Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T23:12:12.430Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Leukocyte telomere length and personality: associations with the Big Five and Type D personality traits

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2017

D. Schoormans*
Affiliation:
Department of Medical and Clinical psychology, CoRPS – Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic diseases, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
J. E. Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
J. Denollet
Affiliation:
Department of Medical and Clinical psychology, CoRPS – Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic diseases, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
L. van de Poll-Franse
Affiliation:
Department of Medical and Clinical psychology, CoRPS – Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic diseases, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands Netherlands Comprehensive Cancer Organization (IKNL), Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Psychosocial Research and Epidemiology, The Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
B. W. J. H. Penninx
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: D. Schoormans, Ph.D., Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, Tilburg University, PO Box 90153, 5000 LE, Tilburg, The Netherlands. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Backgrounds

Accelerated cellular ageing, which can be examined by telomere length (TL), may be an overarching mechanism underlying the association between personality and adverse health outcomes. This 6-year longitudinal study examined the relation between personality and leukocyte telomere length (LTL) across time among adults with a wide age-range.

Methods

Data from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety were used and included patients with a depression and/or anxiety disorder and healthy controls. Overall, 2936 persons (18–65 years, 66% female) had data on LTL at baseline and 1883 persons had LTL at 6-year follow-up. The Big Five personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) and Type D personality were assessed.

Results

Neuroticism was negatively (B = −2.11, p = 0.03) and agreeableness was positively (B = 3.84, p = 0.03) related to LTL measured across two time points, which became just non-significant after adjusting for somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress (B = −1.99, p = 0.06; and B = 3.01, p = 0.10). Type D personality was negatively (B = −50.16, p < 0.01) related to LTL across two time points, which still remained statistically significant after full adjustment (B = −47.37, p = 0.01). Associations did not differ by age, gender, and current psychiatric status.

Conclusions

The Big Five traits high neuroticism and low agreeableness, and Type D personality were associated with shorter LTL measured across a 6-year period. Associations with the Big Five traits became non-significant after controlling for somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress, yet similar trends were observed. Type D personality remained independently associated with shorter LTL after full adjustment.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

Personality can be defined in various ways, but the most widely accepted personality framework is the Five Factor Model (John et al. Reference John, Naumann, Soto, John and Pervin2008). This model consists of the following five personality traits: neuroticism (emotionally unstable and easily anxious), extraversion (outgoing), conscientiousness (efficient and organized), agreeableness (friendly and compassioned), and openness to experience (inventive and curious), the so-called ‘Big Five’. There is increasing evidence that aspects of personality are associated with adverse health outcomes. In detail, individuals who score low on conscientiousness have an increased risk of early mortality (Kern & Friedman, Reference Kern and Friedman2008; Jokela et al. Reference Jokela, Batty, Nyberg, Virtanen, Nabi, Singh-Manoux and Kivimaki2013). Furthermore, high neuroticism (Wilson et al. Reference Wilson, Mendes De Leon, Bienias, Evans and Bennett2004; Chapman et al. Reference Chapman, Fiscella, Kawachi and Duberstein2010) and low agreeableness (Weiss & Costa, Reference Weiss and Costa2005) have been related to an increased risk of mortality, although these findings were not confirmed by a meta-analysis (Jokela et al. Reference Jokela, Batty, Nyberg, Virtanen, Nabi, Singh-Manoux and Kivimaki2013). Moreover, the Big Five personality traits high neuroticism and low conscientiousness have been related to adverse cardiac events among cardiac patients (Jokela et al. Reference Jokela, Pulkki-Raback, Elovainio and Kivimaki2014; McCann, Reference Mccann2014).

More recently, the distressed (Type D) personality was introduced when studying the role of personality in a cardiovascular population (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005). Type D personality refers to the combination of the traits negative affectivity and social inhibition (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005). People scoring high on negative affectivity have a tendency to experience negative emotions, whereas individuals who score high on social inhibition have a tendency to inhibit self-expression. Persons with high scores on both personality traits are classified as having a Type D personality (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005). Although Type D personality is positively and negatively related to the Big Five traits neuroticism and extraversion respectively, it is a distinct construct (De Fruyt & Denollet, Reference De Fruyt and Denollet2002). Studies show mixed findings, where some studies report that persons with a Type D personality have an increased risk of mortality (Denollet, Reference Denollet2013; Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, Pedersen, Vrints and Conraads2013), others did not (Pelle et al. Reference Pelle, Pedersen, Schiffer, Szabo, Widdershoven and Denollet2010). Furthermore studies have linked Type D personality to poor outcomes in aging-related somatic illnesses, such as cardiovascular disease (CVD) and cancer (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005; Mols et al. Reference Mols, Oerlemans, Denollet, Roukema and Van De Poll-Franse2012).

Although the mechanisms underlying the associations between personality traits and health are not fully understood, both behavioural and pathophysiological mechanisms may be involved. For example, higher levels of agreeableness have been related to lower levels of obesity (Sutin et al. Reference Sutin, Ferrucci, Zonderman and Terracciano2011) and might thus be a protective factor, while higher levels of neuroticism, lower levels of agreeableness, and Type D personality have been associated with higher levels of inflammation (Conraads et al. Reference Conraads, Denollet, De Clerck, Stevens, Bridts and Vrints2006; Sutin et al. Reference Sutin, Terracciano, Deiana, Naitza, Ferrucci, Uda, Schlessinger and Costa2010). Furthermore, high neuroticism and Type D personality have been associated with poor health behaviours such as smoking (Lerman et al. Reference Lerman, Caporaso, Audrain, Main, Boyd and Shields2000; Pedersen et al. Reference Pedersen, Lemos, Van Vooren, Liu, Daemen, Erdman, Smits, Serruys and Van Domburg2004). Accelerated cellular ageing may be underlying the relation between these personality traits and poor health outcomes. According to the exposome paradigm of cellular aging, the above mentioned behavioural and pathophysiological factors are exposures possibly impacting cellular ageing (Lyon et al. Reference Lyon, Starkweather, Montpetit, Menzies and Jallo2014), as telomere length (TL) can be used as a proxy of cellular ageing (Sanders & Newman, Reference Sanders and Newman2013). Telomeres cap the ends of DNA, protecting it from damage (Blackburn, Reference Blackburn2001). During each cell division the telomere shortens, with an average yearly attrition rate of 14–20 base pairs per year found in cross-sectional studies and a yearly LTL attrition rate of 32–46 base pairs reported in longitudinal studies (Blackburn, Reference Blackburn1991; Cawthon et al. Reference Cawthon, Smith, O'brien, Sivatchenko and Kerber2003; Muezzinler et al. Reference Muezzinler, Zaineddin and Brenner2013). When telomeres are at a critically short length, cells become susceptible to senescence and apoptosis (Blackburn, Reference Blackburn2001). Moreover, chromosome ends can activate DNA damage-response pathways, in turn this can lead to genetic mutations and altered gene expression, thus leading to susceptibility to various diseases (Lyon et al. Reference Lyon, Starkweather, Montpetit, Menzies and Jallo2014).

