Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T04:09:16.812Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

To what extent do children’s expressions of time actually refer to time? An investigation into the temporal and discursive usages of temporal adverbs in family interaction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 April 2024

Maija Surakka*
Affiliation:
Tampere University, Kalevantie 4, 33014 Tampere
Minna Kirjavainen
Affiliation:
University of the West of England, Coldharbour Lane Bristol, BS16 1QY
*
Corresponding author: Maija Surakka; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Many studies have explored children’s acquisition of temporal adverbs. However, the extent to which children’s early temporal language has discursive instead of solely temporal meanings has been largely ignored. We report two corpus-based studies that investigated temporal adverbs in Finnish child-parent interaction between the children’s ages of 1;7 and 4;11. Study 1 shows that the two corpus children used temporal adverbs to construe both temporal and discursive meanings from their early adverb production and that the children’s usage syntactically broadly reflected the input received. Study 2 shows that the discursive uses of adverbs appeared to be learned from contextually anchored caregiver constructions that convey discourse functions like urging and reassuring, and that the usage is related to the children’s and caregivers’ interactional roles. Our study adds to the literature on the acquisition of temporal adverbs by demonstrating that these items are learned also with additional discursive meanings in family interaction.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Children’s language use can inform us as to how they experience the world and what kinds of strategies they use to convey meaning while their linguistic abilities are developing. In relation to these, researchers have been interested in how children develop in expressing abstract and difficult concepts such as time (Ames, Reference Ames1946; Chejnová, Reference Chejnová2017; Liang et al., Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019; Parm, Reference Parm2013; Surakka, Reference Surakka2019; Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989; Weist & Buczowska, Reference Weist and Buczowska1987). According to these studies, children start using temporal adverbs (e.g., soon, yesterday) during their third year of life indicating that children can refer to abstract concepts (time) relatively early on in development. This is interesting as lexical items referring to easily identifiable and imaginable concepts are easier to learn than those referring to items that are more difficult to identify or imagine (e.g., Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2020; Kranjek & McDonough, Reference Kranjek and McDonough2011), such as time. Thus, young children’s use of adverbs is informative not only in relation to their ability to refer to time, but it also provides a window to children’s conceptual, cognitive, and interactional development. However, even though the order of appearance of temporal adverbs, the temporal meanings of adverbs and the development of temporal concepts in children have received a fair amount of attention from researchers (Ames, Reference Ames1946; Chejnová, Reference Chejnová2017; Liang et al., Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019; Parm, Reference Parm2013; Surakka, Reference Surakka2019; Weist & Buczowska, Reference Weist and Buczowska1987), we still do not fully understand how/why children use temporal adverbs at an early age, given their highly abstract nature. The current study aims to widen the scope of our understanding of adverb acquisition and addresses adverb use in child-parent interaction that emerges through the association of adverbs to specific discourse contexts like urging, threatening, and reassuring.

From the linguistic viewpoint, adverbial expressions form somewhat independent syntactic-semantic units (see Langacker, Reference Langacker, Visapää, Kalliokoski and Sorva2014) that, in the case of temporal adverbs, position the topic or events expressed in an utterance into a temporal frame (telling when something happened or will happen). Examples (1) and (2) represent the kind of uses of temporal adverbs where temporal frame is semantically foregrounded. In (1), the mother is talking to a child (Piia) aged 3 years and 10 months (3;10).

In (1), Piia is playing a board game with her parents. The mother is counting steps forward the child must take on her turn (lines 1 to 3). The mother perceives (line 4) that the child is approaching the home base, i.e., the end of the game, and elaborates that the child will reach it ‘in a moment’ (line 5). The temporal adverb kohta (‘soon’, ‘imminently’, ‘in a minute’, ‘in a moment’) formulates a time frame adverbial for the mother’s declarative clause.

In example (2), a child (Mari), aged 2;1, is having a conversation with her mother.

Mari has scratched her elbow. The mother asks if the scratch has healed already (line 3), to which the child replies ‘yes’ (line 4). Also in this example, temporal adverb, here jo (‘already’ on line 3) temporally frames the referred action (elbow healing). In the current study, we refer to the temporal adverb usage in examples (1) and (2) as temporal.

On the other hand, when a specific discourse function is construed or reinforced by a temporal adverb, the adverb is more difficult, or even impossible, to separate from the other parts of the sentence structure. In these instances, the adverb typically has both temporal and discursive meanings. See example (3), in which Piia (3;6) uses both jo and kohta discursively (but with temporal undertones) as she demands that her mother brings her some paper to cut.

First, even though jo has some temporal meaning, on line 1 Piia is using it to urge her mother to bring some paper; the use of jo reinforces the urgency that is expressed by an interrogative clause. Piia then proceeds from urging to threatening to cut her mother’s trousers if she does not bring the paper (line 5). Similarly to jo with an urging function, kohta with a threatening function (line 5) reinforces the discursive content that is expressed by the utterance, here by a declarative clause. In the current study, we will refer to this kind of adverb use as discursive use.

The temporal meaning can be considered prototypical for temporal adverbs and thus it has been the focus in previous studies of children’s acquisition of these items (e.g., Ames, Reference Ames1946; Chejnová, Reference Chejnová2017; Liang et al., Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019; Parm, Reference Parm2013; Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989; Weist & Buczowska, Reference Weist and Buczowska1987). Our focus on both temporal and discursive uses in child and caregiver language is thus novel and provides a more detailed account of the development of temporal adverbs. In our study, we analyse naturalistic child-parent interaction. The naturalistic contexts allow us to observe various semantic and pragmatic adverb uses relating to children and parent’s interactional roles in relation to functions like urging, threatening or reprimanding, that are more difficult to observe – for example, in experimental contexts or when spontaneous data are collected in a lab or research assistants present (see e.g., Tamis-LeMonda et al., Reference Tamis‐LeMonda, Kuchirko, Luo, Escobar and Bornstein2017).

By adopting the broad framework of Usage-based-constructivism (e.g., Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003), we will present two studies that explore the acquisition of two temporal adverbs jo ‘already’ and kohta ‘in a moment’ in two Finnish child language corpora (Kirjavainen-MPI Finnish corpus child, Kirjavainen et al., Reference Kirjavainen, Kidd and Lieven2017a; Laalo, Reference Laalo2021) between the ages of 1;7 and 4;11 years. Study 1 investigates how jo (JO) and kohta (KOHTA) are syntactically positioned within utterances, the meanings expressed in those positions and whether these positions and meanings mirror the usage in the input the child has received. Study 2 addresses the usage of temporal adverbs as discursive tools of interaction from the perspective of the child’s development and the caregivers’ role in it. Together these two studies shed light on the extent to which children’s early temporal language actually has temporal meaning, or if temporal adverbs are learned and used additionally as discourse items (e.g., to urge or reprimand). They also inform us whether the children’s adverb functions correspond to the functions used by adults.

The development of temporal adverbs in child language

The development of temporal adverbs has been described for a number of languages (e.g., Ames, Reference Ames1946; Chejnová, Reference Chejnová2017; Liang et al., Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019; Parm, Reference Parm2013; Surakka, Reference Surakka2019; Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989; Weist & Buczowska, Reference Weist and Buczowska1987). This body of research suggests that temporal adverbs emerge in children’s language between the ages of 2–3 years, depending on the language.

The development of a conceptual temporal system underlies the linguistic capability of producing temporal adverbs. The temporal system first supports adverbs and particles that refer to the speech time (Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989), i.e., to the event where the child can experience themselves in a concrete manner. Consequently, the adverbs that structure the speech time or the immediate past or future like ‘now’, ‘already’, ‘a moment ago’ and ‘in a moment’ are used more accurately from early in development than the expressions referring to the further past or future (e.g., ‘last week’, ‘in the future’) (e.g., Surakka, Reference Surakka2019; Weist & Buczowska, Reference Weist and Buczowska1987). Even when semantic errors are made and the concepts of time that children express in their utterances are not fully analysed or (completely) accurate (e.g., Child aged 3;4: Ne tuli meille huomenna ‘They came for a visit tomorrow’; Surakka, Reference Surakka2019, p. 34 where the adverb tomorrow is used with a past tense verb), children seem to have an established grammatical understanding that syntactic structures have slots reserved for temporal adverbs (Surakka, Reference Surakka2019, p. 7). This has led to the conclusion that children’s syntactic ability develops early and is relatively well-matured at the age of 2;6 years whereas the abstract and complex conceptual domain of time continues to develop at least until the age of 8 years (Surakka, Reference Surakka2019, p. 78) – after which there is no research-based evidence available.

Given that research shows that adverbs are often polysemous (Hakulinen & Saari, Reference Hakulinen and Saari1995) and that the meaning of the abstract concepts can change depending on the context (Lebois et al., Reference Lebois, Wilson-Mendenhall and Barsalou2015) we assume that our target adverbs JO and KOHTA can also have multiple meanings (see examples (1) – (3) above). The only previous studies that we are aware of that consider discursive meanings in children’s adverb use are Liang et al. (Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019) and Surakka (Reference Surakka2019). Liang et al. (Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019) studied the acquisition of temporal adverbs in Mandarin Chinese, and suggested that by varying the positions of temporal adverbs in utterances, it is possible either to highlight certain meaning components or to constitute a discourse function concerning an ongoing action (e.g., urging). However, Liang et al. reported that their data only contained temporal uses, albeit with some variation in semantic emphasis. Surakka (Reference Surakka2019) studied temporal adverbs in Finnish children’s language and gave some examples of discursive usages of temporal adverbs, especially in relation to kohta (‘in a moment’). Even though Surakka did not systematically analyse temporal and discursive uses, her study indicated that from four years of age, Finnish children can use temporal adverbs to express, for example, politeness and patience (4;1 Voikko sä kohta kiivettää mua siinä isossa kivessä ‘Can you in a moment help me to climb on the big rock’ when the child wants to climb and needs help but also acknowledges that the potential helper is in the middle of garden work), but also as a means of construing threats (5;0 Pistän ton telkkarin koht kiinni ‘I will turn the TV off in a moment’ when the child was playing with memory cards with their caregiver who from time to time stopped and glanced at the television). The present study aims to provide a systematic analysis on Finnish-speaking children’s use of temporal adverbs in discursive (and temporal) manner early in development.

