Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T00:04:52.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early development of negative and positive affect: Implications for ADHD symptomatology across three birth cohorts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 October 2021

Hanna C. Gustafsson*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
Saara Nolvi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, Turku Institute for Advanced Studies, University of Turku, Turku, Finland FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study, Turku Brain and Mind Center, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Elinor L. Sullivan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA Divisions of Neuroscience and Cardiometabolic Health, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, OR, USA
Jerod M. Rasmussen
Affiliation:
Development, Health, and Disease Research Program, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA
Lauren E. Gyllenhammer
Affiliation:
Development, Health, and Disease Research Program, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA
Sonja Entringer
Affiliation:
Development, Health, and Disease Research Program, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Institute of Medical Psychology, Charité University, Berlin, Germany
Pathik D. Wadhwa
Affiliation:
Development, Health, and Disease Research Program, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Department of Epidemiology, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA
Thomas G. O'Connor
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology, Neuroscience, and Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA
Linnea Karlsson
Affiliation:
FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study, Turku Brain and Mind Center, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland Centre for Population Health Research, Turku University Hospital and University of Turku, Turku, Finland Department of Paediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, Turku University Hospital and University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Hasse Karlsson
Affiliation:
FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study, Turku Brain and Mind Center, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland Centre for Population Health Research, Turku University Hospital and University of Turku, Turku, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Turku University Hospital and University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Riikka Korja
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, Turku Institute for Advanced Studies, University of Turku, Turku, Finland FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study, Turku Brain and Mind Center, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Claudia Buss
Affiliation:
Development, Health, and Disease Research Program, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA, USA Institute of Medical Psychology, Charité University, Berlin, Germany
Alice M. Graham
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
Joel T. Nigg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Hanna C. Gustafsson, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road, Multnomah Pavilion, Suite 1400, Mail Code: UHN-80R1 Portland, OR 97239; E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

High levels of early emotionality (of either negative or positive valence) are hypothesized to be important precursors to early psychopathology, with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) a prime early target. The positive and negative affect domains are prime examples of Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) concepts that may enrich a multilevel mechanistic map of psychopathology risk. Utilizing both variable-centered and person-centered approaches, the current study examined whether levels and trajectories of infant negative and positive emotionality, considered either in isolation or together, predicted children's ADHD symptoms at 4 to 8 years of age. In variable-centered analyses, higher levels of infant negative affect (at as early as 3 months of age) were associated with childhood ADHD symptoms. Findings for positive affect failed to reach statistical threshold. Results from person-centered trajectory analyses suggest that additional information is gained by simultaneously considering the trajectories of positive and negative emotionality. Specifically, only when exhibiting moderate, stable or low levels of positive affect did negative affect and its trajectory relate to child ADHD symptoms. These findings add to a growing literature that suggests that infant negative emotionality is a promising early life marker of future ADHD risk and suggest secondarily that moderation by positive affectivity warrants more consideration.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Individual differences in emotionality (also called reactivity in a reactivity-regulation temperament model) emerge very early in development, can be reliably assessed in infants as young as three months of age, and are hypothesized to be transdiagnostic markers of risk for psychopathology (Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard, Reference Cicchetti, Ackerman and Izard1995; Frick, Reference Frick2004; Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003; Nigg, Reference Nigg2006; Nigg, Goldsmith, & Sachek, Reference Nigg, Goldsmith and Sachek2004; Rothbart & Posner, Reference Rothbart, Posner, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; Rothbart & Sheese, Reference Rothbart, Sheese and Gross2007; Rothbart, Posner, & Hershey, Reference Rothbart, Posner, Hershey and Cohen1995; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter and Dollar2016; Whittle, Allen, Lubman, & Yücel, Reference Whittle, Allen, Lubman and Yücel2006). Bottom-up processes such as emotional reactivity, and top-down processes such as effortful control, promote the development of self-regulation in a dynamic manner with bidirectional influences during early development. The emergence of effective self-regulation is a process that may have an expectable range of emotionality with which to contend. When emotional reactivity is very strong, it may disrupt the consolidation of effortful control and associated self-regulation functions (Nigg, Reference Nigg2006; Nigg, Karalunas, et al., Reference Nigg, Karalunas, Gustafsson, Bhatt, Ryabinin, Mooney and Wilmot2020; Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Posner, Hershey and Cohen1995).

Effortful control and related self-regulatory processes are crucial to adjustment, and weak effortful control is a hallmark feature of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (where it is characterized as inattention, impulsivity, or later in development, as poor executive functioning). Further, although emotional reactivity is not part of the diagnostic criteria for ADHD, it is a widely recognized clinical feature that appears to be part of the syndrome (and, historically, has been recognized as such) (Nigg, Sibley, Thapar, & Karalunas, Reference Nigg, Sibley, Thapar and Karalunas2020; Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg, & Leibenluft, Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014). Thus, extremes of reactivity in early infancy are a hypothesized candidate to predict subsequent ADHD and related problems (Frick & Morris, Reference Frick and Morris2004; Kostyrka-Allchorne, Wass, & Sonuga-Barke, Reference Kostyrka-Allchorne, Wass and Sonuga-Barke2020; Nigg, Reference Nigg2006; Nigg et al., Reference Nigg, Goldsmith and Sachek2004; Rothbart, Reference Rothbart2011; Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Posner, Hershey and Cohen1995). To test that hypothesis, however, requires prospective studies from infancy into the early school age years when ADHD is typically first identified. Such studies have been few and thus the present study seeks to move toward filling that gap.

The above-described literature provides ample support for the inclusion of negative and positive valence systems as exemplar domains in the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013; Kozak & Cuthbert, Reference Kozak and Cuthbert2016). Thus, an RDoC-based approach to studying ADHD can profit from consideration of the early emergence of temperamental reactivity in both positive and negative valence domains (Nigg, Sibley, et al., Reference Nigg, Sibley, Thapar and Karalunas2020). Although an association between infant emotionality and internalizing and externalizing symptomatology has been frequently hypothesized (Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Ackerman and Izard1995; Frick & Morris, Reference Frick and Morris2004; Rothbart & Posner, Reference Rothbart, Posner, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter and Dollar2016) and has received some initial empirical support (e.g., Edwards & Hans, Reference Edwards and Hans2015; Morales et al., Reference Morales, Tang, Bowers, Miller, Buzzell, Smith and Fox2021), its association specifically with emerging ADHD symptomatology in children has been very little studied in prospective cohorts.