The first studies relating personality to TL showed mixed results. Sadahiro et al. found a cross-sectional relation between low conscientiousness and shorter TL, yet unexpectedly reported that higher scores on neuroticism were related to longer TL (Sadahiro et al. Reference Sadahiro, Suzuki, Enokido, Matsumoto, Shibuya, Kamata, Goto and Otani2015). In contrast, results of the longitudinal study by van Ockenburg et al. showed that persons who score high on neuroticism have shorter TL (Van Ockenburg et al. Reference Van Ockenburg, De Jonge, Van Der Harst, Ormel and Rosmalen2014). Two cross-sectional null-studies relating TL to the traditional Big Five in an elderly population (Savolainen et al. Reference Savolainen, Eriksson, Kajantie, Pesonen and Raikkonen2015) and the less traditional Type D personality trait in patients with chronic heart failure (Huzen et al. Reference Huzen, Van Der Harst, De Boer, Lesman-Leegte, Voors, Van Gilst, Samani, Jaarsma and Van Veldhuisen2010) were also published. In the latter study, the authors also did not find a relation between depression and TL (Huzen et al. Reference Huzen, Van Der Harst, De Boer, Lesman-Leegte, Voors, Van Gilst, Samani, Jaarsma and Van Veldhuisen2010), whereas previous studies did report a negative association between the two (Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Epel, Lin, Wolkowitz and Penninx2014; Schutte & Malouff, Reference Schutte and Malouff2015). As we know that personality traits (e.g. neuroticism and Type D personality) can be a risk factor for poor mental health, such as the development of depression (Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, Sys, Stroobant, Rombouts, Gillebert and Brutsaert1996; Denollet, Reference Denollet2005; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006) information on mental health is warranted when relating personality to TL. Discrepancies among these studies relating personality to TL may be inherent to the study design as affirmative studies included younger individuals. These disparities may be intrinsic to whether or not researchers controlled for lifestyle factors as in the study by Sadahiro et al. significant associations were found while adjusting for age and gender (Sadahiro et al. Reference Sadahiro, Suzuki, Enokido, Matsumoto, Shibuya, Kamata, Goto and Otani2015), whereas Savolainen and colleagues reported non-significant results while adjusting additionally for education, presence of chronic illnesses, depressive symptoms, and the lifestyle factors: BMI, alcohol use, smoking and physical activity (Savolainen et al. Reference Savolainen, Eriksson, Kajantie, Pesonen and Raikkonen2015).

The objective of this 6-year longitudinal study is to examine the relation between personality (i.e. the Big Five and Type D personality traits) and TL measured in leukocytes (LTL) across two time points among adults with a wide age-range, with and without the inclusion of lifestyle factors. In line with the current literature, we hypothesized that lower conscientiousness is related to shorter LTL. Moreover, we speculated that higher neuroticism, lower agreeableness, and Type D personality may be related to shorter LTL, given their associations with adverse health outcomes. Additionally, we examined whether the relation between personality and LTL differed by having a depression and/or anxiety disorder. Finally, we examined whether psychiatric status influenced the relation between personality and LTL.

Methods

Participants

Data are from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA), an ongoing longitudinal multi-site cohort study examining predictors, course and consequences of depression and anxiety. A detailed description of NESDA can be found elsewhere (Penninx et al. Reference Penninx, Beekman, Smit, Zitman, Nolen, Spinhoven, Cuijpers, De Jong, Van Marwijk, Assendelft, Van Der Meer, Verhaak, Wensing, De Graaf, Hoogendijk, Ormel, Van Dyck and Consortium2008). In short, the NESDA sample consists of 2981 persons between 18 and 65 years old, recruited between September 2004 and February 2007 from community, primary and specialized mental health care settings, including persons with a psychiatric diagnosis (depressive and/or anxiety disorder) and healthy controls. Exclusion criteria were; not being fluent in Dutch, and having a primary clinical diagnosis of another other severe mental disorder (e.g. bipolar disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, severe substance use disorder, or a psychotic disorder), which were either self-reported or reported by their mental health practitioner. A detailed description of the number of excluded participants for each site is described elsewhere (Penninx et al. Reference Penninx, Beekman, Smit, Zitman, Nolen, Spinhoven, Cuijpers, De Jong, Van Marwijk, Assendelft, Van Der Meer, Verhaak, Wensing, De Graaf, Hoogendijk, Ormel, Van Dyck and Consortium2008). The NESDA study was approved by the local Ethics Committees of each participating center and all participants signed an informed consent form.

At baseline, data collection entailed a medical examination, blood draw, filling-out questionnaires and an interview. Overall, 2936 persons had LTL at baseline and 1883 persons had LTL at 6-year follow-up (FU6). Persons who did not participate at FU6 had longer baseline LTL, were slightly younger, had less educational years, were less physically active, were less often a former smoker, and were more often a non-drinker (all p’s < 0.05). They moreover scored slightly higher on neuroticism, yet slightly lower on extraversion, openness to experience, and agreeableness (all p’s < 0.01). Groups did not differ with respect to Type D personality, gender, somatic health, body mass index (BMI), and recent life stress (all p’s > 0.05).

Measurements

Personality

Big Five personality traits

The Big Five personality traits were measured by the revised NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). This widely used 60-item questionnaire is answered on a 5-point Likert scale (range 1–5) and is designed to measure the five personality traits: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and Mccrae1995). All five scales show good internal consistency (Costa & Mccrae, Reference Costa and Mccrae1992). The NEO-FFI was measured at baseline and at 2-year follow up (FU2). At FU4 only the neuroticism and extraversion subscales were re-assessed. To maximize the sample size, an average personality score across time was calculated. That is, mean scores for each personality trait were calculated by dividing the sum of all measurements by the number of available measurements across time. The Big Five personality traits are stable across time (Rantanen et al. Reference Rantanen, Metsapelto, Feldt, Pulkkinen and Kokko2007), which was confirmed in this sample by the strong correlations between scales at all time points (all r’s > 0.70, see online Supplementary Table S1). Mean personality traits correlated very strongly with each individual time point (all r’s > 0.91, see online Supplementary Table S1).

