overall consensus across all tastes and all papers is given at the end of the Fig. and the consensus for each taste modality is given within Fig. 1. The weighting of each study to the consensus is given to the right of the plot; these were derived from the sample size.

Fig. 1 Forest plot from meta-analysis of data from studies measuring taste detection thresholds in younger and older adults (five taste modalities and most commonly studied tastants).

Of the total twenty-three relevant articles that underwent meta-analysis, twenty found taste detection thresholds to significantly increase with age, and these covered all five modalities (Fig. 1). However seven studies found no effect of age for sucrose (four studies), NaCl (two studies), quinine hydrochloride (two studies), caffeine (one study), quinine sulphate (one study), citric acid (one study) and glutamate (one study). One study( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 ) unexpectedly found a significant decrease in taste-detection threshold with age for females only across two taste modalities (sour and salt). It is clear from Fig. 1 that the trend of increasing detection thresholds with age is most conclusive for umami, where all studies have observed thresholds to increase. However, this modality has only been studied by two research groups. Thresholds for salt and sour tastants increase in more than 80% of studies. Bitter and sweet tastants have also been found to be negatively affected by healthy ageing in 70% of studies.

There are numerous reasons for discrepancies across studies, including the widely varying number of participants tested, different age ranges, varying male:female ratios and different exclusion or inclusion of confounding factors such as participants with dentures and smokers. In addition, sensory-testing methodologies varied, as did the tastants used coupled with their concentration ranges and progressions. Many studies commented that there were gender differences in thresholds as well as age differences, so as the genders were not balanced in all studies this will have contributed to discrepancies( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 Reference Simpson, Rae and Parr 9 ).

Of the forty papers investigating detection thresholds, the majority used some form of alternative forced choice (AFC) procedure where tastants were presented in aqueous solution alongside control water samples; either 2-AFC( Reference Cowart 10 , Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 ) where each sample concentration was presented against one water control and the volunteer stated which was the stronger sample, or 3-AFC( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 , Reference Simpson, Rae and Parr 9 , Reference Schiffman, Sattely-Miller and Zimmerman 12 ) where each sample was presented against two controls. In some cases, volunteers were only presented each concentration once (an ascending AFC method)( Reference Schiffman, Sattely-Miller and Zimmerman 12 Reference Spitzer 19 ), whereas more rigorous papers used a staircase methodology where ‘turning points’ are established through presenting the volunteer samples below and above their individual threshold more than once to have more confidence in the individual's threshold( Reference Baker, Didcock and Kemm 8 , Reference Cowart 10 , Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 , Reference Bales, Steinman and Freeland-Graves 20 Reference Wayler, Perlmuter and Cardello 27 ). Hybrids between these two method types do exist, for example where authors have used an ascending AFC method and then repeated the determined threshold( Reference Kaneda, Maeshima and Goto 28 ). The papers in the meta-analysis are predominantly of this type. The AFC approach has also been used where small quantities of samples have been applied directly to the tongue by a pipette( Reference Mavi and Ceyhan 23 , Reference Heft and Robinson 29 ). Simpler methods have been used; for example some researchers have used only single presentation of samples rather than a discrimination test between samples and controls. This has been carried out using solutions presented for normal drinking, either as a series of increasing concentrations( Reference Kennedy, Law and Methven 30 , Reference Watanabe, Fukuoka and Hatakeyama 31 ) or as simply a single tastant solution( Reference Receputo, Mazzoleni and Fazio 32 , Reference Baggio, Dalla and Donazzan 33 ); alternatively as tastants absorbed onto filter paper discs that were placed directly onto the tongue( Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 ), or as small aliquots of tastant solution (1 ml) sprayed directly into the mouth( Reference Yamauchi, Endo and Yoshimura 6 ). The obvious advantage of such simpler methods is to avoid excessive presentation of samples to elderly participants, avoiding fatigue; however, it can lead to less reliable results. Using a single concentration of tastant and determining the proportion of people that can detect its presence, is arguably not a method from which taste-detection thresholds can be quoted. However, it has provided useful data across very large subject cohorts (n 226) where the age of the older cohorts has been higher than in any other studies (101·9 (SD 1·4) compared with 70·5 (SD 5))( Reference Baggio, Dalla and Donazzan 33 ). This Italian study found an overall significant reduction in perception of taste (P < 0·001) in centenarians compared with both adults (mean age 28) and older people (mean age 71)( Reference Receputo, Mazzoleni and Fazio 32 , Reference Baggio, Dalla and Donazzan 33 ). Another method developed to apply tastant solutions directly to the tongue was a gustometer, which enabled continuous presentation for 2 s intervals. Following each 2 s presentation the subjects give their degree of certainty concerning whether a stimulus was present or not. A large number of repetitions per person were possible using this method and results were analysed using a signal detection procedure (R index)( Reference Matsuda and Doty 35 ). This paper investigated salt detection thresholds that were found to increase with age (P < 0·0001), in line with the AFC-type studies, and noted that there was regional sensitivity across the tongue. Finally, taste-detection thresholds can be measured without the presentation of any tastant stimulus, instead through the use of electrogustometry, a technique used to first report differences with age in the 1960s( Reference Hughes 36 ), which has received attention again more recently( Reference Murphy, Quinonez and Nordin 37 , Reference Nakazato, Endo and Yoshimura 38 ). The technique assumes that responses to electrical stimulation of the tongue mimic taste function.