Discursive linguistic units

Reference to discursive linguistic units has been made in child language literature before. In their study on children’s acquisition of verbal aspect, Ibbotson et al. (Reference Ibbotson, Lieven and Tomasello2014) divided verbs into two categories: the ones that referred to actions taking place “here-and-now” (contextually grounded) and those that referred to actions outside of the “here-and-now” (displaced). Their study showed that approx. 30% of the verbs that children aged 2;0 to 3;6 produced were contextually grounded. Additionally, speech directed to young children seemed to carry more grounded references than speech directed to older children. Because we analyse the development of adverbs, which are verb modifiers (and not verbs), it is not as straightforward to identify the meaning of a given target item as being ongoing or displaced as it is with verbs. We will use the term temporal when the temporal adverb refers to time only and discursive when the temporal adverb containing utterance is contextually grounded and additionally has an identifiable discourse function (i.e., directing or impacting the interlocutor’s behaviour).

Diessel and Tomasello (Reference Diessel and Tomasello2000) state that when examined from the usage-based, contextual basis, propositional meanings of the target units may not be fully expressed in children’s utterances but they may function as operators guiding the hearer’s interpretations of the associated discourse and joint actions – as markers of the illocutionary force of an utterance. They explored the development of finite complement clauses in English child language and concluded that complex sentences as constructions convey specific features in child language – instead of two clausal propositions, they convey only one, typically expressed by a complement clause while the main clause functions as an epistemic marker, attention getter or marker of illocutionary force. Linguistic expressions forming certain discourse functions in children’s speech have previously been referred to also as speech act sentences (e.g., De Ruiter et al., Reference De Ruiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021, p. 1153; Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello, Brooks and Barret1999). For the purposes of our work, the term ‘discourse function’ is illustrative as it explicitly emphasizes the centeredness of the ongoing discourse in our analysis. Discourse function also covers the idea of interactional intent(ion) being a motive for formulating utterances with such functions (Athanasiadou, Reference Athanasiadou1991; Ervin-Tripp & Mitchell-Kernan, Reference Ervin-Tripp, Mitchell-Kernan, Ervin-Tripp and Mitchell-Kernan1977, p. 19). When a temporal adverb has a marked discourse function (e.g., warning, threatening), we refer to this as a discursive instance of the adverb. However, importantly, even when JO and KOHTA are denoting discursive aspects, the temporal-propositional (henceforth temporal) meaning is typically not completely absent from these uses.

The term discourse context (e.g., Orsolini, Reference Orsolini1993) in the present study refers to the event of interaction, more specifically to the sequences of conversation where the target adverbs take place. As we conducted an analysis on the transcripts of audio recordings, we analysed the conversational participants, their utterances and actions in 10 lines of discourse context surrounding the target utterance. Our method thus adopts features of Interactional linguistics (Hall, Reference Hall2019) and Conversation Analysis (Hakulinen, Reference Hakulinen, Jucker and Ziv1998; Hakulinen & Saari, Reference Hakulinen and Saari1995). As usage-based constructivism, construction grammar and interactional linguistics share the usage-based view of language, the described methodological combination is theoretically justified (Hall, Reference Hall2019).

The Usage-based-constructivist viewpoint and Construction Grammar

The usage-based-constructivist stance assumes that language acquisition is closely related to usage events and to the language input children receive (e.g., Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003), children’s general cognitive development (Chejnová, Reference Chejnová2017, p. 32) and the development of social cognition (e.g., Liszkowski, Reference Liszkowski, Enfiled and Levinson2006; Olineck & Poulin-Dubois, Reference Olineck and Poulin-Dubois2005). Because language is input driven and children initially base their assumptions of language on the exemplars of constructions they have heard (and their type and token frequencies), children’s language heavily mirrors that of the caregivers’ language (e.g., Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003). However, other factors are also at play in children’s language acquisition. For example, Kauppinen (Reference Kauppinen2020, p. 14, 121, 123) emphasizes the role of the linguistic constructions as “coping strategies” in interaction whereby early in development children will produce utterances as coherent wholes (constructions) with specific discourse functions rather than process the meanings of the utterances’ each individual word separately (see also Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello, Brooks and Barret1999). Also, Ellis (Reference Ellis, Hundt, Pfenninger and Mollin2017) suggests that emotional, cognitive, and motivational factors affect the salience of stimuli: personally and socially significant events and language are easily remembered and learned. Furthermore, in terms of efficiency, children seem to prefer short rather than long and complex constructions (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2019, p. 8).

The concept of construction originates from the field of Construction Grammar (see Hoffmann & Trousdale, Reference Hoffmann, Trousdale, Hoffmann and Trousdale2013) that together with the usage-based approach fall under the broader cognitive linguistics framework. Because they have similar assumptions theoretically, the usage-based approach uses the term construction when referring to conventionalized linguistic items with a specific form and a meaning (Langacker, Reference Langacker2008, p. 167–168). Usage-based child language research has investigated the development of constructions on different levels of language including the analysis of single word and multi-word units (Diessel, Reference Diessel, Hoffmann and Trousdale2013; Diessel & Tomasello, Reference Diessel and Tomasello2000; Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2005; Kirjavainen et al., Reference Kirjavainen, Lieven and Theakston2017b; Lieven & Tomasello, Reference Lieven, Tomasello, Robinson and Ellis2008).

We will conduct a corpus analysis between the children’s age on 1;7–4;11. The current study will examine when the target adverbs start to appear in the children’s language and what their functions are, to investigate if early adverb use consists of temporal and/or discursive uses, and whether this is impacted by the parents’ use of these adverbs. Thus, our investigation follows the usage-based-constructivist theoretical viewpoint.

Finnish

Finnish language has a rich case system and thus does not have many syntactic constraints for word order. Having said that, discourse function and word order patterns are linked in that different word order patterns can give rise to slightly different meanings – even though SVO is considered the unmarked alternative (Vilkuna, Reference Vilkuna1989, Reference Vilkuna and Siewierska1998). Consequently, the adverbial position can affect the interpretation of the whole sentence in Finnish (Huumo, Reference Huumo1997) as in many other languages (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Engelberg, Rauh, Rauh, Engelberg and Austin2004). Furthermore, some adverbials allow a wide range of possible syntactic positions, while others display more restricted behavior (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Engelberg, Rauh, Rauh, Engelberg and Austin2004).

Despite the rich inflection systems in Finnish, there are no morphological future tense markers. Instead, adverbs play an important role in conveying information about time/tense; KOHTA (‘in a moment’) being one example of such an adverbs.

The present study

In the present study, we will first take a syntactic, word-order-based approach to children and their caregivers’ language production and explore if the children’s adverb use syntactically (i.e., in relation to locations within utterances) mirrors that of their parents.

Second, we will investigate the functions (temporal vs. discursive) of the target adverbs in the children and their caregivers’ language – an analysis which, as far as we are aware, has not been reported in the previous literature. For this, we established a discourse context consisting of 10 speaker turns (5 before, 5 after) for each target adverb instance which we used to analyse the function of the target adverb (see also Hakulinen & Saari, Reference Hakulinen and Saari1995). We coded the target adverb uses based on the actions taking place and topics talked about in the discourse contexts (see examples 1–3) as either being temporal or discursive (or ambiguous when the discourse context did not give enough information about the function of the adverb).

For the above, we conducted two corpus analyses (Study 1 and Study 2) that explore the usage of two temporal adverbs, JO ‘already’ and KOHTA ‘in a moment’, in two Finnish-speaking children’s (Piia and Mari) and their caregivers’ language.

These two adverbs were selected as they refer to the immediate past and/or future (jo ‘already’, kohta ‘in a moment’) and are thus among the earliest accurately used temporal adverbs in child language (Surakka, Reference Surakka2019, p. 7; Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989).

Study 1 is a quantitative analysis, in which we investigate the children and their caregivers’ usage of the target adverbs in relation to syntactic (word order) and semantic (temporal vs. discursive) characteristics between the ages of 1;7-4;11. Study 1 seeks answers to the following questions:

1a. What are the syntactic positions the adverbs JO and KOHTA occur in in the corpus children’s language? Does the children’s syntactic usage correspond to that of their parents?

1b. Do the children use adverbs temporally or discursively? – and does this correspond to their parents’ usage?

1c. Are particular meanings (temporal vs. discursive) of JO and KOHTA typically expressed in particular locations within utterances? And are these locations the same in the children and their parents?

Study 2 expands upon Study 1 and investigates how the target adverbs are used as tools of family interaction. We will analyse the discursive uses of JO and KOHTA in child–caregiver interaction in a qualitative manner and will address the following question:

2a. What kinds of discursive constructions do the adverbs JO and KOHTA occur in in child and adult language?

Our predictions are as follows:

1a. The children’s adverb use should mirror the parents’ usage in terms of location within the utterance (Surakka, Reference Surakka2019). However, due to young children’s non-adult-like language representations and language processing, the child vs. adult usage might not be completely identical.

1b. Children’s production of temporal vs. discursive uses of adverbials should broadly reflect adult language (e.g., if a given meaning is infrequent or absent from input it is likely to occur relatively late in development).