Type D personality

The distressed (Type D) personality, was assessed at FU6 by the Dutch 14-item Type D personality scale (DS14) (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005). Type D personality is the tendency to experience both negative affectivity (NA) and social inhibition (SI) (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005) and is stable over time (Kupper et al. Reference Kupper, Boomsma, De Geus, Denollet and Willemsen2011). Items are answered on a 5-point Likert scale (range 0–4). In this study we used two methods to assess Type D personality. First, Type D personality was defined as a cut-off score of ⩾10 on both subscales of the DS14, and coded as a dichotomous variable labeling individuals with v. without a Type D personality (Denollet, Reference Denollet2005). Second, we analyzed the continuous scores on negative affectivity and social inhibition, as well as their interaction term (NA × SI), which reflects a continuous proxy measure of Type D personality (Smith, Reference Smith2011; Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, Pedersen, Vrints and Conraads2013).

Leukocyte telomere length

At baseline and FU6 LTL was measured. Fasting blood samples were drawn from participants between 8:30 and 9:30 AM. Peripheral blood mononuclear cells from all samples were isolated from whole blood using density-gradient centrifugation (with Ficoll-Plaque PLUS) and stored in a −20 °C freezer. DNA was extracted with Gentra PureGene (samples collected 2003–2004), Qiagen FlexiGene (2005–2007), Gentra PureGene (2007–2009) and Chemagen (2009–2014). Quality control checks assured us good quality of the extracted DNA. LTL was determined using an adapted quantitative polymerase chain reaction (PCR) (Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Epel, Lin, Wolkowitz and Penninx2014). LTL at baseline was determined at the laboratory of Telomere Diagnostics, Inc (Menlo Park, CA) and at FU6 at the University of California, San Francisco. Each patients’ sample (T) was run in triplicate wells in the 384-well assay plate and compared with a single gene copy number (S), relative to a reference sample to generate a T/S ratio proportional to mean LTL (Cawthon, Reference Cawthon2002; Aviv et al. Reference Aviv, Hunt, Lin, Cao, Kimura and Blackburn2011). A detailed description of this method is described elsewhere (Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Epel, Lin, Wolkowitz and Penninx2014). The T/S ratio was converted to base pairs (bp) using this formula: bp = 3274 + 2413 × ((T/S − 0.0545)/1.16).

Reliability of both assays was adequate: the inter-assay coefficient variation (CV) (baseline: CV = 4.6%; 6-year: CV = 3.0%) was sufficiently low as shown by the included quality control DNA samples on each PCR run. Similar conclusions can be drawn based on the telomere (baseline: CV = 2.0%; FU6: CV = 5.4%) and the single-gene assay (baseline: CV = 1.6%; FU6: CV = 4.8%) separately. There were systematic differences between baseline and FU6 LTL measurements, as different reference samples were used. Therefore, T/S ratios at FU6 were adjusted relative to the baseline samples, by rerunning and comparing baseline sample plates (n = 226, up to eight samples from each of the baseline plates) to FU6. On average, the T/S ratios of the FU6 runs were at 76% of the T/S ratios of baseline, hence the follow-up T/S ratios were divided by 0.76 to correct for systematic differences. Importantly, DNA samples were de-identified, the laboratories that performed the assays were blind to all other measurements, and case and control samples were randomly distributed over plates.

Covariates

Covariates were assessed at both baseline and FU6 through self-reports via questionnaires or during the interview. Education was assessed in years of education during the interview. The number of medical conditions (i.e. diabetes, osteoarthritis, stroke, cancer, heart, chronic lung, intestinal or thyroid diseases) for which a person was receiving medical treatment was counted during the interview, and was a proxy for somatic health. Furthermore, lifestyle factors were assessed. BMI was calculated by dividing a persons’ weight by their squared height measured at medical examination. Alcohol consumption based on questionnaire data was grouped based on the number of drinks per week, into non-drinker, mild-moderate (<14 and <21 drinks per week for women and men, respectively) and severe drinker (⩾14 and ⩾21 drinks per week for women and men, respectively). Smoking status assessed by a questionnaire was categorized into never smoked, former smoker, and current smoker. Former smokers at baseline were categorized if they indicated to have ever smoked but are currently not smoking. At follow-up they were categorized as former smoker if they answered smoker on a previous measurement but at the current follow-up measurement report that they currently do not smoke. Physical activity as assessed by the International Physical Activity questionnaire (IPAQ) (Craig et al. Reference Craig, Marshall, Sjostrom, Bauman, Booth, Ainsworth, Pratt, Ekelund, Yngve, Sallis and Oja2003) was expressed as overall energy expenditure in Metabolic Equivalent Total (MET) minutes per week. Recent life stress, defined as the count of 12 negative life events during the past year as assessed during the interview by using the Brugha questionnaire (Brugha & Cragg, Reference Brugha and Cragg1990) was considered, as this has shown to be related to LTL (Van Ockenburg et al. Reference Van Ockenburg, Bos, De Jonge, Van Der Harst, Gans and Rosmalen2015; Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Van Oppen, Puterman, Elzinga and Penninx2015b ). Finally, depression and anxiety disorders were assessed at baseline and FU6 by the DSM-IV Composite International Diagnostic Interview version 2.1 and included major depressive disorder, dysthymia, panic disorders, social phobia, agoraphobia, and generalized anxiety disorder. In the NESDA study, psychiatric disorder status (depressed and/or anxious) has been negatively associated with LTL (Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Epel, Lin, Wolkowitz and Penninx2014; Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Van Oppen, Epel, Wolkowitz and Penninx2015a ). To see whether any associations between personality and LTL differed by psychiatric disorder status, we categorized persons as having a current (6-month period) psychiatric diagnosis (depressed and/or anxious). Additionally, because psychiatric status is related to LTL in this patient sample (Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Epel, Lin, Wolkowitz and Penninx2014), and personality (e.g. neuroticism) can be seen as a risk factor for developing psychiatric conditions (Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, Sys, Stroobant, Rombouts, Gillebert and Brutsaert1996; Denollet, Reference Denollet2005; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006), psychiatric status could influence the relation between personality and LTL. We therefore included psychiatric status as a covariate in statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses

Sample characteristics for those included at baseline (n = 2936) and FU6 (n = 1883) are presented as means (standard deviation, s.d.) or numbers (percentages). The relations between sociodemographics, somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress with LTL at both time points were tested by means of generalized estimated equations (GEE) analyses.

