In the past few decades, the prevalence of overweight and obesity has increased drastically in most countries of the world(Reference Abarca-Gómez, Abdeen and Hamid1) and nearly 60 % of the European adult population is currently overweight or obese(2). Overweight and obesity are associated with several adverse health consequences and an increased risk of non-communicable diseases, such as CVD, diabetes, musculoskeletal disorders and certain cancers(3). With the substantial societal as well as personal cost of poor health caused by obesity and related co-morbidities, there is an evident need to address the issue.
A balanced, healthy diet and sufficient physical activity (PA) are key factors in the prevention of overweight and several chronic diseases, and are the cornerstones of health promotion. As in most of the Western world, the diet of the Danish population exhibits inadequate intakes of dietary fibre, fruit and vegetables, while intakes of saturated fat, added sugar, salt and alcohol exceed recommendations(Reference Pedersen, Christensen and Matthiessen4). Several of these dietary patterns have been associated with adverse health effects, such as increased adiposity, type 2 diabetes and CVD(5–Reference Schwab, Lauritzen and Tholstrup8). Furthermore, average PA levels are below recommendations(9,Reference Matthiessen, Andersen and Barbieri10) . While much research has focused on identification of the above-mentioned behavioural patterns, less attention has been paid to temporal variation of dietary intake and PA.
Weekly variation patterns have previously been observed in health behaviours such as sleep duration and sedentary time(Reference Hjorth, Chaput and Michaelsen11), dietary intake(Reference Rothausen, Matthiessen and Hoppe12–Reference Rockell, Parnell and Wilson18) and PA(Reference Hjorth, Chaput and Michaelsen11,Reference Refinetti, Sani and Jean-Louis19–Reference Rowlands, Pilgrim and Eston24) in various populations. In the short term such patterns have been observed to affect health markers, such as fasting insulin and TAG(Reference Hjorth, Damsgaard and Michaelsen25,Reference Jaskolowski, Ritz and Sjodin26) , and in the long term could have potential consequences for body weight and health status(Reference Racette, Weiss and Schechtman27,Reference Orsama, Mattila and Ermes28) . Characterization of food consumption and PA across the days of the week can help target health promotion initiatives striving to improve dietary intake and PA behaviour of the population.
Therefore, the aim of the present study was to compare dietary intake and PA between days of the week, while investigating the potential influence of age and gender, using a large sample of the Danish population having 7 d of dietary recordings and pedometer monitoring. We hypothesized that individuals would display less healthy behaviour during weekends compared with weekdays, and furthermore that this difference would be more pronounced among children and less so among the elderly, as previously indicated for weekly variation in blood markers in a Danish population(Reference Jaskolowski, Ritz and Sjodin26).
Methods
Data were derived from the Danish National Survey of Diet and Physical Activity (DANSDA) 2011–2013; a nationwide, cross-sectional survey assessing diet and PA of the Danish population(Reference Pedersen, Christensen and Matthiessen4,Reference Matthiessen, Andersen and Raustorp29) . For DANSDA 2011–2013, a representative sample of 7356 adults and children aged 4–75 years was randomly drawn from the Danish Civil Registration System(Reference Pedersen30) and data collection took place between April 2011 and August 2013. Danish citizenship was a criterion for inclusion; however, disabled individuals, residents of nursing homes and home-dwelling individuals receiving meals from outside the home were excluded from the sample to ensure that individuals have sufficient knowledge about their dietary intake.