1c. Given that in the Finnish language, different word order patterns can give rise to different meanings or functions (Vilkuna, Reference Vilkuna1989, Reference Vilkuna and Siewierska1998) we predict some adverb-location related form-meaning tendencies to occur.

2a. Our prediction is that discursive uses of adverbs occur as part of constructions that have specific discourse functions in interaction (Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello, Brooks and Barret1999) and that the children’s different participant role from their parents in family interaction can have an impact on the usage (Kauppinen, Reference Kauppinen2020).

If we find that the children’s usage corresponds to their input, that would support the usage-based-constructivist theoretical viewpoint. However, because constructions express communicative intentions (Kauppinen, Reference Kauppinen2020; Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello, Brooks and Barret1999), children’s differential use of adverbs from their caregivers might be to some extent related their own and their parents’ different communicative intentions.

Study 1: Syntactic positions and meanings foregrounded by the target adverbs

Study 1 investigates two aspects of adverb use: syntactic positioning and distinction between temporal and discursive usage of JO and KOHTA in the children’s and their caregivers’ language.

Corpora

Two corpora consisting of child-parent interaction were analysed. These are the only naturalistic transcribed child corpora that sampled child-caregiver language available for Finnish for the relevant age range.

Piia-corpus

The Kirjavainen-Max Planck child corpus (Kirjavainen et al., Reference Kirjavainen, Kidd and Lieven2017a) consists of spontaneous interaction between the corpus child, Piia, and her caregivers – predominantly her parents. Piia is a monolingual first-born child in an upper-working/middle-class family in Kotka, Finland. She was audio-recorded by her parents approx. 3 hours per week between the child’s ages of 1;7–4;1, totaling approx. 255 hours data (278 recordings, approx. 915,000 word tokens, approx. 91,000 child utterances, approx. 185,000 adult utterances). The data were collected in naturalistic contexts without the situations being manipulated in any way. Piia’s younger sibling who was born when Piia was 2;5 was often present in the recordings but did not have a significant role in the interactions – although Piia and her parents did occasionally address her during the sessions. The data were collected in child-caregiver interactional contexts, typically during mealtimes, play sessions, arts and crafts and bath times. The data are transcribed in the CHAT format (MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2000) in Kotka-dialect and also in standard Finnish.

Mari-corpus

Mari-corpus (see Laalo, Reference Laalo2021) was audio collected between the child’s ages of 1;7–4;11, by the parents, one of whom is also a child language researcher. For the current study, we only had the data available between the ages of 2;0–4;11. The corpus contains spontaneous interaction between the child and her caregivers typically during free play, joint book-reading or mealtimes. Socio-economically the family can be described as middle-class. The total number of recordings is 24, totaling approx. 12 hours of data – 13 of the recordings are from the age range of 2;0-2;11 years, 7 from 3;0–3;11, and 4 from 4;0–4;11. The data were transcribed in standard Finnish and also in their phonetic forms.

Method

Searches

As the transcripts of the Piia corpus are in the CHAT format (MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2000), we used the KWAL command in the CLAN software (ibid.) to extract all utterances that contained the words JO and KOHTA (or their variants) along with their 10-utterance discourse contexts (5 utterances before, 5 utterances after the target utterance). As we wanted to only include utterances that showed productivity in the children (and were not retrieved from working- or long-term memory as potentially unanalysed coherent wholes), child utterances that were direct repetitions of the interlocutors’ (coded in the transcripts as [+ I]) or the child’s own speech (coded as [+ SR]) and rote-learned utterances, such as songs and rhymes (coded as [+ R]), were excluded. The child’s utterances that had a target adverb but that were not completed, or that were interrupted by an interlocutor (coded in the transcripts as [+ IN]) were also excluded as it can be difficult to identify the function of the adverb in partial utterances.

Due to the large number of utterances with JO/KOHTA in Piia’s caregivers’ language, and our partially manual analyses (coding each target utterance for their discourse vs. temporal function), we only included every fifth instance of parental use of each adverb in our Piia analysis. Our random sample of Piia’s parents’ data is likely to give us sufficient and reliable information about their usage of JO and KOHTA.

Due to the fact that the Mari corpus is relatively small, the target adverb searches were conducted manually and all child and adult instances of JO and KOHTA were included in the analysis (with the exclusions described above applied to the child’s data).

Coding

The children and caregivers’ data were coded for the following: speaker; age of the child (speaking or spoken to); number of words per utterance; and position of the target adverb in an utterance (utterance-initial; utterance-medial; utterance-final). For the Piia corpus, the coding was done electronically by a Perl script that extracted the above information for the CLAN output into Excel. This Perl output was manually checked by the first author. For the Mari data, the coding was done manually by the first author and checked by the second author. We also coded the data manually for the adverb function (temporal vs. discursive) as we explain below.

Coding of temporal vs. discursive uses of JO and KOHTA

To differentiate temporal and discursive adverb uses, we analysed the 10-turn discourse context. Adverbs that express temporal reference give information about when the action or topic referred to takes or took place, which is typical, for example, in narrative speech. The occurrences of this kind were coded as temporal. In contrast, the criteria for discursive usages were the following:

  1. 1) by the adverb-including utterance, the speaker comments on an action that is either taking place or that immediately preceded or will immediately follow the current event (i.e., is grounded), and

  2. 2) the adverb points out the illocutionary aspects of the utterance by which the speaker typically aims to influence their interlocutor’s behavior.

Both authors (separately) coded all instances of each adverb into the following three categories: temporal, discursive, ambiguous. Target utterances coded as discursive were further coded into specific discursive usages of JO and KOHTA: action completion, urging, reprimanding, reassuring, warning, threatening, complaining, giving permission, correction, directive, indirect no, instruction, invitation, justification, negotiation, persuasion, prohibition, request, suggestion. If the 10-turn discourse context did not provide enough information for assessing the adverb use as being temporal or discursive, the target adverb was marked as ambiguous. The agreement rate between the authors was 89%. The authors negotiated the meanings of differently coded adverbs to reach an agreement of 100%. The aforementioned specific discursive usages were only analysed in Study 2 but we present the coding here to demonstrate that the coding between temporal vs. discursive functions (relevant to Study 1) was carried out by carefully considering the meaning of each target utterance.

Analyses

We do not assume that the development of the two target adverbs is closely related for any other reason than them referring to the immediate past/future and thus likely to occur early in development (Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989). Therefore, we will report our analyses separately for JO and KOHTA.

We utilized generalized linear mixed-effects models (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) to examine the development of Piia’s and Mari’s JO and KOHTA, and whether their usage over time reflects that of their parents regarding utterance position and function. In this context, we follow Van Veen et al. (Reference Van Veen, Evers-Vermeul, Sanders and Van Den Bergh2013), who refer to this type of modeling interchangeably as either a growth curve analysis or as a multilevel logistic regression. However, we prefer to call these models generalized linear mixed-effects models, as they are referred to in Bates et al. (Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015).

In our first analysis in which we investigated the occurrence of JO and KOHTA (see Tables 2 and 3 below), in line with Van Veen et al. (Reference Van Veen, Evers-Vermeul, Sanders and Van Den Bergh2013. p. 1011), we categorized our dependent variables – namely, the utilization of KOHTA and JO, as binary outcomes: either they were present or absent. That is, we employed generalized linear mixed-effects models to assess the likelihood of KOHTA or JO appearing in a recording. Therefore, the analysis does not yield information regarding the frequency of adverbs in a recording. Similarly, as emphasized in Van Veen et al. (Reference Van Veen, Evers-Vermeul, Sanders and Van Den Bergh2013, p. 1012), by treating our data as binary, we sidestep the question as to how frequently adverbs should typically be employed. We acknowledge a limitation in our current GLM analysis pertaining to the frequency of adverb use (our approach focused on the probability of occurrence per recording, rather than per utterance). This methodological choice potentially overlooks finer nuances in the developing frequencies of adverb usage. However, to address the frequencies in more detail, in our other models (syntactic position of the adverbs (Table 5 in Supplementary Materials and Table 6); discursive vs. temporal uses (Tables 8 and 9)), we analysed each specific instance of adverb usage (with the minor exceptions explained below). These analyses move beyond merely noting the presence or absence of an adverb in a recording, aligning with methodologies used in studies by Tribushinina et al. (Reference Tribushinina, Van den Bergh, Kilani-Schoch, Aksu-Koç, Dabašinskienė, Hrzica, Korecky-Kröll, Noccetti and Dressler2013, Reference Tribushinina, Van den Bergh, Ravid, Aksu-Koç, Kilani-Schoch, Korecky-Kröll, Leibovitch-Cohen, Laaha, Nir, Dressler and Gillis2014).

Our independent variable was Age (Table 2) as a fixed effect, with Family (Piia’s and Mari’s) as a random effect. We also included Age as a polynomial of degree 2 or 3; however, these quadratic or cubic polynomials did not yield better predictions. To compare our models with more parameters to a simpler model with fewer parameters, we utilized the anova function. If the model with more parameters demonstrated a significantly improved fit to the data (indicated by a p-value < 0.05 and a smaller AIC (Akaike’s Information Criterion) index), we selected the more complex model. If the comparison did not yield a significant difference, we opted for the simpler model.

Results

We will first report the broad developmental observations for the two adverbs. The total number of occurrences of JO and KOHTA included in the analyses is presented in Table 1.

Table 1. The total number of occurrences included in the analyses.

