Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:39:55.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceived Threat of Disaster, Efficacy, and Psychological Distress Among Individuals with a Physical Disability: A Longitudinal Model

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2023

Ashley E. Reed*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
Cynthia A. Rohrbeck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
Philip W. Wirtz
Affiliation:
Department of Decision Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
Jennifer E. Marceron
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
*
Corresponding author: Ashley Reed; [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Individuals with physical disabilities experience distress when faced with the threat of human-made and natural disasters, yet little is known about how to reduce that distress. This study used Protection Motivation Theory to longitudinally test the relationships between psychological distress and disaster-related cognitive appraisals, including perceived threat, emergency preparedness self-efficacy, and response efficacy, in a sample of individuals with physical disabilities.

Methods:

A nationwide convenience sample of 106 adults completed 2 surveys approximately 5 years apart. Structural equation modeling was used to assess effects of perceived threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy on psychological distress across the 2 waves.

Results:

Our results suggest that the associations of proximal perceived threat and self-efficacy with psychological distress remain stable across time, while the effect of response efficacy is variable and may be more context-specific. Importantly, individuals who reported an increase in self-efficacy over time also reported (on average) a decrease in psychological distress.

Conclusions:

In addition to broadening our understanding of factors related to psychological distress, these results have potentially important intervention implications; for example, to the extent that self-efficacy is a malleable construct, one way of reducing disaster-related psychological distress may be to increase an individual’s self-efficacy.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Society for Disaster Medicine and Public Health, Inc

Psychological distress, also known as emotional suffering, includes general symptoms of stress, anxiety, and depression. It is a transdiagnostic component of several mental disorder diagnoses, including anxiety and depression, and is linked to both mental illness Reference Swartz and Jantz1 and poor health outcomes, even at low to moderate levels of distress. Reference Barry, Stout and Lynch2,Reference McLachlan and Gale3 This is particularly concerning given that approximately 23% of adults in the United States have experienced some level of psychological distress in recent years Reference Daly4 and have reported even higher rates during the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. Reference Twenge and Joiner5 Unfortunately, individuals with physical disabilities are even more likely to experience psychological distress, potentially because of stressors related to societal stereotypes, environmental accessibility challenges, and financial difficulties. Reference Noh, Kwon and Park6,Reference Turner and McLean7

Human-made disasters (eg, terrorism, technological) and natural disasters (eg, earthquakes, hurricanes, pandemics) can occur with little warning and result in severe consequences, including loss of life, physical injury, property damage, and psychological distress. These negative outcomes are multiplying as the frequency and severity of disasters have markedly increased this century. Reference De Smet, Lagadec and Leysen8,Reference Eshghi and Larson9 Awareness of disasters and anticipation about their occurrence can also lead to significant psychological distress. Reference Grupe and Nitschke10,Reference Bults, Beaujean, Richardus and Voeten11 Indeed, previous studies have found a positive association between perceived threat and distress across a variety of disaster and crisis events. Reference Li and Lyu12,Reference Suzuki, Yabe and Yasumura13

Individuals with disabilities have been disproportionately impacted by the negative effects of disasters Reference Castro, Sarmiento and Edwards14Reference Van Willigen, Edwards, Edwards and Hessee17 and often have greater difficulty recovering from disasters. Reference Roubhan18,Reference Sodhi19 Thus, it is likely that individuals with disabilities may be particularly vulnerable to psychological distress when they perceive current or future disaster threats. Given that over 1 billion individuals (or 15%) across the world live with some form of disability, 20 it is crucial to identify ways to impact the role of disaster threat on those individuals’ distress.

Fortunately, other disaster-related factors, such as perceived self-efficacy, may attenuate psychological distress. Self-efficacy, which refers to individuals’ belief in their ability to achieve a specific goal, can help individuals cope effectively in stressful situations. Reference Bandura21,Reference Rabani Bavojdan, Towhidi and Rahmati22 Indeed, self-efficacy has been shown to have a direct negative relationship with distress Reference Abramson, Stehling-Ariza, Garfield and Redlener23,Reference Duygulu, Kuruca-Ozdemir, Erdat and Kocoglu-Tanyer24 and has also been found to moderate the relationship between stressors and mental health outcomes, with better outcomes at higher levels of self-efficacy. Reference Ejeta, Ardalan and Paton25Reference Thormar, Sijbrandij and Gersons27 A disaster domain-specific type of self-efficacy—emergency preparedness self-efficacy—is defined as individuals’ perception of their ability to prepare for and deal with emergency situations Reference Burns28,Reference Wirtz, Rohrbeck and Burns29 and may similarly decrease disaster-related distress. While there has been relatively little research on the role of emergency preparedness and self-efficacy among people with physical disabilities, existing research suggests that individuals with disabilities tend to have lower self-efficacy, in general, due to poor health and mobility limitations. Reference Adams, Eisenman and Glik30,Reference Sullivan-Wiley and Gianotti31

