Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:30:15.868Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

No good deed goes unpunished: the social costs of prosocial behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 July 2021

Nichola J. Raihani*
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, UK
Eleanor A. Power
Affiliation:
Department of Methodology, London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE, UK
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Performing costly helpful behaviours can allow individuals to improve their reputation. Those who gain a good reputation are often preferred as interaction partners and are consequently better able to access support through cooperative relationships with others. However, investing in prosocial displays can sometimes yield social costs: excessively generous individuals risk losing their good reputation, and even being vilified, ostracised or antisocially punished. As a consequence, people frequently try to downplay their prosocial actions or hide them from others. In this review, we explore when and why investments in prosocial behaviour are likely to yield social costs. We propose two key features of interactions that make it more likely that generous individuals will incur social costs when: (a) observers infer that helpful behaviour is motivated by strategic or selfish motives; and (b) observers infer that helpful behaviour is detrimental to them. We describe how the cognition required to consider ulterior motives emerges over development and how these tendencies vary across cultures – and discuss how the potential for helpful actions to result in social costs might place boundaries on prosocial behaviour as well as limiting the contexts in which it might occur. We end by outlining the key avenues and priorities for future research.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media Summary: Good deeds can result in social costs, particularly when observers suspect calculated or malign motives.

Introduction

Investing in costly cooperative behaviour can yield benefits

Humans are outliers in the extent and frequency of our cooperation, particularly when this occurs with non-relatives and in settings where the potential for direct reciprocal benefits is low (Raihani & Bshary, Reference Raihani and Bshary2015; Rand & Nowak, Reference Rand and Nowak2013; Melis & Semmann, Reference Melis and Semmann2010; Bshary & Raihani, Reference Bshary and Raihani2017; Henrich, Reference Henrich2017; Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Boyd, Bowles, Camerer, Fehr, Gintis and Tracer2005; Boyd, Reference Boyd2017). Helping behaviour may be under positive selection because these costly investments allow an actor to signal something about themselves to observers (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Power, Heap, Puurtinen and Sosis2019). For instance, helpful acts can allow an individual to reveal their type (Gintis et al., Reference Gintis, SMITH and Bowles2001) or their intentions (McNamara & Barta, Reference McNamara and Barta2020; Roberts, Reference Roberts2020; Singh & Hoffman, Reference Singh and Hoffman2021). Receivers can benefit from attending to such signals as this allows them to identify and preferentially assort with cooperative interaction partners (Barclay, Reference Barclay2016; Baumard et al., Reference Baumard, André and Sperber2013; McNamara et al., Reference McNamara, Barta, Fromhage and Houston2008). Theoretical models have confirmed the evolutionary logic of reputation-based cooperation, via either indirect reciprocity (Kandori, Reference Kandori1992; Ohtsuki & Iwasa, Reference Ohtsuki and Iwasa2006) or reputation-based partner choice (Fu et al., Reference Fu, Hauert, Nowak and Wang2008; Li, Reference Li2014; Roberts, Reference Roberts1998, Reference Roberts2015). Experimental evidence from laboratory studies (including from non-humans; Bshary & Grutter, Reference Bshary and Grutter2006) shows that the possibility of attracting partners is likely to be an important force for explaining the evolution and persistence of cooperation among non-relatives (Barclay & Barker, Reference Barclay and Barker2020; Barclay & Raihani, Reference Barclay and Raihani2016; Raihani & Barclay, Reference Raihani and Barclay2016; Sylwester & Roberts, Reference Sylwester and Roberts2010, Reference Sylwester and Roberts2013; Barclay & Willer, Reference Barclay and Willer2007; Tognetti et al., Reference Tognetti, Berticat, Raymond and Faurie2012; Van Vugt & Hardy, Reference Van Vugt and Hardy2009; Fehrler & Przepiorka, Reference Fehrler and Przepiorka2016). Data from real-world settings (Hardy & Norgaard, Reference Hardy and Norgaard2016; Milinski et al., Reference Milinski, Semmann and Krambeck2002a; Przepiorka et al., Reference Przepiorka, Norbutas and Corten2017; Raihani & Smith, Reference Raihani and Smith2015; Resnick et al., Reference Resnick, Zeckhauser, Swanson and Lockwood2006; Yoeli et al., Reference Yoeli, Hoffman, Rand and Nowak2013) from many societies around the world (Bliege Bird et al., Reference Bliege Bird, Scelza, Bird and Smith2012; Bliege Bird & Power, Reference Bliege Bird and Power2015; Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Allen-Arave, Hill and Hurtado2000; Lyle & Smith, Reference Lyle and Smith2014; Macfarlan et al., Reference Macfarlan, Remiker and Quinlan2012, Reference Macfarlan, Quinlan and Remiker2013; Macfarlan & Lyle, Reference Macfarlan and Lyle2015; Power & Ready, Reference Power and Ready2018; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Bird and Bird2003; Smith & Bird, Reference Smith and Bird2000; von Rueden et al., Reference von Rueden, Redhead, O'Gorman, Kaplan and Gurven2019) also highlight the benefits that stem from being generous (or being seen to be generous): generous individuals are more likely to be helped themselves (Bliege Bird & Power, Reference Bliege Bird and Power2015; Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Allen-Arave, Hill and Hurtado2000; Lyle & Smith, Reference Lyle and Smith2014; Macfarlan et al., Reference Macfarlan, Remiker and Quinlan2012, Reference Macfarlan, Quinlan and Remiker2013; Power & Ready, Reference Power and Ready2018), are healthier (Lyle & Smith, Reference Lyle and Smith2014) and have more and healthier offspring (von Rueden & Jaeggi, Reference von Rueden and Jaeggi2016).

But benefits do not always follow from prosocial actions

It is perhaps less well appreciated that cooperative actions can result in social costs rather than benefits. By revealing their cooperative intentions, individuals might leave themselves vulnerable to exploitation by others (e.g. Bereczkei et al., Reference Bereczkei, Szabo and Czibor2015), particularly when players only expect to interact once (Roberts, Reference Roberts2020). Yet signalling prosociality can also invite hostility and competition from others. For instance, individuals often refuse offers of help (Ackerman & Kenrick, Reference Ackerman and Kenrick2008), dislike those who offer help (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Nadler and Whitcher-Alagna1982) and may find overly generous individuals unattractive (Bhogal et al., Reference Bhogal, Farrelly, Galbraith, Manktelow and Bradley2020). Those who perform morally laudable behaviours in public also risk being accused of ‘virtue signalling’ (Levy, Reference Levy2020). In laboratory social dilemma games, people's antipathy towards prosocial individuals often manifests as antisocial punishment (Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Thoni and Gachter2008; Sylwester et al., Reference Sylwester, Herrmann and Bryson2013). Although people generally tend to direct punishment at the free-riders in the group (Falk et al., Reference Falk, Fehr and Fischbacher2005; Fehr & Gächter, Reference Fehr and Gächter2000, Reference Fehr and Gächter2002; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Dawes, Fowler, McElreath and Smirnov2009; Nikiforakis & Engelmann, Reference Nikiforakis and Engelmann2011), a persistent fraction of players in social dilemma games sacrifice some of their personal endowment to fine cooperative co-players (Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Thoni and Gachter2008; Raihani & Bshary, Reference Raihani and Bshary2019; Sylwester et al., Reference Sylwester, Herrmann and Bryson2013). Variants of such games that allow players to vote to evict members of the group rather than to financially punish them find similar results: the most cooperative group members are evicted as often as the worst free-riders (Parks & Stone, Reference Parks and Stone2010). The tendency to dislike or to disparage prosocial or morally laudable others has also been studied under the banner of do-gooder derogation (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Wu and Bao2019; Bolderdijk et al., Reference Bolderdijk, Brouwer and Cornelissen2018; Minson & Monin, Reference Minson and Monin2012; Monin, Reference Monin2007; Sparkman & Attari, Reference Sparkman and Attari2020a; Zane et al., Reference Zane, Irwin and Reczek2016) – a phenomenon whereby individuals who perform morally laudable actions (e.g. refraining from eating animal products or defending minority groups) are derogated by peers. This tendency to dislike generous or moral others has also been found in children as young as 8 years old (Tasimi et al., Reference Tasimi, Dominguez and Wynn2015). Although it is clear that good deeds can result in social costs, less work has systematically explored when and why these costs arise. We attempt to do so in this paper.

When and why might helpful signals yield social costs?