The core hypothesis guiding the present study is derived from those findings in addition to studies of ADHD in childhood. Children with ADHD were recalled by caregivers in one study to have displayed high levels of negative affect early in life (Gurevitz, Geva, Varon, & Leitner, Reference Gurevitz, Geva, Varon and Leitner2014). Studies of children with family history of ADHD suggest that at-risk infants as young as 6 to 7 months old display more distress to limitation (a measure of anger/frustration) as well as fewer regulatory behaviors during a task intended to elicit fear or sadness (Auerbach, Atzaba-Poria, Berger, & Landau, Reference Auerbach, Atzaba-Poria, Berger and Landau2004; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Holton, Nousen, Barling, Sullivan, Propper and Nigg2015). As noted, prospective data relating infant temperament and child ADHD symptoms are limited. However, Willoughby, Gottfredson, Stifter, and Investigators (Reference Willoughby, Gottfredson, Stifter and Investigators2017) utilized latent profile analysis (LPA) to summarize temperament data collected between 6 and 36 months of age. These authors report that infants who displayed increased fear and anger exhibited more ADHD symptoms when they were in the first grade.

Research examining the association between infant positive affect and ADHD is much more limited, with only two studies to our knowledge that have addressed this question. Frick, Forslund, and Brocki (Reference Frick, Forslund and Brocki2019) found that maternal report of higher positive affect in 10-month-old infants predicted more ADHD symptoms at 36 months of age (Frick et al., Reference Frick, Forslund and Brocki2019). Miller and colleagues (Miller, Degnan, Hane, Fox, & Chronis-Tuscano, Reference Miller, Degnan, Hane, Fox and Chronis-Tuscano2019) found that parent-rated positive emotionality at 4 months of age predicted more ADHD symptoms at 9 years – but only in girls who experienced less sensitive parenting. Interestingly, neither of these studies found a direct effect of negative emotionality on child ADHD symptoms, highlighting heterogeneity in existing study results. Consideration of relative levels, or balance between infant positive and negative affect, may help to reduce such conflicting findings, and has significant potential to lead to more comprehensive characterization of early emotion-related phenotypes that confer risk for development of ADHD symptoms.

Although individual rankings in temperament scores tend to exhibit stability over time (Pedlow, Sanson, Prior, & Oberklaid, Reference Pedlow, Sanson, Prior and Oberklaid1993), within-individual development of temperament follows a characteristic course in the first year of life (Lemery, Goldsmith, Klinnert, & Mrazek, Reference Lemery, Goldsmith, Klinnert and Mrazek1999; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter and Dollar2016). Previous research suggests that both negative (Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund, & Karrass, Reference Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund and Karrass2010; Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Bridgett, Rothbart, Robertson, Iddins, Ramsay and Schlect2010; Gartstein, Hancock, & Iverson, Reference Gartstein, Hancock and Iverson2018) and positive affect (Bridgett, Laake, Gartstein, & Dorn, Reference Bridgett, Laake, Gartstein and Dorn2013; Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Hancock and Iverson2018; Sallquist et al., Reference Sallquist, Eisenberg, Spinrad, Gaertner, Eggum and Zhou2010) normatively increase across the first year of the child's life. However, positive and negative valence domains are not unitary; rather, for many purposes a more granular, trait-level analysis (e.g., fear, anger/frustration) may be helpful for capturing psychopathology risk (Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter and Dollar2016). Individual dimensions of affectivity, which have been shown to differentiate early infancy (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1997), show unique patterns of change across the first year of life (Gartstein & Hancock, Reference Gartstein and Hancock2019) that are presumed to be related to differential maturation of their underlying neural mechanisms (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Buss, Rasmussen, Entringer, Ramirez, Marr and Wadhwa2019; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Allen, Lubman and Yücel2006).

Although individual differences in the development of negative and positive affect over the first year of life have been noted and have been related to other symptoms of psychopathology (e.g., internalizing and externalizing symptoms in toddlerhood (Giesbrecht, Letourneau, Dewey, & the APrON Study Team, Reference Giesbrecht, Letourneau and Dewey2020), they have not yet been applied to the study of ADHD. Consideration of these trajectories may be important. While prior studies have looked at a single early time point in infancy in relation to later ADHD symptoms, little work has been done to explore how early trajectory variation may set the stage for later ADHD symptomatology. In addition, little attention has been paid to trajectories of positive and negative emotionality in the context of one another, particularly as they may be related to ADHD symptoms.

These gaps in our knowledge lead to the following two research questions: (a) Does considering both the level and trajectory of infant emotionality provide unique information about ADHD symptomatology, or is a static assessment early in infancy sufficient for signaling risk? One possibility is that the answer varies by valence domain. (b) Does considering the developmental course of negative and positive affect together provide additional information about ADHD symptomatology than can be gained by examining the trajectory of either positive or negative emotion traits in isolation?

We consider here both variable-centered and person-centered statistical approaches. Variable-centered approaches report information about average levels of change over time, which can be used to predict factors such as emerging ADHD symptomatology. However, variable-centered approaches can obscure important individual differences in developmental course that can be more readily identified using a person-centered statistical approach (Magnusson, Reference Magnusson2003). Person-centered approaches have the advantage of allowing for the identification of subgroups of individuals who differ in their initial level and pattern of change over time. These models can also consider multiple trajectories in a single model to examine heterogeneity in both. The resultant subgroups can then be compared to one another on ADHD symptoms. Looking at both variable- and person-centered perspectives in the same study provides the most comprehensive characterization of the development of negative and positive affect in relation to ADHD risk. Following previous reports, we included measures of infant sadness, fear, and distress to limitation (dimensions of negative affect) as well as smiling/laughter and high-intensity pleasure (dimensions of positive affect) to provide a more fine-grained perspective on the early development of infant emotionality.

An ongoing and important issue for the field of developmental psychopathology surrounds the issue of replication (Amir & Sharon, Reference Amir and Sharon1990; Francis, Reference Francis2012; Shrout & Rodgers, Reference Shrout and Rodgers2018). Our primary analyses relied on pooled data from participants recruited from Oregon and California (primary analysis). We attempted to replicate the key results in a large Finnish population cohort (replication analysis).

Primary Analysis: Method

Participants

The participants in the primary analysis were 191 children that were pooled from two prospective studies of offspring of women recruited in the first or second trimester of pregnancy whose offspring were followed at regular intervals until 4–8 years of age. The first cohort came from a sample in Oregon (n = 57) (Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Sullivan, Battison, Holton, Graham, Karalunas and Nigg2020). Exclusion criteria included high-risk or medically complicated pregnancy, extreme life circumstances (specifically, homelessness), active substance use (i.e., alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, opioids), and being less than 18 years old. In order to maximize the variation in infant temperamental reactivity and regulation in early life in this sample, an effort was made to over-select women with a family or personal history of ADHD or elevated symptoms of ADHD, as defined by the mother or father carrying a current or childhood diagnosis of ADHD or by the mother scoring ≥80th percentile on the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV) Quick Screen (Barkley, Reference Barkley2011) at enrollment. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Oregon Health & Science University. Participants who at the time of these analyses had completed at least one infant temperament assessment (n = 57) were included in this analysis. A subsample of these children (n = 33) were assessed for ADHD symptoms at 6 years of age. The second cohort was recruited in California (n = 134) (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Rasmussen, Rudolph, Heim, Gilmore, Styner and Buss2018). Exclusionary criteria for infants were birth before 34 weeks’ gestation, and evidence of a congenital, genetic or neurologic disorder. Participants who at the time of these analyses had completed at least one infant temperament assessment were included. A subsample of these children (n = 79) were assessed for ADHD symptoms at 4 to 8 years of age. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of California, Irvine.