It was noted that electrical thresholds increased with age across all three studies( Reference Hughes 36 Reference Nakazato, Endo and Yoshimura 38 ); however, one study noted that there was poor correlation between electrical thresholds and sour or salty taste thresholds( Reference Murphy, Quinonez and Nordin 37 ). It is perhaps reasonable to conclude that although the use of electrogustometry is very useful as a clinical tool to detect substantial increases in taste thresholds, it cannot be used to infer specific taste thresholds.

Where authors investigated the same taste modality using differing tastant compounds, it was found that the taste detection responses with age vary( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 , Reference Easterby-Smith, Besford and Heath 13 , Reference Schiffman, Lindley and Clark 15 , Reference Schiffman, Crumbliss and Warwick 16 , Reference Schiffman, Gatlin and Frey 18 ). In addition, very few papers have evaluated detection thresholds within food or beverage systems( Reference Schiffman, Sattely-Miller and Zimmerman 12 ) although more researchers have considered the perceived intensity of supra-threshold levels of tastants within products( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 , Reference Chauhan, Hawrysh and Ko 39 Reference Zallen, Hooks and O'Brien 45 ).

Despite the overall consensus that taste detection deteriorates with age, there is less evidence to suggest when this decline starts, as most studies have compared a cohort of older adults with one of younger adults. Three recent studies have investigated taste detection thresholds over an age continuum. The electrogustometry method was used with 461 participants with an age range of 15–94 years( Reference Nakazato, Endo and Yoshimura 38 ). It was found that electrogustometry thresholds increased from either age 60+ or 70+ depending upon the exact site of measurement. The study by Yamauchi where aliquots of tastant solution were sprayed directly into participants mouths( Reference Yamauchi, Endo and Yoshimura 6 ) investigated four tastants with 670 participants of age 20–90 years. They found salt thresholds to be significantly higher at 70+ years, bitter at 80 + years, sour was significantly higher at 60+ years in males but not until 80+ years in females, whereas sweet thresholds were not affected by age in their study. A study that presented a series of increasing concentrations of salt solutions to 109 participants of age range 19–95 years( Reference Watanabe, Fukuoka and Hatakeyama 31 ) found no significant effect of age on thresholds. Results from the latter paper conflict not only with the Yamauchi paper, but with almost all papers in the meta-analysis that considered salt (Fig. 1). It is not clear why as all age groups were well represented in the Watanabe paper( Reference Watanabe, Fukuoka and Hatakeyama 31 ); however the difference in sensory method used may have led to discrepancies. Summarising from the limited number of papers that have considered an age continuum, it appears that taste deterioration with age is only noted in later life, beyond at least 60 years of age.

Extent of taste decline with ageing

The extent of taste perception decline with age is rarely quantified and often disputed between research studies. However, it was clear that the effect of sensory decline depended largely upon the taste modality and upon the specific tastant.

Salt

It was clear from the meta-analysis (Fig. 1) that the NaCl taste thresholds increased with age, except for females in one study( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 ). A similar result was found for other salt-taste compounds including potassium chloride( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 ) as well as Na salts of acetate, ascorbate, carbonate, citrate, phosphate, succinate, sulphate and tartrate (all at pH 7)( Reference Schiffman, Crumbliss and Warwick 16 ). Schiffman found the magnitude of salt perception decline varied from 2·7- to 26·7-fold depending on the type of Na salt( Reference Schiffman, Crumbliss and Warwick 16 ). Across studies investigating NaCl thresholds were found to increase between 1·4-( Reference Wayler, Perlmuter and Cardello 27 ) and 6·7-fold( Reference Schiffman, Crumbliss and Warwick 16 ). The mean thresholds quoted over all studies varied considerably; for older people the range was from 4·9( Reference Bales, Steinman and Freeland-Graves 20 ) to 58 mm ( Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 ); with an average across the studies of 21 mM (0·12%, w/w) compared with the average across studies for younger adults of 11 mM (0·06%, w/w). Data from one study were removed from the calculation of mean threshold as the paper quoted means values in different units for younger and older adults, implying a 57-fold decrease in threshold with age, which contradicted the direction of change quoted in the paper( Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 ).