Dietary intake was estimated by 7 d consecutive dietary recording, using food diaries with pre-coded response categories and open answer options. The starting day of recording could be any weekday; however, due to participant preferences, Monday and Tuesday were over-represented while Saturday and Sunday were under-represented. The dietary record was structured according to the typical Danish meal pattern (i.e. breakfast, lunch, dinner and in-between meals) and food items were categorized according to food groups. The pre-coded response options included food items and beverages most commonly consumed in Denmark, with the possibility of adding additional items in an open field. Portion sizes were estimated by predefined household measures (cups, glasses, spoons, slices, etc.) and portion-size pictures. For each participant, the intakes of different food items, and the content of energy and nutrients within, were estimated using the General Intake Estimation System (GIES) software version 1.000.i6 and the Danish Food Composition Databank version 7.0 (www.foodcomp.dk), both developed at the National Food Institute of the Technical University of Denmark. Validation studies of the 7 d pre-coded dietary record used in DANSDA have been carried out in populations of Danish adults and children(Reference Knudsen, Gille and Nielsen31–Reference Rothausen, Matthiessen and Groth33). Compared with a 4 d weighed food record, estimated mean intakes of nutrients and foods were generally consistent with the pre-coded food diary(Reference Knudsen, Gille and Nielsen31); and compared with objectively measured energy expenditure, energy intake was under-reported by 12 % in adults aged 20–59 years and by 7 and 20 % in children aged 7–8 and 12–13 years, respectively(Reference Biltoft-Jensen, Matthiessen and Rasmussen32,Reference Rothausen, Matthiessen and Groth33) . Misreporters of dietary intake in the present study were identified by evaluating reported energy intake against presumed energy requirement, using the Goldberg cut-offs for the ratio between reported energy intake and estimated BMR at the individual level as suggested by Black(Reference Black34,Reference Goldberg, Black and Jebb35) , assuming a moderate physical activity level for all individuals (corresponding to 1·8 for children aged <18 years and 1·6 for adults)(36). BMR was estimated by age- and gender-specific Schofield equations with measured weights(Reference Schofield37). Individuals with at least four valid days of recording, including one weekend day (Saturday or Sunday), were included in the present analysis.
In DANSDA food items have been aggregated into food groups based on nutritional characteristics(Reference Pedersen, Christensen and Matthiessen4), and the presently examined dietary variables include measures of energy, macronutrients, foods and beverages, providing information of diet quantity and quality. Dietary variables include intakes of energy, fruit, vegetables, fish, wholegrain products, red meat (both processed and non-processed meats from four-legged mammals), sugar-sweetened beverages, beer and wine, fast food, discretionary foods, alcohol, carbohydrates, protein, fat, saturated fat, added sugar, fibre and energy density (separately for solid and liquid components of the diet). Foods, beverages and fibre intake are given relative to a 10 MJ diet (i.e. as g/10 MJ), alcohol intake is given as g/d and the remaining macronutrients are given relative to total energy intake (E%), excluding potential energy contribution from alcohol. The variable ‘wholegrain products’ was calculated from the reported intakes of ryebread, oats and coarse wheat bread, which contribute 77–80 % of total wholegrain intake in Denmark(Reference Mejborn, Biltoft-Jensen and Trolle38). The variable ‘sugar-sweetened beverages’ was computed as the sum of soda, non-alcoholic cider, ice teas and sugary fruit drinks. ‘Discretionary foods’ are defined as high-energy-density, low-nutrient-density foods that contribute a considerable amount of ‘empty calories’, while not representing a necessary part of the diet. Both sweet and savoury foods were included, such as cakes, sweets, chocolate, sweet desserts, chocolate spreads, chips and salted nuts. Finally, ‘fast food’ is comprised of readily available convenience foods, often highly processed and energy-dense, such as pizza, burgers, fries, hot dogs, spring rolls, kebab sandwiches and sandwiches consumed between meals as snacks.
