Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T07:41:10.452Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuronal variability of Resting State activity in Eating Disorders: increase and decoupling in Ventral Attention Network and relation with clinical symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Angela V. Spalatro
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Neuroscience, Psychiatry Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Federico Amianto*
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Neuroscience, Psychiatry Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Zirui Huang
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Neuroscience, Psychiatry Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Federico D’Agata
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Neuroscience, Psychiatry Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Mauro Bergui
Affiliation:
cDepartment of Neuroscience, Neuroradiology Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Giovanni Abbate Daga
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Neuroscience, Psychiatry Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Secondo Fassino
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Neuroscience, Psychiatry Section, AOU City of Health and Science of Turin, University of Turin, Via Cherasco 11, 10126, Turin, Italy
Georg Northoff
Affiliation:
dThe Royal’s Institute of Mental Health Research & University of Ottawa Brain and Mind Research Institute, Centre for Neural Dynamics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Ottawa, 145 Carling Avenue, Rm. 6435, Ottawa, ON, K1Z 7K4Canada
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected]

Abstract

Background:

Despite the great number of resting state functional connectivity studies on Eating Disorders (ED), no biomarkers could be detected yet. Therefore, we here focus on a different measure of resting state activity that is neuronal variability. The objective of this study was to investigate neuronal variability in the resting state of women with ED and to correlate possible differences with clinical and psychopathological indices.

Methods:

58 women respectively 25 with Anorexia Nervosa (AN), 16 with Bulimia Nervosa (BN) and 17 matched healthy controls (CN) were enrolled for the study. All participants were tested with a battery of psychometric tests and underwent a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) resting state scanning. We investigated topographical patterns of variability measured by the Standard Deviation (SD) of the Blood-Oxygen-Level-Dependent (BOLD) signal (as a measure of neuronal variability) in the resting-state and their relationship to clinical and psychopathological indices.

Results:

Neuronal variability was increased in both anorectic and bulimic subjects specifically in the Ventral Attention Network (VAN) compared to healthy controls. No significant differences were found in the other networks. Significant correlations were found between neuronal variability of VAN and various clinical and psychopathological indices.

Conclusions:

We here show increased neuronal variability of VAN in ED patients. As the VAN is relevant for switching between endogenous and exogenous stimuli, our results showing increased neuronal variability suggest unstable balance between body attention and attention to external world. These results offer new perspective on the neurobiological basis of ED. Clinical and therapeutic implication will be discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © European Psychiatric Association 2019

1. Introduction

Eating Disorders (ED) represent the psychiatric disorder with the highest rate of mortality in adolescent and young women [Reference American Psychiatric Association1]. To date, the physiopathology of these disorders is only partially understood. Clinicians suggest a multifactorial approach, in which neurobiological factors can contribute to the vulnerability, onset and outcome of ED [Reference Amianto, Northoff, Abbate Daga, Fassino and Tasca2Reference Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay and Schmidt4]. Recently a growing number of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have contributed to clarify neuro-functional alterations in eating disorders [Reference Gaudio, Wiemerslage, Brooks and Schiöth5]. Based on these studies, a neurobiological model of ED suggests that there could be an imbalance in information processing between ventral limbic and dorsal executive circuits [Reference Wagner, Aizenstein, Mazurkewicz, Fudge, Frank and Putnam6]. These circuits are primarily implicated in inhibitory cognitive processes and reward-related behavior, which are altered in ED [Reference D’Agata, Caroppo, Amianto, Spalatro, Caglio and Bergui7Reference Kaye, Wierenga, Bailer, Simmons, Wagner and Bischoff-Grethe9].

Several groups have used resting-state fMRI [Reference Fox and Greicius10] that is thought to reflect internally oriented thinking and consciousness [Reference Harmelech, Preminger, Wertman and Malach11, Reference Northoff and Stanghellini12]. This method measures brain activity in absence of any stimulus and allows to calculate temporal correlations between different brain areas, i.e., functional connectivity [Reference Biswal, Mennes, Zuo, Gohel, Kelly and Smith13, Reference Carnell, Gibson, Benson, Ochner and Geliebter14].

Despite the different interpretations of the Resting State studies on ED, the results are generally overlapping in that they show functional alterations in various networks and/or areas. Specifically, alterations in the fronto-parietal network (a brain network involved in cognitive control functions) were found in both patients with Anorexia Nervosa (AN) and participants recovering from AN [Reference Boehm, Geisler, King, Ritschel, Seidel and Deza Araujo15]. These resting-state findings may underlie the impaired cognitive control of appetitive processes as well as ruminations concerning the self and body image [Reference Abbate-Daga, Buzzichelli, Amianto, Rocca, Marzola and McClintock16, Reference Canna, Prinster, Monteleone, Cantone, Monteleone and Volpe17].