This difference in means across studies equated to a mean increase between younger and older adults of 2·0-fold. It is interesting to consider whether this extent of increase could effect detection of salt in meals by older people, certainly a level of 0·12% (w/w) would be below the level of salt in most foods. The British Dietetic Association class low, medium and high-salt food to contain levels within the ranges of ⩽0·3 g, 0·3–1·5 g and >1·5 g per 100 g product. Although the distribution of individual salt detection thresholds is wide, a large study (n 146) by Baker( Reference Baker, Didcock and Kemm 8 ) found that very few individuals had thresholds above 50 mm (0·3% NaCl).

Sour

The tastants included in the meta-analysis plot (Fig. 1) were citric, tartaric and acetic acids, the thresholds for which were found to increase with age, except in one out of five studies on citric acid where thresholds were found to be higher in a younger group of females( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 ). A study of hydrochloric acid with males found thresholds to increase with age( Reference Spitzer 19 ). From four citric acid studies, the reported increase in threshold varied between 1·4-( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 ) and 11-fold( Reference Bartoshuk, Rifkin and Marks 46 ); however, the actual thresholds measured were much lower in the latter study where the total sample size was small (n 36) and there was a disproportionate number of females (89%). Across the studies the mean thresholds for younger adults was 0·4 mM and for older adults 0·7 mM, representing a 1·5-fold increase with age.

Bitter

The effect of age on bitter detection thresholds has been reported for thirteen different compounds across nine different studies( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 , Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 , Reference Schiffman, Gatlin and Frey 18 , Reference Spitzer 19 , Reference Cowart, Yokomukai and Beauchamp 21 , Reference Mavi and Ceyhan 23 , Reference Stevens 24 , Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 , Reference Bartoshuk, Rifkin and Marks 46 ) and in all but one case( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 ) have been found to increase with age. The most common tastants studied are quinine derivatives and caffeine (Fig. 1). The extent of increase reported for quinine detection thresholds was between 1·5-( Reference Schiffman, Gatlin and Frey 18 ) and 7·4-fold( Reference Bartoshuk, Rifkin and Marks 46 ). For quinine hydrochloride the mean thresholds across four studies for the younger and older adult groups were 0·002 and 0·009 mM, respectively, representing a 4·1-fold increase. For quinine sulphate the means were 0·005 M and 0·019 mM, respectively, similarly, a 4·0-fold increase. Interestingly, the results for caffeine were less notable; the extent of increase with age reported to be from 1·1-( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 ) to 1·6-fold( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 ). Across four studies the mean caffeine thresholds for younger and older groups was 1·4 and 1·8 mM, respectively, representing an overall mean increase of 1·2-fold with age.

It was noted that some studies took into account genetic differences in ability to detect the bitter phenylthiocarbamide or propylthiouracil, whereas others did not, which would be a confounding factor. However, studies investigating these tastants found thresholds to increase with age( Reference Schiffman, Gatlin and Frey 18 , Reference Bartoshuk, Rifkin and Marks 46 ). Schiffman( Reference Schiffman, Gatlin and Frey 18 ) evaluated a wide range of thirteen bitter tastants in one study where participants were first screened into phenylthiocarbamide taste/non-taster groups. The detection thresholds were significantly (P < 0·05) higher for the older group for seven of the compounds, with the extent of difference within the non-phenylthiocarbamide taster group ranging from 1·5-fold for quinine hydrochloride to 33-fold for magnesium nitrate. However, the total sample size was small (n 34; sixteen younger, eighteen older; half phenylthiocarbamide non-tasters).

Sweet

Sucrose-detection thresholds were measured in ten studies, four of which found no effect of age. The remaining seven studies did find an increase with age varying from 1·2-( Reference Kaneda, Maeshima and Goto 28 ) to 2·6-fold( Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 ). The mean threshold across the studies was 12·4 mM for younger adults and 16·8 mM for older adults, representing a 1·4-fold increase. A limited number of studies have investigated sweeteners. Two studies on saccharin both concluded thresholds were approximately 4-fold higher in older adults( Reference Easterby-Smith, Besford and Heath 13 , Reference Schiffman, Lindley and Clark 15 ). Regarding aspartame, one study found thresholds to increase 4·1-fold with age( Reference Schiffman, Lindley and Clark 15 ), whereas two other studies found no difference between age groups( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 , Reference Easterby-Smith, Besford and Heath 13 ). Schiffman evaluated a further nine sweeteners and found detection thresholds of all to increase with age by between 1·5-fold (sodium cyclamate) to 4·7-fold (monellin)( Reference Schiffman, Lindley and Clark 15 ).