Measures

Infant temperament

Infant negative and positive affect were assessed using the revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) (Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003) which was administered in the Oregon study at 3, 6, and 12 months of age and in the California sample at 3, 6, 9, and 12 months of age. The current analyses utilized the fear, distress to limitation, and sadness subscales as measures of negative affect. We used the smiling/laughing and high-intensity pleasure subscales as measures of positive affect. These dimensions were selected based on previous literature implicating them in the emergence of ADHD symptomatology.

Child ADHD dimension

When children were approximately 6 years old (mean age = 6.12 years, SD = .13; range = 5.97–6.35) for the Oregon sample and between 4 and 8 years old for the California sample (mean age = 6.92 years, SD = 1.07; range = 4.73–8.56), mothers completed the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; n = 33 Oregon sample, n = 79 California sample) (Goodman, Reference Goodman2001). The hyperactivity/inattention subscale (hereafter, SDQ hyperactivity) was used in the current analyses as a proxy for level of ADHD dimensional symptomatology. Twenty-four percent of the Oregon sample had SDQ hyperactivity at or above the clinical cut point (8 or above) (Silva, Osório, & Loureiro, Reference Silva, Osório and Loureiro2015), as compared to 8% of the California sample, χ 2 = 4.34, p = .04.

Covariates

Mothers reported on their current depressive symptoms using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). CES-D scores were collected at the time of the 6- and 12-month assessments in the Oregon sample and at 3-, 6-, and 12-month assessments in the California sample. These scores were averaged to capture maternal depressive symptoms across the child's first year of life and were controlled for in analyses predicting SDQ hyperactivity as a way to account for possible effects of maternal mood on reports of infant temperament. Child age at the time of the ADHD assessment (in years) and child sex (0 = female, 1 = male) were also included as covariates, given potential sex differences in ADHD symptom severity (Arnett, Pennington, Willcutt, DeFries, & Olson, Reference Arnett, Pennington, Willcutt, DeFries and Olson2015).

Analytic strategy

Combining the cohorts

The decision to combine the Oregon and California samples was made to increase sample size and statistical power, and to capture a wider range of symptoms and ADHD risk. Unsurprisingly, the cohorts differed on the child SDQ hyperactivity (Oregon mean = 4.34, SD = 3.20, California mean = 3.08, SD = 2.61; t = 2.10, p = .04) and on IBQ high-intensity pleasure at 3 months; sadness at 6 months, and fear and smiling at 6 and 12 months (all p < .05). However, the correlations among the IBQ-R subscales, and between the IBQ-R subscales and child SDQ hyperactivity scores, were grossly similar across the two cohorts. Though these comparisons support the decision to combine the datasets, the data collection site was controlled for in all analyses predicting child SDQ hyperactivity scores to account for potential variability in findings attributable to site.

Research question 1: Relating baseline and change in emotionality to ADHD symptoms

The average trajectories of each dimension of negative and positive affect were modeled using latent curve modeling (LCM) (Bollen & Curran, Reference Bollen and Curran2006), a variable-centered statistical technique. Unconditional models were first estimated to establish the functional form of each trajectory; each IBQ-R subscale (i.e., distress to limitation, fear, sadness; smiling/laughter, high-intensity pleasure) was considered in its own model due to prior evidence that these dimensions may have distinct neural underpinnings and implications for subsequent mental health risk (Nigg, Reference Nigg2006; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Buss, Rasmussen, Entringer, Ramirez, Marr and Wadhwa2019; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Allen, Lubman and Yücel2006). Both linear and quadratic effects were tested. Models were estimated using Mplus 8.5 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998-2017) with the robust maximum likelihood estimator. Full information maximum likelihood (Allison, Reference Allison2003) was used to handle missing data. Model fit was evaluated using the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). CFI and TLI values above .90 and RMSEA values below .08 indicate adequate model fit (Browne & Cudeck, Reference Browne, Cudeck, Bollen and Long1993; Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). To test associations between each IBQ dimension and child SDQ hyperactivity scores, a series of conditional LCMs were estimated (one for each dimension of affect), where the SDQ hyperactivity score was regressed on the intercept and slope of the IBQ scale, as well as on child sex, age at the SDQ assessment, data collection site, and maternal depressive symptoms during infancy.

To test whether there were subgroups of individuals who differed in their initial levels of and/or their slope of each IBQ dimension, we used latent class growth analysis (LCGA) (Jung & Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008), a person-centered statistical technique. Again, separate LCGAs were estimated for each IBQ dimension. To determine the best class-solution, Bayesian information criteria (BIC) values and the results of Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin likelihood ratio tests (VLMR LRT) for the k-class versus k−1 class model were examined. The best class solution is generally one that has a lower BIC value, paired with a significant VLMR LRT, and where all resulting classes contain at least 5% of the sample (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2012; Jung & Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008). One- through five-class unconditional LCGA models were fit to the data. After the best-class solution was selected, the resulting classes were compared to one another on their average child SDQ hyperactivity scores using Mplus. Dummy coded variables that contrasted the various classes were created and the SDQ hyperactivity score was regressed on these dummy coded variables in a series of multiple regressions that also controlled for child sex, age, maternal depressive symptoms, and data collection site. This approach was selected over comparing these classes using an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), despite their conceptual similarity, because it allowed us to account for missing data and therefore yielded model results that more closely parallel the LCM results.

Research question 2: Evaluating joint contribution of components of positive and negative affect and their trajectories

The second research question was also tested using LCGA. Six LCGAs were estimated. In each model, two trajectories were considered, one for a single dimension of negative affect (distress to limitation, fear, or sadness) and one for a single dimension of positive affect (smiling/laughter or high-intensity pleasure); the six models corresponded to the six combinations of these variables (e.g., fear and smiling/laughter; sadness and high-intensity pleasure). Resultant classes were compared to one another on child SDQ hyperactivity (controlling for child sex, age, maternal depressive symptoms, and data collection site), as above.

Primary analysis: Results

Sample description

Sample demographics and average SDQ scores appear in Table 1. Mean levels of IBQ-R scores at each assessment time point appear in Table 2.