Perceived response efficacy, defined as an individual’s perception that suggested actions will be effective in achieving desired outcomes, can also impact psychological distress. Indeed, recent research has linked higher levels of response efficacy to lower psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Reference Ambelu, Birhanu and Yitayih32 Similarly, Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Xiong, Zheng and Wu33 found that both response efficacy and self-efficacy mediated the relationship between perceived susceptibility to COVID-19 and anxiety, such that individuals who perceived a greater likelihood of infection believed that actions to avoid COVID-19 would be less effective, and in turn experienced greater distress. Limited research has investigated response efficacy among individuals with disabilities, but current findings suggest that this population tends to report lower levels of response efficacy with regard to disaster situations. Reference Adams, Eisenman and Glik30,34 Fortunately, efficacy beliefs are malleable, Reference Bandura21,Reference Bandura35 and emergency preparedness self-efficacy and response efficacy represent potential targets to decrease disaster-related psychological distress among individuals with physical disabilities.

Recent research has adapted Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) when modeling the additive effects of perceived threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy on emotional responses. In its original formulation, PMT provides a basic model for understanding how individuals’ appraisals of risk and efficacy result in protective actions. Reference Maddux and Rogers36 According to the PMT, an assessment of threat (severity and vulnerability) and efficacy (response and self-efficacy) operates on protection motivation additively. Kim et al.’s Reference Kim, Yang, Min and White37 work extended the PMT by proposing that such appraisals can elicit emotional responses such as hope or fear, which in turn can influence future protective behavior. In line with this suggestion, a recent study found that college students who perceived greater susceptibility to COVID-19 also reported lower perceived efficacy to cope with this threat, which in turn predicted higher levels of anxiety. Reference Zhang, Xiong, Zheng and Wu33

The current research seeks to extend past work on the PMT by examining the relationship between disaster-related cognitive appraisals and negative affective responses in greater detail. Research to date linking perceived threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy to psychological distress has been exclusively correlational, which can only suggest causal relationships. Findings may guide future interventions to mitigate disaster-related psychological distress among individuals with disabilities. Therefore, the present research used structural equation modeling to examine a 2-wave longitudinal model of these linkages among 106 individuals with physical disabilities (Figure 1). That model was used to test the following research questions and hypotheses:

RQ1

Do threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy each independently relate to psychological distress? We hypothesized that at both waves (ie, cross-sectionally), higher psychological distress would be independently associated with higher levels of perceived threat and with lower levels of perceived self-efficacy and response efficacy.

RQ2

Do the independent effects of threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy on psychological distress differ at different points in time? The stability of these relationships over time has been largely unexplored in existing literature; therefore, this question was examined without a specific hypothesis.

RQ3

Is change in perceived threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy related to change in psychological distress? We hypothesized that across the 2 waves (ie, longitudinally), increased psychological distress would be independently associated with increased perceived threat and reduced self-efficacy and response efficacy.

RQ4

Are there distal (ie, cross-wave) effects of threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy on psychological distress, independent of any proximal (ie, within-wave) effects? We hypothesized that higher Wave 2 psychological distress would be independently associated with higher perceived threat (at both waves), lower self-efficacy (at both waves), and lower response efficacy (at both waves).

Figure 1. The proposed model.

Methods

Sample

Data were collected from 106 individuals with a physical disability in 2015 (Wave 1) Reference Marceron and Rohrbeck38 and again in 2020 (Wave 2). Participants were recruited nationwide with the help of various organizations that serve individuals with disabilities. Individuals were eligible for participation if they: (1) were 18 years of age or older, (2) self-identified as someone with a physical disability as defined by the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), and (3) resided in the United States.