Umami

Detection thresholds for umami have been evaluated in fewer studies. Three studies of monosodium glutamate all found detection threshold to increase with age. The mean threshold across studies was 2·5 mM for younger adults and 5·5 mM for older, representing a 2·2-fold increase. Two studies of inosine monophospate reported 4·4-fold higher detection thresholds in older adults (1·5 mM compared with 0·3 mM). Schiffman investigated thresholds of a further four glutamate salts all of which were between 3·7- and 8·5-fold higher for older adults( Reference Schiffman, Frey and Luboski 17 ). Glutamates and inosine monophospate interact synergistically to increase overall umami taste. Thresholds for all four glutamate salts tested in combination with inosine monophospate also had higher thresholds in older adults compared with younger( Reference Schiffman, Frey and Luboski 17 ).

Effects of ageing on taste identification thresholds

Eighteen studies considered identification thresholds( Reference Wardwell, Chapman-Novakofski and Brewer 5 , Reference Yamauchi, Endo and Yoshimura 6 , Reference Schiffman, Sattely-Miller and Zimmerman 12 , Reference Schiffman, Lindley and Clark 15 Reference Schiffman, Frey and Luboski 17 , Reference Kennedy, Law and Methven 30 , Reference Receputo, Mazzoleni and Fazio 32 Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 , Reference Chang and Ou 42 , Reference Kim, Hur and Cho 47 Reference Yang, Wei and Yu 53 ). Some authors using the AFC methodology report identification thresholds post-detection within the same procedure. Others used simpler methods such as ascending presentation of a series (state when identified), either as solutions( Reference Chang and Ou 42 , Reference Kim, Hur and Cho 47 ) or taste discs( Reference Fukunaga, Uematsu and Sugimoto 34 ), presenting a single concentration of a tastant and determining the proportion of subjects that can name it( Reference Receputo, Mazzoleni and Fazio 32 , Reference Baggio, Dalla and Donazzan 33 ). A recent simple but effective means to determine identification thresholds has been the use of taste strips; four different concentrations per tastant, applied directly to the tongue( Reference Mueller, Pintscher and Renner 48 , Reference Welge-Lussen, Dorig and Wolfensberger 52 ); or similarly using four concentrations of solution applied as drops( Reference Pingel, Ostwald and Pau 50 , Reference Yang, Wei and Yu 53 ). Seventeen studies found taste-identification thresholds to increase with age, although one study reported only a weak relationship between age and taste strip results( Reference Welge-Lussen, Dorig and Wolfensberger 52 ). Only one study found no significant difference between age groups( Reference Yang, Wei and Yu 53 ); however, their older group was younger than in all other studies reported in this review (age 51–65), and therefore outside of our inclusion criteria.

Effects of ageing on perception of taste intensity at supra-threshold levels

Table 1 summarises the twenty-five extracted studies which report perceived intensity of tastants by younger and older adults. A similar review was done by Mojet in 2001( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 54 ). The most common assessment method was magnitude estimation followed by the use of various category scales and also the calculation of Weber ratios through just noticeable difference discrimination tests.

Table 1 Review of studies comparing perceived intensity of tastants and supra-threshold levels in younger and older adults

DB, Downs and Black score; OP, older participants; YP, younger participants; f, female; SU, sucrose; NaCl, sodium chloride; CA, citric acid; QS, quinine sulphate; Caf, caffeine; IMP, inosine monophsophate; AA, acetic acid; MSG, monosodium glutamate; KCl, potassium chloride; QHCl, quinine hydrochloride.

* Number of OP (x,y denotes two groups of older volunteers).

Number of YP.

Weber fraction calculated from just noticeable difference (JND)/point of subjective equality (PSE); by 2-AFC (alternative forced choice).

§ Number in brackets correspond to number of levels that tastants tested.

Indicates whether a significant difference was found between YP and OP.