Functional connectivity describes the relationship, i.e., synchronization, between different regions and their activities in the resting state. This leaves out the neuronal features of the region itself independent of its relationship to others. For that, one could investigate the variability of neural activity in the resting state as it has been introduced recently [Reference Chen, Zhou, Chen, Dong, Zang and Qiao18Reference Garrett, Samanez-Larkin, MacDonald, Lindenberger, McIntosh and Grady21]. This approach, called “standard deviation (SD) of blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal” describes the fMRI fluctuations of the BOLD signal in a specific cerebral region, resulting in a more direct measure of a brain region activity [Reference Garrett, Kovacevic, McIntosh and Grady19Reference Garrett, Samanez-Larkin, MacDonald, Lindenberger, McIntosh and Grady21]. To date, studies conducted with this approach are still very limited. Studies on psychiatric subjects show that in depression and bipolar disorders [Reference Huang, Davis, Yue, Wiebking, Duncan and Zhang22Reference Yue, Jia, Hou, Zang and Yuan27], the analysis of neuronal variability allows us to emphasize more precisely the neuro-functional alterations in terms of specific regions or networks including their relation to behavior and clinical symptoms [Reference Northoff28, Reference Northoff and Duncan29].

The aim of this study is to explore for the first time fMRI resting state neuronal variability in ED patients including both anorectic (AN) and bulimia (BN) subjects and to correlate any differences with psychopathological and clinical symptoms. As no previous studies investigated SD in ED, our study must be considered exploratory. Based on previous findings [Reference D’Agata, Caroppo, Amianto, Spalatro, Caglio and Bergui7, Reference Kaye, Wierenga, Bailer, Simmons, Wagner and Bischoff-Grethe9, Reference Northoff and Stanghellini12] we tentatively hypothesized changes in SD in specifically the attention networks like ventral attention network (VAN) in ED including their relationship to psychopathological measures.

2. Methods and material

2.1 Participants

Forty-one women who were consecutively assessed after their first contact at the Center for Eating Disorders of the University of Turin diagnosed respectively with Anorexia Nervosa (25) and Bulimia Nervosa (16) were recruited for this study between January 2012 and December 2014. A diagnosis of AN and BN was established by a psychiatrist following DSM-IV-TR criteria [Reference American Psychiatric Association30] and confirmed using DSM 5 criteria before the data elaboration. Inclusion criteria were: female sex; age of illness < 1.5 years; right-handed; BMI < 18 and > 14. Exclusion criteria were major medical illness, neurological disorders, use of psychotropic medications or psychotherapeutic treatments in the last 6 months, presence of other relevant psychiatric diagnosis, contraindication for MRI. Seventeen matched healthy women were enrolled for the study as control group.

2.2 Ethics

The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the San Giovanni Battista Hospital, Turin. (CEI/17 0028836) according to the Helsinki Declaration as revised in Edinbourgh in 2000. All patients and controls gave their written informed consent to the study.

2.3 Measures

During a screening interview, an expert psychiatrist assessed every patient and control with the support of the SCID [Reference First Michael, Spitzer Robert, Miriam, Williams and BW31] and conducted a clinical evaluation to determine possible inclusion in the study. Clinical data such as length of disease, BMI, binge/purging episodes, physical hyperactivity and use of laxative per week were also collected. According to Clinical Global Index (CGI), the psychiatrist assigned a severity index for the disease for each patient. All the participants completed a battery of self-administered psychometric tests including Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI [Reference Cloninger, Przybeck and Svrakic32]), Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2 [Reference Garner33]), Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20 [Reference Bagby, Taylor, Parker and Loiselle34]), Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90 [Reference Derogatis35]), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II [Reference Beck, Steer, Brown and BDI-II36]) and Attachment Style Questionnaire (ASQ [Reference Feeney, Noller and Hanrahan37]) to evaluate psychopathological symptoms and personality features.

2.4 fMRI data acquisition

Functional data were collected at the Neuroscience Department AOU San Giovanni Battista, Turin, Italy on a Philips Achieva 1.5 T scanner (Erlangen, Holland) equipped with dual gradient system (Quasar, Philips) of 40 m T/m and a Sense high-field, high resolution eight-channel head coil optimized for functional imaging. The resting-state scan comprised 200 continuous functional volumes (repetition time = 1966 ms, echo time = 50 ms, flip angle = 90°, 24 axial slices, matrix = 128 × 128, slice thickness = 5 mm, acquisition voxel size = 1.8 mm × 1.8 mm × 6 mm, field of view = 25 cm). The total acquisition time was 6 min and 33 s; the patients were scanned with their eyes closed. Foam pads were used to reduce head motion and scanner noise. All participants reported keeping their eyes closed and being awake during the scanning.