Pedometer-determined step counts were measured during the same 7 d monitoring period as the dietary assessment. Trained interviewers instructed participants to wear the pedometer (Yamax SW-200, Tokyo, Japan) on the hip during waking hours and to fill in step diaries. Step diaries were adapted from Tudor-Locke et al.(Reference Tudor-Locke, Lind and Reis39) and included wear time and time spent cycling and doing exercise. Pedometers do not capture non-ambulatory PA, such as cycling, swimming and weight training. To give a more accurate estimate of the total volume of PA in the Danish population, a cycling-adjusted step count was estimated, as cycling is the key non-ambulatory activity in Denmark(Reference Matthiessen, Andersen and Raustorp29). Cycling-adjusted step counts were estimated by adding 160 steps/min of cycling to the crude step count, although not exceeding an addition of 10 000 steps from cycling to avoid overestimation(Reference Matthiessen, Andersen and Raustorp29). At least four valid days, including one weekend day (Saturday or Sunday), were required for inclusion in the analysis of PA(Reference Tudor-Locke, Mire and Dentro40). A valid day was defined as having a pedometer wear time ≥10 h and a step count between 100 and 50 000 steps/d(Reference Matthiessen, Andersen and Raustorp29). Average step counts for the monitoring period of <1000 or >25 000 steps/d (>30 000steps/d for children) were treated as outliers and excluded from analysis, as suggested by Tudor-Locke et al.(Reference Tudor-Locke, Bassett and Shipe41), unless the high or low step count could be explained by deviating activity level in the step diary. Step counts and cycling-adjusted step counts are given both in absolute amounts (steps/d) and relative to pedometer wear time during a valid day (cadence; steps/min).
Educational level and weight status were determined by trained interviewers during baseline face-to-face interviews. Weight was measured to the nearest 0·1 kg using an electronic scale (ADE BE 822; ADE Germany, Hamburg, Germany) and standing height to the nearest 0·1 cm using a portable stadiometer (Charder HM200P PortStad; Charder Electronic Co., Taichung City, Taiwan), with the participant’s head positioned at the Frankfurt horizontal plane. Participants wore light clothing and no shoes and were encouraged to empty their bladder prior to measurement. Measurements were repeated twice and the average was reported. For adults, weight status was classified based on international BMI standards into underweight (BMI < 18·5 kg/m2), normal weight (18·5 kg/m2 ≤ BMI < 25 kg/m2), overweight (25 kg/m2 ≤ BMI < 30 kg/m2) and obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2); the corresponding classification for children was based on age- and gender-specific International Obesity Task Force BMI cut-offs(Reference Cole, Bellizzi and Flegal42). Educational level was classified into six levels based on the length of the education: basic school (<12 years), upper secondary school (12 years), vocational education (13 years, practical), short higher (13–14 years), medium higher (15–16 years) and long higher (≥17 years).
We divided participants into four age groups to capture the different life stages represented in the population and enable identification of differing health behaviour patterns therein: children aged 4–13 years (children under parental influence); adolescents and young adults aged 14–24 years (youth typically still undergoing education); adults aged 25–59 years (adults in family forming and parenting age); and elderly aged 60–75 years (older adults at or close to retirement age). Preliminary analysis and previous research have indicated that patterns of dietary intake and PA are rather stable through Monday to Thursday, but may follow distinct patterns on Friday, Saturday and Sunday(Reference Rothausen, Matthiessen and Hoppe12,Reference An13,Reference Jahns, Conrad and Johnson43) . Consequently, we constructed a four-level categorical variable with Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday and Thursday belonging to the same category (i.e. ‘weekdays’) and Friday, Saturday and Sunday being separate categories. For ease of reading, Saturday and Sunday are collectively referred to as ‘weekend days’ when appropriate.
Statistical analysis
The study population was described by means of counts and proportions and means and standard deviations.
Weekday variation in dietary and PA variables was analysed by means of linear mixed models. One model per outcome was constructed, with weekday category (Monday–Thursday, Friday, Saturday and Sunday), gender and age as fixed effects and subject as random effect. All linear mixed models were validated by inspection of residual plots, normal quantile–quantile plots of the residuals and normal quantile–quantile plots for predicted random effects. Estimated marginal means for weekday categories and corresponding 95 % confidence intervals were obtained using the R package lsmeans version 2.27-61(Reference Lenth44). Differences between weekday categories were assessed by pairwise comparisons using post hoc t tests. Adjustment of P values was based on the single-step method(Reference Hothorn, Bretz and Westfall45). For consumption of foods and beverages, the means are based on ‘intake days’ only, that is when the intake of a specific food product on an observation day was >0. Skewed outcome variables were transformed when appropriate by logarithmic transformation, square root transformation or Box–Cox transformation, and subsequently back transformed.