Table 1. Sample demographics

Note: SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

Table 2. Mean Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) scores for the US and Finnish samples

Research question 1: Trajectories of infant negative or positive affect

LCMs (variable-centered analyses)

The parameters and results from the LCMs that examined the trajectory of the individual subscales of infant affect are described in detail in Online Supplement A and are depicted in Figure 1. On average, each dimension of affect increased significantly across the first year of the child's life, though the shape of the trajectory varied across individual dimensions of affect. Specifically, a linear model appeared to characterize distress to limitation and sadness trajectories best, while a model that included both a linear and a quadratic slope term best characterized fear, smiling/laughter, and high-intensity pleasure. In the case of the nonlinear trajectories, there was an increase in each construct over time, but this rate of change appeared to slow toward the end of the first year of life.

Figure 1. Primary analysis, research question 1: Visual depiction of the model-implied average trajectories from the latent curve models.

Note: IBQ-R = revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire.

The intercept (B = 1.44, p = .01) but not slope (p = .87) of sadness predicted greater SDQ hyperactivity scores, controlling for child sex, age, maternal depressive symptoms, and data collection site. Neither the intercept nor the slope of the other dimensions of affect were associated with SDQ hyperactivity scores (ps > .28).

LCGAs (person-centered analyses)

Results from the LCGAs that considered one dimension of affect at a time suggest that for some, but not all, dimensions of affect there were meaningful subgroups of infants who differed from one another in their overall level and slopes. Detailed description of the results of these models appear in Online Supplement A and LCGA model fit statistics appear in Supplemental Table S1. Specifically, fear was best characterized by three classes (“low, stable,” “low increasing,” and “high, stable”), sadness by two classes (“high, stable” and “lower, increasing”) and smiling/laughter by three classes (“low, stable,” “moderate, increasing,” and “high, increasing”). These results are depicted in Figure 2. The models that considered distress to limitations and high-intensity pleasure did not yield meaningful subgroups.

Figure 2. Primary analysis, research question 1: Depiction of results of latent class growth analyses that considered a single dimension of affect.

Note: IBQ-R = revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire.

The presence of these subgroups suggests that there is meaningful variability in individual level and trajectory of these dimensions of infant emotionality. However, these classes did not differ in terms of their average SDQ hyperactivity scores (ps > .35; means are presented by class in Supplemental Table S3), suggesting that the rate of change in a single dimension of affect does not provide meaningful additional information about emergent ADHD risk.

Research question 2: LCGAs that considered both positive and negative affect

LCGAs (person-centered analyses)

The LCGAs that considered different combinations of positive and negative affect trajectories also suggested that there were meaningful subgroups in some, but not all models. The LCGA that considered distress to limitation with smiling/laughter and the LCGA that included sadness with smiling/laughter both yielded four-class solutions, whereas the LCGA that included fear with smiling/laughter and the LCGA with sadness and high-intensity pleasure yielded 2-Class solutions. The distress to limitation with high-intensity pleasure and fear with high-intensity pleasure models did not produce meaningful subgroups. See Online Supplement B for full details related to these analyses, Supplemental Table S2 for fit statistics and Supplemental Figures S2–S4 for a depiction of the resultant classes. Mean SDQ hyperactivity scores for each class are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Primary analysis, research question 2: Raw attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptom means presented by subgroup for latent class growth analysis that considered both positive and negative affect

Note: SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

Subgroup differences in SDQ hyperactivity scores

Interesting differences in SDQ hyperactivity scores emerged between the classes capturing different combinations of distress to limitation and smiling/laughter, which yielded a four-class solution (see Figure 3): Class 1 (41%; n = 78) “moderate, increasing distress/high, increasing smiling”; Class 2 (17%; n = 33) “high, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling”; Class 3 (12%; n = 23) “low, increasing distress/high, increasing smiling”; and Class 4 (30%; n = 57) “moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling”). Specifically, Class 4 (“moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling”) had significantly lower SDQ hyperactivity scores than Class 2 (17%) (“high, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling”) (B = −1.67, p = .027), controlling for age, sex, site, and maternal depressive symptoms. None of the other classes differed from one another in terms of ADHD symptoms, ps > .28.

Figure 3. Primary analysis, research question 2: Depiction of results of latent class growth analysis considering both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter (four-class solution). Note: LCGA = latent class growth analysis. SDQ hyperactivity = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, hyperactivity/inattention subscale. SD = standard deviation. Distress = distress to limitation subscale of the revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) (Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003) smiling = smiling/laughter subscale of the IBQ-R. Means and SDs presented here are raw means (unadjusted for covariates).

Summary of primary analysis

Results from our primary analyses suggest that the individual dimensions of negative and positive emotionality show different patterns of change over the first year of life, a finding that supports our decision to consider these dimensions individually. This conclusion is based on observed differences in the intercept and/or slope of these dimensions, differences in the shape of the trajectories (linear vs. quadratic), as well as differences in the subgroups produced using LCGA. The intercept (3-month assessment), but not slope, of sadness predicted greater SDQ hyperactivity scores in middle childhood, suggesting rate of change in a single dimension of affect does not provide meaningful additional information about emergent ADHD risk in this sample. If replicated, this result may suggest that even a single assessment of emotionality, obtained during the first months of life, may be helpful for identifying at-risk children.

Results also suggest that there were subgroups of individuals who differed in their intercept and/or slope of both negative and positive emotionality. Of note, there were four classes of infants who differed in terms of their distress to limitation and smiling/laughter. Two of these groups (one whose distress to limitation was higher than their smiling/laughter, and the other who exhibited moderate, increasing distress and moderate smiling/laughter) differed on their average SDQ hyperactivity scores, suggesting that additional information is gained by considering both positive and negative affect in the same model.

Based on these results, and in an effort to address issues of nonreplicability in psychological research, we aimed to test similar research questions using data from a replication sample. Specifically, we looked to see if both level and trajectory of individual dimensions of affect were associated with emerging ADHD symptoms. To most comprehensively characterize associations between early affect and child ADHD symptomatology, the decision was made to examine both overall level and change in each dimension of affect as they may relate to child symptomatology rather than focusing only on the sadness–ADHD association observed in the primary analyses (i.e., analyses were conducted “blind” to the results of the primary analysis). To replicate our second set of findings, we also examined whether there were subgroups of individuals who showed different patterns of distress to limitation and smiling/laughter, and if so, whether they differed on SDQ hyperactivity scores.