Measures

Perceived threat of disaster

Based on the PMT’s conceptualization of risk perception, Reference Maddux and Rogers36 participants were asked about the estimated likelihood of disaster (In your view, what is the likelihood of a natural or human-made disaster in your city or town in the next six months?) and the estimated severity of disaster (In your view, how negative would the consequences be for you if a natural or human-made disaster occurred in your city or town in the next six months?). Answers were reported on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (“None”) to 5 (“Extreme”). In line with past research, perceived likelihood and severity were then combined into a composite perceived threat of disaster variable by multiplying the variables together and performing a square root transformation. Reference Vacondio, Priolo, Dickert and Worry39

Emergency preparedness self-efficacy

The Emergency Preparedness Self-Efficacy (EPSE) scale was used to assess self-efficacy beliefs regarding disaster preparedness activities. Reference Burns28,Reference Wirtz, Rohrbeck and Burns29 This scale includes 7 items, such as “I can protect myself and my property in an emergency” and “I can maintain food and water supplies in an emergency.” Participants rate their confidence in their ability to successfully complete each of the items in emergency events other than the COVID-19 pandemic on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (“Not at all capable”) to 5 (“Totally capable”). Previous research has found strong internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.89) and test-retest reliability (r = 0.83, P < 0.001) for this measure. Reference Burns28,Reference Marceron, Rohrbeck and Burns40 Internal consistency was also strong in this study (Cronbach’s α = 0.81).

Response efficacy

This 3-item measure is rated on a 5-point Likert scale and asks participants about the extent to which they believe that emergency preparedness actions will effectively mitigate the negative effects of disaster events. For example, participants rated the extent to which they believed that “following emergency preparedness recommendations will be effective in reducing the impact of disasters.” Internal consistency for this measure was strong in the present study (Cronbach’s α = 0.80).

Psychological distress

The Kessler 6 scale (K6) Reference Kessler, Andrews and Colpe41 was administered to assess for non-specific psychological distress. This 6-item measure is rated on a 5-point Likert scale and asks participants about how frequently they have experienced various symptoms of psychological distress in the past 30 days. Internal consistency for this measure was strong in past research (Cronbach’s α = 0.89) Reference Kessler, Andrews and Colpe41 and in the current research (Cronbach’s α = 0.82).

Disability severity

The World Health Organization’s Disability Assessment Schedule (WHODAS 2.0) was administered to assess for functional disability in day-to-day life. Reference Üstün, Kostanjsek, Chatterji and Rehm42 This 36-item measure is rated on a 5-point Likert scale and asks participants to rate their difficulty across 6 life domains, including communication, mobility, self-care, interpersonal skills, household responsibilities, and participation in society. Example items include “In the last 30 days, how much difficulty did you have in taking care of your household responsibilities?” and “In the last 30 days, how much difficulty did you have in moving around inside your home?” The average general disability and domain scores can be used to more clearly interpret the severity of functional impacts based on the WHODAS 5-point scale, which ranges from 1 (“None”) to 5 (“Extreme”). Reference Üstün43 These scores can be calculated by summing the responses for either the entire measure or the desired domain and dividing by the total number of items in either the whole measure or the specified domain. Internal consistency for this measure was strong in past research (Cronbach’s α = 0.98) Reference Üstün, Kostanjsek, Chatterji and Rehm42 and in the current study (Cronbach’s α = 0.95).

Procedure

At Wave 1, participants were recruited nationally with the help of various organizations that serve individuals with disabilities, such as United Cerebral Palsy, the DC Office of Disability Rights, and the American Association on Health and Disability. Reference Marceron and Rohrbeck38 These organizations advertised the study through website posts, social media and networking sites (Twitter, Facebook, LinkedIn, Instagram, MeetUp.com), and emails. Individuals were eligible for participation if they: (1) were 18 years of age or older, (2) self-identified as someone with a physical disability as defined by the ADA, and (3) resided in the United States. Reference Marceron and Rohrbeck38 Eligible participants received a link to the Qualtrics survey or arranged a time for a telephone interview, depending on participant preference. Participants who completed the survey were compensated with a $10 gift card.

The follow-up data collection occurred 5 years after the original data collection, from June to September 2020. From the original Wave 1 sample of 294 individuals, 277 indicated that they were interested in participating in future research and consented to be contacted again in the future. Out of these 277 potential participants, 61 had invalid contact information, for a total contactable sample of 216 individuals. Upon IRB approval, each of these 216 individuals were re-contacted to ask whether they would like to participate in a follow-up study. Potential participants who did not respond were contacted again approximately 2 weeks after initial contact, and those who did not respond again were re-contacted for a final time approximately 2 weeks after the second contact. In response to these emails, 136 individuals indicated that they were interested in participating, and 4 individuals indicated that they did not want to participate. Interested participants were sent an email link to the Qualtrics survey, structured similarly to that in the original study. Reference Marceron and Rohrbeck38 Upon completion of the survey, participants were compensated for their time with a $10 gift card that was electronically sent or physically mailed, depending on each participant’s preference.