Variables representing intakes of foods and beverages typically contained large proportions of zeros and, consequently, logistic mixed-effects regression models were fitted to estimate the consumption probability (Prob.) of foods and beverages on different weekdays. Pairwise comparisons were performed on the log-odds scale, and the estimated Prob. and the corresponding 95 % confidence interval were obtained through back transformation. The logistic models contained weekday category, gender and age as fixed effects and subject as random effect.
To investigate the potential influence of gender and age as determinants for a weekly variation in dietary intake and PA, stratified analyses were performed for different age groups and for males and females separately. Finally, sensitivity analysis excluding under- and over-reporters of dietary intake were performed for energy intake.
Statistical significance in all pairwise comparisons was indicated by compact letter display. Significance was displayed at P < 0·001 in main analyses and subset analyses by gender, while P < 0·01 was used in age subsets. Data preparation and descriptive statistics were performed in the statistical software package IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows version 25 and all other statistical analyses were carried out in R statistical software version 3.3.2(46).
Results
The final study population represents 54·3 % of the originally drawn survey sample. The reasons for loss and exclusion of participants are described in more detail in the flow diagram (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Fig. S1). A total of 3940 individuals (51·1 % female) with a mean age of 38·7 (sd 21·2) years and 47·4 % being overweight or obese were included in analysis, of which 89·4 % had valid recordings for both PA and dietary intake (Table 1). Details about the analytical samples are presented in Supplemental Table S1.
* Highest completed education. Adolescents undertaking upper secondary school education are excluded (n 115). For children (<15 years), the educational level of the parent who filled in the interview form is given.
† Based on measured weight and height. International Obesity Task Force BMI cut-offs were used to determine weight status in children.
PA, with and without adjustment for cycling and pedometer wear time (cadence), was lower on weekend days compared with weekdays (Monday–Thursday; P < 0·001; Table 2). Measured as steps/d, PA was 10 and 17 % lower on Saturday and Sunday, respectively, compared with weekdays. No difference in PA was found between weekdays and Friday (P > 0·001). A decrease in PA was observed in all age groups, the weekday–weekend difference declining with each age group from the youngest to the oldest, and the patterns observed were similar for raw steps and cycling-adjusted steps (Fig. 1 and online supplementary material, Supplemental Table S2). The decrease in step count from weekdays to Saturday and Sunday, respectively, was 21 and 30 % in the youngest age group (4–13 years), whereas it was only 7 and 8 % in the oldest age group (60–75 years). Only minor differences in weekly variation in PA were observed between genders (Supplemental Table S2).
Prob., probability of consumption; E%, percentage of total energy; SSB, sugar-sweetened beverages.
a,b,c,dProbabilities and mean values within a row with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·001), as estimated from post hoc pairwise comparisons. P values for added sugar, foods and beverages were obtained from Box–Cox transformed models, physical activity from square root transformed models, and all others from normal models.
* Data are presented as means and 95 % CI, as estimated from linear mixed models. Regression models contained main fixed effects of weekday (four levels), gender and age, and random effect of subject. Outcome variables of physical activity were transformed by square root transformation, food and beverage variables were transformed by logarithmic transformation. Back transformed estimates presented.
† Probability of consumption as estimated from logistic regression. Regression model contained main effects of weekday (four levels), gender and age, and random effect of subject. Estimates and 95 % CI are back transformed from logit scale.
‡ Logistic regression model failed to converge. Fixed effects of age and gender removed from model.
§ Logistic regression model failed to converge. Fixed effect of age removed from model.
‖ Logistic regression model failed to converge. Model fitted without random effect and weekday as only explanatory variable.