We propose that generosity is least likely to yield benefits and most likely to result in social costs in two, non-mutually exclusive scenarios:

  1. (1) observers infer that helpful behaviour is motivated by strategic or selfish motives;

  2. (2) observers infer that helpful behaviour is detrimental to them.

In what follows, we use the word observer to mean ‘someone who learns about the prosocial action of another individual’. In principle, the observer could learn because they are the recipient of the prosocial act or because they are an uninvolved bystander. We use the word observer to denote both possibilities. We expand on these predictions below.

Observers infer strategic or selfish motives

Praiseworthiness depends on attributing intentions to actors (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Crockett and Pizarro2020; Bénabou & Tirole, Reference Bénabou and Tirole2006; Pizarro et al., Reference Pizarro, Uhlmann and Salovey2003; Siegel et al., Reference Siegel, Crockett and Dolan2017), but the motives underpinning helpful actions are not visible to an observer. Instead, motives are typically inferred based on the helper's behaviour (e.g. the type and extent of help offered), any emotions revealed during the behaviour and the context (including the sociocultural setting, see Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Bolyanatz, Crittenden, Fessler, Fitzpatrick, Gurven and Laurence2016; Henrich, Reference Henrich2020) in which the behaviour occurs.

Motives and outcomes do not always cohere: bad outcomes can stem from benign intentions, and good outcomes can stem from malign or selfish intentions. In principle, helpful actions can stem from proximately ‘altruistic’ motivations – that is, a genuine desire to help others (e.g. Andreoni, Reference Andreoni1990; Batson et al., Reference Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley and Birch1981; Bénabou & Tirole, Reference Bénabou and Tirole2006; Harbaugh et al., Reference Harbaugh, Mayr and Burghart2007). Nevertheless, at least part of the motive underpinning helpful actions may also be strategic or self-interested. For example, in experimental Dictator and Ultimatum games, a standard result is that people typically offer more money to a partner in the Ultimatum than in the Dictator game (Camerer, Reference Camerer2011). This result can be understood in light of the strategic nature of the Ultimatum game (where an offer that is perceived to be too low can be rejected by the responder) compared with the non-strategic nature of the Dictator game (where receivers must accept whatever share the dictator sends). Similarly, people often behave more generously in situations where their behaviour can be observed (e.g. Andreoni & Petrie, Reference Andreoni and Petrie2004; Bereczkei et al., Reference Bereczkei, Birkas and Kerekes2007; Harbaugh, Reference Harbaugh1998; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, McCabe, Shachat and Smith1994; Yoeli et al., Reference Yoeli, Hoffman, Rand and Nowak2013) and can affect how others may treat them in future (Barclay & Willer, Reference Barclay and Willer2007; Milinski et al., Reference Milinski, Semmann and Krambeck2002b; Semmann et al., Reference Semmann, Krambeck and Milinski2004; Sylwester & Roberts, Reference Sylwester and Roberts2010, Reference Sylwester and Roberts2013), which also indicates that prosocial actions are influenced by strategic concerns.

These patterns hint at the complexity of decoding the motives underpinning generosity: some helpful behaviour may well be motived by pure altruism, but it is clear that humans are also sensitive to the possibility of personal gains and adjust their investments in helpful behaviour accordingly (Semmann et al., Reference Semmann, Krambeck and Milinski2004), even from a young age (Warneken et al., Reference Warneken, Sebastián-Enesco, Benjamin and Pieloch2019). Observers (and recipients) of prosocial acts therefore face the difficult task of reconciling the actions they observe with a range of possible underlying motives. Some of these motives are likely to be more indicative of the helper's underlying cooperative disposition than others.

We propose that observers will be more likely to infer that help stems from strategic rather than altruistic motives when helpful actions appear to be aggrandising, for example if they are performed in public or are actively broadcast to others by the helpful individual. Several studies have shown that individuals who advertise their good deeds (Berman et al., Reference Berman, Levine, Barasch and Small2015; De Freitas et al., Reference De Freitas, DeScioli, Thomas and Pinker2019; Siem & Stürmer, Reference Siem and Stürmer2018) or who know that their good deeds will be visible to others (Gambetta & Przepiorka, Reference Gambetta and Przepiorka2014) risk being perceived as less generous by observers (Srna et al., Reference Srna, Barasch and Small2017). Similar results have been obtained in the context of corporate social responsibility (CSR). Specifically, consumers are more sceptical of CSR activities when they suspect that these deeds stem from profit-seeking rather than benevolent motives (Berge & Arshad, Reference Berge and Arshad2020; Chernev & Blair, Reference Chernev and Blair2015; Sen et al., Reference Sen, Du and Bhattacharya2016) and they downgrade their assessment of CSR initiatives when they learn of these from the company's advertising rather than from other sources (Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Gürhan-Canli and Schwarz2006).

In general, positive acts are more likely to result in reputation benefits when observers can infer that the helper is motivated by empathic concern or that they are eager and willing to help. Indeed, the reputation consequences of actions may be affected more by the emotions attributed to the actor than to the outcome of the action itself (Yudkin et al., Reference Yudkin, Prosser and Crockett2019). People rely more on emotions than on outcomes when judging helpful individuals because emotions provide a more reliable cue as to the person's underlying character or disposition (Barasch et al., Reference Barasch, Levine, Berman and Small2014; Frank, Reference Frank1991; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Yoeli and Nowak2015; Levine et al., Reference Levine, Barasch, Rand, Berman and Small2018; Reed et al., Reference Reed, Zeglen and Schmidt2012) or to the value they place on the relationship (Aktipis et al., Reference Aktipis, Cronk, Alcock, Ayers, Baciu, Balliet and Winfrey2018; Ames et al., Reference Ames, Flynn and Weber2004; Delton & Robertson, Reference Delton and Robertson2016; Frank, Reference Frank1991; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Yoeli and Nowak2015) – and are therefore a more reliable guide to how the person might behave in future (see also Hirshleifer, Reference Hirshleifer1987; Singh & Hoffman, Reference Singh and Hoffman2021). For instance, people who perform prosocial actions but display reluctance or negative emotions while doing so are unlikely to accrue reputation benefits as the negative emotional signal nullifies the positive act (Ames & Johar, Reference Ames and Johar2009; Carlson & Zaki, Reference Carlson and Zaki2018; Krull et al., Reference Krull, Seger and Silvera2008). Similarly, people who express mixed motives for helping or who help without expressing empathy are also less positively evaluated by others (Erlandsson et al., Reference Erlandsson, Wingren and Andersson2020). When evaluating the moral consequences of charitable donations, people rate those who donate time as being more praiseworthy than those who donate money (even though people also acknowledge that time donations are often less efficient; Johnson & Park, Reference Johnson and Park2019). This evaluative premium on time arises in part because people perceive time donations as signalling a greater emotional investment in the cause and therefore a better indicator of the donor's underlying character.

The idea that observers try to discern a helper's underlying character or disposition when evaluating prosocial acts is also evident in the negative reaction to hypocrisy or inconsistent behaviour (Effron et al., Reference Effron, O'Connor, Leroy and Lucas2018; Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Sommers, Bloom and Rand2017). For example, in one vignette-based study, individuals who signalled moral virtue (by criticising others) but subsequently behaved immorally themselves were more negatively judged than individuals who admitted their moral failings (Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Sommers, Bloom and Rand2017). Similar reactions are observed in the domain of CSR and can help to explain why these activities sometimes backfire: if companies with bad reputations engage in CSR, consumers are more likely to infer that these socially responsible actions stem from a desire to whitewash a bad reputation (Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Gürhan-Canli and Schwarz2006). CSR activities that seem at odds with the broader mission of the company can also negatively impact brand evaluations (Silver et al., Reference Silver, Newman and Small2021; Torelli et al., Reference Torelli, Monga and Kaikati2012). Indeed, companies that emphasise the business case for CSR tend to be more positively evaluated than those who emphasise the moral case because emphasising the business case allows the company to avoid accusations of moral hypocrisy (Hafenbrädl & Waeger, Reference Hafenbrädl and Waeger2019). A key point here seems to be that being upfront about the underlying reasons (or potential benefits) associated with performing prosocial deeds can quell the suspicion these deeds might otherwise arouse in some cases.