Our data show that both children started to produce temporal adverbs soon after turning two years of age (Mari: jo: 2;1, kohta: 2;1; Piia: jo: 2;2, kohta: 2;2). Given that for Mari we do not have data before the age of 2;0, it is possible that her earlier data might have had instances of JO and KOHTA. Figures 1 and 2 display the outcomes of generalized linear mixed-effects models for JO and KOHTA, respectively (see also Tables 2 and 3). These figures illustrate that a child’s use of both JO and KOHTA increases with development, whereas the adults’ usage of neither adverb changes with the children’s development. Note that the figures suggest that approximately 40-50% of the recordings contain at least one instance of JO, and around 60% contain an instance of KOHTA in adult language. However, recall that we only included every fifth instance of adult target adverbs in the analysis, which led to the adult production predictions being lower than they should be. If all instances of JO and KOHTA were included, the adult usage would have been (almost) 100%.

Figure 1. The predicted probabilities of JO (y-axes) for both the children and their parents (shown over the period of data collection; x-axes represent children’s age measured in days).

Figure 2. The predicted probabilities of KOHTA (y-axes) for both the children and their parents (shown over the period of data collection; x-axes represent children’s age measured in days).

Table 2. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb JO in recordings.

Table 3. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb KOHTA in recordings.

Second, we investigated the syntactic positions in which the children and adults produced JO and KOHTA. Table 4 shows the number of utterance initial, medial and final uses of JO and KOHTA. All speakers produce JO typically in medial and final positions. In fact, the only utterance-initial uses of JO were produced by Piia’s parents in frozen idiomatic phrases ‘Jo nyt on kumma’ (Lit: Already now is weird ‘Have I not told you already’). When it comes to KOHTA Piia produced a relatively similar number of utterances with the initial, medial and final uses, Mari seemed to favour utterance initial and medial uses, while both Piia and Mari’s parents used KOHTA typically in utterance medial positions.

Table 4. Instances of utterances with JO and KOHTA, that had at least 3 words.

For a generalized mixed-effects model, we excluded all one and two-word utterances because in these utterances it is impossible for the adverb to occur in utterance medial positions. Initially, we analyzed whether the children’s usage changes with development, and then we investigated if the children’s language mirrors the usage in adults.

The model for the adverb JO suggests that there are no significant differences in the syntactic position of JO usage between the adults and the children (see Table 5 in Supplementary Materials). However, the model for the adverb KOHTA (see Figure 3 and Table 6) indicates that while children and adults used KOHTA in initial positions with no significant difference, adults are significantly more likely to produce KOHTA in utterance-medial positions than children. On the other hand, children are more likely than adults to produce KOHTA in utterance-final positions. Table 6 illustrates that there is no developmental change in the distribution of KOHTA usage between children and adults; while there are differences between child and parent uses, these distributions remain consistent over time. Put simply, within the model, ‘age’ as a fixed effect emerges as a significant predictor (Table 6). However, the interaction term between ‘age’ and ‘position’ does not demonstrate statistical significance. This model diverges from our first analyses (Tables 2 and 3) in its approach to the response variable. Instead of utilizing a binary coding to represent the presence or absence of the target adverb in a recording, it employs a binary coding based on the speaker: 0 for an adult using KOHTA and 1 for a child using KOHTA. Consequently, Figure 3 depicts the distribution of KOHTA within a sentence, showing the probabilities of its usage. The lower end of the y-axis represents adult usage, while the upper end indicates child usage. The red lines in the graph represent the confidence intervals.

Figure 3. The predicted probabilities of KOHTA for the children and their parents (y-axes) (x-axes represent the adverb’s location in the utterance: final, initial, or medial).

Table 6. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb KOHTA in adult vs child utterances.

Third, we analysed the functions (temporal vs. discursive) of the adverbs JO and KOHTA. Table 7 lists the first instances of temporal and discursive uses for JO and KOHTA for the two corpus children. The first instance of JO, at 2;2, that were found in Piia’s data was used discursively (Mä söin jo ‘I finished eating already’ (action completion)). Temporal uses were found about a month later. On the other hand, Mari’s data only had temporal uses initially and the first discursive use was found several months after the temporal JO usage. For KOHTA, the first occurrences in Piia and Mari’s language were temporal. The discursive usages were found several months later (Piia: 2;8 Saat ruokaa kohta ‘You’ll get some food in a moment’ (reassuring) Mari: 2;6 Tulen kohta ‘I’ll be there in a moment’ (reassuring)). Table 8 illustrates the number of temporal, discursive and ambiguous uses of JO and KOHTA Piia, Mari and their parents’ language. When it comes to JO, the table shows that when Mari, Mari’s parents and Piia’s parents produce JO, they typically express temporal meanings. However, Piia typically uses JO discursively.

Table 7. The first occurrence of temporal and discursive usage of JO and KOHTA in the children’s data.

Table 8. The number of temporal, discursive and ambiguous uses of JO and KOHTA.

We then ran a generalised mixed effects model on children’s and their parents’ data for function (temporal vs. discursive). We applied binary coding: 0 for temporal use and 1 for discursive use. These dummy coded variables function as the dependent variables. Figure 4 and Table 9 show that the children are predicted to start by using JO initially temporally but to produce increasingly more discursive meanings with development. This is driven by Piia’s usage given that Mari’s data contained only one discursive use of JO. The parents produce JO typically temporally and do not change their behaviour with the children’s development.

Figure 4. The predicted probabilities of JO in its temporal vs. discursive function (in the y-axis, discursive is coded as 1 and temporal is coded as 0, thus, 100% probability means discursive use only and 0% means temporal use only) in adults’ and children’s utterances (shown over the period of data collection; x-axis represent children’s age measured in days).

Table 9. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb JO in its temporal vs. discursive function in adult and child utterances.

Table 8 above shows that overall Piia, Mari and Mari’s parents produced similar proportions of temporal and discursive uses of KOHTA. We ran a generalised mixed effect model on the children and the adults’ data (see Table 10). Figure 5 shows that the children are initially predicted to produce more temporal uses of KOHTA but with age start producing more discursive uses. The model predicts that the parents produce more discursive than temporal uses of KOHTA, i.e., the children’s usage starts mirroring the caregivers’ language with development.

Table 10. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb KOHTA in its temporal vs. discursive function in adults’ and children’s utterances.

Figure 5. The predicted probabilities of KOHTA in its temporal vs. discursive function (y-axis) in adults’ and children’s utterances (shown over the period of data collection; x-axis represent children’s age measured in days).

Finally, we examined the interaction between semantics (temporal vs. discursive) and the syntactic location of the adverb (initial, medial, final) for both children and adults (Tables 11 and 12). To address this research question, we ran a model separately for JO and KOHTA in which the temporal/discursive distinction was dummy coded as 0 and 1, functioning as the dependent variable. The variables Age, Speaker (child vs parent), and Location (Initial, Medial, Final) were utilized as independent variables, including terms for their interaction. This interaction term did not yield significance in either the model for JO or for KOHTA (See Tables 13 and 14 in Supplementary Materials).

Table 11. Meanings of JO by position in utterances that had at least 3 words.

Table 12. Meanings of KOHTA by position in utterances that had at least 3 words.

Discussion

We investigated the syntactic (utterance initial, medial, final) and semantic (temporal vs. discursive) development of adverbs JO ‘already’ and KOHTA ‘in a moment’. We found that both adverbs emerged in the children’s data roughly at the same time (2;1-2;2), which is likely to be related to the input of both being present in the caregiver data but also due to these adverbs having similar, immediate future/past, meanings. As expected, there was an increase in the children’s usage of both adverbs with development. Even though initially there was no distinct difference between the frequency of use of the two adverbs, the children produced more instances of JO than KOHTA later in development. This might be explained to some extent by the differences in the frequency of JO and KOHTA in the parental use (Piia’s input: JO: 1181, KOHTA: 940; Mari’s input: JO: 33, KOHTA: 22) but also by the fact that JO is compatible with past and present tense verb forms while KOHTA is only compatible with the present tense, providing different levels of opportunity to use the two adverbs. In addition, as we explain above, in this analysis, we coded occurrences as 0 or 1 depending on whether there was at least one instance of the target adverb in a given recording. Consequently, our method does not take into consideration the overall frequencies of the adverbs.

In terms of the syntactic locations, the children’s usage of JO was similar to that of the parents’ use; JO typically occurred in utterance-medial, and, to some extent, final positions in all speakers. However, Piia seemed to produce more utterance-final uses, which reflects her parents’ usage, while Mari typically produced utterance-medial uses, reflecting her parents’ usage. JO’s unpopularity in the initial position overall can be explained by the fact that JO does not have a completely free sentence internal distribution – it occurs sentence initially only in some fixed, idiomatic, phrases (e.g., Jo nyt on kumma Lit: Already now is weird ‘Have I not told you already’).

The positioning of KOHTA within utterances was more mixed. Both children and the adults were found to produce KOHTA in all three syntactic positions. However, while the children and adults were equally likely to produce KOHTA utterance initially, the adults were more likely to produce KOHTA in utterance-medial positions than the children, and the children were more likely to produce KOHTA utterance finally than the adults. This preference for beginnings and ends of the utterances (recency/primacy effects) is likely to relate to processing limitations in early development (e.g., Freudenthal et al., Reference Freudenthal, Pine, Jones and Gobet2015; Seidl & Johnson, Reference Seidl and Johnson2006). That is, from a processing perspective it is easier to attach complements to the initial or final positions of already acquired utterances than insert them in an utterance internally. Adding extra/new material to the beginning/end of phrases/utterances can be seen, for example, in children’s relative clause acquisition, as relative clauses modifying sentence subjects (e.g., The dog that chased the cat is over there) are practically absent from children’s early naturalistic language production, while those utterances where relative clauses modify, for example, a predicate nominal and are attached to the end of the utterance (That’s the dog that chased the cat ), are produced early in development (Diessel & Tomasello, Reference Diessel and Tomasello2000). An additional explanation for the children’s initial/final adverb use is that the children’s utterances are shorter than those of the caregivers and therefore the children’s adverbs have fewer opportunities to occur utterance medially.