When aiming to relate taste perception to food liking and choice, it is perhaps perceived intensity at supra-threshold levels that is most important if the tastant levels in foods are likely to be above detection thresholds. As noted in Table 1, a wide range of tastants have been investigated and some researchers have measured perceived intensities in products( Reference Mojet, Christ-Hazelhof and Heidema 11 , Reference Chauhan, Hawrysh and Ko 39 , Reference Chauhan and Hawrysh 40 , Reference Drewnowski, Henderson and Driscoll 43 Reference Zallen, Hooks and O'Brien 45 , Reference DeJong, DeGraaf and VanStaveren 56 , Reference Zandstra and de Graaf 62 Reference Chauhan 64 ). Sixteen of the twenty-five studies noted that age had a significant negative impact on the intensity of perception, and a further two reported non-significant trends. This finding was relatively consistent for caffeine, citric acid, quinine and NaCl. Regarding sucrose, six studies found no significant effect of age on perceived intensity, which was disputed in a further three studies. Magnitude estimation studies where psychophysical functions could be calculated by plotting log perceived intensity against log concentration, tended to find that the slope was flatter for older volunteers, particularly as higher concentrations of tastants were perceived as less intense than for younger volunteers. Only three studies reported no age-related differences in intensity scoring. The extent of effect was not frequently reported in the supra-threshold studies. However, Schiffman's magnitude estimation studies found the slope of perceived intensity against tastant concentration to be steeper for young adults than for older adults by a mean factor of 2·06 for sweet compounds( Reference Schiffman, Lindley and Clark 15 ) and 1·76 for bitter compounds( Reference Schiffman, Gatlin and Frey 18 ). Four studies investigating both pure solutions and products found a significant decrease in perception with age in both cases( Reference Chauhan, Hawrysh and Ko 39 Reference Chauhan 41 , Reference Mojet, Heidema and Christ-Hazelhof 44 ). In the Mojet paper, this effect was consistent over a wide range of tastants( Reference Mojet, Heidema and Christ-Hazelhof 44 ). However, one study found a significant effect of age for salt solution intensity which was not supported in both products( Reference Drewnowski, Henderson and Driscoll 43 ).

Quality of data and reporting of studies

The Downs and Black scores for the reviewed articles ranged between eleven and twenty-one out of a possible twenty-seven with an average rating of 16 (SD 2). This average is low, with many studies failing to fully incorporate and describe confounding factors, and very few reporting blinding of both the participants and the organisers throughout the investigation, usually typical of clinical trials. Furthermore, the use of various sensory methods, and small participant numbers in most studies, reduces the ability to collate and compare results without over-emphasising methodological noise.

Conclusion

Overall, this systematic review generally found an age-related decrease in taste thresholds and sensitivity with age. However, the extent and significance of this decline varied between taste modalities, tastants and studies.

The effect of age on sensory perception, and specifically taste perception, is complex, due to the highly heterogeneous nature of the older community. The main conclusion to be drawn from the studies reviewed in this paper is that taste perception declines with age. Understanding this decline in taste ability could help the development of specifically enhanced foods for older adults to compensate for sensory losses. While deterioration in salt perception should not be compensated for by the addition of extra salt in food for elderly people who may already be at risk of hypertension, CVD or hypernatraemia, authors have suggested that increased levels of umami tastants can improve liking and consumption of foods by older adults. Although this has typically been achieved through the direct addition of monosodium glutamate( Reference Schiffman 2 ), it can also be achieved through the use of natural ingredients rich in umami taste compounds( Reference Dermiki, Suwankanit and Nilkamhaeng 65 ).

Sensory decline is a generic process and happens to everyone, yet several factors can influence the extent of this sensory decline. Nutritional status, vitamin and micronutrient intakes can all influence sensory perception, and the extent of decline with age, with research focussing on the involvement of Zn in taste perception( Reference Stewart-Knox, Simpson and Parr 66 ). Dentition in older adults could also influence sensory perception, especially if portions of the palate are covered, as well as impacting on salivation( Reference Wayler, Perlmuter and Cardello 27 ).

Although the majority of studies reviewed reported a significant age-related decline in perceived intensity at supra-threshold levels, the extent of decline was under-reported. Yet, in order to determine how this should be addressed when developing foods and beverages for the older adult market, it is the extent of decline that is important to establish. Across a range of ten tastants in five product types, Mojet found no correlations between detection threshold sensitivity and preferred tastant concentration. However, there was evidence of a negative correlation between supra-threshold perceived intensity and preferred concentration in products for salt (P < 0·05), caffeine (P < 0·001), aspartame (P < 0·01) and inosine monophospate (P < 0·05). In other words, people with reduced intensity perception preferred higher concentrations of these tastants. Knowledge of the decline of taste and olfactory perception with age has led to the use of taste and flavour enhancement of foods, aiming to improve liking and ultimately consumption by older adults. Although this approach has been successful in some studies( Reference Schiffman 2 ), it has not in others( Reference Essed, Kleikers and van Staveren 3 ). In order for such studies to succeed it may be important to know the extent of decline in intensity perception for more complex mixtures of tastants within real food systems, as well as to account for the numerous confounding factors within individual perception.