Relatedly, observers might also suspect that helping stems from strategic rather than altruistic motives if the helper deliberates or assesses the costs before deciding whether to help (Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Yoeli and Nowak2015). Participants who cooperate without knowing the cost of doing so (Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Hoffman, Nowak and Rand2016) – or who make their decision quickly (Critcher et al., Reference Critcher, Inbar and Pizarro2013; Evans & van de Calseyde, Reference Evans and van de Calseyde2017) – are perceived as more prosocial or trustworthy by others – as are those who make deontological, rather than utilitarian, decisions when faced with moral dilemmas (Everett et al., Reference Everett, Pizarro and Crockett2016). Similarly, individuals who donate based on the cost-effectiveness of the charity, rather than based on the degree of empathy they feel for the beneficiaries, are evaluated less positively by others (Montealegre et al., Reference Montealegre, Bush, Moss, Pizarro and Jimenez-Leal2020). Subjects who cooperate in seemingly one-shot encounters are also perceived as more trustworthy than those who cooperate in encounters that they know will be repeated (Johnsen & Kvaløy, Reference Johnsen and Kvaløy2016), perhaps because cooperating in interactions that are known to be repeated is more indicative of strategic motives.

The appearance of strategic, calculating decision-making is particularly costly in long-term reciprocal exchanges (‘friendships’), when an effort to quickly reciprocate and erase a debt may reduce what could be seen as an open-ended cooperative partnership to a disinterested series of transactions (Bourdieu, Reference Bourdieu and Nice1990; Silk, Reference Silk and Hammerstein2003). The logic of tit-for-tat in a long-term relationship can be damaging because it implies low interdependence (Aktipis et al., Reference Aktipis, Cronk, Alcock, Ayers, Baciu, Balliet and Winfrey2018; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Zhang and Norton2019; Roberts, Reference Roberts2005). This can explain why subjects tend to repay favours from strangers more quickly than those from friends (Boster et al., Reference Boster, Rodriguez, Cruz and Marshall1995) and can take offence – and judge friendships as less close – when friends immediately reciprocate kind acts (Shackelford & Buss, Reference Shackelford and Buss1996; Silk, Reference Silk and Hammerstein2003) or give gifts with the expectation of adulation or return favours (Lee, Reference Lee1969).

Motives might also be inferred based on whether the helper materially benefits from their actions. For example, donors to charity are perceived as less charitable if they donate to a cause to which they have a personal connection (Lin-Healy & Small, Reference Lin-Healy and Small2012), and advocates for a charitable cause are perceived as less sincere and garner fewer donations for the cause if the advocates reap financial benefits from this association (Barasch et al., Reference Barasch, Berman and Small2016). In corporate settings, organisations that perform good deeds are given less credit if doing good helps their bottom line (Makov & Newman, Reference Makov and Newman2016; Newman & Cain, Reference Newman and Cain2014), an effect known as ‘tainted altruism’ (Newman & Cain, Reference Newman and Cain2014). In keeping with this view, people more positively judge helpful actions when these involve a degree of personal sacrifice (Bigman & Tamir, Reference Bigman and Tamir2016; Carlson & Zaki, Reference Carlson and Zaki2018; Johnson, Reference Johnson2018; Kawamura et al., Reference Kawamura, Ohtsubo and Kusumi2020; Nelissen, Reference Nelissen2008; Visserman et al., Reference Visserman, Righetti, Impett, Keltner and Van Lange2018). Indeed, in the context of charitable donations, people are more sensitive to personal sacrifice than social benefit when evaluating prosocial behaviour (Johnson, Reference Johnson2018). This increased perception of costs, and so an assumed lesser material benefit, can explain why lower status helpers are sometimes perceived in a more positive light. A vignette study found that lower class donors (measured in terms of occupation and salary) were more likely to be seen as prosocial, compared with higher class donors, when giving the same (either relative or absolute) donation to a charity, as the lower-status individuals were seen as more authentically motivated (Yuan et al., Reference Yuan, Wu and Kou2018).

Observers infer that helping behaviour is detrimental to them

Although prosocial acts are typically interpreted as yielding benefits to others, this may not always be the case. Instead, observers might sometimes perceive (accurately or not) that a prosocial act is likely to harm, rather than to benefit, them. For this reason, the reputation consequences of helping are also likely to depend on who is watching: helpful acts might improve your reputation in the eyes of some individuals but not others. Broadly, it seems plausible that observers who incur costs or infer that they are disadvantaged as a consequence of the helpful actions of another individual will be more likely to evaluate the helpful individual negatively. We outline some specific cases below.

One of the most obvious ways that observers might incur costs from the actions of helpful individuals is due to social comparison. A good reputation is, by definition, a positional good – a person's reputation is ‘good’ in relation to the reputations of other individuals to whom that individual is compared (Barclay, Reference Barclay2011, Reference Barclay2013, Reference Barclay2016; Samu et al., Reference Samu, Számadó and Takács2020). Prosocial actions that improve one person's reputation (or can be construed as potentially doing so) can therefore provoke competitive responses from those whose reputation may suffer by comparison (e.g. Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Engelmann and Tomasello2019; Macfarlan et al., Reference Macfarlan, Remiker and Quinlan2012; McAndrew & Perilloux, Reference McAndrew and Perilloux2012; Pleasant & Barclay, Reference Pleasant and Barclay2018; Raihani & Smith, Reference Raihani and Smith2015; Sylwester & Roberts, Reference Sylwester and Roberts2013). Similarly, if status hierarchies are formed in part on the basis of patronage and largesse, then one person's generous acts may have the effect of lowering others’ relative positions within that hierarchy. Gift-giving or hosting large feasts, for example, may be arenas for status competition and the demonstration of power and authority (Boone, Reference Boone1998); such acts may therefore invite negative reactions from those displeased with how they will look in comparison. Studies exploring intergroup relations have shown that help directed from higher to lower status groups can be resisted as it can be interpreted as an attempt to assert or maintain dominance. For example, when asked to imagine receiving help from an out-group, Israeli Arabs were more likely to judge help from Israeli Jews as being directed at reinforcing existing power dynamics (Halabi et al., Reference Halabi, Dovidio and Nadler2016; Nadler & Halabi, Reference Nadler and Halabi2006). Cross-cultural work using economic games also suggests that people resist generous overtures that can be interpreted as attempts to establish dominance (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Boyd, Bowles, Camerer, Fehr, Gintis and Tracer2005).

Antisocial punishment and other hostile reactions to cooperators observed in social dilemmas are thought to be prompted at least in part by social comparison. For example, in a public goods game, where players could vote to evict one player from the group after each round, players often voted to evict the most generous member from the group (Parks & Stone, Reference Parks and Stone2010). When asked to justify these decisions, participants frequently referenced their own reputation, for example ‘No one else is doing what he does. He makes us all look bad’ and ‘Next to Person Blue I look like a bad guy, but I don't think I am’. Other studies have also shown that excessive generosity is judged unfavourably and may even be punished (Bahry & Wilson, Reference Bahry and Wilson2006; Irwin & Horne, Reference Irwin and Horne2013; Kawamura & Kusumi, Reference Kawamura and Kusumi2020). In contrast, antisocial punishment of overly cooperative individuals is rare when the punisher is a third party, rather than a player who was involved in the public goods game (Bone et al., Reference Bone, S. and Raihani2014). Since the third party cannot contribute to the public good, their reputation as a generous or cooperative individual is not affected by the investments of the other players. The fact that uninvolved parties do not tend to antisocially punish therefore supports the idea that hostile reactions to helpful individuals are mediated by social comparison. In a more recent study, Pleasant and Barclay (Reference Pleasant and Barclay2018) showed that antisocial punishment in a public goods game was most prevalent when individuals were being evaluated as a partner for a subsequent Trust Game, which further supports the idea that antisocial punishment operates via concern for reputation and relative standing. As with antisocial punishment, experimental studies have shown that do-gooder derogation frequently stems from competitive motives – because people who appear to be morally superior can prompt less moral individuals to evaluate themselves more negatively (Minson & Monin, Reference Minson and Monin2012; Monin, Reference Monin2007; Monin et al., Reference Monin, Sawyer and Marquez2008).

Observers might also negatively evaluate helpful individuals when the helper's and the observer's goals are misaligned (Melnikoff & Bailey, Reference Melnikoff and Bailey2018). For example, people prefer moral traits in others (such as mercifulness, loyalty and trustworthiness) only when these traits are conducive to their current goal. In scenarios where it would benefit these individuals to interact with merciless, fickle or dishonest others, then these traits are more positively evaluated (Melnikoff & Bailey, Reference Melnikoff and Bailey2018). A similar discrepancy has been observed in moral dilemmas, such as the trolley problem, where people prefer deontological agents when choosing social or romantic partners but prefer consequentialist (utilitarian) individuals when electing political leaders (Everett et al., Reference Everett, Faber, Savulescu and Crockett2018).