Replication Analysis: Method

Participants

The replication sample is part of a longitudinal FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study that researches environmental and genetic influences on child brain and behavioral development (Karlsson et al., Reference Karlsson, Tolvanen, Scheinin, Uusitupa, Korja and Ekholm2018). Pregnant women were recruited to the study after their participation in an ultrasound at gestational week 12. The inclusion criteria were a verified pregnancy and knowledge of Finnish and/or Swedish, the official languages in Finland. The parents gave their own consent and consented on behalf of their child. All procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Hospital District of Southwest Finland. Participants included in the current study (n = 1,032) were selected based on the available assessment of infant temperament at both 6 and 12 months and child ADHD symptom assessment at 5 years of age. Children with known congenital central nervous system or major health conditions diagnosed in infancy were excluded.

Measures

Infant temperament was assessed using the IBQ-R at 6 and 12 months of child age. ADHD symptoms were assessed using the SDQ hyperactivity subscale at 5 years (mean age = 5.21 years, SD = .20, range = 4.69–6.01 years). Child sex and age at the SDQ assessment as well as maternal depressive symptoms were considered as covariates. Maternal depressive symptoms at 6 and 12 months were assessed using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) (Cox, Holden and Sagovsky, Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987) at 6 and 12 months. Scores on the EPDS were averaged, as was done for the primary analyses. Infant gestational age at birth (weeks) was also included as a covariate, given known variation in this cohort (33–42 gestational weeks).

Analytic strategy

Because the study used for the replication analyses did not collect at least three assessments of temperament in the first year of life (which is required to model trajectories), LCM could not be conducted. Instead, change scores that capture the change in that dimension from 6 to 12 months were calculated for each construct. Both the baseline measure of each trait at 6 months and the change score (controlling for the baseline of each trait) were then entered into a general linear model predicting SDQ hyperactivity scores. Child sex, age at the SDQ assessment, maternal depressive symptoms, and gestational age at delivery were included as covariates in these models. Each dimension of affect (distress to limitation, fear, sadness, smiling/laughter, and high-intensity pleasure) was considered in a separate model, with multiple comparisons corrected using Bonferroni adjustment. To impute the missing data in the covariates (mainly age at SDQ assessment), Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) multiple imputation with 10 datasets was used, with virtually no change in the results when compared to nonimputed data.

To replicate the results addressing research question 2 in the primary analysis, and specifically the finding that considering both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter trajectories in the same model may help in the prediction of child ADHD symptoms, a latent class analysis (LCA) (Lanza & Cooper, Reference Lanza and Cooper2016) including distress to limitation and smiling/laughter at 6- and 12-month time points was conducted. The models were estimated using Mplus 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2017) using the robust maximum likelihood estimator. The latent class analysis classes were examined based on the same indices as in the primary analysis (BIC and VLMR LRT). Finally, child SDQ hyperactivity scores at 5 years were regressed on dummy coded variables that captured the various contrasts between the classes, controlling for the same factors as the models described above. The class sizes and means are reported from the unconditional models, although the classes remained highly similar in the models with predictors.

Replication Analysis: Results

Sample description

Sample characteristics are displayed in Table 1. Mean levels of IBQ-R scores at each assessment time point appear in Table 2. Fifty-nine (5.7%) children in the sample scored at or above 8 on the SDQ hyperactivity. As was observed in the primary analyses, children on averaged increased in each dimension of negative and positive emotionality between 6 and 12 months of age (ps < .001).

Research question 1: Relating baseline and change in emotionality to ADHD symptoms

Regression models (variable-centered analysis)

Results from the regression models that related baseline and change in emotionality to SDQ hyperactivity scores at age 5 years are displayed in Table 4. There was a significant positive association between distress to limitation and sadness at 6 months and SDQ hyperactivity scores at 5 years, even after controlling for child sex, age, maternal depressive symptoms, and gestational age at birth. Infant smiling/laughter at 6 months was negatively associated with ADHD symptoms. However, when the change scores were examined, only the increase in distress to limitation from 6 to 12 months positively predicted SDQ hyperactivity scores when controlled for the baseline and the covariates (p = .009). In this model, the 6-month score was also associated with child SDQ hyperactivity scores (p < .001) which suggests that both level and change are important here.

Table 4. Replication analysis: The general linear model for the baseline of infant emotional reactivity at 6-months and the change of emotional reactivity from 6 to 12 months of age and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) hyperactivity/inattention symptoms at 60 months (n = 1,039)

Note: aadjusted for child sex, gestational age, maternal Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) (averaged from 6- and 12-month time points) and child age at the ADHD symptom assessment. Higher change scores reflect more positive change in the temperament trait in question.

Research question 2: LPAs that considered both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter

LPAs (person-centered analyses)

Fit statistics associated with the LPAs are presented in Table 5. The LPA suggested a four-class solution (see Figure 4 for a depiction of these classes). In Class 1 (29%; n = 295), “moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling,” distress to limitation at 6 months was moderate (intercept = 4.09) and increased to 12 months (intercept = 4.52) and smiling was moderate (intercept = 4.67) and only slightly increased to 12 months (intercept = 5.13). In Class 2 (17%; n = 173) “moderate, increasing distress/low, increasing smiling,” where distress to limitation was moderate (intercept = 3.68) and increased to 12 months (intercept = 4.28), and 6-month smiling/laughter was low (intercept = 2.92) and increased to 12 months (intercept = 3.68). In Class 3 (35%; n = 365) “low, increasing distress/moderate, increasing smiling,” distress to limitation at 6 months was low (intercept = 2.88) and increased to 12 months (intercept = 3.35), and smiling/laughter was moderate (intercept at 6 months = 4.01) and increased to 12 months (intercept = 4.59). Finally, in Class 4 (19%; n = 199) “low, increasing distress/high, stable smiling” distress to limitation at 6 months was low (intercept = 2.72) and increased to 12 months (intercept = 3.22), and the smiling/laughter was high at 6 months (intercept = 5.67) and remained stable (intercept at 12 months = 5.82).

Figure 4. Replication analysis: Depiction of results of latent profile analysis considering both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter at 6 and 12 months of Age. Note: C1 (29%) = “moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling,” C2 (17%) = “moderate, increasing distress/low, increasing smiling,” C3 (35%) = “low, increasing distress/moderate, increasing smiling,” and C4 (19%) = “low, increasing distress/high, stable smiling.”

Table 5. Replication analysis: Fit statistics for the latent class analysis including distress to limitations and smiling/laughter at 6 and 12 months

Note: BIC = Bayesian information criterion, VLMR LRT = Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin likelihood ratio test for the k versus k−1 class solution. The bolded values describe the class solution best supported by the fit statistics.

Similar to the primary analysis, the class that showed higher distress than smiling (Class 2) had significantly higher SDQ hyperactivity scores than Class 4 (B = .18, p = .022). Class 1 also had significantly higher SDQ hyperactivity scores than Class 4 (B = .21, p = .002). The other class comparisons were not significant (ps > .06).