2.5 fMRI data processing

Preprocessing steps were implemented in Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software [Reference Cox38]. We discarded the first five volumes in each scanning using the AFNI command “3dTcat”. The functional images from each scan were then slice-timing corrected (via AFNI’s 3dTshift), aligned to the second volume using AFNI’s “3dvolreg”, transformed into Talaraich space (Talairach J and P Tournoux 1988) using and resampled to 3 × 3×3mm3 using AFNI’s “adwarp”, and then spatially smoothed with a 6-mm full width at half maximum Gaussian blur (AFNI’s “3dmerge”). Subjects with head motion larger than ±3 mm translation or ±1° rotation were removed. Within our sample two subjects exceeded this threshold (they have been eliminated from the following analysis); data of all the other participants were used for the following analysis. Time series for six estimated parameters of head motion and mean time series from the white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) were regressed out from grey matter. According to previous literature, the data was then filtered with a band-pass filter preserving signals between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz, which was thought to reflect fluctuations of spontaneous brain activity [Reference Cordes, Haughton, Arfanakis, Carew, Turski and Moritz39Reference Zhang and Raichle41].

2.6 Resting state analysis

Resting-state data analysis was performed using AFNI (https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/ afni) (38). First, the global signal variance was calculated to account for possible global changes in all subsequent analyses. Specifically, according to literature, we calculated the SD of BOLD signal changes, a measure of neuronal variability in the resting state.

Following Yang et al. [Reference Yang, Murray, Repovs, Cole, Savic and Glasser42, Reference Yang, Murray, Wang, Glahn, Pearlson and Repovs43] we choose to calculate the global signal using two measures, the mean of the global signal (GM) and the global signal standard deviation (GSD). This yielded no significant differences between groups and between GM and GSD. Because these measures did not reveal significant differences in each subgroups subject-level voxel-wise, SD maps (p <.305) were standardized and normalized onto subject-level z-score maps per brain volume by subtracting the mean voxel-wise SD obtained for the entire brain (global mean of SD), then dividing by the SD across voxels [Reference Taylor, Chen, Cox and Saad44]. That’s the reason why our SD showed negative values within our sample (as reported in Fig. 1 and 2) and in all the analysis that we have done. Next, according to the literature, spherical regions of interest (ROIs) with a radius of 6 mm were placed in the Talairach coordinates of all cortical nodes of each network. We adopted a well-established node template from a previous study [Reference Laird, Eickhoff, Li, Robin, Glahn and Fox45Reference Talairach and Tournoux47], containing 264 putative functional areas (10 mm diameter spheres, 32 voxels per sphere) across the whole brain. We extract 11 different networks: the Default Mode Network (DMN), the Somato Motor Network (SMN), the Salience Network (SN), the Auditory Network (AUD), the Visual Network (VS), the Dorsal Attention Network (DAN), the Ventral Attention Network (VAN), the Cerebellum Network (CB), the Frontal Parietal Temporal Network (FPTC), the Medial Regional Network (MR), the Cingular Operculum Task Control Network (COTC) and the Subcortical Network (SUB). We extracted the SD from all of the nodes and we then calculated the mean SD of all nodes within each resting-state network. Data were then filtered with two separate bands within the standard range of 0.01–0.10 Hz: Slow5 (0.01–0.027 Hz) and Slow4 (0.027–0.073 Hz) [Reference Huang, Davis, Yue, Wiebking, Duncan and Zhang22, Reference Huang, Zhang, Wu, Qin, Wu and Wang23, Reference Martino, Magioncalda, Huang, Conio, Piaggio and Duncan26].

Fig. 1. Networks distribution across groups and differences between AN, BN and CN.

Legend: CN = control subjects; BN = bulimic patients; AN = anorectic patients. CB = Cerebellum Network; COTC = Cingular Operculum Task Control Network; FPTC = Frontal Parietal Temporal Network; SUB = Subcortical Network; DA = Dorsal Attention Network; MR = Medial Regional Network; (MR), VAN = Ventral Attention Network; AUD = Auditory Network; DMN = Default Mode Network; SAL = Salience Network; SS = Somato Sensitive Network.

***p <.05.

Fig. 2. SLOW 5 SLOW 4 differentiation between Patients (AN + BN) and Control (2a) and between AN, BN and CN (2b).

Legend: Fig. 2a. Comparison of Slow 5 and Slow 4 SD values between Patients (PAT = Anorectic patients and Bulimic Patients) and Controls (CN).

*** p <.05. Footnote Fig. 2b Comparison of Slow 5 and Slow 4 SD values between Anorectic patients, Bulimic Patients and Controls. AN = Anorectic patients; BN = Bulimic patients; CN = healthy controls.

*** p <.05

2.7 Statistical analysis

Firstly, Analysis of variance (ANOVA) and age/BDI-controlled Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were performed to compare all variables between AN, BN and controls and between the two groups of subjects distinguished in patients (AN + BN) and controls using one sample t-test. A Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was applied accepting only results with p < 0.001. Only the variables that significantly differed between groups at this step were considered in the further analysis.