Another way in which a helper's goals might be misaligned with an observer's is when the help offered to one person is traded off against the help that can be offered to another (Law et al., Reference Law, Campbell and Gaesser2019). For example, in Western cultures, helping strangers is typically perceived as being a morally positive thing to do (Henrich, Reference Henrich2020; McManus et al., Reference McManus, Kleiman-Weiner and Young2020). However, even in a sample of US residents, people who help strangers instead of helping family were generally judged more negatively (McManus et al., Reference McManus, Kleiman-Weiner and Young2020), indicating that helpers’ reputations are formed on the basis of who is helped and the trade-offs involved. Related to the above, helpful acts can sometimes threaten the observer's existing relationship with the helper – for example by calling into question the closeness of the relationship (Law et al., Reference Law, Campbell and Gaesser2019) and thereby potentially sparking jealousy (Krems et al., Reference Krems, Williams, Aktipis and Kenrick2021).

Helpers might also be judged more negatively when the help disadvantages the observer's in-group or is directed towards the observer's out-group (cf. Fessler et al., Reference Fessler, Barrett, Kanovsky, Stich, Holbrook, Henrich and Laurence2015). For example, one study measured participants’ reactions to another individual who confronted discrimination, involving both sexism and racism as a function of whether the confronter was part of the disadvantaged group (e.g. the confronter is also a woman in the sexism example) or part of the advantaged group (e.g. the confronter is a man in the sexism example; Kutlaca et al., Reference Kutlaca, Becker and Radke2020). In this study, participants were more likely to judge that the confronter had over-reacted if both the participant and the confronter were members of the same advantaged in-group. Similarly, whistleblowers within organisations are seldom rewarded for their actions and are often more likely to be bullied than those who keep quiet (Bjørkelo et al., Reference Bjørkelo, Einarsen, Nielsen and Matthiesen2011). The evaluation of any particular ‘helpful act’ must, then, lie in the eye of the beholder.

Reputation management strategies in humans

Given the potential social costs associated with performing overtly generous or moral actions, it is not surprising that people manage their reputations in quite sophisticated ways, for example by refraining from doing good deeds in public or by actively hiding their good deeds from others. While many studies have shown that people are more cooperative when their behaviour is public (e.g. Andreoni & Petrie, Reference Andreoni and Petrie2004; Bekkers & Wiepking, Reference Bekkers and Wiepking2011; Bereczkei et al., Reference Bereczkei, Birkas and Kerekes2007; Bohnet & Frey, Reference Bohnet and Frey1999; Piazza & Bering, Reference Piazza and Bering2008; Yoeli et al., Reference Yoeli, Hoffman, Rand and Nowak2013), other studies report mixed or contrasting results. Notably, a recent meta-analysis found that although there is a small and statistically significant positive effect of observability on prosocial behaviour, the effect is highly heterogeneous and context-dependent (Bradley et al., Reference Bradley, Lawrence and Ferguson2018). For example, public recognition can decrease people's willingness to donate blood (Shi, Reference Shi2011) or to donate to charity (Denis et al., Reference Denis, Pecheux and Warlop2020; Savary & Goldsmith, Reference Savary and Goldsmith2020; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, White and Laran2018). Experimental studies have shown that individuals feel uncomfortable when they learn that they are morally superior to their peers (Cowgill, Reference Cowgill2017) or when their moral actions are highlighted to others (Wang & Tong, Reference Wang and Tong2015) and these negative feelings might prompt individuals to hide their good deeds from others. For instance, when making charitable donations online, people often hide their donations from others by making them anonymously (Big Charitable Gifts, 2020; Imada, Reference Imada2020) and the tendency to donate anonymously (either by hiding their name or the amount donated) is higher when the donation is especially generous (Mokos & Scheuring, Reference Mokos and Scheuring2019; Peacey & Sanders, Reference Peacey and Sanders2014; Raihani, Reference Raihani2014). Similar results have been obtained in experimental settings (Jones & Linardi, Reference Jones and Linardi2014), although we note that most of these studies involve WEIRD (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) populations and it remains an open question as to whether these patterns would generalise to different cultures.

People also seem to be sensitive to the possibility that their good deeds will reveal self-interested intentions, and often work to prevent such inferences from being made. For example, when donating to charity, people are more likely to perform charitable actions that are perceived as being painful or difficult (Olivola & Shafir, Reference Olivola and Shafir2013), perhaps to lessen any suspicions of self-interest in ostensibly altruistic behaviour. Similarly, time donations are viewed as more morally praiseworthy than money donations and – as we might expect – people often prefer to donate time than money, even when the latter could have a larger impact (Johnson & Park, Reference Johnson and Park2019). In studies where people could cooperate in a calculated or uncalculated manner (by revealing the cost associated with cooperating), people are more likely to opt for uncalculated cooperation when they know their decision is observed by others (Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Hoffman, Nowak and Rand2016). Similarly, hunters, especially of large game, may practice ‘pecuniary distancing’ by actively avoiding having control over the divvying up and sharing of their catch, so that others cannot infer selfish intent (Bliege Bird & Power, Reference Bliege Bird and Power2015).

Individuals also manage their prosocial reputation in other ways, for example by selectively investing in subtle, rather than ostentatious signals (Bliege Bird et al., Reference Bliege Bird, Ready and Power2018). The very subtlety and discreetness of acts may help ensure that they are seen as generous and well intentioned: when no audience is there to provide a reputational boon, it should be much harder to infer that the intention was one of self-aggrandisement. Helpful acts which are experienced and observed only (or primarily) by the intended recipient may then do much to assure that person of the giver's commitment to their continued relationship.

Recent theoretical work has also shown how obscuring or ‘burying’ costly signals can also be under positive selection (Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Hilbe and Nowak2018). This occurs because buried signals come with a tag that identifies them as having been buried (e.g. we might know whether our wealthy friend chose to donate anonymously on a fundraising platform). This allows the fact that the signal has been buried to act as a signal in itself. Although initial work found that only the ‘best’ signallers would evolve to bury their signals, a subsequent analysis found that a more universal modesty norm can also spread through populations (Quillien, Reference Quillien2019). Empirical studies support these models, by showing that modesty begets modesty and that modest actions are likely to be socially learned. For example, in one study of online charitable giving, the presence of anonymous donations encouraged subsequent donors to anonymise their own donations (Burtch et al., Reference Burtch, Ghose and Wattal2016).

In general, we suggest that concern for reputation might often manifest as behaviour aimed at preventing reputational damage, rather than behaviour aimed at actively improving reputation. This is partly because, as the evidence above indicates, prosocial acts do not always result in social benefits. Not only that, but there is a further asymmetry in the social consequences of blameworthy and praiseworthy acts: there is often more to be lost from being seen as blameworthy than to be gained from being seen as praiseworthy (reviewed in Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Crockett and Pizarro2020). For example, breaking a promise is viewed as morally contemptible, but over-delivering on a promise carries no reputation benefits relative to simply keeping it (Gneezy & Epley, Reference Gneezy and Epley2014). Similarly, moral judgements of selfish actions are moderated by the magnitude of the selfishness (more selfish actions are judged morally worse), but the converse is not true for selfless actions: the benefits of behaving prosocially do not scale linearly with the magnitude of the prosocial deeds (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Grossmann, Uskul, Kraus and Epley2015; Klein & Epley, Reference Klein and Epley2014). Other work has shown that blameworthiness judgements are often more extreme than praise judgements (Guglielmo & Malle, Reference Guglielmo and Malle2019) and more sensitive to outcomes. Given all this, the risk of a generous act resulting in social costs may weigh more heavily in people's minds than the potential benefits that could stem from it.

The cognitive underpinnings of managing and evaluating reputations

There is clearly more to reputation management – and impression formation – than the simple performance of prosocial actions to reap social benefits. Instead, when forming an impression of others, we have to be able to infer the motives and intentions that accompany positive actions. When managing our own reputation, we also have to be able to infer how our own actions – and potentially divergent perceptions of our intentions – will affect how others form and update their assessments of us. The ability to manage one's own reputation and to evaluate the reputations of others relies on ‘epistemic vigilance’ (Sperber et al., Reference Sperber, Clément, Heintz, Mascaro, Mercier, Origgi and Wilson2010) – a suite of cognitive abilities which probably evolved to help us discern reliable from unreliable communication. These abilities emerge at different stages of development (Banerjee, Reference Banerjee2000; Banerjee et al., Reference Banerjee, Heyman, Lee and Jensen2020; Engelmann & Rapp, Reference Engelmann and Rapp2018; Silver & Shaw, Reference Silver and Shaw2018; Sperber et al., Reference Sperber, Clément, Heintz, Mascaro, Mercier, Origgi and Wilson2010) and show differential expression in reputation-based cooperation across societies (Amir & McAuliffe, Reference Amir and McAuliffe2020; Cowell et al., Reference Cowell, Lee, Malcolm-Smith, Selcuk, Zhou and Decety2017).