Summary of replication analysis

Consistent with previous research and the results of the primary analyses, the replication analysis found that, on average (variable-centered analysis), there was a significant increase in all dimensions of affect over the first year of life. Similar to the primary analysis, the replication analysis found that early negative emotionality was predictive of emerging ADHD symptomatology. However, the specific dimensions of negative affect were different (distress to limitation vs. sadness), and in the replication study, both the baseline (6 months) and the change (from 6 to 12 months) in negative affect were related to greater ADHD symptoms at 5 years. Though the results of the primary analysis were not replicated in this case, these results are broadly consistent with the primary analysis conclusion that early life measures of negative emotionality may be helpful for identifying at-risk children.

Results from the (person-centered) LPAs more closely replicated the findings of the primary analysis and confirm the utility of considering both positive and negative emotionality in the same model. Several parallels between these results and those of the primary analysis were observed. First, both sets of analyses resulted in a four-class solution, and the resultant classes captured different combinations of positive and negative affect. In both sets of models, two classes exhibited stable or relatively stable positive affect, whereas the other two classes showed growth in smiling/laughter. As was true for the results of the primary analysis, child distress to limitations increased in each class, but initial levels varied across classes. In addition, in both sets of analyses, there was one class that showed a unique pattern of higher levels of distress than smiling/laughter. Most importantly, both the primary and replication analyses found that these groups differed from one another on their average SDQ hyperactivity scores, suggesting that important information is gained by considering both positive and negative affect in the same model.

Discussion

Dysregulation of both positive and negative emotionality has been hypothesized to be a route to ADHD and other disorders (Nigg, Reference Nigg2006; Nigg et al., Reference Nigg, Goldsmith and Sachek2004; Nigg, Karalunas, et al., Reference Nigg, Karalunas, Gustafsson, Bhatt, Ryabinin, Mooney and Wilmot2020). The present study partially supports that hypothesis and provides relatively novel information by prospectively examining these associations from infancy to early childhood and by confirming results in an international sample. Utilizing both variable-centered and person-centered approaches to analyzing longitudinal infant temperament data, the goal of the current study was to examine whether trajectories of infant negative and positive temperament traits (which evaluate emotionality), considered either in isolation or together, were associated with children's ADHD symptoms in middle childhood as estimated by the SDQ hyperactivity scale. Two important findings emerged that provided partial support for our hypotheses.

Our first research question centered on whether the level and/or trajectory of infant emotionality provided unique information about risk for ADHD symptomatology. Results from variable-centered analyses suggest that overall level of negative emotionality, but not rate of change across the first year, signaled risk for ADHD. Specifically, infant sadness at 3 months of age predicted SDQ hyperactivity scores at 4 to 8 years of age, but the rate of change (in this and the other dimensions of emotionality) was not related to ADHD symptoms when each dimension was considered in a trajectory model by itself. Contrary to our hypothesis, infant positive emotionality was not associated with ADHD symptoms in our primary analyses and was negatively associated with ADHD symptoms in our replication cohort. Aside from the possibility that the primary sample was too small to detect positive emotion effects, it is also possible that the meaning of positive emotions in infancy is somewhat different than later in life, or that low positive affect signals later behavior problems. However, given lack of replication these surmises require further evaluation.

The person-centered analysis that considered each dimension of emotionality separately also was consistent with the notion that overall level of emotionality was more predictive of ADHD symptoms than trajectories were. We make this statement based on the fact that the individual subgroups, many of whom differed in terms of slope, did not differ from one another on SDQ hyperactivity scores. This finding did not replicate (in the Finnish cohort both baseline and the change in distress to limitation was related to greater ADHD symptoms at 5 years of age). Perhaps lower power was a factor in the US sample. While these two samples converged on the idea that early negative affect may be an important marker of risk, caution is needed in interpreting these results, particularly in light of the nonreplication. Additional caution is needed in interpreting the lack of slope findings in our primary analyses, due to this study's observation that there was limited variability in individual trajectories of child temperament in this sample (as indicated by a nonsignificant slope variance in some models), which may have limited our ability to detect such effects.

Our second research question centered on whether considering the developmental course of negative and positive affect together provided additional information about ADHD symptomatology than was gained by examining the trajectory of either positive or negative emotion traits in isolation. Across several person-centered trajectory models (which considered different combinations of the various dimensions of emotionality) we found that there were subgroups of individuals who differed from one another in the intercept and/or slope of both positive and negative affect. Of note, in the case of the model that considered both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter, we were able to identify subgroups who differed significantly from one another on their ADHD symptoms. In the original sample, the “high, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling” class exhibited significantly more ADHD symptoms than the “moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling” groups; the two classes characterized by high, increasing smiling (and varying levels of distress) had average ADHD scores that were between the means for these two classes. In the replication sample, the classes with “moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling” and “moderate, increasing distress/low, increasing smiling” exhibited greater ADHD symptoms than the group with “moderate, increasing distress/high, stable smiling.” This pattern suggests that, only when exhibiting moderate, stable or low levels of positive affect (vs. high, increasing levels), does negative affect and its trajectory appear to be related to child ADHD symptoms. Further, the pattern of having higher distress than smiling also appears to predispose children to ADHD symptoms. Though the most marked difference between the classes in our analyses appears to be the infant's distress to limitation, these ADHD differences only emerged in the models that considered both positive and negative affect (and are only apparent under conditions of moderate, stable smiling or higher distress than smiling). Thus, these findings support the hypothesis that considering both positive and negative affect is important for determining ADHD risk. These results do not provide direct support for our hypothesis that high levels of positive affect signal risk for ADHD.

Replication is a prevailing issue in developmental psychopathology where replications of findings are rarely attempted or reported. To address this, we utilized data from a large cohort of children living in Finland to replicate a portion of the findings (replication analysis). The sample core measures (the IBQ-R for infant temperament and the SDQ for ADHD symptoms) were used across studies, though the timing and frequency of the measurement of temperament (6 and 12 months) and ADHD (5 years) in this study were slightly different than in the primary analysis. Through parallel, but not identical, analytic models we found evidence of some consistency of results, which provides us with greater confidence in our study conclusions. However, full replication was not possible due to the different timing of data collection (and specifically, the inability to model trajectories) in the study used in the replication analysis, which may explain some of the differences in the consequent findings. In addition, there are cultural factors that may play a role in explaining the slight differences across cohorts, which were recruited from different countries. First, there are differences in the age of school entry between the Finland and the United States; 5-year-olds in Finnish society do not yet attend formal school, whereas 6-year-old children in the United States typically do. The school setting typically places higher pressure on child's attention and inhibitory control and thus may amplify the ADHD symptoms reflected in caregiver reports. Differences such as these must be taken into account in the interpretation of the findings. Second, some differences in the levels of emotionality were observed between the US and Finnish cohorts (though of note, the change over time appears similar). These differences may reflect cultural factors and expectations and could contribute to a partially differential pattern observed in the consequent analyses.