To examine whether personality was consistently related to LTL, GEE analyses were conducted with personality as predictors (mean scores for each of the Big Five personality traits and Type D information at FU4) and LTL at both time points as outcome variable. To take within-person correlations as a result of multiple observations per person into account, GEE analyses were performed with exchangeable correlation structure and identity log-link function (Twisk, Reference Twisk2004). Time, that is the within subject variable defining the order of measurements, was categorized as 1 (baseline) and 2 (FU6). Separate analyses were performed for each of the Big Five personality traits and Type D personality.

Analyses included covariates measured at both time points, which were entered in two steps. First, we adjusted for sociodemographic factors (age, gender and education), which were seen as potential confounders. Secondly, somatic health, lifestyle factors (smoking alcohol use, BMI and physical activity), and recent life stress at both time points were considered as possibly explanatory variables and added to the model. In order to examine whether effects were consistent across time, age, and gender, interaction terms between personality trait (mean Big Five traits/Type D at FU4)×time/age/gender together with their standardized main effects were added to the sociodemographic adjusted models. Adding time interactions allowed us to examine the association between personality and LTL attrition. No interaction terms for the continuous Type D personality scoring was calculated, as a third way interaction (NA × SI × time/age/gender/psychiatric status) is less comprehensible.

Because short LTL is associated with psychiatric disorders such as depression and anxiety disorders (Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Epel, Lin, Wolkowitz and Penninx2014; Verhoeven et al. Reference Verhoeven, Revesz, Van Oppen, Epel, Wolkowitz and Penninx2015a ), we examined whether the relation between personality and LTL differed by depression and/or anxiety disorder status by adding the interaction term personality × psychiatric disorder. Furthermore, we examined whether psychiatric status influenced the relation between personality and LTL, by adding psychiatric status as a covariate to the model.

Because there was attrition from baseline to follow-up due to drop-out of 36%, which was not completely at random, analyses were performed using multiple imputation obtaining 40 imputed datasets. Details on the imputation method are provided in the online Supplementary Material File. Pooled estimates were used to obtain statistical interference. Additionally, we performed two types of sensitivity analyses: (1) using the original non-imputed data; and (2) including only the 64% individuals who had data available at both measurement points.

All analyses were conducted in SPSS version 22. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05, evaluation of trends was set at p < 0.10. We chose not to use a more stringent alpha level since this is the first study relating both the Big Five personality traits and Type D personality to LTL in both a healthy sample and psychiatric patients, hence we wanted to avoid making a type-2 error.

Results

Study population

Table 1 shows characteristics of the study population at baseline (n = 2936) and FU6 (n = 1883). Participants were on average 41.8 years old at baseline and two-third was women. Most participants were current or former smokers and modest drinkers. The average BMI was approximately 25, whereas little more than half of participants had a current psychiatric disorder. The number of negative life events during the past year was 0.2 at baseline and 1.3 at FU6. Mean scores on the Big Five personality traits ranged from 35 for neuroticism to 44 on agreeableness. Nearly 32% of participants were classified as having a Type D personality (Table 2).

Table 1. Participant characteristics at baseline and FU6

N, number; %, percentage; M, mean; s.d., standard deviation; FU6, 6-year follow-up; BMI, body mass index; MET, metabolic equivalent total; min/wk, minutes per week.

Table 2. Mean scores on personality traits

NA×SI, the standardized interaction term between negative affectivity (NA) and social inhibition (SI), representing Type D personality; s.d., standard deviation; N, number; %, percentage.

LTL was 5468 (s.d. = 617) at baseline and 5387 (s.d. = 433) at FU6. GEE analyses relating covariates to LTL at both time points showed that age was negatively related to LTL (B = −12.99, p < 0.01). Moreover, results showed that females had longer LTL (B = 60.98, p < 0.01), whereas current smokers had shorter LTL (B = −76.57, p < 0.01). The other covariates – education, alcohol use, somatic health, BMI, physical activity, and recent life stress – were not significantly related to TL (p’s > 0.10, data not shown).

LTL and personality

Table 3 shows the results of the GEE analyses, based on the imputed data, relating personality to LTL while adjusting for (1) sociodemographics, and (2) somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress. For the Big Five personality traits, we found a significant relation for neuroticism (B = −2.11, p = 0.03) and a positive relation for agreeableness (B = 3.84, p = 0.03) with LTL, after adjustment for sociodemographics. The negative relation for both neuroticism (B = −1.99, p = 0.06) and agreeableness (B = 3.01, p = 0.10) with LTL became non-significant, yet there was still a trend after adding the covariates somatic health (i.e. the number of treated medical conditions), lifestyle variables (i.e. smoking, alcohol use, BMI and physical activity), and recent life stress (i.e. the number of negative life events in the past year). There was a statistically significant sociodemographic-adjusted negative relation between Type D and LTL (B = −50.16, p < 0.01), which did not change after adjusting for somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress (B = −47.37, p = 0.01). Analyses relating Type D to LTL by including the continuous scores showed similar results: Type D (NA × SI) was negatively related to LTL (sociodemographic adjustment: B = −20.22, p = 0.04, full adjustment: B = −20.23, p = 0.04). Both sensitivity analyses showed similar regression coefficients (see online Supplementary Table S2); yet, in the analyses including only individuals with data at both measurements, some associations became non-significant, which is likely due to reduced power.

Table 3. The relation between personality and leukocyte telomere length across two time points

Note: Results are based on the imputed dataset. Sociodemographic adjusted models are presented: adjusted for age at baseline, gender, and years of education at both time points. Full adjustment: adjusted for sociodemographics and somatic health, lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol use, BMI, and physical activity), and recent life stress measured at both time points. , reference is non Type D personality; NA×SI, the standardized interaction term between negative affectivity (NA) and social inhibition (SI) representing Type D personality. Both standardized main effects (NA and SI) and its interaction term representing Type D personality were entered simultaneously to the model. *p < 0.05 and †p < 0.10. B-values for significant results and trends are noted in bold.