A key feature of epistemic vigilance is the ability to infer intentions that appear to be at odds with observed actions (e.g. The actions suggest X but the motive is Y). This form of intention attribution is distinct from and computationally more demanding than the simpler version of intention attribution (likely to be present in non-human apes and very young children; Tomasello et al., Reference Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne and Moll2005), where observers impute intentions that cohere with a target's observed behaviour. The development of scepticism – where we suspect that an agent is trying to present themselves in a favourable light – involves doing something even more sophisticated, however: we may intuit not only that A's intentions are at odds with their actions, but also that A would like us (or other observers) to form a false belief about their intentions (e.g. A wants me to believe their motive is X when I suspect in fact it is Y). This latter awareness involves reasoning at a higher meta-representational level (Sperber et al., Reference Sperber, Clément, Heintz, Mascaro, Mercier, Origgi and Wilson2010), which may account for its relatively late emergence during childhood (Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Fu and Lee2007; Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Barner, Heumann and Schenck2014).

Children start to manage their own reputation – and to evaluate the reputations of others – in early childhood (Banerjee et al., Reference Banerjee, Heyman, Lee and Jensen2020; Engelmann & Rapp, Reference Engelmann and Rapp2018; Silver & Shaw, Reference Silver and Shaw2018). The ability to form impressions of others seems to emerge earlier in development than the ability to manage one's own reputation: indeed, some studies have even found that pre-verbal infants evaluate others on the basis of helpfulness (Hamlin, Reference Hamlin2013; Hamlin et al., Reference Hamlin, Wynn and Bloom2011; but see Salvadori et al., Reference Salvadori, Blazsekova, Volein, Karap, Tatone, Mascaro and Csibra2015; Schlingloff et al., Reference Schlingloff, Csibra and Tatone2020 for failed replications). Whether or not the ability to evaluate others emerges in preverbal infants, this ability does seem to be reliably evident by early childhood. By the time they are around two and a half years old, children can discriminate a nice partner from a mean one, only on the basis of observation, and prefer to interact with the nice partner (Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Keupp, Hare, Vaish and Tomasello2013). Children as young as three years old expect that individuals will share information that presents them in a good light, for example, by revealing their positive performance on a puzzle but withholding their failures (Hicks et al., Reference Hicks, Liu and Heyman2015); and by the age of five, children begin to gossip, offering their opinion on the character of others to help a third party to decide who to interact with (Engelmann et al., Reference Engelmann, Herrmann and Tomasello2016). Four-year-olds appreciate that others will lie to benefit themselves and, by the time they are six, children distinguish between self-interested lies and lies intended to benefit others (Banerjee et al., Reference Banerjee, Heyman, Lee and Jensen2020; Mills & Keil, Reference Mills and Keil2005). In one study, children were asked to evaluate a protagonist who either plagiarised someone else's story (and took the credit for themselves) or who lied to give the credit for their own story to another child (Shaw & Olson, Reference Shaw and Olson2015); children were more likely to evaluate the person who stole the credit negatively – and to evaluate the person who gave credit away positively.

In contrast to the early emergence of impression formation, children do not begin managing their own reputation until they are around five years old. By this age, children (but not chimpanzees) are sensitive to the presence of an audience when making prosocial decisions (Engelmann et al., Reference Engelmann, Herrmann and Tomasello2012) – and give more when their decisions will be revealed to others (Leimgruber et al., Reference Leimgruber, Shaw, Santos and Olson2012). Nevertheless, before middle childhood (around the age of 8) children tend not to reason explicitly about how their actions will affect other individuals’ impressions; rather, reputation management in young children has been referred to as ‘implicit’ and may overlap with what has been called ‘audience effects’ in non-human species. By middle childhood, children start to explicitly reason about how their actions will affect other people's impressions of them (Banerjee et al., Reference Banerjee, Heyman, Lee and Jensen2020; Engelmann & Rapp, Reference Engelmann and Rapp2018)– and it is at this point that children also start to understand that other individuals may do the same (Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Fu and Lee2007, Reference Heyman, Fu, Barner, Zhishan, Zhou and Lee2016; Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Barner, Heumann and Schenck2014). For example, although young children do not differentiate between acts of generosity that are performed in public vs. those that are done in private, by the time they are eight years old, children reliably rate private acts of beneficence as more morally praiseworthy (Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Fu, Barner, Zhishan, Zhou and Lee2016; Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Barner, Heumann and Schenck2014). In middle childhood, children also begin to exhibit scepticism about self-serving claims of prosociality (Amemiya et al., Reference Amemiya, Liu, Compton and Heyman2020; Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Fu and Lee2007; Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Barner, Heumann and Schenck2014), although this ability continues to develop over this period. For example, older children (aged nine to 10) appreciate that self-promotional statements are more morally permissible when they can also be construed as implicit offers of help (Amemiya et al., Reference Amemiya, Liu, Compton and Heyman2020).

At the same time as children start to appreciate that others strategically manage their reputation – and develop a sense of scepticism about people who use self-promotional strategies – they also begin to understand the benefits of modest actions and to evaluate modest others as more morally praiseworthy (Banerjee, Reference Banerjee2000). The importance of modesty – and the extent to which children both strive to appear modest and approve of modesty in others – varies cross-culturally, being more strongly emphasised in East-Asian than in Western societies. For example, in a study involving Japanese and American schoolchildren (aged seven to 11 years), the tendency to approve of modest individuals (who lied about their prosocial actions) increased with age in both groups (Heyman et al., Reference Heyman, Itakura and Lee2011). Nevertheless, the Japanese children were also more likely to disapprove of those who truthfully accepted the praise for their good deed, a pattern that was not observed among the American children. A more recent study comparing self-effacement in a sample of Chinese and Canadian children of the same age found that the tendency to eschew moral praise increases with age – but that this effect was more pronounced among the Chinese children (Fu et al., Reference Fu, Heyman, Cameron and Lee2016). These and related studies strongly suggest a role for social learning in the development of modesty traits (see also Banerjee, Reference Banerjee2000) and emphasise the fact that modesty norms (and associated reputation management strategies) are likely to vary cross-culturally (Genyue et al., Reference Genyue, Heyman and Lee2011; Kanagawa et al., Reference Kanagawa, Cross and Markus2001; Yamagishi et al., Reference Yamagishi, Hashimoto, Cook, Kiyonari, Shinada, Mifune and Li2012). Cross-cultural evidence generally suggests that responsiveness to local norms solidifies in middle childhood, and that this can precipitate important variation in prosocial behaviour (Amir & McAuliffe, Reference Amir and McAuliffe2020; Blake et al., Reference Blake, McAuliffe, Corbit, Callaghan, Barry, Bowie and Warneken2015; Cowell et al., Reference Cowell, Lee, Malcolm-Smith, Selcuk, Zhou and Decety2017; House et al., Reference House, Silk, Henrich, Barrett, Scelza, Boyette and Laurence2013, Reference House, Kanngiesser, Barrett, Broesch, Cebioglu, Crittenden and Silk2020a, Reference House, Kanngiesser, Barrett, Yilmaz, Smith, Sebastian-Enesco and Silkb).

In sum, several cognitive abilities are implicated in even basic reputational models of cooperation – including the ability to mind-read and attribute intentions and motives to others, to be sensitive to the local norms in order to judge whether behaviours are praiseworthy or contemptible, and to infer how others might perceive us based on our actions and their own perceptions of our hidden intentions. These skills do not all emerge at a single set point in development, but are instead honed over childhood and into adolescence, are responsive to local cultural norms, and are generally highly contingent and situational.