Secondly the SD values of the mean in each network and the SD values in each frequency band (Slow5 and Slow4) were entered into a one way ANOVA to compare group-level differences in the variability between each subgroup: AN, BN and CN. The networks’ SD that were significantly different between subgroups were entered into a post hoc test to detect differences in neuronal variability between the various subgroups (i.e., AN, BN and CN). A one sample t-test was then performed to study differences between the two groups of subjects distinguished in patients (AN + BN) and controls. Third, a Pearson correlation analysis was performed between SD and the clinical variables (BMI and hyperactivity) and TCI, EDI-2, TAS-20, ASQ, SCL-90 subscales which differed between probands and controls.

Unless otherwise stated, all resulting t-maps were thresholded at a corrected p-value of < 0.05. That is, the multiple-comparison error was corrected using Monte Carlo simulation as implemented in AFNI program AlphaSim, yielding a family-wise error rate (FWER) at p < 0.05 with a minimal cluster size of 97 voxels. The smoothness used in the AlphaSim was the average smoothness across subjects. The statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

3.1 Psychopathological assessment

We observed significant differences between anorectic subjects, bulimic subjects and healthy controls in education [F = 6.296; p < 0.004], in BMI [F = 68.856; p < 0.000] and physical hyperactivity [F = 10.829; p < 0.000]. (Table 1). Some subscales of the EDI-2 (drive to thinness [F = 34.382; p < 0.000], social insecurity [F = 12.073; p < 0.000], interpersonal distrust [F = 10.652; p < 0.000] and interoceptive awareness [F = 16.220; p < 0.000]) showed significant differences between the three groups with both anorectic and bulimic subjects scoring higher values than healthy subjects (Table 1). Moreover, both anorectic and bulimic subjects also showed higher alexithymia scores compared with healthy subjects [F = 19.150; p < 0.000] (Table 1). Anorectic and bulimic patients showed higher scores in many other psychopathological scales and personality dimensions (Table 2). TCI scales (persistence, cooperativeness and self-transcendence) and ASQ subscales (second nature of relationships and discomfort about intimacy) were non-significantly different.

Table 1 Demographical, clinical and psychopathological characteristicsof the sample.

AN = anorectic patients; BN = bulimic patients; CN = control subjects. BMI = Body Mass Index; EDI-2= Eating Disorder Inventory 2; TAS = Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20.

Table 2 Personality and general psychopathological assessment of the sample.

AN = Anorectic Patients; BN = Bulimic Patients; CN= Healthy Controls. TCI = Temperament and Character Inventory; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory II; SCL-90= Symptom Check List-90; ASQ = Attachment Style Questionnaire.

3.2 Head motion analysis

In order to exclude the possible bias of head motion, we firstly calculated the mean of motion shift and motion rotation of the whole sample (shift: 0.06 ± 0.03; rotation: 0.06 ± 0.02) and then we conducted an ANOVA to detect if there are any differences between groups. No significant differences were detected between AN, BN and healthy subjects in terms of motion shift (AN = 0.06 ± 0.02; BN = 0.07 ± 0.02; CN = 0.06 ± 0.03 p <. 319) and motion rotation (AN = 06 ± 0.02; BN = 0.06 ± 0.01; CN 0.06 ± 0.02; p <.320).

3.3 Global signal - global neuronal variability

In a first step, following Yang et al. [Reference Yang, Murray, Repovs, Cole, Savic and Glasser42, Reference Yang, Murray, Wang, Glahn, Pearlson and Repovs43] we calculated the global signal using two measures, the mean of the global signal (GM) and the global signal standard deviation (GSD). This yielded no significant differences between groups (for GM: F =.696; p <.503; for GSD: F =.871; p <.425) and between GM and GSD (t=-1.307; p <.305).

3.4 Neural networks - neuronal variability

We then investigated neuronal variability operationalized as standard deviation of BOLD signal changes (SD) in the resting state within the 11 networks of interest. ANOVA revealed that the SD differed significantly between anorectic subjects, bulimic subjects and healthy controls in specifically the Ventral Attention Network (VAN) [F = 7.311 p < 0.002] (Fig. 1). No other significant differences were found within the other networks (Table S1).

Following the literature, we also focused on Slow-5 and Slow-4 [Reference Huang, Davis, Yue, Wiebking, Duncan and Zhang22, Reference Huang, Zhang, Wu, Qin, Wu and Wang23, Reference Buzsáki and Draguhn48Reference Zuo, Di Martino, Kelly, Shehzad, Gee and Klein51]. We first performed a two-sample test between patients (anorectic and bulimic subjects together) and healthy subjects for slow 5 and slow 4 (Fig. 2 a) followed by one way ANOVA between the three group separately for slow 5 and slow 4 (Fig. 2 b). This yielded significant SD differences in VAN specifically in slow 4 between patients (AN + BN) and CN: [t=-2.754; p <.008] and within the three subgroups (AN, BN and CN separately): [F = 4.12; p < 0.02]. No significant differences were found in slow 5. For details see also Table S2.