Semantically (temporal vs. discursive), the children became more adult-like with development for KOHTA, going from typically producing temporal uses initially to using KOHTA more discursively. However, the children and adults’ semantic use of JO showed some differences. Namely, while Piia’s parents produced significantly more temporal uses of JO, Piia started to produce JO typically with a discourse function with development. Mari seemed to mirror her parents in the semantic use of JO. We assume that the difference in the functions of JO between Piia and Mari can be explained by the different data collection contexts – Piia’s data were typically sampled when engaging in family routines while Mari’s data contained frequent joint book reading contexts which can create different levels of opportunity to express discursive meanings. In addition, family dynamics and children’s personalities can impact the discursive usage of adverbs.

To recap, even though syntactically the children’s adverb usage was relatively similar to that of their parents’ usage, there were some major differences in the semantic usage. To investigate these differences further, we conducted Study 2.

Study 2: Temporal adverbs in discursive constructions

In study 2, we will conduct a qualitative analysis on utterances in which JO and KOHTA have discursive uses in the Piia corpus. We are especially interested in the ways that children use established linguistic constructions for the purposes of expressing their communicative intentions (see Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello, Brooks and Barret1999, p. 163–164). The target constructions are closely related to events in the child–caregiver interaction in which the participant aims to influence the ongoing or immediately following actions (i.e., they all have discursive functions). In study 2, we shed further light onto the usage of JO and KOHTA in Piia’s and her caregivers’ language found in Study 1.

Method

Like a number of studies in the field of usage-based approach to language acquisition (e.g., Diessel & Tomasello, Reference Diessel and Tomasello2000; Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2005; Lieven & Tomasello, Reference Lieven, Tomasello, Robinson and Ellis2008), in our study 2 we will adopt the views and analytical tools from the field of Construction Grammar. We analyze the use of adverb-containing discursive constructions in child and adult language and take into consideration that the child’s repertoire of constructions might not correspond to that of the adults’ due to different interactional roles (caregiver vs. cared).

Data

Study 2 further analyses the occurrence of JO and KOHTA and their discourse contexts in the Piia data that were coded as discursive in Study 1. The Mari-corpus was not included in this analysis because of the low number (JO: 1; KOHTA: 9) of discursively used adverbs in her data.

Coding and analyses

When temporal adverbs are used in their typical function i.e., as time-frame adverbials, they are syntactically and semantically independent from their host sentences (see Langacker, Reference Langacker, Visapää, Kalliokoski and Sorva2014) – sometimes so much so that the sentence can be fully comprehensible and its propositional value remains largely intact even in the absence of the adverb (‘I am already two years of age’ vs. ‘I am two years of age’). On the other hand, when temporal adverb reinforces the discourse functions of its host utterance, the adverb can be regarded as being inseparable from the structure of the utterance. This distinction between independent and inseparable is the main linguistic-methodological starting point when identifying and describing adverb containing constructions in our study 2.

Discursive usages were first coded into the following categories:

action completion, urging, reprimanding, reassuring, warning, threatening, complaining, giving permission, correction, directive, indirect no, instruction, invitation, justification, negotiation, persuasion, prohibition, request, suggestion.

We then identified expressions that met the criteria of a conventionalized construction, i.e., those that were repeatedly used (i.e., at least three times) in a somewhat similar linguistic structure and function (Langacker, Reference Langacker2008, p. 167–168). Next, we described the constructions by their discourse function and syntactic structure (re S=subject, V=verb, O=object), separately for JO and KOHTA.

We ended up with the constructions listed below. The constructions contain fixed and variable parts. The variable parts or parts with more than one possible syntactic location are marked with parenthesis. In the analysis, the form of each instance is given in brackets.

Discourse functions of the constructions containing jo

Action completion

Form: [(S) VPST (O) (JO)]

Form: [VPST-Q.CLI (S) (O) JO]

Criteria: The speaker comments on their own action that has ended in the immediate past OR asks the interlocutor if the interlocuter has completed the action the interlocutor had been engaged in.

Urging

Form: [VIMP/PASS (O) (JO)]

Form: [VQ.CLI (S) (O) JO]

Form: [NIIN KUIN ‘as if’ VBE.COND JO], urging-reprimanding idiom construction

Criteria: The speaker expects something from their interlocutor and urges them.

Reprimanding

Form: [JO NYT ON XDA]; disapproval (DA) idiom construction ‘have I not told you already’

Criteria: The speaker takes a stand against the interlocutor’s ongoing action with an affective idiom construction.

Discourse functions of the constructions containing kohta

Reassuring

Form: [(S) V (O) (MOD) (KOHTA) (joo)]

Criteria: Reassuring utterances contain meaning structures of ‘demand to wait’ (typically implicit) and ‘promising the expected action to take place soon’.

Warning

Form: [(S) (V) (O/P) (MOD) (KOHTA)]

Criteria: The speaker expresses that the interlocutor’s ongoing action can result in a negative consequence.

Threatening

Form: [(S1SG) V1SG (KOHTA) (O)]

Criteria: The speaker expresses the interlocutor’s ongoing action can result in a negative reaction from the speaker.

(MOD = modifier; joo = affirmative discourse marker, O/P = In the copular clause in the Finnish language, the verb complement is called predicative (VISK § 943; Hakulinen et al., Reference Hakulinen, Vilkuna, Korhonen, Koivisto, Heinonen and Alho2004). It alternates with the grammatical object in the warning construction.)

From the basis of the discourse context, we will next analyse instances of these constructions: their forms and meanings in relation to the ongoing actions. The forms of the instances are given in brackets above the numbered examples, and the pieces of contextual information are given in parentheses after the samples.

Results

JO

Action completion

Action completion construction was the earliest (2;2) and the most common (n=38) discourse function in the child’s data. The verb syödä ‘to eat’ occurs in more than one third of the instances of the action completion construction (examples 4, 5). This highlights the construction’s grounded nature: eating is a core routine in a family-life context and the one action pattern to which the child is expected to contribute at the early age. However, with development (3;4-4;1), a number of different verbs emerged in the construction (e.g., tein ‘I made’; ‘pesin ‘I washed’; kokeilin; ‘I tried’; osasin ‘I was able to’).

In the child’s use of the action completion construction, the verb occurred in the 1st person past tense form and the adverb was typically in the utterance-final position (n = 29/50), but later in development JO was also often found in utterance-medial positions (e.g., Mie isi tein jo sen ‘Daddy, I already did it’). The salience of agency in the construction reflects the child’s developing ability to conceptualise the action-related person perspective, and means–end differentiation as the basis of behaving according to the expectations (Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2020, p. 103). From that viewpoint, action completion is also a token of the child experiencing success in social events.

A small number of action completion utterances was found also in the caregivers’ data (n=8). On these occasions, the agent was rarely in the 1st person form, reflecting the differences in the communicative content between children and their caregivers. The parents were more likely to produce, for example, questions with the action completion function (FAT: Söit sie kaikki jo? ‘Have you finished eating already?’) that most probably motivated the child’s usage of action completion construction with a first-person reference.

From the cognitive viewpoint, JO in the action completion construction seems to reinforce the action boundary expressed by the past tense verb form (see Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2020, p. 59). When the ongoing action is referred to, the boundary effect of the past tense verb form is more concrete compared to references to actions completed further in the past. Consequently, within the ongoing event, by marking the action boundaries, a speaker can influence the action taking place next, which can be considered a root for the construction’s discursive nature.

Urging

The urging function occurs in Piia’s data from the age of 2;8 (n = 28). Together with action completion, the urging construction makes up the majority of the discursive utterance-final JO instances in the children’s language (see Table 11). The urging construction is rare in the input data (n = 6, FAT: Ota se tyttö täält jo ‘take the girl from here already’) which suggests that caregivers use some other linguistic patterns to urge. In the child’s urging construction, the function is construed by a verb in a second person imperative form (6), or passive form (7), and the utterance is typically two to four words long.

Throughout, the verbs instantiated in the construction refer to concrete actions. The ones found in the imperative form were tule ‘come’; mene ‘go’; anna ‘give’; kato ‘look’; kokeile ‘try’. When the imperative form is applied, the urge is directed at the interlocutor (6). The passive form (7), which in Finnish is used similarly to the phrase Let’s-Verb in English, occurs only with the verb mennä ‘go’ reflecting the fact that the action of going is often a collaborative action in child-caregiver contexts and is speaker-inclusive: the action referred to will be accomplished together with the caregiver who also makes decisions about these joint actions. However, the child can influence those decisions, for example, by urging the decision maker.

In the data, Piia’s caregivers urge only by one construction (8).

The construction niin ku(i)n be cond jo is an example of a far conventionalised idiom construction.Footnote 1 The construction is emotionally loaded and implies that the child is not performing the needed action as quickly as expected or is refusing to do something requested. As an idiom construction, it allows no variation in word order, which is also the case for the reprimand construction that will be looked at next.

Reprimand

Another idiom construction occurring only in the input data constitutes the function of reprimand (n = 6). The form of the construction is fixed JO NYT ON XDA, the only lexically open slot being reserved for the various expressions of disapproval (XDA). This construction also has emotional dimensions, and the rate of intensity expressed can be modified by the lexical choice made in the utterance-final slot. The construction is applied in the events where the child does not appear to be following instructions from the caregiver.

In (9), the adjective kumma ‘weird’ constitutes a relatively neutral stance whereas Jo nyt on himputti that occurs twice in the data is a modification of the swear word Helvetti ‘Hell’. However, himputti is so remote in regard to the source swear word that it suits the family context as an expression of intensity, similarly to using ‘What the heck’ for ‘What the Hell’ in English.