Larger-scale consequences of perverse reputational incentives

Considering the social costs incurred by prosocial individuals and organisations has broader implications for the emergence and persistence of cooperation within groups, as well as for the effectiveness of these altruistic efforts. Many of the social interactions we face can be broadly described as social dilemmas: mutual cooperation yields the best collective payoffs, but individuals can derive a larger short-term benefit from acting selfishly. Finding ways to resolve such social dilemmas – and to encourage mutual cooperation – is both theoretically and practically important (e.g. Raihani & Aitken, Reference Raihani and Aitken2011; Ostrom et al., Reference Ostrom, Burger, Field, Norgaard and Policansky1999; Van Vugt, Reference Van Vugt2009; Penn, Reference Penn2003). A key insight from experimental economics and social psychology is that many people are conditional cooperators (Fischbacher et al., Reference Fischbacher, Gachter and Fehr2001). Under this view, good deeds can spark further cooperation (Gino et al., Reference Gino, Gu and Zhong2009; Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Rand, Arbesman, Fowler and Christakis2013; McAuliffe et al., Reference McAuliffe, Wrangham, Glowacki and Russell2015; Pfeiffer et al., Reference Pfeiffer, Rutte, Killingback, Taborsky and Bonhoeffer2005; Raihani & Hart, Reference Raihani and Hart2010) and uncooperative acts prompt reciprocal defection, potentially leading to the unravelling of cooperation (e.g. Raihani & Hart, Reference Raihani and Hart2010). Although we do not want to challenge the notion of conditional cooperation, we also want to emphasise that cooperative individuals can also sometimes undermine the emergence of large-scale cooperation. For example, people often hold negative stereotypes of socially conscious individuals – and these perceptions can dissuade others from cooperating in these domains (Bashir et al., Reference Bashir, Lockwood, Chasteen, Nadolny and Noyes2013; Fruhen & Flin, Reference Fruhen and Flin2015). The investments of prosocial individuals can also deter individuals who might otherwise cooperate, either because they do not feel the same degree of commitment to the cause as these moral minorities (Kurz et al., Reference Kurz, Prosser, Rabinovich and O'Neill2020) or because they fear moral reproach if their own efforts fall short by comparison (Howe & Monin, Reference Howe and Monin2017; Sparkman & Attari, Reference Sparkman and Attari2020b; Stouten et al., Reference Stouten, van Dijke, Mayer, De Cremer and Euwema2013).

Moreover, negatively evaluating prosocial individuals and companies can act as a further obstacle to achieving large-scale cooperation because these social costs act as an additional disincentive to pay the costs associated with cooperative behaviour. For example, people and companies whose actions yield personal and social benefits are typically evaluated more negatively than counterparts whose actions yield no social benefits at all (Makov & Newman, Reference Makov and Newman2016; Newman & Cain, Reference Newman and Cain2014). By disparaging people who personally benefit from performing good deeds – or by enacting preferences against for-profit, for-good companies – people might disincentivise those who might otherwise invest in socially beneficial activities.

The special credit we afford to deeds seemingly motivated by ‘pure’ emotions compared with those that stem from ‘deliberative’ calculation can also have unwanted large-scale consequences. For instance, despite the large sums that are collectively donated to charity (around $400 billion per year in the US), many people do not prioritise ‘effectiveness’ in the decision of which charity to support (Caviola et al., Reference Caviola, Schubert and Nemirow2020). Instead, people support charities based on their subjective preferences (Berman et al., Reference Berman, Barasch, Levine and Small2018), being more likely to donate to disaster-relief campaigns than to charities that aim to resolve recurring problems (Caviola et al., Reference Caviola, Schubert and Nemirow2020) or donating to causes that are associated with single identifiable victims rather than those which will help larger numbers (Jenni & Loewenstein, Reference Jenni and Loewenstein1997). Ineffective altruism can nevertheless make sense in light of its reputation consequences. For example, those who deliberate over which charity to support are evaluated less positively than those who use empathy as their moral compass (Montealegre et al., Reference Montealegre, Bush, Moss, Pizarro and Jimenez-Leal2020) and, despite the fact that donating to charities in lower-income countries is more effective than donating to more local causes (Caviola et al., Reference Caviola, Schubert and Nemirow2020), people often disapprove of those who forego opportunities to help at a local scale in favour of helping more distant others (Law et al., Reference Law, Campbell and Gaesser2019; McManus et al., Reference McManus, Kleiman-Weiner and Young2020). More generally, the social rewards that flow from prosocial acts tend to be sensitive to the costs and observability of the good deeds and typically do not scale with the efficacy of the helpful act, which can explain why people engage in ineffective helping (Burum et al., Reference Burum, Nowak and Hoffman2020).

The tendency to seek ulterior motives behind ostensibly prosocial actions can also have effects on self-assessments of our own helpful deeds, which subsequently affect the tendency that we will repeat them. Specifically, it appears to be easier to maintain a positive self-image and to derive a warm glow from giving when we can be assured that our own motives were not corrupted by extrinsic incentives (Gneezy et al., Reference Gneezy, Gneezy, Riener and Nelson2012; Savary et al., Reference Savary, Li and Newman2020; Savary & Goldsmith, Reference Savary and Goldsmith2020; Small & Cryder, Reference Small and Cryder2016). It is now well established that external incentives for prosocial behaviour – such as thank you gifts or financial incentives, like tax breaks – frequently reduce people's willingness to perform these prosocial acts (Chao, Reference Chao2017; Gneezy & Rustichini, Reference Gneezy and Rustichini2000; Mellström & Johannesson, Reference Mellström and Johannesson2008; Moussaoui et al., Reference Moussaoui, Herren-Vichet, Baldelli, Waldvogel and Desrichard2019; Newman & Jeremy Shen, Reference Newman and Jeremy Shen2012; Savary et al., Reference Savary, Li and Newman2020; Shi, Reference Shi2011). These negative effects can be ameliorated by giving people the opportunity to forego the external incentive (Mellström & Johannesson, Reference Mellström and Johannesson2008) – and other work indicates that people often engage in such ‘motivational laundering’: foregoing an external incentive to remove ambiguity about the motives underlying their charitable actions (Kirgios et al., Reference Kirgios, Chang, Levine, Milkman and Kessler2020).

Conclusions and future directions

In this final section, we outline some specific directions for future research, focusing on research gaps and open questions. One key hypothesis stemming from this review is that people might often seek to avoid social costs rather than to garner social benefits – particularly when it comes to reputation management in daily life. Predictions stemming from this hypothesis cannot easily be tested with experimental paradigms that constrain the ways in which individuals are allowed to manage their reputation (for example, using experiments that only allow individuals to signal prosociality to attract interaction partners). Instead, we believe that a more naturalistic, observational approach as used in studies of animal behaviour would be helpful to address this hypothesis. The rich insights from ethnography and participant observation will surely have a role here. Alongside this, new technologies allow for experience sampling methods and have been used to great effect to address other aspects of social behaviour in daily life (e.g. Molho et al., Reference Molho, Tybur, Van Lange and Balliet2020; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Michels, Burgmer, Mussweiler, Ockenfels and Hofmann2020). Vignette studies, wherein individuals are presented with a situation and give their reaction or outline potential courses of action may also provide a promising avenue (cf. Mathew & Boyd, Reference Mathew and Boyd2014). Such methods could be used to measure the various ways that people manage their reputations and form and update impressions of others and to test the idea that avoiding social costs takes precedence over seeking reputation benefits.

Another key theme coming out of the existing literature on reputation-based cooperation is that both reputation management and impression formation rely on a suite of cognitive abilities, which seem to emerge during middle childhood. This is now a burgeoning area of research and great strides are being made in identifying the cognitive abilities that scaffold reputational strategies. However, it is still unclear which cognitive mechanisms are key to these abilities and how exactly these abilities are attained. Specifically, it is not clear whether the cognition supporting sophisticated forms of reputation-based cooperation emerges at relatively fixed points in development or whether the abilities have to be culturally learned and practised during childhood (Heyes, Reference Heyes2019; Luhrmann, Reference Luhrmann2011). We also know rather little about how individual variation in socio-cognitive abilities affects reputational strategies. For example, do individuals vary in their ability to both manage their own reputation and to form accurate impressions of others; and, if so, where does this variation come from? It may also be fruitful to explore metacognitive accuracy – specifically whether individuals are able to judge how signalling their prosocial deeds might be interpreted by others, as well as the factors that predict performance in this domain. We also note that previous studies have documented societal variation in the tendency to account for mental states when judging harmful outcomes (Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Bolyanatz, Crittenden, Fessler, Fitzpatrick, Gurven and Laurence2016; Curtin et al., Reference Curtin, Barrett, Bolyanatz, Crittenden, Fessler, Fitzpatrick and Henrich2020): in countries that have stronger kin-based institutions, people are less likely to take an actor's mental state into consideration when forming moral judgements. As we have argued, intention attribution is also pivotal in determining how people response to actions with beneficial outcomes but much of this evidence is derived from Western societies. A clear research priority is therefore to determine whether consideration of mental states when judging beneficial acts varies in a similar manner around the world.