Overall, energy intake, energy density (solids and liquids), intakes of added sugar, discretionary foods, alcohol, and beer and wine were higher, while intakes of fruit, vegetables, fibre and wholegrain products were lower, during weekend days (Saturday and Sunday) and Friday, compared with weekdays (P < 0·001; Table 2 and online supplementary material, Supplemental Figs S2 and S3). Only minor differences in dietary intake during the week were observed between genders (Supplemental Tables S3 and S4). Overall, the weekly variation in dietary intake appeared most pronounced for children (4–13 years) and least pronounced for the elderly (60–75 years; Figs 2 and 3, and Supplemental Tables S5 and S6), as seen for the relative increase in E% of added sugar from weekdays to Saturday, which was 76 % for 4–13-year-olds, 34 % for 14–24-year-olds, 29 % for 25–59-year-olds and 8 % for 60–75-year-olds (P < 0·01 for all). The same observation was made for sugar-sweetened beverages and discretionary foods. The mean intake of alcohol increased by 94 % on Saturdays compared with weekdays in 16–24-year-olds, by 81 % in 25–59-year-olds and by 42 % in 60–75-year-olds.
Overall, results remained the same when removing under-reporters (13·4 %) and over-reporters (1·0 %) from the analyses (data not shown).
Discussion
As hypothesized, dietary intake and PA were found to fluctuate during the week in this large representative sample of Danes, with weekend days displaying lower dietary quality and less PA compared with weekdays. Saturday displayed in general the poorest dietary quality, while Sunday was the least active day of the week. In terms of dietary intake, Friday resembled a weekend day, while it resembled a weekday in terms of PA. Overall, similar patterns were observed across genders and age groups, although the differences in diet and PA between weekdays and weekend days were most pronounced among children and relatively modest among the elderly population.
Previous studies investigating weekly variation in dietary intake and PA have generally found less healthy behaviour during the weekend. Larger intakes of total energy, fat, added sugar, different discretionary foods and alcohol have consistently been observed during the weekend in studies on Western populations(Reference Rothausen, Matthiessen and Hoppe12–Reference Haines, Hama and Guilkey17,Reference Jahns, Conrad and Johnson43,Reference de Castro47–Reference Svensson, Larsson and Eiben54) . However, the increase in total energy intake during weekend days, compared with weekdays, observed in the present study was larger than demonstrated in other large-scale, national studies(Reference An13,Reference Yang, Black and Barr15–Reference Haines, Hama and Guilkey17) . In agreement with our findings, previous studies using pedometry or accelerometery have consistently found lower PA level during weekends compared with weekdays in both adults and children(Reference Hjorth, Chaput and Michaelsen11,Reference Refinetti, Sani and Jean-Louis19–Reference Rowlands, Pilgrim and Eston24,Reference Brusseau, Kulinna and Tudor-Locke55–Reference Matthews, Ainsworth and Thompson63) ; however, most studies are based on specific populations and narrow age spans. A more detailed summary of previous research on weekly variation in dietary intake and PA can be found in the online supplementary material, Supplemental Tables S7 and S8 and supplementary literature review.
A rationale for the existence of distinct temporally conditioned behavioural patterns, as observed in the present study, could be provided by the fact that human behaviour during weekdays is highly structured and generally dictated by time spent at work/in school(Reference McCarthy64), and many health promotion initiatives target the weekdays. A change in daily structures during the weekend is likely to have consequences for habitual behaviours like eating and PA. Friday is a transition day between the week and the weekend, with the first half of the day being lived like a weekday and the second half like a weekend day, explaining the observed Friday PA behaviour, which resembles weekdays, and the observed Friday dietary intake behaviour, which resembles weekends. Some of the observed weekly variation in dietary intake may arise from differences in social, environmental and contextual factors of food intake during weekdays and weekend days. De Castro demonstrated that meal sizes during weekends were larger, meal duration was longer, and meals were ingested later and in the company of more people(Reference de Castro47,Reference de Castro65,Reference de Castro66) . The observed decrease in PA during the weekend could be related to a difference in leisure activities and increased sedentary activities, for instance screen time, as indicated in previous studies(Reference Hjorth, Chaput and Michaelsen11,Reference Hart, Raynor and Osterholt67,Reference Rey-López, Vicente-Rodriguez and Ortega68) .