The reprimand construction is the only context in the data where JO does not seem to construe any temporal reference at all but has become a marker of a certain discursive practice. In this construction, JO additionally co-occurs with NYT from which the temporal reference has reduced to the point where the word is considered only homonymous with the adverb nyt (‘now;’ No emmä nyt tiedä ‘Well, I don’t NOW know’ (Hakulinen, Reference Hakulinen, Jucker and Ziv1998, p. 87), meaning ‘Well, I don’t think so’). The similar grammaticalization processes can be found also from other languages, the bi-partite clause negation ne pas in French as one example (see Hansen, Reference Hansen2009).

With the reprimand and urging idiom constructions, the caregivers strongly set limits for the child’s behavior. This supports the child’s developing understanding of the event-based action patterns and the related desirable behavior. The likely explanations as to why these constructions are not manifested in the child’s language are that these constructions are more tightly linked to the caregivers’ interactional role and that children acquire abstract idiomatic expressions relatively late (e.g., Theakston et al., Reference Theakston, Lieven and Tomasello2003).

KOHTA

Reassuring

Caregivers use KOHTA frequently (n = 55) in situations where they acknowledge that the child is expecting something from them. The reassuring construction consists of all possible adverbial word order variants (utterance-initial, -medial, and -final).

Example (10) with KOHTA in utterance-medial position illustrates the nature of reassurance as a discourse function. The mother encourages the child to wait some more (oota hetki ‘wait a moment’) and additionally promises that the action referred to will take place in the immediate future. In typical instances of the reassuring construction, however, asking the child to wait remains implicit.

In 12 occasions, the reassuring kohta is utterance-initial. In (11), KOHTA occurs with an intensifier ihan ‘just’/‘quite’/’right’. In (12), the utterance ends with an affirmative discourse marker joo that also reinforces the given promise. The slots of the potential intensifiers ihan and joo are open in the construction.

In 16 occasions, kohta takes place is in an utterance-final position (13).

The reassuring KOHTA together with the child’s urging construction illustrate the impact that interactional roles have on the linguistic patterns in language use between children and caregivers. There are, however, instances (n = 23) of the reassuring construction in Piia’s data, starting already before the age of 3;0. Even if Piia follows the construction model relatively early in development, these early instances of KOHTA are often (n = 13) in the utterance-final position, suggesting that the child is not necessarily fully integrating the adverb into the utterance yet.

In (14), the child is talking to her baby sister and reformulates what the mother had said earlier. The example illustrates the usage-based learning of language: the child adopts form-meaning pairings from the caregivers’ language. Thus, language acquisition, adverbs included, and social interaction are interconnected.

Warning and threatening

In the input data, warning is one salient function construed by KOHTA (n = 38). The construction is tightly connected to the caregivers’ role, and is absent from the child’s data. By warnings, the caregivers explicitly estimate the presumable outcome of the child’s ongoing action. This way caregivers support the child’s development in causal reasoning and behavior regulation and bind this conceptual causal knowledge to the affective, formulaic language making the learning contexts more salient (cf. Ellis, Reference Ellis, Hundt, Pfenninger and Mollin2017).

The warning construction displays a lot of word-order-related variation – KOHTA takes place in utterance-initial (n = 6), utterance-medial (n = 25) and utterance-final (n = 7) positions. Example (15) illustrates the most frequent utterance-medial variant.

The warning function may additionally convey an aspect of a threat (n = 7, example 16). The threatening construction differs from the warning construction by its solely speaker-centered perspective.

From the variety of warnings in the input data, the child starts using the threatening construction between the ages of 3;4–4;0 (n = 5, see example 3).

Despite being relatively infrequent in the input data, the child starts using the threatening construction between the ages of 3;4–4;0 – probably due to the highly salient and memorable contexts they occur in. As in the input language, KOHTA in the threats produced by the child takes an utterance-initial (n = 3) or utterance-medial (n = 2) position.

Discussion

Discursive constructions containing JO in the child’s data were action completion and urging, the first function emerging in the child language being action completion (2;2). In this construction, JO was concluded to reinforce action boundaries expressed by the past tense verb form. Action completion also manifests the child’s ability to co-ordinate agency and perspective co-ordination as the basis of acquiring event-related desirable behavior. The child using an urging construction conveys to the caregiver that they should transition from the current to the desired next action. JO in the urging construction thus marks the “patience boundary” related to the implicit waiting process. The constructions containing JO in the input data were urging and reprimand. These were both highly idiomatic, and by using them the caregivers aimed to co-ordinate the child’s behavior. In all, the constructions containing JO in the input are not neatly mirrored in the child’s language, which can be explained by the difference in the interactional roles between the child and her caregivers affecting the usage.

Unlike the JO constructions, KOHTA constructions are similar in the child and the caregivers’ data, both expressing reassuring and threatening. However, the caregivers also use KOHTA construction with a warning function. In the input language, KOHTA constructions allow for several word order variations from which the child selects and practices some particular forms. KOHTA co-occurs with three variants of reinforcements in the caregivers’ language: the intensifiers ihan ‘quite’/‘right’ or sitten ‘then’, or an affirmative discourse marker joo.

We found that even though Piia and her parents used to some extent the same constructional types (e.g., reassuring), they also used different constructions that reflected their participant roles in family interaction (e.g., reprimand). These differences can at least partly explain the differences in the semantic and syntactic-semantic use between the children and their parents in Study 1. For example, the fact that we found that Piia’s discursive use of JO significantly increased with development can be explained by her increased use of urging and action completion functions in which JO typically occurred in Piia’s speech utterance finally. However, it is important to note that even though the constructional repertoires seem to be different in the child’s and the adults’ use, this does not mean that the child does not have the ability to use adult-like constructions. For example, the child produces a relatively large number of KOHTA with a reassuring function (23). Even though many of these were directed at the parents, Piia also produced reassuring uses of KOHTA when interacting with her baby sister and mirrored adults lexically specific construction (Saat ruokaa kohta ‘You’ll get some food in a moment’ 2;8). In the same vein, children’s pretend play contexts in which they are a caregiver (for example, to a toy) can contain constructions that would more typically occur in adult language. However, other than reassuring, Piia’s data contained only a small number of relatively typical adult constructions (reprimand: 0; warning: 2; threat: 5) probably due to the fact that the Piia-corpus consists of contexts in which the parent interacts with the child (i.e., the child was rarely left on her own to play with her toys during the recordings giving rise to pretend play opportunities).

General discussion

We investigated the syntactic and semantic acquisition of two temporal adverbs from their earliest occurrences (2;1/2;2) until 4;11/4;1. We found that both temporal and discursive uses emerged relatively early – before the age of 2;9 in both children’s language for JO and KOHTA. Some previous studies have reported that while temporal uses are present early in development, discursive uses have been absent early on (Liang et al., Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019; Surakka, Reference Surakka2019) while others have not made a distinction between temporal and discursive meanings in their analyses (e.g., Weist, Reference Weist, Levin and Dakay1989; Weist & Buczowska, Reference Weist and Buczowska1987). Thus, as far as we are aware, our study is the first to report both temporal and discursive meanings for early temporal adverbs. Due to lack of space, we only included two adverbs in our analysis. It is thus important to extend the work to other temporal adverbs to paint a clearer picture as to when different meanings emerge in child language. It is useful to point out that, to our knowledge, the two corpora included in the current study are the only naturalistic Finnish child corpora available in transcribed form for our target age range. The Piia corpus is relatively densely collected and large (approx. 3h per week between 1;7-4;1, approx. 255 h of data). Nevertheless, the occurrence of the target adverbs even in this corpus is relatively sparse. Thus, further analyses with bigger datasets, when available, are also encouraged.

Did the child’s usage mirror that of the adults?

Syntactic use (word order) of adverbs

When it comes to the location of the adverbs within utterances, we found that the children’s use of JO and KOHTA within utterances largely mirrored their parents’ use. This fits in with the usage-based viewpoint of language development which holds that children’s language use and acquisition is heavily influenced by the language they have been exposed to (e.g., Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003). In addition, this finding is in line with Surakka’s (Reference Surakka2019) study that found that children aged 2;6 years have a good syntactic understanding of how to position temporal adverbs in utterances. However, we did observe some important differences and developmental changes. First, the adults were more likely to use the adverbs, in particular, KOHTA, utterance medially than the children. It is likely that the early utterance-initial/final positions were produced at least partly due to the following two factors. First, as we discussed earlier, it is easier to add components to the beginning or end of units that the speaker has (some) ability over than somewhere in the middle of the unit. Second, it is possible that due to the early instances of JO and KOHTA occurring predominantly in short utterances (1–2 words long) the slot reserved for these adverbials was very specific and systematic (first/last word). This influenced the child’s preferred positioning of these due to practice, even with longer utterances that gave the option for utterance medial use. Also, the following are likely to have had a role in the child’s later production of adverbs in more varied positions: (a) the child’s increasing linguistic ability and production of longer utterances; and (b) when it comes to languages like Finnish that have a relatively flexible word order, the child’s developing understanding of the range of syntactic locations a given word can slot into.