Statistical Analysis

All analytical procedures were completed in SAS 9.4. Data were examined for outliers, missing data, or errors and corrected, if possible, prior to testing the hypotheses. Four participants who reported not having a physical disability at Time 2 were excluded from the analysis. Additionally, participants with missing data on any of the key variables at either time point were excluded from the analysis, for a final analytical sample of 106 participants who completed the survey at both Wave 1 and Wave 2. Internal reliabilities and test-retest correlations were calculated for all multi-item scales. PROC CALIS in SAS was used to test the full model specified in Figure 1. Because inclusion of demographic control variables (age, education, gender, and race) did not significantly alter the results, the unadjusted parameter estimates are presented here for the sake of parsimony.

Results

Sociodemographic Characteristics of the Sample

Demographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1. At the time of interview completion, the sample was predominantly single (59%), lived in a suburban area (49%), identified as female (74%), owned their home (58%), lived with others (71%), was not a parent (58%), had earned at least a bachelor’s degree (68%), earned at least $35 000 (38%), had no children living with them (79%), and was white (89%). The average age was 46. The average general disability severity within the sample was mild to moderate (mean = 2.25, SD = 0.66). The average mobility-related impairment was moderate (mean = 2.92, SD = 0.96).

Table 1. Sample Characteristics

1Respondents could specify more than one ethnicity.

Univariate Characteristics and Bivariate Relationships Among Key Study Variables

Means, standard deviations, and bivariate relationships among the central variables employed in the study are presented in Table 2. Increased averages from Wave 1 to Wave 2 were observed in perceived threat (difference = 0.26, t = 3.70, P < 0.001), perceived self-efficacy (difference = 1.33, t = 2.80, P < 0.01), response efficacy (difference = 0.28, t = 1.01, P > 0.05), and psychological distress (difference = 0.80, t = 2.02, P < 0.05). Positive Wave 1 to Wave 2 correlations were found for perceived threat (r = 0.41, P < 0.001), perceived self-efficacy (r = 0.57, P < 0.001), response efficacy (r = 0.40, P < 0.001), and psychological distress (r = 0.58, P < 0.001).

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Note: * p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001.

At Wave 1, psychological distress was positively correlated with perceived threat (r = 0.28, P < 0.01) and negatively correlated with perceived response efficacy (r = −0.20, P < 0.05) and self-efficacy (r = −0.40, P < 0.001). At Wave 2, perceived threat (r = 0.27, P < 0.01) and self-efficacy (r = −0.45, P < 0.001) showed significant correlations with psychological distress. In contrast, the correlation between psychological distress and response efficacy (r = −0.16), which was statistically significant at Wave 1, dropped to non-significance (P > 0.05) at Wave 2, suggesting the possibility of non-stability in that relationship across time.

RQ1 and RQ2: Do Threat, Self-Efficacy, and Response Efficacy Each Independently Relate to Psychological Distress, and Are These Relationships Stable Across Time?

Standardized parameter estimates of psychological distress regressed on perceived threat, response efficacy, and self-efficacy are presented in Table 3, separately by wave. The estimate for perceived threat at the first (b = 0.27, P < 0.001) and second (b = 0.25, P < 0.001) waves were very similar. This similarity extended to the estimates for self-efficacy at Wave 1 (b = −0.28, P < 0.001) and at Wave 2 (b = −0.32, P < 0.001). However, the significant estimate for response efficacy at Wave 1 (b = −0.19, P < 0.05) dissipated to non-significance at Wave 2 (b = −0.04, P > 0.05), suggesting the possible instability of this effect across time.

Table 3. Parameter Estimates: Cotemporaneous Model

RQ3: Is Change in Perceived Threat, Self-Efficacy, and Response Efficacy Related to Change in Psychological Distress?