It is also apparent that the content of the norms we use to guide our behaviour and form impressions of others varies hugely around the world. There is a clear need to understand how cognition and culture exert their independent and interacting effects on reputational strategies in humans; and this field would benefit from theoretically informed work addressing how and why such variation arises. For example, although it seems apparent from the studies above that modesty norms are more strongly endorsed in East Asian (compared with Western) societies, it is not especially clear why this is the case: the observation that modesty is more highly prized in some societies as compared with others falls short as an explanation as it simply restates the puzzle.

Crucial to any explanation of cross-cultural variation in the importance of reputation and social costs/benefits will be an understanding of the nature and structure of social relations: for example, the extent of people's interdependence (Aktipis et al., Reference Aktipis, Cronk, Alcock, Ayers, Baciu, Balliet and Winfrey2018; Roberts, Reference Roberts2005), their embeddedness in dense kin networks (Enke, Reference Enke2019; Henrich, Reference Henrich2020; Schulz et al., Reference Schulz, Bahrami-Rad, Beauchamp and Henrich2019), their ability to extricate themselves from existing relationships (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Rodriguez-Barraquer and Tan2012) and the importance of social relations to future generations (Bowles et al., Reference Bowles, Smith and Borgerhoff Mulder2010; Borgerhoff Mulder et al., Reference Borgerhoff Mulder, Bowles, Hertz, Bell, Beise and Wiessner2009). Such variation in the form and salience of social relations, which can stem from differences in ecology and mode of production (Bowles et al., Reference Bowles, Smith and Borgerhoff Mulder2010; Lamba & Mace, Reference Lamba and Mace2011), as well as historical contingency and cultural norms, should help account for variations in social norms and institutions around the world. Comparative, cross-cultural work that captures these aspects of social structure (whether through social network collection or field experiments that retain elements of people's social network position and connections) is needed to help us understand these points.

Finally, just as there is a need to explain variation across societies, so too is there a need to explain variation in the strategies of reputation management and impression formation used within societies. If the reaction to any prosocial act is contingent and context-specific, then we can expect that people's exposure to and concern for social costs will vary. An individual's social standing may have particular bearing on how prosocial acts are interpreted (and so, on the individual's propensity to carry them out). However, it is not immediately clear how this might feed in. For example, we may expect people of lower social standing to be viewed in a positive light because of the greater relative cost they incur for any helpful act (e.g. Yuan et al., Reference Yuan, Wu and Kou2018). Or, we could expect the opposite, with people of lower social standing being viewed with more scepticism: if they are more in need of the potential social benefits that stem from helpful acts, then they may more readily be assumed to have strategic aims, as compared with better off individuals who could be doing things not out of need, but out of ‘the goodness of their heart’. Vignette studies could extend earlier work (Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Bolyanatz, Crittenden, Fessler, Fitzpatrick, Gurven and Laurence2016) to determine whether and, if so, how judgments are dependent on the identity and status(es) of the actor involved. If there are indeed systematic biases in who faces derision for their prosocial acts and who garners praise, then it is also worth investigating the aggregate consequences of these inequalities. Formal and agent-based models may be particularly useful in studying the structural consequences of particular patterns of biases in interpretation.

To sum up, prosocial acts commonly yield reputation benefits, which can be advantageous in the context of partner choice. Nevertheless, there is also the potential for these investments to result in social costs – and this is most likely when observers can infer self-interested intentions or when observers believe that helpful actions have the potential to harm them. We have emphasised the contingent and context-specific nature of such evaluations. There is a clear need for more cross-cultural work in this field so that we address the extent to which some of the most frequently observed patterns are culturally specific or are more likely to be human universals.

Author contributions

Both authors contributed equally to this paper.

Financial support

NR is supported by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship and by the Leverhulme Trust.

Conflict of interests

NR is the author of the forthcoming trade book, The Social Instinct: How Cooperation Shaped the World.

Data availability

There are no data to report.