In order to examine whether associations were consistent across time or not, and hence whether personality was associated with LTL attrition, we added personality × time interaction terms to the sociodemographic models. These analyses showed no statistically significant interactions (Table 4). Both sensitivity analyses showed a significant time interaction effect for Type D (both measurements, B = 58.05, p = 0.04; and non-imputed data, B = 61.16, p = 0.02), where persons with Type D personality showed a slower LTL shortening than those without Type D (Table 4). Analyses using the non-imputed data showed an additional significant time interaction for openness to experience (Table 4). Differences in LTL between those scoring high on openness to experience had a faster shortening of LTL than those scoring low on openness to experience (B = −26.59, p = 0.02).

Table 4. The relation between personality and leukocyte telomere length attrition

Note: Sociodemographic adjustment: adjusted for age at baseline, gender, and years of education at both time points. , reference is non Type D personality. The number of observations in the non-imputed dataset differed for each analysis ranging from n = 3766 (1883 + 1883) when relating Type D personality to LTL, up to n = 4819 (2936 + 1883) when relating the Big Five personality traits to LTL. No time interaction for the continuous Type D personality scoring was calculated, as a third way interaction (NA × SI × time) is less comprehensible. *p < 0.05; †p < 0.10* B-values for significant results and trends in the main (imputed) analyses are denoted in bold for all three datasets.

There were no age and gender moderation effects for LTL (p’s > 0.10, data not shown). Furthermore, we examined whether the relation between personality and LTL was different by psychiatric status, that is, those with a current depression and/or anxiety disorder v. those without. No significant interaction effects were found for psychiatric status for the relation between personality and baseline LTL (ps > 0.10, data not shown). Hence, the relation between personality and baseline LTL was similar among those with and without a current depression and/or anxiety disorder. Both types of sensitivity analyses provided similar results (data not shown). Entering psychiatric status as a covariate showed similar associations between LTL and neuroticism (B = −1.69, p = 0.12); and agreeableness (B = 3.44, p = 0.05), yet relations became non-significant. Type D personality remained significantly associated with LTL after adjustment for psychiatric status (dichotomized: B = −45.86, p = 0.02; and NA × SI: B = −20.14, p = 0.04).

Discussion

This longitudinal study related the well-established Big Five personality traits and the more recently developed Type D personality construct to LTL measured across two time points. Results of this study confirmed our expectations that high neuroticism and low agreeableness were related to shorter LTL measured at two time points. However, these associations became statistically non-significant after adjusting for somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress. Importantly, Type D personality was negatively related to LTL measured at two time points, which remained significant after full adjustment. Personality was not associated with LTL attrition across time in our main analyses. Associations between personality and LTL did not differ by age, gender, and current psychiatric status. Adding psychiatric status to the model, resulted in non-significant associations for neuroticism and agreeableness, whereas Type D personality remained significantly related to LTL.

Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find an association between conscientiousness and LTL. This lack of association between conscientiousness and LTL was unexpected, as previous research has consistently reported that higher conscientiousness is related to decreased mortality (Kern & Friedman, Reference Kern and Friedman2008; Jokela et al. Reference Jokela, Batty, Nyberg, Virtanen, Nabi, Singh-Manoux and Kivimaki2013) and more recently to LTL (Sadahiro et al. Reference Sadahiro, Suzuki, Enokido, Matsumoto, Shibuya, Kamata, Goto and Otani2015). Dissimilarities between the significant association with LTL found by Sadahiro et al. and our study may be allocated to differences in age; they included a group of Japanese students with a mean age of 23 (s.d. = 1.7) (Sadahiro et al. Reference Sadahiro, Suzuki, Enokido, Matsumoto, Shibuya, Kamata, Goto and Otani2015), whereas we included an older sample with a broad age range (mean age = 42, s.d. = 13.1). This could be the result of different attrition rates across the life span as research has shown that early in life one has a fast LTL attrition rate, whereas during adulthood attrition is considered relatively small (Hjelmborg et al. Reference Hjelmborg, Dalgard, Moller, Steenstrup, Kimura, Christensen, Kyvik and Aviv2015). The relation between lower agreeableness and shorter LTL, is in agreement with previous findings of Savolainen and colleagues who found a similar relation among women but not in men in an elderly population (Savolainen et al. Reference Savolainen, Eriksson, Kajantie, Pesonen and Raikkonen2015). Our findings underscore the previously described protective character of higher agreeableness with respect to mortality (Weiss & Costa, Reference Weiss and Costa2005). Contrary to the null finding in the study by Sadahiro et al. (Reference Sadahiro, Suzuki, Enokido, Matsumoto, Shibuya, Kamata, Goto and Otani2015) we confirmed the relation found by others (Van Ockenburg et al. Reference Van Ockenburg, De Jonge, Van Der Harst, Ormel and Rosmalen2014) relating higher neuroticism to shorter LTL among adults of a broad age-range. Even after full adjustment there was still a trend. Some associations between the Big Five personality traits and LTL became non-significant after adjustment for covariates but similar trends still remained. Hence, the relations are influenced but not completely driven by differences in somatic health, lifestyle or experienced negative life events. Moreover, our study showed that persons with a Type D personality had shorter LTL measured at both time points, even after full adjustment.

Although there were no significant time interactions in the main analyses, the analyses based on the non-imputed data showed a significant time interaction for openness to experience and Type D personality, which are somewhat unexpected based on the accelerated aging hypothesis. These time interactions were not confirmed in our main analyses nor in the sensitivity analyses using individuals with complete LTL assessments, therefore it is unclear whether we found a true effect in the non-imputed analyses.

Important in the interpretation of our results is the crucial question as to how Type D personality is understood in terms of the renowned Big Five personality traits. The Type D personality sub-traits negative affectivity and social inhibition are known to correlate most strongly with neuroticism and extraversion (reversed) of the Big Five taxonomy (De Fruyt & Denollet, Reference De Fruyt and Denollet2002; Horwood et al. Reference Horwood, Anglim and Tooley2015), which was confirmed in our study. Previous studies have estimated that neuroticism and extraversion, although related to Type D personality, are not similar to Type D personality, given that both traits together explained half of the variance of the Type D construct (De Fruyt & Denollet, Reference De Fruyt and Denollet2002). A second important issue is the relation between neuroticism and Type D personality with depression as the NESDA sample consists of persons with a current depression and/or anxiety disorder and healthy controls. Although some overlap exists regarding neuroticism, the Type D sub-trait negative affectivity, and depression, the conceptual difference lies within the fact that personality is a permanent trait while depression is a disorder and thus a more temporary state (Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, De Jonge, Kuyper, Schene, Van Melle, Ormel and Honig2009; Karsten et al. Reference Karsten, Penninx, Riese, Ormel, Nolen and Hartman2012). Psychometrically, depression loads on different higher-order constructs or factors than Type D personality and its sub-traits negative affectivity and social inhibition (Pelle et al. Reference Pelle, Denollet, Zwisler and Pedersen2009). Underscoring the distinctiveness between depression and the personality trait neuroticism and Type D personality, we found that the relations between neuroticism and Type D with LTL were similar among those with and without a current depressive and/or anxiety disorder. Results of the analyses where we added current psychiatric status as a covariant to the model – examining whether psychiatric status influences the relation between personality and LTL – showed similar but non-significant associations for neuroticism and agreeableness with LTL. Contrary, the relation between Type D personality and LTL was unchanged after adding current psychiatric status. These findings suggest that current psychiatric status – that is, being currently diagnosed with a depression and/or anxiety disorder – could be a potential confounder or may be a possible mechanism or pathway for the relation between neuroticism and agreeableness with LTL, but not so much for Type D personality.