Meaning

In Study 1, we found that even though syntactically the children’s language broadly speaking corresponded to their parents’ language, semantically (temporal vs. discursive) Piia’s usage differed from that of her parents, from Mari and Mari’s parents. More specifically, with age, Piia started to produce more discursive uses of JO while the other speakers produced JO more commonly with temporal meanings. The opposite was the case for KOHTA – with development the children’s usage started to reflect that of their parents, i.e., the children, like the parents, used KOHTA typically discursively. The increase in discursive uses through development might be linked to the child’s developing understanding of the different uses of a given adverb (e.g., urging, reassuring). This gives them an opportunity to produce discursive meanings more frequently (because they have a bigger repertoire of discursive meanings to express with a given adverb). One of our key findings thus is that discursive uses of temporal adverbs seem to be commonplace in early child language – a finding previous studies have failed to observe. This can be explained by the following. First, the relatively infrequent occurrence of discursive uses early in development means that densely collected corpora (such as the Piia corpus) are needed to find instances of relatively infrequent items, possibly explaining some of the discrepancy between our study and some previous studies. Second, the data analysed need to be collected in naturalistic family contexts where discursive meanings can surface (as opposed to e.g., in a lab where parents are less likely to reprimand, threaten or even urge their children, or in child-child interactions in which these might not be relevant). For example, Liang et al. (Reference Liang, Dandan and Hui2019) found no discursive uses in their data which consisted of preschool children playing in peer dyads with toys instead of making conversation with each other. Consequently, the absence of discursive meanings in their data does not necessarily mean that the children do not know or use discursive adverbs – it is just that the contexts sampled might not have given rise to those meanings being expressed. Also, we observed some differences in the usage (temporal vs. discursive) between our two corpus children. These differences could be related to the differences in the interactional contexts of the two corpora. Namely, the Piia-corpus was much richer in discursive usages of temporal adverbs compared to the Mari-corpus, which we assume relates to the Piia-corpus having been typically sampled in daily routine contexts in which the joint activities with certain goals (e.g., finishing lunch at mealtime) are constantly negotiated (see Spagnola & Fiese, Reference Spagnola and Fiese2007). Mari’s interaction with her parents was sampled in contexts in which the child and parents engaged in joint play and book reading sessions in which discursive usage of adverbs might have been irrelevant. Lastly, the child’s and their parents’ personalities are likely to impact the frequency with which the child uses language discursively. For example, some children might be more assertive and strong-willed and would be more confident to command, threaten, warn and reprimand their caregivers than some other children; and some parents might allow such behaviour more readily than others. Further studies on the topic with additional children’s corpus data would be highly beneficial to make more far-reaching generalizations on the development of temporal adverbs.

Construction selection

Study 2 showed that interactional intentions affect the construction selection and thus the acquisition of constructions with a specific discourse function. In relation to JO, the child used constructions that reflected their subordinate interactional role. However, even the role-related choices were not invented in linguistic isolation, but the constructions produced by the child and caregivers seem to formulate functional pairs: for example, when a caregiver asked Söit sie kaikki jo? (‘Have you finished eating already?’), the child learned that an affirmative answer is expected from them and that behaving accordingly (e.g., finishing her meals) will license the use of the action completion construction. Orsolini (Reference Orsolini1993) suggests that through linguistic routines, children learn when certain speech acts are expected from them. For example, if the child has learned that the parent will commonly urge them to finish their dinner, they will produce the expected response already before the parent has urged them. We consider this script-based interactional setting a valid explanation for how the constructions that are used by the child in the absence of a parental prompt or significant input become part of the child’s linguistic repertoire – like the ACTION COMPLETION construction (Mä söin jo ‘I ate already’).

Piia’s selections of KOHTA constructions seemed to follow a more expected pattern of language acquisition: the child gradually developed in producing the same/similar constructions as her caregivers – highly abstract idiom constructions as exceptions (see also Theakston et al., Reference Theakston, Lieven and Tomasello2003, p. 876). The acquisition of KOHTA constructions, especially reassuring, indicated dependence in the discourse context: the child was practicing transition between her two social roles (being a child for the caregivers and a caregiver for her baby sister).

Our results suggest that in addition to expressing what seems to be temporal reference, temporal adverbs can also be used to express discursive functions from 2;2 years of age and that these uses are learned as tools of interaction. The variety of meanings construed by the target adverbs in the data thus supports the idea of Lebois et al. (Reference Lebois, Wilson-Mendenhall and Barsalou2015) in that the meanings of abstract, polysemic concepts may vary from one context to another. The linguistic constructions adopted from the input language appeared to work as concrete communicative tools in interaction and offer formulaic models for children that ease the adverb production interaction (cf. Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003). Also, the analysis in Study 2 illustrated how abstract concepts develop from the contextual experiences as they are applied as tools that organize interaction with concretely perceivable results (see also Schwanenflugel & Shoben, Reference Schwanenflugel and Shoben1983). For example, the affective and thus salient threatening construction was adopted by Piia from her caregivers at age of 3;6 (see example 3).

To what extent do children’s early temporal adverbs refer to time?

Previous studies have found that even though children produce temporal adverbs already early in development, they might not have a fully-fledged understanding of the meaning of the adverb. For example, Surakka (Reference Surakka2019) reports that the adverb huomenna ‘tomorrow’ was used with a past tense verb, indicating that the child had not fully understood the meaning of the adverb but was nevertheless using it.

We found that our corpus children produced instances of JO and KOHTA from early in development (2;1-2;2), seemingly accurately (i.e., we did not find any instances of incongruent uses of an adverb with the rest of the utterance) and that the first 6 months of each target adverb instantiated both temporal and discursive meanings – in fact, for Piia, discursive meanings (2;2) of JO were found before temporal meanings (2;3). As discursive adverbs have complex meaning consisting typically of temporal reference and discourse function, it entails that at least some of the early adverb use cannot be taken as strong evidence for fully developed understanding of temporal meaning of JO ‘already’ and KOHTA ‘in a moment’. Furthermore, given that even in the temporal uses of JO and KOHTA, their temporal meaning is relatively vague (for example, in comparison to adverbials such as ‘yesterday’ or ‘in an hour’) the temporal adverbs that were found in the early data might not give us strong evidence of the child’s understanding of the relevant temporal meanings, but are using the adverbs as learned parts of utterances (Lieven & Tomasello, Reference Lieven, Tomasello, Robinson and Ellis2008) in relation to the concrete joint actions (Mazzuca et al., Reference Mazzuca, Fini, Michalland, Falcinelli, Da Rold, Tummolini and Borghi2021; Schwanenflugel & Shoben, Reference Schwanenflugel and Shoben1983). That is, it might be that even when children produce utterances like Nalle syö kohta (‘Teddy will eat in a moment’) in which they appear to express temporal reference with KOHTA ‘in a moment’ , they might not fully understand what the time reference is, but are producing the adverb because they have heard it being used by parents in similar contexts (mother saying Mummo tulee kohta ‘grandma is coming over in a moment’ or Isi nousee ylös kohta ‘daddy will get up in a moment’).

Conclusion

We investigated early temporal adverb acquisition by looking at adverbs JO ‘already’ and KOHTA ‘in a moment’. Our study showed that the children used the target adverbs to construe both temporal and discursive meanings, that the syntactic location of the adverbs relatively closely reflected that of the adult use and that the discursive uses appeared to be learned from interactional contexts and typically had the functions of urging, action completion and reassuring in the child’s language. The daily-routine-related everyday discussions evoked linguistic patterns corresponding to the child’s and caregivers’ interactional roles. Further study into this area with additional languages and corpora is highly encouraged.

Abbreviations used in glossing

aff

affirmative

all

allative

cli

clitic

cond

conditional

dm

discourse marker

gen

genitive

ill

illative

imp

imperative mood

ine

inessive

neg

negative

pass

passive voice

par

partitive

pst

past tense

q

question

1sg

first person singular

2sg

second person singular

3sg

third person singular

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000114.

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank Prof Klaus Laalo for sharing his child corpus with us. We would also like to thank the three anonymous reviewers and the action editor for their helpful comments on the earlier versions of the article.

Competing interest

The authors declare none.

Funding

This project was funded by an Ella and Georg Ehrnrooth grant to the first author.

Footnotes

1 Hoffman and Trousdale (Reference Hoffmann, Trousdale, Hoffmann and Trousdale2013) introduce the idea of constructions taking places in the lexicon-syntactic continuum. The meaning of idiom construction is not completely compositional (i.e. transparent by meaning built by the “bricks” involved) but must be stored in a speaker’s mental lexicon like words. Constructions at the other end of the continuum are those that occur in the child’s speech also. They are more schematic by nature (i.e., allow lexical variation) and are thus more compositional and transparent by meaning.