The Downs and Black scores obtained by these studies were low, and in order to judge the abilities of older cohorts and understand effects of ageing, more robust and larger cohort studies are needed. There are potentially a large number of factors beyond age which may differ between young and older adult groups. Longitudinal studies would clarify whether variation between cohorts is due to individuals or the effect of time and larger, more robust cohort studies maybe more practical at controlling some of these variables.

Acknowledgements

The authors declare no known conflicts of interest. The authors were partly supported by Research into Ageing (Age UK) and/or by Nestec Ltd in related research projects. L. M. conducted the systematic review jointly with V. A. and wrote the manuscript. V. A. jointly conducted the systematic review, carried out the Downs and Blacks scoring of the included papers, carried out the meta-analysis and edited the manuscript. C. W. carried out the Downs and Blacks scoring of the included papers and edited the manuscript. M. G. read, reviewed and improved the manuscript for submission.

References

1. de Jong, N, Mulder, I, de Graaf, C et al. (1999) Impaired sensory functioning in elders: the relation with its potential determinants and nutritional intake. J Geront A Biol Sci Med Sci 54, B324B331.Google Scholar
2. Schiffman, SS (1998) Sensory enhancement of foods for the elderly with monosodium glutamate and flavors. Food Rev Int 14, 321333.Google Scholar
3. Essed, NH, Kleikers, S, van Staveren, WA et al. (2009) No effect on intake and liking of soup enhanced with monosodium glutamate and celery powder among elderly people with olfactory and/or gustatory loss. Int J Food Sci Nutr 60, 143154.Google Scholar
4. Downs, SH & Black, N (1998) The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomized and non-randomized studies of health care interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health 52, 377384.Google Scholar
5. Wardwell, L, Chapman-Novakofski, K & Brewer, MS (2009) Effects of age, gender and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease on taste acuity. Int J Food Sci Nutr 60, 8497.Google Scholar
6. Yamauchi, Y, Endo, S & Yoshimura, I (2002) A new whole-mouth gustatory test procedure. II. Effects of aging, gender and smoking. Acta Otolaryngol 122, Suppl 546, 4959.Google Scholar
7. Hyde, RJ & Feller, RP (1981) Age and sex effects on taste of sucrose, NaCl, citric acid and caffeine. Neurobiol Aging 2, 315318.Google Scholar
8. Baker, KA, Didcock, EA, Kemm, JR et al. (1983) Effect of age, sex and illness on salt taste detection thresholds. Age Ageing 12, 159165.Google Scholar
9. Simpson, EEA, Rae, G, Parr, H et al. (2012) Predictors of taste acuity in healthy older Europeans. Appetite 58, 188195.Google Scholar
10. Cowart, BJ (1989) Relationships between taste and smell across the adult life span. Ann N Y Acad Sci 561, 3955.Google Scholar
11. Mojet, J, Christ-Hazelhof, E & Heidema, J (2005) Taste perception with age: pleasantness and its relationships with threshold sensitivity and supra-threshold intensity of five taste qualities. Food Quality Preference 16, 413423.Google Scholar
12. Schiffman, SS, Sattely-Miller, EA, Zimmerman, IA et al. (1994) Taste perception of monosodium glutamate (MSG) in foods in young and elderly subjects. Physiol Behav 56, 265275.Google Scholar
13. Easterby-Smith, V, Besford, J & Heath, MR (1994) The effect of age on the recognition thresholds of three sweeteners: sucrose, saccharin and aspartame. Gerodontology 11, 3945.Google Scholar
14. Schiffman, SS, Hornack, K & Reilly, D (1979) Increased taste thresholds of amino acids with age. Am J Clin Nutr 32, 16221627.Google Scholar
15. Schiffman, SS, Lindley, MG, Clark, TB et al. (1981) Molecular mechanism of sweet taste: relationship of hydrogen bonding to taste sensitivity for both young and elderly. Neurobiol Aging 2, 173185.Google Scholar
16. Schiffman, SS, Crumbliss, AL, Warwick, ZS et al. (1990) Thresholds for sodium salts in young and elderly human subjects: correlation with molar conductivity of anion. Chem Senses 15, 671678.Google Scholar
17. Schiffman, SS, Frey, AE, Luboski, JA et al. (1991) Taste of glutamate salts in young and elderly subjects: role of inosine 5′-monophosphate and ions. Physiol Behav 49, 843854.Google Scholar