In order to address Research Question 3, the difference in psychological distress (Wave 2–Wave 1) was regressed on the difference in perceived threat, the difference in response efficacy, and the difference in self-efficacy. The results revealed that, on average, the difference in psychological distress was positively associated with the difference in perceived threat (b = 1.95, P < 0.001) and negatively associated with the difference in self-efficacy (b = −0.21, P < 0.01) but not significantly related to the difference in response efficacy (b = −0.10, P > 0.05).

RQ4: Are There Distal (that is, cross-wave) Effects of Threat, Self-efficacy, and Response Efficacy on Psychological Distress, Independent of Any Proximal (that is, within-wave) Effects?

In order to address Research Question 4, the full structural equation model depicted in Figure 1 was examined and tested. The results are presented in Table 4 and summarized in Figure 2. Of the 4 primary measures (psychological distress, perceived threat, response efficacy, and self-efficacy), only psychological distress (b = 0.57, P < 0.0001) and perceived threat (b = −0.20, P = 0.017) from Wave 1 were found to independently relate to Wave 2 psychological distress. Proximal Wave 2 measures of perceived threat (b = 0.26, P < 0.002) and self-efficacy (b = −0.31, P < 0.001) were also found to be significantly related to Wave 2 psychological distress.

Table 4. Parameter Estimates: Full Model

Figure 2. Final model.

Discussion

Although cross-sectional research has shown that perceived levels of self-efficacy and response efficacy protect individuals against the psychological distress accompanying the perceived threat of stressors, Reference Zhang, Xiong, Zheng and Wu33,Reference Bandura44Reference Myall, Hine and Marks47 the stability of these relationships across time has remained largely unexplored. In a 2-wave longitudinal study spanning 5 years, we investigated the proximal and distal effects of perceived threat, self-efficacy, and response efficacy on reported symptoms of psychological distress among individuals with physical disabilities—a population for whom psychological distress is known to be significantly higher than the general population. Reference Noh, Kwon and Park6

Our results suggest that the association of proximal perceived threat and emergency preparedness self-efficacy with psychological distress remains stable across time, while the effect of response efficacy may be more context-specific, and therefore more variable across time. Because Wave 2 data were collected at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, it is possible that response efficacy would be impacted by political beliefs or trust in government given the politicization of this event. Indeed, recent research has found that political conservatism was associated with lower levels of response efficacy during the COVID-19 pandemic. Reference Winter, Riordan, Scarf and Jose48 However, this effect was stronger among US citizens relative to those from other countries, Reference Stroebe, vanDellen and Abakoumkin49 which suggests that the specific effects of response efficacy depend on contextual factors, like the strong politicization of COVID-19 in the United States. To the extent that response efficacy is context-specific, its effect on psychological distress may vary across time.

In contrast to response efficacy, self-efficacy appears to have a more stable relationship with psychological distress across time. Regardless of wave, the level of perceived emergency preparedness self-efficacy consistently negatively predicted psychological distress. Of particular note, individuals who reported an increase in self-efficacy across the 2 waves of the study also reported (on average) a decrease in psychological distress. In addition to broadening our understanding of factors related to psychological distress, this result has potentially important intervention implications: Given that self-efficacy is a malleable construct, 1 way of reducing psychological distress may be to increase an individual’s self-efficacy level. Past research suggests that there are 4 major methods of improving self-efficacy beliefs, including mastery performance, vicarious experience (modeling), verbal persuasion, and physiological arousal. Reference Bandura44 Therefore, interventions that aim to improve emergency preparedness self-efficacy and, in turn, decrease disaster-related psychological distress, could do so by creating opportunities to practice preparedness behavior, observe peers engage in preparedness behavior, and receive verbal encouragement. In addition to promoting better mental health by promoting self-efficacy, such interventions may also promote general resilience to disasters, as prior research has found a positive link between self-efficacy and preparedness behavior. Reference Ejeta, Ardalan and Paton25,Reference Marceron and Rohrbeck38

The results of this study also underscore the stable association of perceived threat with the level of psychological distress across time. Regardless of wave, a higher perceived threat was associated with greater psychological distress, and an increased perceived threat across waves was associated, on average, with increased psychological distress. This highlights the importance of coping mechanisms (such as self-efficacy) to overcome the barrier imposed by perceived threat.

These results also suggest that any distal effects of Wave 1 emergency preparedness self-efficacy are mediated by Wave 1 psychological distress. Once prior distress level and the proximal effects of threat and emergency preparedness self-efficacy have been accounted for, there is no indication that distal effects of emergency preparedness self-efficacy have any direct bearing on psychological distress. In particular, there is no suggestion from these results of any lagged effect of emergency preparedness self-efficacy.