3.5 Neuronal variability in networks - relationship to psychopathology

In a final step, we correlated the neuronal data on VAN SD with psychopathological scores. Significant results showed that altered SD in VAN directly correlated with BMI [p < 0.01] and inversely correlated with TAS total [p < 0.01], physical hyperactivity [p < 0.04], drive of thinness [p < 0.03] and impulsivity [p < 0.03] (Table 3). Other correlation between SD and significantly different psychopathological scores (including some EDI-2 subscales interpersonal distrust, interoceptive awareness and interpersonal distrust, TAS-20 subscale difficulty in describing feelings, some TCI subscales novelty seeking, harm avoidance and self-directedness and ASQ subscales worry about relationship and need for approval) were non-significant.

Table 3 Correlation between VAN and psychopathology in ED patients.

ED = Eating Disorders (Anorectic patients and Bulimic patients); VAN = Ventral Attention Network; TAS-20 = Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20.

4. Discussion

4.1 Main findings

We here investigated resting state activity in patients with Eating Disorders ED with a novel measure, that is, neuronal variability. Moreover we investigated for the first time the global signal as well as neuronal variability in specific networks. This yielded the following main findings: (i) no differences in global signal between ED and healthy subjects; (ii) increased neuronal variability in specifically the Ventral Attention Network (VAN) in ED; and (iii) correlation of increased neuronal variability in VAN with various psychopathological measures.

4.2 Global signal

We for the first time investigated the global signal (GS) in resting state of ED. Following Yang et al. [Reference Yang, Murray, Repovs, Cole, Savic and Glasser42, Reference Yang, Murray, Wang, Glahn, Pearlson and Repovs43], we applied two different measures of GS, that is GS mean and GS SD. Previous investigations yielded increased GS in schizophrenia on both a global level and a regional level [Reference Yang, Murray, Repovs, Cole, Savic and Glasser42, Reference Yang, Murray, Wang, Glahn, Pearlson and Repovs43]. In contrast, patients with bipolar disorder did not exhibit any change in the global component of GS [Reference Yang, Murray, Repovs, Cole, Savic and Glasser42]. However, a recent study showed that GS differed on the regional level in manic and depressed phases of bipolar disorder [Reference Magioncalda, Martino, Conio, Escelsior and Piaggio25].

The present data investigate GS for the first time in ED. We here show no GS changes in ED on the global level. This suggests the GS to be different in ED when compared to the previous findings in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. However, to further strengthen that claim future studies may want to directly compare ED, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder within one and the same investigation. This perspective probably will confirm the hypothesis that GS could have a precise and clear role in the diagnostic classification of psychiatric disorders.

4.3 Neuronal variability in ventral attention network

Our main finding consists in increased neuronal variability in specifically the ventral attention network (VAN). The whole group of ED as well as both subgroups showed increased neuronal variability in specifically the VAN whereas no changes were observed in other networks. Moreover, we observed specific increase in slow 4, the faster frequency, whereas no such changes occurred in the slower frequency of slow 5. This means that in ED patients the faster frequency was affected and not the slower one concerning VAN. This result differs from what has been highlighted in bipolar disorder where the slow 5 is more compromised even if this impairment concerns different networks (posterior Default Mode Network and Salience Network) [Reference Magioncalda, Martino, Conio, Escelsior and Piaggio25]. This difference in the involved frequencies of the VAN may represent a further specific marker for psychiatric disorders to be explored with further studies.

Our findings are partially in contrast with a previous investigation by Collantoni et al. [Reference Collantoni, Michelon, Tenconi, Degortes, Titton and Manara52] who observed reduced functional connectivity in this network in ED. Rather than applying functional connectivity, we here measured neuronal variability which was increased in VAN and which correlated with various psychopathological measures.

The VAN is involved in sudden shifts and redirection between different stimuli [Reference Corbetta and Shulman53]. In this context, VAN works as a mediator between externally- and internally-directed stimuli with their respective activities. Especially the TPJ as core region of the VAN takes on a central role in encoding body-related stimuli and their discrimination form non-bodily-related stimuli [Reference Blanke, Slater and Serino54Reference Graziano and Kastner56]. The impairment of neuronal variability in VAN in ED thus suggests that these patients are less able to redirect and shift their focus from body-related stimuli to non-bodily-related environmental stimuli: their attention is thus fixed and “stuck” on the own body without being able to shift their attention away from the own body. This is certainly well in line with the clinical symptom of an “increased bodily-focus” (see [Reference Northoff57, Reference Northoff58] for such bodily-focus in the context of depression).

Moreover, the assumption of such deficit in internal-external shifting and re-direction is further supported by our findings of correlation. Increased neuronal variability in VAN was significantly correlated with various psychopathological measures like BMI, TAS, and typical eating disorder symptoms like physical hyper intensity, drive of thinness, and impulsivity. This suggests that an impaired neuronal variability in VAN could be the basis of some clinical symptoms of patients with EDs, even if the underlying neurobiological mechanisms should be confirmed in future research.