Neuroticism, agreeableness, Type D personality and shorter LTL are associated with morbidity, such as the age-related diseases cancer and CVD, and mortality (Wilson et al. Reference Wilson, Mendes De Leon, Bienias, Evans and Bennett2004; Denollet, Reference Denollet2005; Weiss & Costa, Reference Weiss and Costa2005; Chapman et al. Reference Chapman, Fiscella, Kawachi and Duberstein2010; Mols et al. Reference Mols, Oerlemans, Denollet, Roukema and Van De Poll-Franse2012; Denollet, Reference Denollet2013; Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, Pedersen, Vrints and Conraads2013; Lyon et al. Reference Lyon, Starkweather, Montpetit, Menzies and Jallo2014; McCann, Reference Mccann2014). Similar underlying risk factors, such as the amount of oxidative stress exposure, inflammation and a genetic vulnerability may be responsible for these associations. Experiencing psychological stress leads to the release of the stress hormone cortisol, which increases damage by oxidative stress (McIntosh et al. Reference Mcintosh, Hong and Sapolsky1998) and inflammation (Epel, Reference Epel2009). Neuroticism and Type D personality are associated with experiencing psychological stress (Denollet et al. Reference Denollet, Sys, Stroobant, Rombouts, Gillebert and Brutsaert1996; Denollet, Reference Denollet2005; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006), which is accompanied by increased oxidative stress and inflammation. Hence, elevated stress hormones may be a mediating factor partly explaining the relations between personality (i.e. neuroticism, agreeableness, and Type D personality) and LTL, mortality and morbidity. In clinical practice, the primary focus should not be on changing personality itself, but rather on providing these individuals with the skills necessary to more effectively cope with stressful events.

A major strength of this study is its large sample size and longitudinal design. Moreover, our study included well characterized patients with a current depressive and/or anxious disorder together with healthy controls. Also, our sample comprised a wide age range, and included information on important covariates such as somatic health, lifestyle factors and recent life stress. Furthermore, LTL was reliably measured with qPCR, whereas the intra-assay coefficients of variation were sufficiently low. As a result we could comprehensively examine the relation between personality and LTL measured across a 6-year period.

It is however also worth mentioning the limitations of this study. First, longitudinal data was analyzed where Type D personality was measured 6 years after baseline LTL was assessed, whereas the Big Five personality traits were measured at several occasions and scores were averaged. The Big Five and Type D personality are considered to be stable across time (Rantanen et al. Reference Rantanen, Metsapelto, Feldt, Pulkkinen and Kokko2007; Kupper et al. Reference Kupper, Boomsma, De Geus, Denollet and Willemsen2011), which is supported by the high correlations between mean Big Five personality traits and scores on each measurement occasion. We therefore believe that averaging personality scores across time had little impact on our results. Furthermore, no conclusions regarding causality can be drawn. Following from this, we have assumed that personality may affect LTL, yet research has also shown that LTL deletion can cause neuropsychological abnormalities (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Dilley and Mattson2007). By analogy, short LTL might also have an impact on personality through increasing levels of inflammation (Conraads et al. Reference Conraads, Denollet, De Clerck, Stevens, Bridts and Vrints2006; Sutin et al. Reference Sutin, Terracciano, Deiana, Naitza, Ferrucci, Uda, Schlessinger and Costa2010). Additionally, the difference in the effects between Type D personality as a dichotomized measure and the continuous Big Five traits may be an artifact of the nature of the variable rather than a true difference. Although the small differences in results for the imputed analyses compared with both sensitivity analyses suggest that missing data had limited impact on the observed findings, as LTL at both measurements was not missing at random. There seems to be an impact of missingness of LTL when examining whether personality is associated with LTL attrition, given the discrepancy in results from the imputed v. sensitivity analyses. Furthermore, as is common in studies examining TL, LTL was used as a valid and often used indicator of cellular ageing. Nonetheless, average TL consists of several cell types complicating the distinguishing between TL differences due to actual shortening or lengthening or the reorganization of cell types (Lin et al. Reference Lin, Damjanovic, Metter, Nguyen, Truong, Najarro, Morris, Longo, Zhan, Ferrucci, Hodes and Weng2015). Additionally, no information on telomerase activity was available in our study, inhibiting the studying of its important role in maintaining TL. Also, LTL from baseline and FU6 was measured in two different batches two years apart which could have introduced noise between time-points. To adjust for possible systematic differences, samples from both time points were re-rerun together and FU TLT was converted accordingly.

In conclusion, this study demonstrates that high neuroticism and low agreeableness were associated with shorter LTL measured across a 6-year period. After controlling for somatic health, lifestyle factors, and recent life stress (full adjustment) associations between these Big Five traits became non-significant, although similar trends were still observed. However, the association between Type D personality and shorter LTL measured across a 6-year period remained significant, even after full adjustment. It is important to note, that the relationship between personality and LTL did not differ by age, gender or a current depressive and/or anxiety disorder. Future research should explore causality as personality may lead to a predisposed vulnerability leading to biological ageing.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002471.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a Social Psychology Fellowship from the Dutch Cancer Society (#UVT2013-5893) granted to Dounya Schoormans and a NWO-VICI grant (91811602) awarded to Brenda Penninx. The infrastructure for the NESDA study (http://www.nesda.nl) is funded through the Geestkracht program of the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (Zon-Mw, grant number 10-000-1002) and is supported by participating universities and mental health care organizations (VU University Medical Center, GGZ inGeest, Arkin, Leiden University Medical Center, GGZ Rivierduinen, University Medical Center Groningen, Lentis, GGZ Friesland, GGZ Drenthe, Institute for Quality of Health Care (IQ Healthcare), Netherlands Institute for Health Services Research (NIVEL) and Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction (Trimbos).