Interestingly, the results show distal effects of Wave 1 perceived threat on Wave 2 psychological distress even after accounting for prior distress level and proximal effects of perceived threat and emergency preparedness self-efficacy. Specifically, there is a significant negative relationship between Wave 1 perceived threat and Wave 2 psychological distress. This is unexpected, given that past studies have found a positive association between perceived threat and psychological distress. Reference Grupe and Nitschke10,Reference Li and Lyu12,Reference Suzuki, Yabe and Yasumura13 Although stressors, such as perceived threat, are often associated with increased psychological distress, extant research has also found evidence for stress-related growth, in which individuals who are able to cope successfully with stressful events report positive adjustments in their lives. Reference Park and Fenster50Reference Weinrib, Rothrock, Johnsen and Lutgendorf52 Participants in the current study may have experienced such stress-related growth and found positive strategies to cope with their perceived threat over the 5 years between waves, resulting in a negative distal effect of Wave 1 perceived threat on Wave 2 psychological distress. These findings highlight the importance of finding ways to promote stress-related growth in order to support positive psychological outcomes in the long term.

Limitations

These results should be interpreted with caution. They are based on 106 individuals with physical disabilities and may not generalize to other populations (including populations with different sociodemographic characteristics). All measures were based on self-reports. The 5-year time span between waves did not permit a more fine-tuned longitudinal analysis; additional waves of data would have permitted more insight into factors affecting individual trajectories of psychological distress across time. Although the analysis of differences across time is suggestive of a causal effect of threat and self-efficacy on psychological distress, experimental studies are needed to confirm the plausibility of the implied causal model. It should also be noted that Wave 2 data collection occurred shortly after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. The extent to which COVID-19 affected the relationships reported here—if at all—is unclear.

Conclusions

These concerns, notwithstanding the results of this study, suggest perceived threat and emergency preparedness self-efficacy may impact the psychological distress of individuals with physical disabilities in a consistent manner across time. In line with Kim et al.’s Reference Kim, Yang, Min and White37 work, these findings suggest that the PMT should be extended to consider how disaster-related cognitive appraisals may impact negative affective responses, like psychological distress. This study is the first known attempt to examine this extension of the PMT longitudinally and within a sample of individuals with disabilities. Overall, these results suggest the potential importance of perceived threat and self-efficacy in the etiology of disaster-related psychological distress among individuals with a physical disability and indicate possible avenues for intervention to prevent or mitigate this distress.

Author contribution

Ashley E. Reed—writing and editing of this paper, completed follow-up data collection; Cynthia A. Rohrbeck—assisted with writing discussion and editing entire paper; Philip W. Wirtz—completed data analysis, assisted with writing methods and results; Jennifer E. Marceron—completed the first wave data collection.

Funding statement

The first wave data collection was supported by the Society for Community Research and Action, The American Psychological Association of Graduate Students, Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues, and a George Washington University Summer Dissertation Fellowship, all awarded to the fourth author. Follow-up data collection was supported by an APAGS/Psi Chi Junior Scientist Fellowship awarded to the first author.