The psychopathological symptoms in ED may thus be traced to an abnormal temporal feature in VAN neural activity, i.e., increased neuronal variability: if the neuronal activity is less variable, the likelihood of shifting and re-directing attention between internal bodily-related and external, i.e., environmental non-bodily-related stimuli is decreased so that the patients’ focus remain on the internal bodily-related stimuli. That, in turn, induces the various symptoms one can observe in these patients [Reference Abbate-Daga, Buzzichelli, Amianto, Rocca, Marzola and McClintock16, Reference Lang, Lloyd, Khondoker, Simic, Treasure and Tchanturia59]. Accordingly, psychopathological symptoms in ED may have temporal and spatial basis in abnormal spatiotemporal patterns of the resting state’s neural activity – this amounts to what has recently been introduced as “Spatiotemporal psychopathology” [Reference Northoff and Stanghellini12, Reference Northoff and Huang60].

4.4 Conclusions

We here investigate for the first time global signal and neuronal variability in resting state in ED. While we did not find any changes in global signal in ED, our results demonstrated increased neuronal variability in a faster frequency, i.e., slow 4 in the ventral attention network in ED. Moreover, changes in resting state neuronal variability in VAN correlated with various psychopathological measures in ED. This further underlines the central relevance of VAN in ED and its altered capacity of shifting between internal body-related and external environmental non-bodily-related stimuli in ED as it can be observed in psychopathological symptoms. More generally, our findings support the recently suggested novel form of psychopathology, that is spatiotemporal psychopathology [Reference Northoff and Stanghellini12, Reference Blanke, Slater and Serino54] to bridge the gap between neuronal and psychopathological levels.

Two mainstreams of clinical implications may derive from our findings. The first is the possibility to support a sequential treatment of these disorders considering the impairment of the cognitive dimension of EDs as one of the possible motives of the ED psychopathology. In fact neurocognitive impairment may represent a consequence of specific neurofunctional impairment on which both relational and emotional deficits are articulated. In light of this specific therapeutic protocols such as Cognitive Remediaton Therapy may become primary-line interventions in the early stages of the ED treatment, which can be then followed by more dynamic psychotherapeutic approaches.

The second regards the possibility to act on these specific neurofunctional alterations with primarily biological instruments such as TMS, tDCS or DBS in association with psychological and nutritional therapies. In both cases the study of neurofunctional markers of EDs offers the possibility to integrate in a multidisciplinary approach both biological and psychological treatments that have been considered separately from each other from a long time.

4.5 Limitations

The first limitation concerns the relatively short resting state which, based on a repetition time of 2 s, did not yield a high number of data points. Future studies may thus want to apply longer resting state with for instance 15 min and a lower TR (like 1 s).

The second limitation concerns the relative small sample of subjects. Future studies with the same approach need to be done to confirm our preliminary results in order to generalize our hypothesis and considerations on different neurobiological aspects of ED patients.

The third limitation concerns the use of a 1.5 T scanner. Future confirmatory studies should be conducted on scanners of at least 3 T power.

Declarations of interest

None.

Contributors

All the authors contributed to the paper. AVS contributed in sample recruitment, data collection and elaboration and in writing the paper. FA, ZH, FDA, MB, AB, GN, GAD and SF contributed in the data collection and elaboration and in the revision of the paper.

Disclosures

The lead author, AVS, affirms that the manuscript is honest, accurate, and transparent. Any discrepancies of the study have been explained within limitations.

Funding source

The present study was sponsored by the “Compagnia di San Paolo” Bank Foundation with the “Bando Neuroscienze” grant [code: 3929IT/PF 2008.2242] assigned in 2009 to Prof. Secondo Fassino and Dr. Federico Amianto and by IARPsrl generic [20BW320110719008513915033] Casale Monferrato “Resistenza ai trattamenti nei dca”

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr Carmen Settanta, Dr Caglio Marcella and Dr Paola Caroppo for their help in fMRI data acquisition and patients recruitment. The authors wish to acknowledge the fundings of San Paolo Bank Fundation and Cassa di Risparmio di Torino Bank Fundation given to the Professor Fassino for the support to the Pilot Centre for Eating Disorders in the years 2005–2015.

Appendix A Supplementary data

Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.08.005.