This study has a number of strengths. Despite great interest in the developmental origins of ADHD and related disorders, this is the first study to examine trajectories of infant temperament in relation to ADHD symptoms and the first to show that the combination of negative and positive emotionality may be helpful for understanding heterogeneity in ADHD symptoms. This is also the first study to report a prospective association between 3-month temperament and child ADHD symptoms, which is earlier in development than previous studies have reported. This is a nontrivial contribution of this study, as it suggests that, if this finding is replicated, that ADHD risk may be able to be identified and intervened upon earlier than previously described. Further, this study used both person-centered and variable-centered approaches to understanding developmental trends in temperament. Though variable-centered trajectory has gained in popularity in this research area, this is only the second paper to our knowledge to look at subgroups of trajectories or latent classes of temperament, and the first to test for subgroups in the individual dimensions of affectivity. Our study is also the first to examine such trajectories as they related to child functioning years later. Another strength of this study was our use of a replication sample which, despite some differences in data collection and analysis, broadly validated our study's conclusions.

Despite these strengths, this study had several limitations. We relied on parent report of both temperament and ADHD symptoms. Though parent report is commonly used in clinical settings, and we controlled for maternal depressive symptoms in analyses to help alleviate concerns of a common reporter bias, future research should replicate these findings using observer ratings of temperament and/or data provided by other reporters. This study focused on infant temperament during the first year of life (a developmental period with particular relevance, given the substantial development of emotionality during this time period and previously described association with later ADHD); however, different insights may be gained if later assessments of temperament (including assessments of later-emerging dimensions of temperament, such as effortful control) were also considered. Future research should also consider a number of possible moderators, including child sex and early caregiving behaviors, as well as the moderating and/or mediating role of executive functioning/effortful control in transmitting the influence of temperament on later ADHD risk. In the current analyses, we controlled for maternal depression as a way to minimize possible bias in parental reports of temperament; however, maternal depressive symptoms have also been shown to contribute to infant temperament and its development (Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Bridgett, Rothbart, Robertson, Iddins, Ramsay and Schlect2010), so its inclusion here may have weakened meaningful associations. Although all of the LCGA classes produced in our primary analyses met recommended criteria for the percentage of participants in each class (i.e., at least 5%), some classes included a small number of participants due to the modest sample size of our primary cohort.

Summary and Conclusion

Results from this study add to a growing literature that suggests that it may be possible to detect ADHD risk early in life and that infant emotionality is one important marker of such risk. This study reports the earliest prediction of ADHD symptoms from temperament measures to date (3-month sadness), highlights the importance of considering individual differences in overall level of emotionality in infancy, and provides evidence that considering both infant positive and negative affect together can yield important information about later risk for ADHD symptomatology. Results were broadly confirmed using a large replication sample, validating the conclusions of this study. However, additional research investigating potential mediators and moderators of the associations described herein is needed.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001012

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health, via grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (HG, K01-MH120507), (ES & JN, R01-MH117177 and R01-MH124824), (JN, R37-59105), (CB & PDW, R01-MH091351 and R01-MH105538), the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (JR, K99-HD100593), (PDW, R01-HD060628), (SE & PDW, R01-HD065825), (LG, K99-HD097302) and the Office of the Director (PDW, CB, & TOC, UH3-OD023349); the Abracadabra Foundation (JN); the Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation (HK, SN, LK); the Emil Aaltonen Foundation (SN); State Grants for Clinical Research (ERVA) (SN, LK, HK); the Academy of Finland (HK, grant number 26080089), (RK, grant number 308252), (LK, grant number 308176); the Jane and Aatos Erkko Foundation (HK); and The Finnish Cultural Foundation.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Footnotes

This is an updated version of the original article. For details please see the notice at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001425