18. Schiffman, SS, Gatlin, LA, Frey, AE et al. (1994) Taste perception of bitter compounds in young and elderly persons: relation to lipophilicity of bitter compounds. Neurobiol Aging 15, 743750.Google Scholar
19. Spitzer, ME (1988) Taste acuity in institutionalized and noninstitutionalized elderly men. J Gerontol 43, P71P74.Google Scholar
20. Bales, CW, Steinman, LC & Freeland-Graves, JH (1986) The effect of age on plasma zinc uptake and taste acuity. Am J Clin Nutr 44, 664669.Google Scholar
21. Cowart, BJ, Yokomukai, Y & Beauchamp, GK (1994) Bitter taste in aging: compound-specific decline in sensitivity. Physiol Behav 56, 12371241.Google Scholar
22. Grzegorczyk, PB, Jones, SW & Mistretta, CM (1979) Age-related differences in salt taste acuity. J Gerontol 34, 834840.Google Scholar
23. Mavi, A & Ceyhan, O (1999) Bitter taste threshold and its relation to number of circumvallate papillae in the elderly. Aging Clin Exp Res 11, 6163.Google Scholar
24. Stevens, JC (1996) Detection of tastes in mixture with other tastes: issues of masking and aging. Chem Senses 21, 211221.Google Scholar
25. Stevens, JC, Cruz, LA, Hoffman, JM et al. (1995) Taste sensitivity and aging: high incidence of decline revealed by repeated threshold measures. Chem Senses 20, 451459.Google Scholar
26. Stevens, JC, Cruz, LA, Marks, LE et al. (1998) A multimodal assessment of sensory thresholds in aging. J Gerontol B, Psychol Sci Soc Sci 53, P263P272.Google Scholar
27. Wayler, AH, Perlmuter, LC, Cardello, AV et al. (1990) Effects of age and removable artificial dentition on taste. Spec Care Dentist 10, 107113.Google Scholar
28. Kaneda, H, Maeshima, K, Goto, N et al. (2000) Decline in taste and odor discrimination abilities with age, and relationship between gustation and olfaction. Chem Senses 25, 331337.Google Scholar
29. Heft, MW & Robinson, ME (2010) Age differences in orofacial sensory thresholds. J Dent Res 89, 11021105.Google Scholar
30. Kennedy, O, Law, C, Methven, L et al. (2010) Investigating age-related changes in taste and affects on sensory perceptions of oral nutritional supplements. Age Ageing 39, 733738.Google Scholar
31. Watanabe, M, Fukuoka, Y, Hatakeyama, A et al. (2008) Salt taste perception and salt intake in older people. Geriatr Gerontol Int 8, 6264.Google Scholar
32. Receputo, G, Mazzoleni, G, Fazio, ID et al. (1996) Study on the sense of taste in a group of Sicilian centenarians. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 22, Suppl 1, 411414.Google Scholar
33. Baggio, G, Dalla, M, Donazzan, S et al. (1998) Assessment of sense of taste in Italian centenarians. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 26, 177183.Google Scholar
34. Fukunaga, A, Uematsu, H & Sugimoto, K (2005) Influences of aging on taste perception and oral somatic sensation. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 60, 109113.Google Scholar
35. Matsuda, T & Doty, RL (1995) Regional taste sensitivity to NaCl: relationship to subject age, tongue locus and area of stimulation. Chem Senses 20, 283290.Google Scholar
36. Hughes, G (1969) Changes in taste sensitivity with advancing age. Gerontol Clini 11, 224230.Google Scholar
37. Murphy, C, Quinonez, C & Nordin, S (1995) Reliability and validity of electrogustometry and its application to young and elderly persons. Chem Senses 20, 499503.Google Scholar
38. Nakazato, M, Endo, S, Yoshimura, I et al. (2002) Influence of aging on electrogustometry thresholds. Acta Otolaryngol 122, Suppl 546, 1626.Google Scholar
39. Chauhan, J, Hawrysh, ZJ, Ko, C et al. (1987) Taste perception of salt in young, old, and very old adults. Ann N Y Acad Sci 510, 222223.Google Scholar
40. Chauhan, J & Hawrysh, ZJ (1988) Suprathreshold sour taste intensity and pleasantness perception with age. Physiol Behav 43, 601607.Google Scholar
41. Chauhan, J (1989) Relationships between sour and salt taste perception and selected subject attributes. J Am Diet Assoc 89, 652658.Google Scholar
42. Chang, Y-F & Ou, AS-M (1999) Taste impairment and related anthropometric factors in aging. Food Sci Agric Chem 1, 257263.Google Scholar