Declaration of Interest

The authors declare no relevant conflict of interest.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Aviv, A, Hunt, SC, Lin, J, Cao, X, Kimura, M, Blackburn, E (2011). Impartial comparative analysis of measurement of leukocyte telomere length/DNA content by Southern blots and qPCR. Nucleic Acids Research 39, e134.Google Scholar
Blackburn, EH (1991). Structure and function of telomeres. Nature 350, 569573.Google Scholar
Blackburn, EH (2001). Switching and signaling at the telomere. Cell 106, 661673.Google Scholar
Brugha, TS, Cragg, D (1990). The list of threatening experiences: the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 82, 7781.Google Scholar
Cawthon, RM (2002). Telomere measurement by quantitative PCR. Nucleic Acids Research 30, e47.Google Scholar
Cawthon, RM, Smith, KR, O'brien, E, Sivatchenko, A, Kerber, RA (2003). Association between telomere length in blood and mortality in people aged 60 years or older. Lancet 361, 393395.Google Scholar
Chapman, BP, Fiscella, K, Kawachi, I, Duberstein, PR (2010). Personality, socioeconomic status, and all-cause mortality in the United States. American Journal of Epidemiology 171, 8392.Google Scholar
Conraads, VM, Denollet, J, De Clerck, LS, Stevens, WJ, Bridts, C, Vrints, CJ (2006). Type D personality is associated with increased levels of tumour necrosis factor (TNF)-alpha and TNF-alpha receptors in chronic heart failure. International Journal of Cardiology 113, 3438.Google Scholar
Costa, PT, Mccrae, RR (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five Factor Inventory: Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.: Lutz, Florida.Google Scholar
Costa, PT Jr., Mccrae, RR (1995). Domains and facets: hierarchical personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment 64, 2150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, CL, Marshall, AL, Sjostrom, M, Bauman, AE, Booth, ML, Ainsworth, BE, Pratt, M, Ekelund, U, Yngve, A, Sallis, JF, Oja, P (2003). International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 35, 13811395.Google Scholar
De Fruyt, F, Denollet, J (2002). Type D personality: a five-factor model perspective. Psychology and Health 17, 671683.Google Scholar
Denollet, J (2005). DS14: standard assessment of negative affectivity, social inhibition, and Type D personality. Psychosomatic Medicine 67, 8997.Google Scholar
Denollet, J (2013). Interpersonal sensitivity, social inhibition, and Type D personality: how and when are they associated with health? Comment on Marin and Miller (2013). Psychological Bulletin 139, 991997.Google Scholar
Denollet, J, De Jonge, P, Kuyper, A, Schene, AH, Van Melle, JP, Ormel, J, Honig, A (2009). Depression and Type D personality represent different forms of distress in the myocardial INfarction and depression – intervention trial (MIND-IT). Psychological Medicine 39, 749756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denollet, J, Pedersen, SS, Vrints, CJ, Conraads, VM (2013). Predictive value of social inhibition and negative affectivity for cardiovascular events and mortality in patients with coronary artery disease: the Type D personality construct. Psychosomatic Medicine 75, 873881.Google Scholar
Denollet, J, Sys, SU, Stroobant, N, Rombouts, H, Gillebert, TC, Brutsaert, DL (1996). Personality as independent predictor of long-term mortality in patients with coronary heart disease. Lancet 347, 417421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epel, ES (2009). Psychological and metabolic stress: a recipe for accelerated cellular aging? Hormones (Athens) 8, 722.Google Scholar
Hjelmborg, JB, Dalgard, C, Moller, S, Steenstrup, T, Kimura, M, Christensen, K, Kyvik, KO, Aviv, A (2015). The heritability of leucocyte telomere length dynamics. Journal of Medical Genetics 52, 297302.Google Scholar
Horwood, S, Anglim, J, Tooley, G (2015). Type D personality and the five-factor model: a facet-level analysis. Personality and Individual Differences 83, 5054.Google Scholar
Huzen, J, Van Der Harst, P, De Boer, RA, Lesman-Leegte, I, Voors, AA, Van Gilst, WH, Samani, NJ, Jaarsma, T, Van Veldhuisen, DJ (2010). Telomere length and psychological well-being in patients with chronic heart failure. Age and Ageing 39, 223227.Google Scholar
John, OP, Naumann, LP, Soto, CJ (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative big-five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (ed. John, O. P., R. R., Pervin, L. A.). Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Jokela, M, Batty, GD, Nyberg, ST, Virtanen, M, Nabi, H, Singh-Manoux, A, Kivimaki, M (2013). Personality and all-cause mortality: individual-participant meta-analysis of 3,947 deaths in 76,150 adults. American Journal of Epidemiology 178, 667675.Google Scholar
Jokela, M, Pulkki-Raback, L, Elovainio, M, Kivimaki, M (2014). Personality traits as risk factors for stroke and coronary heart disease mortality: pooled analysis of three cohort studies. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 37, 881889.Google Scholar
Karsten, J, Penninx, BW, Riese, H, Ormel, J, Nolen, WA, Hartman, CA (2012). The state effect of depressive and anxiety disorders on big five personality traits. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 644650.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Gatz, M, Gardner, CO, Pedersen, NL (2006). Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 11131120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kern, ML, Friedman, HS (2008). Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychology 27, 505512.Google Scholar
Kupper, N, Boomsma, DI, De Geus, EJ, Denollet, J, Willemsen, G (2011). Nine-year stability of Type D personality: contributions of genes and environment. Psychosomatic Medicine 73, 7582.Google Scholar
Lerman, C, Caporaso, NE, Audrain, J, Main, D, Boyd, NR, Shields, PG (2000). Interacting effects of the serotonin transporter gene and neuroticism in smoking practices and nicotine dependence. Molecular Psychiatry 5, 189192.Google Scholar
Lin, Y, Damjanovic, A, Metter, EJ, Nguyen, H, Truong, T, Najarro, K, Morris, C, Longo, DL, Zhan, M, Ferrucci, L, Hodes, RJ, Weng, NP (2015). Age-associated telomere attrition of lymphocytes in vivo is co-ordinated with changes in telomerase activity, composition of lymphocyte subsets and health conditions. Clinical Science 128, 367377.Google Scholar
Lyon, DE, Starkweather, AR, Montpetit, A, Menzies, V, Jallo, N (2014). A biobehavioral perspective on telomere length and the exposome. Biological Research for Nursing 16, 448455.Google Scholar