Competing interests

Authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

Swartz, JA, Jantz, I. Association between nonspecific severe psychological distress as an indicator of serious mental illness and increasing levels of medical multimorbidity. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(12):2350-2358. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, V, Stout, ME, Lynch, ME, et al. The effect of psychological distress on health outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. J Health Psychol. 2020;25(2):227-239. doi: 10.1177/1359105319842931 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLachlan, KJ, Gale, CR. The effects of psychological distress and its interaction with socioeconomic position on risk of developing four chronic diseases. J Psychosom Res. 2018;109:79-85. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, M. Prevalence of psychological distress among working-age adults in the United States, 1999–2018. Am J Public Health. 2022;112(7):1045-1049. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2022.306828 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, JM, Joiner, TE. Mental distress among US adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Clin Psychol. 2020;76(12):2170-2182. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noh, JW, Kwon, YD, Park, J, et al. Relationship between physical disability and depression by gender: a panel regression model. PLoS One. 2016;11(11):e0166238. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166238 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, RJ, McLean, PD. Physical disability and psychological distress. Rehabil Psychol. 1989;34(4):225-242. doi: 10.1037/h0091727 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Smet, H, Lagadec, P, Leysen, J. Disasters out of the box: a new ballgame? J Contingencies Crisis Manag. 2012;20(3):138-148. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5973.2012.00666.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eshghi, K, Larson, RC. Disasters: lessons from the past 105 years. Disaster Prev Manag Int J. 2008;17(1):62-82. doi: 10.1108/09653560810855883 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grupe, DW, Nitschke, JB. Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(7):488-501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bults, M, Beaujean, DJ, Richardus, JH, Voeten, HA. Perceptions and behavioral responses of the general public during the 2009 influenza A (H1N1) pandemic: a systematic review. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2015;9(2):207-219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X, Lyu, H. Epidemic risk perception, perceived stress, and mental health during COVID-19 pandemic: a moderated mediating model. Front Psychol. 2021;11:1-9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.563741 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y, Yabe, H, Yasumura, S, et al. Psychological distress and the perception of radiation risks: the Fukushima health management survey. Bull World Health Organ. 2015;93:598-605. doi: 10.2471/BLT.14.146498 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, CP, Sarmiento, JP, Edwards, R, et al. Disaster risk perception in urban contexts and for people with disabilities: case study on the city of Iquique (Chile). Nat Hazards. 2017;86(1):411-436. doi: 10.1007/s11069-016-2698-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chou, YJ. Who is at risk of death in an earthquake? Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160(7):688-695. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh270 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quaill, J, Barker, R, West, C. Experiences of individuals with physical disabilities in natural disasters: an integrative review. Aust J Emerg Manag. 2018;33(3):58-63. doi: 10.3316/ielapa.793049280261310 Google Scholar
Van Willigen, M, Edwards, T, Edwards, B, Hessee, S. Riding out the storm: experiences of the physically disabled during Hurricanes Bonnie, Dennis, and Floyd. Nat Hazards Rev. 2002;3(3):98-106. doi: 10.1061/(ASCE)1527-6988(2002)3:3(98)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roubhan, B. Natural hazards: enhancing disaster preparedness and resilience of people with disabilities. In: Crises, Conflict, and Disability: Ensuring Equality. Routledge; 2014:75-83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sodhi, MS. Natural disasters, the economy and population vulnerability as a vicious cycle with exogenous hazards. J Oper Manag. 2016;45(1):101-113. doi: 10.1016/j.jom.2016.05.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Disability Fact Sheet. World Health Organization. Published 2022. Accessed December 12, 2022. https://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/disability-and-health Google Scholar
Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84(2):191. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabani Bavojdan, M, Towhidi, A, Rahmati, A. The relationship between mental health and general self-efficacy beliefs, coping strategies and locus of control in male drug abusers. Addict Health. 2011;3(3-4):111-118.Google ScholarPubMed
Abramson, D, Stehling-Ariza, T, Garfield, R, Redlener, I. Prevalence and predictors of mental health distress post-Katrina: findings from the Gulf Coast Child and Family Health Study. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2008;2(2):77-86. doi: 10.1097/DMP.0b013e318173a8e7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duygulu, S, Kuruca-Ozdemir, E, Erdat, Y, Kocoglu-Tanyer, D. University students’ risk perception, protective measures, and general health during the COVID-19 pandemic in Turkey. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. Published online August 22, 2022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ejeta, LT, Ardalan, A, Paton, D. Application of behavioral theories to disaster and emergency health preparedness: a systematic review. PLoS Curr. 2015;7. doi: 10.1371/currents.dis.31a8995ced321301466db400f1357829 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, MT, Rohrbeck, CA, Wirtz, PW. The importance of self-efficacy in parental emergency preparedness: a moderated mediation model. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2018;12(3):345-351. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2017.80 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thormar, SB, Sijbrandij, M, Gersons, BPR, et al. PTSD symptom trajectories in disaster volunteers: the role of self-efficacy, social acknowledgement, and tasks carried out: PTSD symptom trajectories in volunteers. J Trauma Stress. 2016;29(1):17-25. doi: 10.1002/jts.22073 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, KM. Emergency Preparedness Self-Efficacy and the Ongoing Threat of Disasters. [PhD thesis]. Washington, DC, USA: The George Washington University; 2014. http://proxygw.wrlc.org/login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1614471823?accountid=11243 Google Scholar