References

American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5th ed. 2013, American Psychiatric Publishing Washington, DC.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amianto, FNorthoff, GAbbate Daga, GFassino, STasca, GAIs anorexia nervosa a disorder of the self? A psychological approach. Front Psychol 2016; 14:7849.Google Scholar
Fassino, SAbbate-Daga, GResistance to treatment in eating disorders: a critical challenge. BMC Psychiatry 2013; 7:13282.Google Scholar
Zipfel, SGiel, KEBulik, CMHay, PSchmidt, UAnorexia nervosa: aetiology, assessment, and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry 2015; 2:10991111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaudio, SWiemerslage, LBrooks, SJSchiöth, HBA systematic review of resting-state functional-MRI studies in anorexia nervosa: evidence for functional connectivity impairment in cognitive control and visuospatial and body-signal integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 71:578589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, AAizenstein, HMazurkewicz, LFudge, JFrank, GKPutnam, K et al. Altered insula response to taste stimuli in individuals recovered from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33:513523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Agata, FCaroppo, PAmianto, FSpalatro, ACaglio, MMBergui, M et al. Brain correlates of alexithymia in eating disorders: a voxel-based morphometry study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2015; 69:708716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Favaro, AClementi, MManara, RBosello, RForzan, MBruson, A et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype modifies executive functioning and prefrontal functional connectivity in women with anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2013; 38:241248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, WHWierenga, CEBailer, UFSimmons, ANWagner, ABischoff-Grethe, ADoes a shared neurobiology for foods and drugs of abuse contribute to extremes of food ingestion in anorexia and bulimia nervosa?. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 73:836842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, MDGreicius, MClinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci 2010; 17:419.Google Scholar
Harmelech, TPreminger, SWertman, EMalach, RThe day-after effect: long term, Hebbian-like restructuring of resting-state fMRI patterns induced by a single epoch of cortical activation. J Neurosci 2013; 33:94889497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, GStanghellini, GHow to link brain and experience? Spatiotemporal psychopathology of the lived body. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 28:1076.Google Scholar
Biswal, BBMennes, MZuo, XNGohel, SKelly, CSmith, SM et al. Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010; 107:47344739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carnell, SGibson, CBenson, LOchner, CNGeliebter, ANeuroimaging and obesity: current knowledge and future directions. Obes Rev 2012; 13:4356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boehm, IGeisler, DKing, JARitschel, FSeidel, MDeza Araujo, Y et al. Increased resting state functional connectivity in the fronto-parietal and default mode network in anorexia nervosa. Front Behav Neurosci 2014; 2:8346.Google Scholar
Abbate-Daga, GBuzzichelli, SAmianto, FRocca, GMarzola, EMcClintock, SM et al. Cognitive flexibility in verbal and nonverbal domains and decision making in anorexia nervosa patients: a pilot study. BMC Psychiatry 2011; 7:11162.Google Scholar
Canna, APrinster, AMonteleone, AMCantone, EMonteleone, PVolpe, U et al. Interhemispheric functional connectivity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Eur J Neurosci 2017; 45:11291140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, CZhou, XChen, CDong, QZang, YQiao, S et al. The neural basis of processing anomalous information. Neuroreport 2007; 18:747751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, DDKovacevic, NMcIntosh, ARGrady, CLBlood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. J Neurosci 2010; 30:49144921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, DDKovacevic, NMcIntosh, ARGrady, CLThe importance of being variable. J Neurosci 2011; 31:44964503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, DDSamanez-Larkin, GRMacDonald, SWLindenberger, UMcIntosh, ARGrady, CLMoment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping?. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37:610624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, ZDavis, HHYue, QWiebking, CDuncan, NWZhang, J et al. Increase in glutamate/glutamine concentration in the medial prefrontal cortex during mental imagery: a combined functional mrs and fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36:32043212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, ZZhang, JWu, JQin, PWu, XWang, Z et al. Decoupled temporal variability and signal synchronization of spontaneous brain activity in loss of consciousness: an fMRI study in anesthesia. Neuroimage 2016; 124:693703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, JCorbera, SWexler, BENeural activation abnormalities during self-referential processing in schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res 2014; 222:65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magioncalda, PMartino, MConio, BEscelsior, APiaggio, N et al. Functional connectivity and neuronal variability of resting state activity in bipolar disorder--reduction and decoupling in anterior cortical midline structures. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36:666682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martino, MMagioncalda, PHuang, ZConio, BPiaggio, NDuncan, NW et al. Contrasting variability patterns in the default mode and sensorimotor networks balance in bipolar depression and mania. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113:48244829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yue, YJia, XHou, ZZang, YYuan, YFrequency-dependent amplitude alterations of resting-state spontaneous fluctuations in late-onset depression. Biomed Res Int 2015; 505479.Google Scholar
Northoff, GPsychoanalysis and the brain - why did freud abandon neuroscience?. Front Psychol 2012; 3:71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, GDuncan, NWHow do abnormalities in the brain’s spontaneous activity translate into symptoms in schizophrenia? From an overview of resting state activity findings to a proposed spatiotemporal psychopathology. Prog Neurobiol 2016; 146:2645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 4th ed., text rev. 2010, American Psychiatric Publishing Washington, DC.Google Scholar
First Michael, BSpitzer Robert, LMiriam, GibbonWilliams, JanetBW, Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disordersClinician version (SCID-CV) 1996, American Psychiatric Press, Inc. Washington DC.Google Scholar