Mccann, SJH (2014). Higher resident neuroticism is specifically associated with elevated state cancer and heart disease mortality rates in the United States. Sage Open, 115.Google Scholar
Mcintosh, LJ, Hong, KE, Sapolsky, RM (1998). Glucocorticoids may alter antioxidant enzyme capacity in the brain: baseline studies. Brain Research 791, 209214.Google Scholar
Mols, F, Oerlemans, S, Denollet, J, Roukema, JA, Van De Poll-Franse, LV (2012). Type D personality is associated with increased comorbidity burden and health care utilization among 3080 cancer survivors. General Hospital Psychiatry 34, 352359.Google Scholar
Muezzinler, A, Zaineddin, AK, Brenner, H (2013). A systematic review of leukocyte telomere length and age in adults. Ageing Research Reviews 12, 509519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, SS, Lemos, PA, Van Vooren, PR, Liu, TK, Daemen, J, Erdman, RA, Smits, PC, Serruys, PW, Van Domburg, RT (2004). Type D personality predicts death or myocardial infarction after bare metal stent or sirolimus-eluting stent implantation: a rapamycin-eluting stent evaluated at rotterdam cardiology hospital (RESEARCH) registry substudy. Journal of the American College of Cardiology 44, 9971001.Google Scholar
Pelle, AJ, Denollet, J, Zwisler, AD, Pedersen, SS (2009). Overlap and distinctiveness of psychological risk factors in patients with ischemic heart disease and chronic heart failure: are we there yet? Journal of Affective Disorders 113, 150156.Google Scholar
Pelle, AJ, Pedersen, SS, Schiffer, AA, Szabo, B, Widdershoven, JW, Denollet, J (2010). Psychological distress and mortality in systolic heart failure. Circulation: Heart Failure 3, 261267.Google Scholar
Penninx, BW, Beekman, AT, Smit, JH, Zitman, FG, Nolen, WA, Spinhoven, P, Cuijpers, P, De Jong, PJ, Van Marwijk, HW, Assendelft, WJ, Van Der Meer, K, Verhaak, P, Wensing, M, De Graaf, R, Hoogendijk, WJ, Ormel, J, Van Dyck, R, Consortium, NR (2008). The Netherlands study of depression and anxiety (NESDA): rationale, objectives and methods. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 17, 121140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rantanen, J, Metsapelto, RL, Feldt, T, Pulkkinen, L, Kokko, K (2007). Long-term stability in the Big Five personality traits in adulthood. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 48, 511518.Google Scholar
Sadahiro, R, Suzuki, A, Enokido, M, Matsumoto, Y, Shibuya, N, Kamata, M, Goto, K, Otani, K (2015). Relationship between leukocyte telomere length and personality traits in healthy subjects. European Psychiatry: the Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists 30, 291295.Google Scholar
Sanders, JL, Newman, AB (2013). Telomere length in epidemiology: a biomarker of aging, age-related disease, both, or neither? Epidemiologic Reviews 35, 112131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savolainen, K, Eriksson, JG, Kajantie, E, Pesonen, AK, Raikkonen, K (2015). Associations between the five-factor model of personality and leukocyte telomere length in elderly men and women: the Helsinki birth cohort study (HBCS). Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79, 233238.Google Scholar
Schutte, NS, Malouff, JM (2015). The association between depression and leukocyte telomere length: a meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety 32, 229238.Google Scholar
Smith, TW (2011). Toward a more systematic, cumulative, and applicable science of personality and health: lessons from Type D personality. Psychosomatic Medicine 73, 528532.Google Scholar
Sutin, AR, Ferrucci, L, Zonderman, AB, Terracciano, A (2011). Personality and obesity across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101, 579592.Google Scholar
Sutin, AR, Terracciano, A, Deiana, B, Naitza, S, Ferrucci, L, Uda, M, Schlessinger, D, Costa, PT Jr. (2010). High neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with interleukin-6. Psychological Medicine 40, 14851493.Google Scholar
Twisk, JWR (2004). Longitudinal data analysis. A comparison between generalized estimating equations and random coefficient analysis. European Journal of Epidemiology 19, 769776.Google Scholar
Van Ockenburg, SL, Bos, EH, De Jonge, P, Van Der Harst, P, Gans, RO, Rosmalen, JG (2015). Stressful life events and leukocyte telomere attrition in adulthood: a prospective population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine 45, 29752984.Google Scholar
Van Ockenburg, SL, De Jonge, P, Van Der Harst, P, Ormel, J, Rosmalen, JG (2014). Does neuroticism make you old? Prospective associations between neuroticism and leukocyte telomere length. Psychological Medicine 44, 723729.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, JE, Revesz, D, Epel, ES, Lin, J, Wolkowitz, OM, Penninx, BW (2014). Major depressive disorder and accelerated cellular aging: results from a large psychiatric cohort study. Molecular Psychiatry 19, 895901.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, JE, Revesz, D, Van Oppen, P, Epel, ES, Wolkowitz, OM, Penninx, BW (2015 a). Anxiety disorders and accelerated cellular ageing. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science 206, 371378.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, JE, Van Oppen, P, Puterman, E, Elzinga, B, Penninx, BW (2015 b). The association of early and recent psychosocial life stress with leukocyte telomere length. Psychosomatic Medicine 77, 882891.Google Scholar
Weiss, A, Costa, PT Jr. (2005). Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychosomatic Medicine 67, 724733.Google Scholar
Wilson, RS, Mendes De Leon, CF, Bienias, JL, Evans, DA, Bennett, DA (2004). Personality and mortality in old age. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 59, P110P116.Google Scholar
Zhang, P, Dilley, C, Mattson, MP (2007). DNA damage responses in neural cells: focus on the telomere. Neuroscience 145, 14391448.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant characteristics at baseline and FU6

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean scores on personality traits

Figure 2

Table 3. The relation between personality and leukocyte telomere length across two time points

Figure 3

Table 4. The relation between personality and leukocyte telomere length attrition

Supplementary material: File

Schoormans et al supplementary material

Schoormans et al supplementary material 1

Download Schoormans et al supplementary material(File)
File 23.7 KB