References

Ames, L. B. (1946). The development of the sense of time in the young child. Journal of Genetic Psychology 68(1), 97126. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1946.10533358Google ScholarPubMed
Athanasiadou, A. (1991). The discourse function of questionsPragmatics 1.1 107122.Google Scholar
Austin, J. R., Engelberg, S., & Rauh, G. (2004). Current issues in the syntax and semantics of adverbials. In Rauh, G., Engelberg, S., & Austin, J. R. (Eds.), Adverbials: The interplay between meaning, context, and syntactic structure (Vol. 70, pp. 144). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1075/la.70.02ausCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67 ( 1), 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chejnová, P. (2017). Acquisition of adverbs and pronominal adverbs in a Czech childTopics in Linguistics18(1), 3247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Ruiter, L. E., Lemen, H. C., Lieven, E. V., Brandt, S., & Theakston, A. L. (2021). Structural and interactional aspects of adverbial sentences in English mother-child interactions: an analysis of two dense corporaJournal of Child Language48(6), 11501184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diessel, H. (2013). Construction Grammar and First Language Acquisition. In Hoffmann, T., & Trousdale, G. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp. 327345). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.013.0019Google Scholar
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11 ( 1/2), 131151. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2001.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2017). Salience in Language Usage, Learning, and Change. In Hundt, M., Pfenninger, S., & Mollin, S. (Eds.), The changing English language. The Psycholinguistic Perspectives (pp. 7192). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (1977). Introduction. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.), Child discourse (pp. 123). New York: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Jones, G., & Gobet, F. (2015). Simulating the cross-linguistic pattern of Optional Infinitive errors in children’s declaratives and Wh- questionsCognition143, 6176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, A. (2005). Constructions at Work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199268511.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. (2019). Explain me this: creativity, competition, and the partial productivity of constructions. Princeton: Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586520000049Google Scholar
Hakulinen, A. (1998). The use of Finnish nyt as discourse particle. In Jucker, A. H., & Ziv, Y. (Eds.), Discourse markers. Descriptions and theory (pp. 8396). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.57.06hakCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakulinen, A., & Saari, M. (1995). Temporaalisesta adverbista diskurssipartikkeliksi [From temporal adverb to discourse particle]Virittäjä 99(4), 481500.Google Scholar
Hakulinen, A., Vilkuna, M., Korhonen, R., Koivisto, V., Heinonen, T. R., & Alho, I. (2004). VISK. Iso suomen kielioppi [Descriptive grammar of Finnish]. Helsinki: Suomalaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura. http://scripta.kotus.fi/visk/etusivu.php.Google Scholar
Hall, J. K. (2019). The contributions of conversation analysis and interactional linguistics to a usage-based understanding of language: Expanding the transdisciplinary frameworkThe Modern Language Journal103, 8094. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, M. M. (2009). The grammaticalization of negative reinforcers in old and middle French: A discourse-functional approachStudies in Pragmatics7, 227251. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004253216_013Google Scholar
Hoffmann, T., & Trousdale, G. (2013). Construction Grammar: Introduction. In Hoffmann, T., & Trousdale, G. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of construction grammar (pp. 112). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huumo, T. (1997). Lokatiivit lauseen semanttisessa tulkinnassa. Ajan, omistajan, paikan ja tilan adverbiaalien keskinäiset suhteet suomen kielessä. [Locatives and semantic interpretation of the sentence: On mutual relations of adverbial indicating time, possession, space and (internal) state in Finnish]. Turku: University of Turku.Google Scholar
Ibbotson, P. (2020). What it takes to talk. Exploring developmental cognitive linguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110647914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, P., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2014). The communicative contexts of grammatical aspect use in EnglishJournal of Child Language41(3), 705723. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kauppinen, A. (2020). Mistä kieli meihin tulee [Language – and where it comes from]. Tampere: Vastapaino.Google Scholar
Kirjavainen, M., Kidd, E., & Lieven, E. (2017a). How do language-specific characteristics affect the acquisition of different relative clause types? Evidence from Finnish. Journal of Child Language 44, 120157. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000768CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirjavainen, M., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. (2017b). Can infinitival to omissions and provisions be primed? An experimental investigation into the role of constructional competition in infinitival to omission errors. Cognitive Science 41(5), 12421273. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kranjek, A., & McDonough, L. (2011). The implicit and explicit embodiment of time. Journal of Pragmatics 43, 735748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2010.07.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laalo, K. (2021). Derivation in Finnish child speech and child-directed speechThe Acquisition of Derivational Morphology: A cross-linguistic perspective66, 237. https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.66.10laaGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226709005799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2014). Subordination in a dynamic account of grammar. In Visapää, L., Kalliokoski, J., & Sorva, H. (Eds.), Contexts of subordination. Cognitive, typological and discourse perspectives (pp. 1772). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Lebois, L. A. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., & Barsalou, L. W. (2015). Are automatic conceptual cores the gold standard of semantic processing? The context-dependence of spatial meaning in grounded congruency effects. Cognitive Science, 39, 17641801. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, L. Y., Dandan, W. U., & Hui, L. I. (2019). Chinese preschoolers’ acquisition of temporal adverbs indicating past, present, and future: a corpus-based studyJournal of child language 46(4), 760784. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, E. V., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Children’s first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective. In Robinson, P., & Ellis, N. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 168196). New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203938560-16Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U. (2006). Infant Pointing at 12 Months: Communicative Goals, Motives, and Social-Cognitive Abilities, In Enfiled, N. J., & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction (pp.153178). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.1162/coli.2000.26.4.657CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzuca, C., Fini, C., Michalland, A. H., Falcinelli, I., Da Rold, F., Tummolini, L., & Borghi, A. M. (2021). From Affordances to Abstract Words: The Flexibility of Sensorimotor GroundingBrain Sciences 11(10), 1304. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11101304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olineck, K. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2005). Infants’ ability to distinguish between intentional and accidental actions and its relation to internal state languageInfancy8(1), 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orsolini, M. (1993). Dwarfs do not shoot: An analysis of children’s justifications. Cognition and Instruction 11, 281297. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.1993.9649026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parm, S. (2013). Eesti keele ajasõnade omandamine [ Acquisition of time adverbs in Estonian]. Dissertationes linguisticae universitatis Tartuensis 17. Tartu: Tartu Ülikooli Kirjastus.Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1983). Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9(1), 82102. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.9.1.82Google Scholar
Seidl, A., & Johnson, E. K. (2006). Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extractionDevelopmental Science9(6), 565573. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00534.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spagnola, M., & Fiese, B. H. (2007). Family Routines and Rituals: A Context for Development in the Lives of Young Children. Infants & Young Children 20(4), p. 284299. DOI: 10.1097/01.IYC.0000290352.32170.5aCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Surakka, (2019). Ajan paikka. Ajanilmausten kehityspolkuja lasten kielessä [A location of time. The development of the expressions of time in children’s language]. Publications of the University of Eastern Finland Dissertations in Education, Humanities, and Theology. Joensuu: University of Eastern Finland.Google Scholar
Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., Luo, R., Escobar, K., & Bornstein, M. H. (2017). Power in methods: Language to infants in structured and naturalistic contextsDevelopmental science20(6), e12456. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of the input in the acquisition of third singular verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46, 863877. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/067)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. A usage-based approach to language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Brooks, P. J. (1999). Early syntactic development: A construction grammar approach. In Barret, M. (ed.), The development of language (pp. 161190). New Yourk: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tribushinina, E., Van den Bergh, H., Kilani-Schoch, M., Aksu-Koç, A., Dabašinskienė, I., Hrzica, G., Korecky-Kröll, K., Noccetti, S., & Dressler, W. (2013). The role of explicit contrast in adjective acquisition: A cross-linguistic longitudinal study of adjective production in spontaneous child speech and parental inputFirst Language33(6), 594616. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723713503146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E., Van den Bergh, H., Ravid, D., Aksu-Koç, A., Kilani-Schoch, M., Korecky-Kröll, K., Leibovitch-Cohen, I., Laaha, S., Nir, B., Dressler, W., & Gillis, S. (2014). Development of adjective frequencies across semantic classes: A growth curve analysis of child speech and child-directed speechLIA : Language, Interaction and Acquisition5(2), 185226. https://doi.org/10.1075/lia.5.2.02triCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Veen, R., Evers-Vermeul, J., Sanders, T., & Van Den Bergh, H. (2013). The influence of input on connective acquisition: a growth curve analysis of English because and German weil. Journal of child language, 40 ( 5), 10031031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vilkuna, M. (1989). Free word order in Finnish: Its syntax and discourse functions. Helsinki: Suomalaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura.Google Scholar
Vilkuna, M. (1998). Word order in European Uralic. In Siewierska, A. (Ed.), Constituent order in the languages of Europe (pp. 173233). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weist, R. M. (1989). Time concepts in language and thought: Filling the Piagetian void from 2 to 5 years. Levin, I., & Dakay, D. (Eds.), Time and human cognition. A life-span perspective (pp. 63118). Advances in psychology 59. Amsterdam: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61039-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weist, R. M., & Buczowska, E. (1987). The emergence of temporal adverbs in child Polish. First Language 7, 217229. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272378700702105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. The total number of occurrences included in the analyses.

Figure 1

Figure 1. The predicted probabilities of JO (y-axes) for both the children and their parents (shown over the period of data collection; x-axes represent children’s age measured in days).

Figure 2

Figure 2. The predicted probabilities of KOHTA (y-axes) for both the children and their parents (shown over the period of data collection; x-axes represent children’s age measured in days).

Figure 3

Table 2. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb JO in recordings.

Figure 4

Table 3. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb KOHTA in recordings.

Figure 5

Table 4. Instances of utterances with JO and KOHTA, that had at least 3 words.

Figure 6

Figure 3. The predicted probabilities of KOHTA for the children and their parents (y-axes) (x-axes represent the adverb’s location in the utterance: final, initial, or medial).

Figure 7

Table 6. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb KOHTA in adult vs child utterances.

Figure 8

Table 7. The first occurrence of temporal and discursive usage of JO and KOHTA in the children’s data.

Figure 9

Table 8. The number of temporal, discursive and ambiguous uses of JO and KOHTA.

Figure 10

Figure 4. The predicted probabilities of JO in its temporal vs. discursive function (in the y-axis, discursive is coded as 1 and temporal is coded as 0, thus, 100% probability means discursive use only and 0% means temporal use only) in adults’ and children’s utterances (shown over the period of data collection; x-axis represent children’s age measured in days).

Figure 11

Table 9. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb JO in its temporal vs. discursive function in adult and child utterances.

Figure 12

Table 10. Estimated coefficients, standard errors, z-values, and p-values for the generalized mixed model fitted to the occurrence of the adverb KOHTA in its temporal vs. discursive function in adults’ and children’s utterances.

Figure 13

Figure 5. The predicted probabilities of KOHTA in its temporal vs. discursive function (y-axis) in adults’ and children’s utterances (shown over the period of data collection; x-axis represent children’s age measured in days).

Figure 14

Table 11. Meanings of JO by position in utterances that had at least 3 words.

Figure 15

Table 12. Meanings of KOHTA by position in utterances that had at least 3 words.

Supplementary material: File

Surakka and Kirjavainen supplementary material

Surakka and Kirjavainen supplementary material
Download Surakka and Kirjavainen supplementary material(File)
File 119.9 KB