References

Allison, P. D. (2003). Missing data techniques for structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 545557. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amir, Y., & Sharon, I. (1990). Replication research: A” must” for the scientific advancement of psychology. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 51.Google Scholar
Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Willcutt, E. G., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2015). Sex differences in ADHD symptom severity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 632639. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12337CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2012). Using Mplus TECH11 and TECH14 to test the number of latent classes. Mplus Web Notes, 14, 22.Google Scholar
Auerbach, J. G., Atzaba-Poria, N., Berger, A., & Landau, R. (2004). Emerging developmental pathways to ADHD: Possible path markers in early infancy. Neural Plasticity, 11, 2943. doi:10.1155/NP.2004.29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R. A. (2011). Barkley adult ADHD rating scale-IV (BAARS-IV). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2006). Latent curve models: A structural equation perspective (Vol. 467). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 791. doi:10.1037/a0019673CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Laake, L. M., Gartstein, M. A., & Dorn, D. (2013). Development of infant positive emotionality: The contribution of maternal characteristics and effects on subsequent parenting. Infant and Child Development, 22, 362382. doi:10.1002/icd.1795CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In Bollen, K. A. & Long, J. S. (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Focus Editions.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., & Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 110. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 11, 18. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-11-126sCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, R. C., & Hans, S. L. (2015). Infant risk factors associated with internalizing, externalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems in young children. Developmental Psychology, 51, 489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, G. (2012). The psychology of replication and replication in psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 585594. doi:10.1177/1745691612459520CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, M. A., Forslund, T., & Brocki, K. C. (2019). Can reactivity and regulation in infancy predict inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive behavior in 3-year-olds? Development and Psychopathology, 31, 619629. doi:10.1017/S0954579418000160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J. (2004). Integrating research on temperament and childhood psychopathology: Its pitfalls and promise. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 27. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 5468. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, M. A., Bridgett, D. J., Rothbart, M. K., Robertson, C., Iddins, E., Ramsay, K., & Schlect, S. (2010). A latent growth examination of fear development in infancy: Contributions of maternal depression and the risk for toddler anxiety. Developmental Psychology, 46, 651668. doi:10.1037/a0018898CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, M. A., & Hancock, G. R. (2019). Temperamental growth in infancy: Demographic, maternal symptom, and stress contributions to overarching and fine-grained dimensions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 65, 121157. doi:10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.65.2.0121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gartstein, M. A., Hancock, G. R., & Iverson, S. L. (2018). Positive affectivity and fear trajectories in infancy: Contributions of mother–child interaction factors. Child Development, 89, 15191534. doi:10.1111/cdev.12843CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 6486. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giesbrecht, G. F., Letourneau, N., Dewey, D., & the APrON Study Team (2020). Latent class trajectories of infant temperament and associations with problem behavior at two years of age. Development and Psychopathology, 116. doi:10.1017/S0954579420000991Google Scholar
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 13371345. doi:10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, A. M., Rasmussen, J. M., Rudolph, M. D., Heim, C. M., Gilmore, J. H., Styner, M., … Buss, C. (2018). Maternal systemic interleukin-6 during pregnancy is associated with newborn amygdala phenotypes and subsequent behavior at 2 years of age. Biological Psychiatry, 83(2), 109119. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.05.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurevitz, M., Geva, R., Varon, M., & Leitner, Y. (2014). Early markers in infants and toddlers for development of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 18, 1422. doi:10.1177/1087054712447858CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafsson, H. C., Sullivan, E. L., Battison, E. A., Holton, K. F., Graham, A. M., Karalunas, S. L., … Nigg, J. T. (2020). Evaluation of maternal inflammation as a marker of future offspring ADHD symptoms: A prospective investigation. Brain, behavior, and immunity, 89, 350356. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.07.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L. t., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, T., & Wickrama, K. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 302317. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson, L., Tolvanen, M., Scheinin, N. M., Uusitupa, H. M., Korja, R., Ekholm, E.FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study Group. (2018). Cohort profile: the FinnBrain birth cohort study (FinnBrain). International journal of epidemiology, 47(1), 1516j. doi:10.1093/ije/dyx173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kostyrka-Allchorne, K., Wass, S. V., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2020). Research review: Do parent ratings of infant negative emotionality and self-regulation predict psychopathology in childhood and adolescence? A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 401416. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozak, M. J., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2016). The NIMH research domain criteria initiative: Background, issues, and pragmatics. Psychophysiology, 53, 286297. doi:10.1111/psyp.12518CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanza, S. T., & Cooper, B. R. (2016). Latent class analysis for developmental research. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 5964. doi:10.1111/cdep.12163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament. Developmental Psychology, 35, 189. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnusson, D. (2003). The person approach: Concepts, measurement models, and research strategy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2003, 323. doi:10.1002/cd.79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, N. V., Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Fox, N. A., & Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2019). Infant temperament reactivity and early maternal caregiving: Independent and interactive links to later childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60, 4353. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, S., Tang, A., Bowers, M. E., Miller, N. V., Buzzell, G. A., Smith, E., … Fox, N. A. (2021). Infant temperament prospectively predicts general psychopathology in childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2017). Mplus user's guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395422. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sachek, J. (2004). Temperament and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The development of a multiple pathway model. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 4253. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T., Karalunas, S. L., Gustafsson, H. C., Bhatt, P., Ryabinin, P., Mooney, M. A., … Wilmot, B. (2020). Evaluating chronic emotional dysregulation and irritability in relation to ADHD and depression genetic risk in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 205214. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T., Sibley, M. H., Thapar, A., & Karalunas, S. L. (2020). Development of ADHD: Etiology, heterogeneity, and early life course. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 2, 559583. doi:10.1146/annurev-devpsych-060320-093413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedlow, R., Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Stability of maternally reported temperament from infancy to 8 years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 998. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.6.998CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: Temperament and personality in development. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2006). Temperament, attention, and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Developmental neuroscience (pp. 465501). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. L. (1995). Temperament, attention, and developmental psychopathology. In Cohen, D. J. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, pp. 465501). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Sheese, B. E. (2007). Temperament and Emotion Regulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Sallquist, J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Gaertner, B. M., Eggum, N. D., & Zhou, N. (2010). Mothers’ and children's positive emotion: Relations and trajectories across four years. Social Development, 19, 799821. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00565.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P., Stringaris, A., Nigg, J., & Leibenluft, E. (2014). Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 276293. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shrout, P. E., & Rodgers, J. L. (2018). Psychology, science, and knowledge construction: Broadening perspectives from the replication crisis. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 487510. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011845CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, T. B., Osório, F. L., & Loureiro, S. R. (2015). SDQ: Discriminative validity and diagnostic potential. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 811. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00811CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stifter, C., & Dollar, J. (2016). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Developmental Psychopathology, 162. doi:10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy411Google Scholar
Sullivan, E. L., Holton, K. F., Nousen, E. K., Barling, A. N., Sullivan, C. A., Propper, C. B., & Nigg, J. T. (2015). Early identification of ADHD risk via infant temperament and emotion regulation: A pilot study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 949957. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, E., Buss, C., Rasmussen, J. M., Entringer, S., Ramirez, J. S., Marr, M., … Wadhwa, P. D. (2019). Newborn amygdala connectivity and early emerging fear. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 37, 100604. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, S., Allen, N. B., Lubman, D. I., & Yücel, M. (2006). The neurobiological basis of temperament: Towards a better understanding of psychopathology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 511525. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M. T., Gottfredson, N. C., Stifter, C. A., & Investigators, F. L. P. (2017). Observed temperament from ages 6 to 36 months predicts parent-and teacher-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in first grade. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 107120. doi:10.1017/S0954579415001236CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample demographics

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) scores for the US and Finnish samples

Figure 2

Figure 1. Primary analysis, research question 1: Visual depiction of the model-implied average trajectories from the latent curve models.Note: IBQ-R = revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Primary analysis, research question 1: Depiction of results of latent class growth analyses that considered a single dimension of affect.Note: IBQ-R = revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire.

Figure 4

Table 3. Primary analysis, research question 2: Raw attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptom means presented by subgroup for latent class growth analysis that considered both positive and negative affect

Figure 5

Figure 3. Primary analysis, research question 2: Depiction of results of latent class growth analysis considering both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter (four-class solution). Note: LCGA = latent class growth analysis. SDQ hyperactivity = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, hyperactivity/inattention subscale. SD = standard deviation. Distress = distress to limitation subscale of the revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) (Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003) smiling = smiling/laughter subscale of the IBQ-R. Means and SDs presented here are raw means (unadjusted for covariates).

Figure 6

Table 4. Replication analysis: The general linear model for the baseline of infant emotional reactivity at 6-months and the change of emotional reactivity from 6 to 12 months of age and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) hyperactivity/inattention symptoms at 60 months (n = 1,039)

Figure 7

Figure 4. Replication analysis: Depiction of results of latent profile analysis considering both distress to limitation and smiling/laughter at 6 and 12 months of Age. Note: C1 (29%) = “moderate, increasing distress/moderate, stable smiling,” C2 (17%) = “moderate, increasing distress/low, increasing smiling,” C3 (35%) = “low, increasing distress/moderate, increasing smiling,” and C4 (19%) = “low, increasing distress/high, stable smiling.”

Figure 8

Table 5. Replication analysis: Fit statistics for the latent class analysis including distress to limitations and smiling/laughter at 6 and 12 months

Supplementary material: File

Gustafsson et al. supplementary material

Gustafsson et al. supplementary material

Download Gustafsson et al. supplementary material(File)
File 31.1 KB