43. Drewnowski, A, Henderson, SA, Driscoll, A et al. (1996) Salt taste perceptions and preferences are unrelated to sodium consumption in healthy older adults. J Am Diet Assoc 96, 471474.Google Scholar
44. Mojet, J, Heidema, J & Christ-Hazelhof, E (2003) Taste perception with age: generic or specific losses in supra-threshold intensities of five taste qualities? Chem Senses 28, 397413.Google Scholar
45. Zallen, EM, Hooks, LB & O'Brien, K (1990) Salt taste preferences and perceptions of elderly and young adults. J Am Diet Assoc 90, 947950.Google Scholar
46. Bartoshuk, LM, Rifkin, B, Marks, LE et al. (1986) Taste and aging. J Gerontol 41, 5157.Google Scholar
47. Kim, WY, Hur, M, Cho, MS et al. (2003) Effect of olfactory function on nutritional status of Korean elderly women. Nutr Res 23, 723734.Google Scholar
48. Mueller, CA, Pintscher, K & Renner, B (2011) Clinical test of gustatory function including umami taste. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 120, 358362.Google Scholar
49. Nilsson, B (1979) Taste acuity of the human palate. III. Studies with taste solutions on subjects in different age groups. Acta Odontol Scand 37, 235252.Google Scholar
50. Pingel, J, Ostwald, J, Pau, HW et al. (2010) Normative data for a solution-based taste test. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngol 267, 19111917.Google Scholar
51. Schiffman, SS, Gatlin, LA, Sattely-Miller, EA et al. (1994) The effect of sweeteners on bitter taste in young and elderly subjects. Brain Res Bull 35, 189204.Google Scholar
52. Welge-Lussen, A, Dorig, P, Wolfensberger, M et al. (2011) A study about the frequency of taste disorders. J Neurol 258, 386392.Google Scholar
53. Yang, L, Wei, Y, Yu, D et al. (2010) Olfactory and gustatory function in healthy adult Chinese subjects. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 143, 554560.Google Scholar
54. Mojet, J, Christ-Hazelhof, E & Heidema, J (2001) Taste perception with age: generic or specific losses in threshold sensitivity to the five basic tastes? Chem Senses 26, 845860.Google Scholar
55. Enns, MP, Van, TBT & Grinker, JA. (1979) Contributions of age, sex and degree of fatness on preferences and magnitude estimations for sucrose in humans. Physiol Behav 22, 9991003.Google Scholar
56. DeJong, N, DeGraaf, C & VanStaveren, WA (1996) Effect of sucrose in breakfast items on pleasantness and food intake in the elderly. Physiol Behav 60, 14531462.Google Scholar
57. Schiffman, SS & Clark, TB 3rd. (1980) Magnitude estimates of amino acids for young and elderly subjects. Neurobiol Aging 1, 8191.Google Scholar
58. Weiffenbach, JM, Cowart, BJ & Baum, BJ (1986) Taste intensity perception in aging. J Gerontol 41, 460468.Google Scholar
59. Gilmore, MM & Murphy, C (1989) Aging is associated with increased Weber ratios for caffeine, but not for sucrose. Percept Psychophys 46, 555559.Google Scholar
60. Murphy, C & Gilmore, MM (1989) Quality-specific effects of aging on the human taste system. Percept Psychophys 45, 121128.Google Scholar
61. Weiffenbach, JM, Tylenda, CA & Baum, BJ (1990) Oral sensory changes in aging. J Gerontol 45, M121M125.Google Scholar
62. Zandstra, EH & de Graaf, C (1998) Sensory perception and pleasantness of orange beverages from childhood to old age. Food Quality Preference 9, 512.Google Scholar
63. Nordin, S, Razani, LJ, Markison, S et al. (2003) Age-associated increases in intensity discrimination for taste. Exp Aging Res 29, 371381.Google Scholar
64. Chauhan, J (1989) Pleasantness perception of salt in young vs. elderly adults. J Am Diet Assoc 89, 834835.Google Scholar
65. Dermiki, M, Suwankanit, C, Nilkamhaeng, S et al. (2011) Taste enhancement of savoury food aiming to increase acceptance by an elderly cohort. Proc Nutr Soc 70, E177.Google Scholar
66. Stewart-Knox, BJ, Simpson, EEA, Parr, H et al. (2005) Zinc status and taste acuity in older Europeans: the ZENITH study. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, S31S36.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Forest plot from meta-analysis of data from studies measuring taste detection thresholds in younger and older adults (five taste modalities and most commonly studied tastants).

Figure 1

Table 1 Review of studies comparing perceived intensity of tastants and supra-threshold levels in younger and older adults