Only a few other studies have included analysis of weekly variation in different age strata, showing that weekly variation in dietary intake is smaller in elderly compared with younger adults(Reference An13,Reference Thompson, Larkin and Brown14,Reference Haines, Hama and Guilkey17,Reference de Castro65) . However, children have been found to have smaller weekly variation compared with adults(Reference Monteiro, Hassan and Estima16,Reference Haines, Hama and Guilkey17) . In the present analysis, children were the main drivers of the weekly variation of added sugar, sugar-sweetened beverages, discretionary foods and step counts. Children and teens may display the most distinct weekly patterns because their weekdays are highly structured and influenced by time spent at school, while adults’ behaviour may be subject to additional complexity posed by different work routines and preferences. Among older adults, retirement brings an obvious change to the structure of the week and is a likely determinant of a lesser day-of-the-week effect in free-living elderly. Children’s dietary habits are also subject to parental influence(Reference Taylor, Evers and McKenna69). A study of Danish parents’ motives for giving their children sugar-rich foods found that parents try to limit children’s sugar intake during weekdays and as a consequence sugar-rich foods are ‘reserved’ for the weekend(Reference Iversen, Matthiessen and Fagt70).
Present findings pinpoint weekends as the time of the week when physical inactivity and unfavourable dietary behaviour dominate, and should be targeted by public health interventions. An evaluation of potential long-term adverse health effects of the observed weekly variation in diet and activity would be important for assessing the public health relevance of such patterns, especially the potential contribution to overweight/obesity and related co-morbidities. Acute effects of weekend health behaviour have been observed for different blood markers of metabolic health in Danish populations(Reference Hjorth, Damsgaard and Michaelsen25,Reference Jaskolowski, Ritz and Sjodin26) . These studies demonstrated that TAG, leptin and homeostatic model assessment of insulin resistance were highest immediately after the weekend and lowered gradually during weekdays, whereas ghrelin increased(Reference Hjorth, Damsgaard and Michaelsen25), and the effects were especially large among children and adolescents(Reference Jaskolowski, Ritz and Sjodin26). Plausible contributors to such changes in blood markers could be the components of dietary intake and PA that displayed substantial weekly variation in the present study.
Measurement of both dietary intake and PA in the same individuals allows for consideration of the combined effect of both sides of the energy balance equation: energy intake and energy expenditure. Energy intake increased by 7–20 % on Friday and weekend days in the present study. On a population level there is no indication of compensatory increased energy expenditure by PA, since step counts per day decreased by 10–17 % on Saturday and Sunday, and therefore positive energy balance is a concern. Few studies have investigated the impacts of weekly variation in health behaviour on long-term health outcomes or weight control, but weekly weight fluctuations have been observed, indicating a small weight increase during weekends(Reference Racette, Weiss and Schechtman27,Reference Orsama, Mattila and Ermes28) . A positive energy balance during the weekend, without compensation during the weekdays, could have a cumulative effect on body weight in the long term, but such effects remain to be investigated.