Wirtz, PW, Rohrbeck, CA, Burns, KM. Anxiety effects on disaster precautionary behaviors: a multi-path cognitive model. J Health Psychol. 2019;24(10):1401-1411. doi: 10.1177/1359105317720277 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, RM, Eisenman, DP, Glik, D. Community advantage and individual self-efficacy promote disaster preparedness: a multilevel model among persons with disabilities. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(15):2779. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16152779 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan-Wiley, KA, Gianotti, AGS. Risk perception in a multi-hazard environment. World Dev. 2017;97:138-152. doi: 10.1016/j.worlddev.2017.04.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambelu, A, Birhanu, Z, Yitayih, Y, et al. Psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic in Ethiopia: an online cross-sectional study to identify the need for equal attention of intervention. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2021;20(1):22. doi: 10.1186/s12991-021-00344-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, W, Xiong, S, Zheng, Y, Wu, J. Response efficacy and self-efficacy mediated the relationship between perceived threat and psychic anxiety among college students in the early stage of the CoViD-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(5):2832. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19052832 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preparedness in America: Research Insights to Increase Individual, Organizational, and Community Action. Federal Emergency Management Agency. Published 2014. Accessed December 12, 2022. https://www.ready.gov/sites/default/files/2020-08/Preparedness_in_America_August_2014.pdf Google Scholar
Bandura, A. Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52(1):1-26. doi: 10.1111/1467-839X.00024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddux, JE, Rogers, RW. Protection motivation and self-efficacy: a revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1983;19(5):469-479. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(83)90023-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J, Yang, K, Min, J, White, B. Hope, fear, and consumer behavioral change amid COVID-19: application of protection motivation theory. Int J Consum Stud. 2022;46(2):558-574. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12700 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marceron, JE, Rohrbeck, CA. Disability and disasters: the role of self-efficacy in emergency preparedness. Psychol Health Med. 2019;24(1):83-93. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2018.1492730 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vacondio, M, Priolo, G, Dickert, S, Worry, Bonini N., perceived threat and media communication as predictors of self-protective behaviors during the COVID-19 outbreak in Europe. Front Psychol. 2021;12:577992. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.577992 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marceron, JE, Rohrbeck, CA, Burns, KM. A self-efficacy measure for both human-made and natural disasters. In: Poster Session for the Annual Federal Alliance for Safe Homes Conference. Lake Buena Vista, FL, USA; 2013.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Andrews, G, Colpe, LJ, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32(6):959-976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Üstün, TB, Kostanjsek, N, Chatterji, S, Rehm, J. Measuring health and disability: manual for WHO disability assessment schedule WHODAS 2.0. World Health Organization; 2010.Google Scholar
Üstün, TB. Measuring health and disability: manual for WHO disability assessment schedule WHODAS 2.0. World Health Organization; 2010.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. W. H. Freeman; 1997.Google Scholar
Ding, Y, Xu, J, Huang, S, et al. Risk perception and depression in public health crises: evidence from the COVID-19 crisis in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(16):5728. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mireault, GC, Bond, LA. Parental death in childhood: perceived vulnerability, and adult depression and anxiety. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1992;62(4):517-524. doi: 10.1037/h0079371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myall, BR, Hine, DW, Marks, AD, et al. Assessing individual differences in perceived vulnerability in older adults. Pers Individ Differ. 2009;46(1):8-13. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, T, Riordan, BC, Scarf, D, Jose, PE. Conspiracy beliefs and distrust of science predicts reluctance of vaccine uptake of politically right-wing citizens. Vaccine. 2022;40(12):1896-1903. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2022.01.039 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroebe, W, vanDellen, MR, Abakoumkin, G, et al. Politicization of COVID-19 health-protective behaviors in the United States: longitudinal and cross-national evidence. PLoS One. 2021;16(10):e0256740. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256740 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, C, Fenster, J. Stress-related growth: predictors of occurrence and correlates with psychological adjustment. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2004;23:195-215. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.2.195.31019 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tedeschi, RG, Calhoun, LG. Posttraumatic growth: conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychol Inq. 2004;15(1):1-18. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinrib, AZ, Rothrock, NE, Johnsen, EL, Lutgendorf, SK. The assessment and validity of stress-related growth in a community-based sample. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74(5):851. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.851 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The proposed model.

Figure 1

Table 1. Sample Characteristics

Figure 2

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Figure 3

Table 3. Parameter Estimates: Cotemporaneous Model

Figure 4

Table 4. Parameter Estimates: Full Model

Figure 5

Figure 2. Final model.