Cloninger, CRPrzybeck, TRSvrakic, DMThe Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): a guide to its development and use center for psychobiology of personality 1996, University St. Louis, MO Washington.Google Scholar
Garner, DMEating disorder inventory-2: professional manual 1991, Psychological Assessment Resources Odessa, FL.Google Scholar
Bagby, RMTaylor, GJParker, JDLoiselle, CCross-validation of the factor structure of the Toronto alexithymia scale. J Psychosom Res 1990; 34:4751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, L.R.The SCL-90 manual I: scoring, administration and procedures for the SCL-90 1997, Johns Hopkins Univ. Sch. Med. Baltim: Clin. Psychom. Unit.Google Scholar
Beck, ATSteer, RABrown, GKBDI-II, Beck depression inventory: manual 1996, Consulting Psychologists Press San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Feeney, JANoller, PHanrahan, MAttachment in adults: clinical and developmental perspectives. Assessing adult attachment 1994, Guilford Press New York128152.Google Scholar
Cox, RWAFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 1996; 29:162173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordes, DHaughton, VMArfanakis, KCarew, JDTurski, PAMoritz, CH et al. Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in "resting-state" data. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2001; 22:13261333.Google ScholarPubMed
Fox, MDGreicius, MClinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci 2010; 17:419.Google Scholar
Zhang, DRaichle, MEDisease and the brain’s dark energy. Nat Rev Neurol 2010; 6:1528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, GJMurray, JDRepovs, GCole, MWSavic, AGlasser, MF et al. Altered global brain signal in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014; 111:74387443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, GJMurray, JDWang, XJGlahn, DCPearlson, GDRepovs, G et al. Functional hierarchy underlies preferential connectivity disturbances in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113:E21928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, PAChen, GCox, RWSaad, ZSOpen environment for multimodal interactive connectivity visualization and analysis. Brain Connect 2016; 6:109121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, AREickhoff, SBLi, KRobin, DAGlahn, DCFox, PTInvestigating the functional heterogeneity of the default mode network using coordinate-based meta-analytic modeling. J Neurosci 2009; 29:1449614505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, MWBassett, DSPower, JDBraver, TSPetersen, SEIntrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron 2014; 83:238251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talairach, JTournoux, PCo-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-Dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging 1988, Thieme Medical Publishers Inc. New York.Google Scholar
Buzsáki, GDraguhn, ANeuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 2004; 304:19261929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, YWang, JZhao, ZMin, BLu, JLi, K et al. Frequency-dependent changes in the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a resting-state fMRI study. Neuroimage 2011; 55:287295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoptman, MJZuo, XNButler, PDJavitt, DCD’Angelo, DMauro, CJ et al. Amplitude of low-frequency oscillations in schizophrenia: a resting state fMRI study. Schizophr Res 2010; 117:1320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuo, XNDi Martino, AKelly, CShehzad, ZEGee, DGKlein, DF et al. The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage 2010; 49:14321445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collantoni, EMichelon, STenconi, EDegortes, DTitton, FManara, R et al. Functional connectivity correlates of response inhibition impairment in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res 2016; 247:916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, MShulman, GLControl of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3:201215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanke, OSlater, MSerino, ABehavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness. Neuron 2015; 88:145166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geng, JJVossel, SRe-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: contextual updating?. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37:26082620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, MSKastner, SHuman consciousness and its relationship to social neuroscience: a novel hypothesis. Cogn Neurosci 2011; 2:98113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, GPsychopathology and pathophysiology of the self in depression - neuropsychiatric hypothesis. J Affect Disord 2007; 104:114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, GSelf and brain: what is self-related processing?. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 2011; 15:186187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, KLloyd, SKhondoker, MSimic, MTreasure, JTchanturia, KDo children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa display an inefficient cognitive processing style?. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0131724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, GHuang, ZHow do the brain’s time and space mediate consciousness and its different dimensions? Temporo-spatial theory of consciousness (TTC). Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 80:630645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Networks distribution across groups and differences between AN, BN and CN.Legend: CN = control subjects; BN = bulimic patients; AN = anorectic patients. CB = Cerebellum Network; COTC = Cingular Operculum Task Control Network; FPTC = Frontal Parietal Temporal Network; SUB = Subcortical Network; DA = Dorsal Attention Network; MR = Medial Regional Network; (MR), VAN = Ventral Attention Network; AUD = Auditory Network; DMN = Default Mode Network; SAL = Salience Network; SS = Somato Sensitive Network.***p <.05.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. SLOW 5 SLOW 4 differentiation between Patients (AN + BN) and Control (2a) and between AN, BN and CN (2b).Legend: Fig. 2a. Comparison of Slow 5 and Slow 4 SD values between Patients (PAT = Anorectic patients and Bulimic Patients) and Controls (CN).*** p <.05. Fig. 2b Comparison of Slow 5 and Slow 4 SD values between Anorectic patients, Bulimic Patients and Controls. AN = Anorectic patients; BN = Bulimic patients; CN = healthy controls.*** p <.05

Figure 2

Table 1 Demographical, clinical and psychopathological characteristicsof the sample.

Figure 3

Table 2 Personality and general psychopathological assessment of the sample.

Figure 4

Table 3 Correlation between VAN and psychopathology in ED patients.

Supplementary material: File

Spalatro et al. supplementary material

Spalatro et al. supplementary material
Download Spalatro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.8 MB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.