The current analysis of both PA and dietary intake in a large, nationally representative sample of individuals from 4 to 75 years of age enables consideration of weekly patterns across the lifespan and is to our knowledge the first of its kind. A particular strength of the present study is the wide array of dietary variables examined, providing a versatile presentation of dietary quality and consumption habits. Furthermore, PA is objectively measured and step counts were expressed as both absolute (per day) and relative to wear-time (per minute), as well as adjusted for cycling, which is important since Danes frequently cycle to work as active transportation(Reference Hallal, Andersen and Bull71). That said, other non-ambulatory activities, such as weightlifting and swimming, are not captured by pedometers and the volume and/or intensity of such activities could be different in weekdays and weekend days. However, at a population level, non-ambulatory activities (apart from cycling) contribute only a little to the total volume of PA in Denmark(Reference Matthiessen, Andersen and Raustorp29). Previous studies in Danish children and adolescents using accelerometers corroborate our finding of lower PA levels during weekends(Reference Hjorth, Chaput and Michaelsen11,Reference Kristensen, Korsholm and Møller20) , and self-reported activity data in Danish adults indicate further that Sunday is the least active day of the week(Reference Matthiessen, Rothausen and Sørensen72). Another limitation of the study is that the response rate in DANSDA was 54 %. This rather low response rate may compromise the representativeness of the sample. Compared with the general Danish population, the study population in DANSDA was approximately representative in terms of gender and age, but individuals with a short education were under-represented(Reference Pedersen, Christensen and Matthiessen4). In a large study of US adults, individuals with high-school education and below appeared to have a smaller weekly variation for selected variables of dietary intake compared with individuals with college education and above(Reference An13). Provided that these patterns can be extended to the Danish population, an overestimation of our findings due to non-response bias cannot be excluded. Furthermore, Monday and Tuesday as starting days for dietary recording were over-represented, while Saturday and Sunday were under-represented. Previous analyses on DANSDA have indicated moderately larger under-reporting of energy intake on the first day (experimenter effect) and the last day (fatigue effect) of recording(Reference Biltoft-Jensen, Matthiessen and Rasmussen32), which may give rise to an underestimated energy intake on weekdays due to relatively higher occurrence of these days as start and end days. Finally, under-reporting of dietary intake is inevitably present and imprecision of estimates is inherent, but pose a threat to the legitimacy of the observed weekly patterns only if the misreporting and imprecision are differential with respect to weekday. A higher energy intake at weekends could provoke a larger absolute under-reporting for weekends but is unlikely a substantial source of error, since relative under-reporting is likely to be present on all days of the week and sensitivity analysis with and without misreporters showed similar results. Further, a tendency towards a selective under-reporting of energy-dense, high-sugar and high-fat foods has been documented(Reference Rasmussen, Matthiessen and Biltoft-Jensen73,Reference Heitmann, Lissner and Osler74) , but such misreporting would, if anything, give rise to an underestimation of the observed weekday–weekend difference, since these types of foods are more commonly consumed at weekends.
Conclusion
In conclusion, results of the present study indicate that dietary intake and PA of Danes fluctuate substantially during the week, with weekend days having consistently lower dietary quality and less PA compared with weekdays. The weekly variation appears especially large among children and modest among the elderly population. Present findings prompt the question whether the large variation could be deconstructed and health behaviour in the weekend improved in an effort to improve public health. This effort should primarily target children and adolescents and their parents to combat the large weekly differences observed and to impact long-term behaviour. Additionally, present findings suggest that proportional sampling of weekdays and weekend days is important when monitoring average diet and PA. Finally, the potential long-term implications of weekly variation on weight management and development of non-communicable diseases need to be explored to enhance understanding of the public health impacts of substantial weekly rhythms in health behaviour.
Acknowledgements
Acknowledgements: The authors are grateful to data warehouse manager Karsten Kørup and senior advisor Tue Christensen for contributions to data processing in DANSDA and provision of data in the present study. Financial support: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: M.N., M.F.H., J.M. and A.B.-J. designed the research and formulated the research question; M.N. performed the statistical analysis; C.R. provided statistical counselling; M.N. and M.F.H. wrote the manuscript; J.M. and A.B.-J. were responsible for the design and collection of data in the Danish National Survey of Dietary Habits and Physical Activity; all authors contributed to the discussion of analyses, critically reviewed the manuscript and approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency. Verbal informed consent was obtained from all participants. The Danish National Committee on Health Research Ethics reviewed the study protocol and all procedures involving research study participants and concluded that DANSDA did not require approval by this authority according to Danish law.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980019003707