References

Ackerman, J. M., & Kenrick, D. T. (2008). The costs of benefits: Help-refusals highlight key trade-offs of social life. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(2), 118140. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308315700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aktipis, A., Cronk, L., Alcock, J., Ayers, J. D., Baciu, C., Balliet, D., … Winfrey, P. (2018). Understanding cooperation through fitness interdependence. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(7), 429431. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0378-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amemiya, J., Liu, Z., Compton, B. J., & Heyman, G. D. (2020). Children's judgements of positive claims people make about themselves. Infant and Child Development, e2212. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2212Google Scholar
Ames, D. R., Flynn, F. J., & Weber, E. U. (2004). It's the thought that counts: On perceiving how helpers decide to lend a hand. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(4), 461474. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203261890CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, D. R., & Johar, G. V. (2009). I'll know what you're like when i see how you feel: How and when affective displays influence behavior-based impressions. Psychological Science, 20(5), 586593. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02330.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amir, D., & McAuliffe, K. (2020). Cross-cultural, developmental psychology: Integrating approaches and key insights. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 430444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. A., Crockett, M. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2020). A theory of moral praise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 694703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreoni, J. (1990). Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm-glow giving. The Economic Journal, 100(401), 464. https://doi.org/10.2307/2234133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Petrie, R. (2004). Public goods experiments without confidentiality: A glimpse into fund-raising. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7), 16051623. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2727(03)00040-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahry, D. L., & Wilson, R. K. (2006). Confusion or fairness in the field? Rejections in the ultimatum game under the strategy method. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 60(1), 3754. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2004.07.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bai, F., Wu, W., & Bao, S. (2019). Moral but dominant: When do-gooders get derogated. Academy of Management Proceedings, 2019(1), 14563. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMBPP.2019.14563abstractCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banerjee, R. (2000). The development of an understanding of modesty. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 499517. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151000165823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banerjee, R., Heyman, G. D., & Lee, K. (2020). The development of moral self-presentation. In Jensen, L. A. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of moral development (pp. 91109). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190676049.013.6Google Scholar
Barasch, A., Berman, J. Z., & Small, D. A. (2016). When payment undermines the pitch: On the persuasiveness of pure motives in fund-raising. Psychological Science, 27(10), 13881397. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616638841CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barasch, A., Levine, E. E., Berman, J. Z., & Small, D. A. (2014). Selfish or selfless? On the signal value of emotion in altruistic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(3), 393413. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barclay, P. (2011). Competitive helping increases with the size of biological markets and invades defection. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 281(1), 4755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.04.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 164175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P. (2016). Biological markets and the effects of partner choice on cooperation and friendship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 3338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P., & Barker, J. L. (2020). Greener than thou: People who protect the environment are more cooperative, compete to be environmental, and benefit from reputation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 72, 101441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P., & Raihani, N. J. (2016). Partner choice versus punishment in human Prisoner's Dilemmas. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(4), 263271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.12.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P., & Willer, R. (2007). Partner choice creates competitive altruism in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1610), 749753. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.0209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, J. L., Power, E. A., Heap, S., Puurtinen, M., & Sosis, R. (2019). Content, cost, and context: A framework for understanding human signaling systems. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 28(2), 8699. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21768CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, H. C., Bolyanatz, A., Crittenden, A. N., Fessler, D. M. T., Fitzpatrick, S., Gurven, M.,… Laurence, S. (2016). Small-scale societies exhibit fundamental variation in the role of intentions in moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(17), 46884693. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1522070113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bashir, N. Y., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, A. L., Nadolny, D., & Noyes, I. (2013). The ironic impact of activists: Negative stereotypes reduce social change influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7), 614626. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1983CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T., & Birch, K. (1981). Is empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 290302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N., André, J.-B., & Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 5978. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11002202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bekkers, R., & Wiepking, P. (2011). A literature review of empirical studies of philanthropy: Eight mechanisms that drive charitable giving. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 40(5), 924973. https://doi.org/10.1177/0899764010380927CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bénabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2006). Incentives and prosocial behavior. American Economic Review, 96, 16521678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bereczkei, T., Birkas, B., & Kerekes, Z. (2007). Public charity offer as a proximate factor of evolved reputation-building strategy: An experimental analysis of a real-life situation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(4), 277284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.04.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bereczkei, T., Szabo, Z. P., & Czibor, A. (2015). Abusing good intentions: Machiavellians strive for exploiting cooperators. SAGE Open, 5(2), 2158244015593119. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244015593119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berge, J. W., & Arshad, F. M. (2020). Doing well while doing good: Do firms’ profit motives for doing good matter to employees? http://scholarspace.manoa.hawaii.edu/handle/10125/70512Google Scholar
Berman, J. Z., Barasch, A., Levine, E. E., & Small, D. A. (2018). Impediments to effective altruism: The role of subjective preferences in charitable giving. Psychological Science, 29(5), 834844. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617747648CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, J. Z., Levine, E. E., Barasch, A., & Small, D. A. (2015). The braggart's dilemma: On the social rewards and penalties of advertising prosocial behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 52(1), 90104. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhogal, M. S., Farrelly, D., Galbraith, N., Manktelow, K., & Bradley, H. (2020). The role of altruistic costs in human mate choice. Personality and Individual Differences, 160, 109939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.109939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigman, Y. E., & Tamir, M. (2016). The road to heaven is paved with effort: Perceived effort amplifies moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 16541669. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjørkelo, B., Einarsen, S., Nielsen, M. B., & Matthiesen, S. B. (2011). Silence is golden? Characteristics and experiences of self-reported whistleblowers. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 20(2), 206238. https://doi.org/10.1080/13594320903338884CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, P. R., McAuliffe, K., Corbit, J., Callaghan, T. C., Barry, O., Bowie, A., … Warneken, F. (2015). The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature, 528(7581), 258261. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliege Bird, R., & Power, E. A. (2015). Prosocial signaling and cooperation among Martu hunters. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(5), 389397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliege Bird, R., Ready, E., & Power, E. A. (2018). The social significance of subtle signals. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(2), 16. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0298-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliege Bird, R., Scelza, B., Bird, D. W., & Smith, E. A. (2012). The hierarchy of virtue: Mutualism, altruism and signaling in Martu women's cooperative hunting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(1), 6478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnet, I., & Frey, B. S. (1999). Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games: Comment. American Economic Review, 89(1), 335339. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.89.1.335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolderdijk, J. W., Brouwer, C., & Cornelissen, G. (2018). When do morally motivated innovators elicit inspiration instead of irritation? Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02362Google ScholarPubMed
Bone, J., S., A. S., & Raihani, N. J. (2014). Defectors, not norm violators, are punished by third-parties. Biology Letters, 10(7), 20140388. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boone, J. L. (1998). The evolution of magnanimity. Human Nature, 9(1), 21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Bowles, S., Hertz, T., Bell, A., Beise, J., … Wiessner, P. (2009). Intergenerational wealth transmission and the dynamics of inequality in small-scale societies. Science, 326(5953), 682688. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1178336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boster, F. J., Rodriguez, J. I., Cruz, M. G., & Marshall, L. (1995). The relative effectiveness of a direct request message and a pregiving message on friends and strangers. Communication Research, 22(4), 475484. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365095022004005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1990). The logic of practice (Nice, R., Trans.). Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Bowles, S., Smith, E. A., & Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2010). The emergence and persistence of inequality in premodern societies: Introduction to the special section. Current Anthropology, 51(1), 717. https://doi.org/10.1086/649206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R. (2017). A different kind of animal: How culture transformed our species. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, A., Lawrence, C., & Ferguson, E. (2018). Does observability affect prosociality? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1875), 20180116. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bshary, R., & Grutter, A. S. (2006). Image scoring and cooperation in a cleaner fish mutualism. Nature, 441, 975978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bshary, R., & Raihani, N. J. (2017). Helping in humans and other animals: A fruitful interdisciplinary dialogue. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1863), 20170929. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0929Google ScholarPubMed
Burtch, G., Ghose, A., & Wattal, S. (2016). Secret admirers: An empirical examination of information hiding and contribution dynamics in online crowdfunding. Information Systems Research, 27(3), 478496. https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2016.0642CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burum, B., Nowak, M. A., & Hoffman, M. (2020). An evolutionary explanation for ineffective altruism. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(12), 12451257. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00950-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. F. (2011). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Carlson, R. W., & Zaki, J. (2018). Good deeds gone bad: Lay theories of altruism and selfishness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75, 3640. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.11.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caviola, L., Schubert, S., & Nemirow, J. (2020). The many obstacles to effective giving. Judgment and Decision Making, 15(2), 159172.Google Scholar
Chao, M. (2017). Demotivating incentives and motivation crowding out in charitable giving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(28), 73017306. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1616921114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chernev, A., & Blair, S. (2015). Doing well by doing good: The benevolent halo of corporate social responsibility. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(6), 14121425. https://doi.org/10.1086/680089CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowell, J. M., Lee, K., Malcolm-Smith, S., Selcuk, B., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2017). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, 20(4), e12403. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowgill, C. M. (2017). Good, but not a goody two-shoes: Self-presentation concerns of appearing overly moral. Ohio University. https://etd.ohiolink.edu/apexprod/rws_olink/r/1501/10?clear=10&p10_accession_num=ohiou1492991431186931Google Scholar
Critcher, C. R., Inbar, Y., & Pizarro, D. A. (2013). How quick decisions illuminate moral character. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(3), 308315. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612457688CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtin, C. M., Barrett, H. C., Bolyanatz, A., Crittenden, A. N., Fessler, D. M. T., Fitzpatrick, S., … Henrich, J. (2020). Kinship intensity and the use of mental states in moral judgment across societies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 415429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Freitas, J., DeScioli, P., Thomas, K. A., & Pinker, S. (2019). Maimonides’ ladder: States of mutual knowledge and the perception of charitability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(1), 158173. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2016). How the mind makes welfare tradeoffs: Evolution, computation, and emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 1216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denis, E., Pecheux, C., & Warlop, L. (2020). When public recognition inhibits prosocial behavior: The case of charitable giving. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 0899764020911203. https://doi.org/10.1177/0899764020911203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Effron, D. A., O'Connor, K., Leroy, H., & Lucas, B. J. (2018). From inconsistency to hypocrisy: When does ‘saying one thing but doing another’ invite condemnation? Research in Organizational Behavior, 38, 6175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.riob.2018.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations. PLOS ONE, 7(10), e48433. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Preschoolers affect others’ reputations through prosocial gossip. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 447460. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12143CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelmann, J. M., & Rapp, D. J. (2018). The influence of reputational concerns on children's prosociality. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 9295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enke, B. (2019). Kinship, cooperation, and the evolution of moral systems. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 134(2), 9531019. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjz001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erlandsson, A., Wingren, M., & Andersson, P. A. (2020). Type and amount of help as predictors for impression of helpers. PLOS ONE, 15(12), e0243808. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243808CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, A. M., & van de Calseyde, P. P. F. M. (2017). The effects of observed decision time on expectations of extremity and cooperation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 5059. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.05.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, J. A. C., Faber, N. S., Savulescu, J., & Crockett, M. J. (2018). The costs of being consequentialist: Social inference from instrumental harm and impartial beneficence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 200216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everett, J. A. C., Pizarro, D. A., & Crockett, M. J. (2016). Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 772787. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, A., Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2005). Driving forces behind informal sanctions. Econometrica, 73, 20172030. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0262.2005.00644.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980994. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.90.4.980CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137140. https://doi.org/10.1038/415137aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehrler, S., & Przepiorka, W. (2016). Choosing a partner for social exchange: Charitable giving as a signal of trustworthiness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 129, 157171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T., Barrett, H. C., Kanovsky, M., Stich, S., Holbrook, C., Henrich, J., … Laurence, S. (2015). Moral parochialism and contextual contingency across seven societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1813), 20150907. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0907CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischbacher, U., Gachter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71(3), 397404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, J. D., Nadler, A., & Whitcher-Alagna, S. (1982). Recipient reactions to aid. Psychological Bulletin, 91(1), 2754. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1991). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions (new ed.). W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Fruhen, L. S., & Flin, R. (2015). Car driver attitudes, perceptions of social norms and aggressive driving behaviour towards cyclists. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 83, 162170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2015.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, F., Hauert, C., Nowak, M. A., & Wang, L. (2008). Reputation-based partner choice promotes cooperation in social networks. Physical Review E, 78(2), 026117. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevE.78.026117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, G., Heyman, G. D., Cameron, C. A., & Lee, K. (2016). Learning to be unsung heroes: Development of reputation management in two cultures. Child Development, 87(3), 689699. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gambetta, D., & Przepiorka, W. (2014). Natural and strategic generosity as signals of trustworthiness. PLOS ONE, 9(5), e97533. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genyue, F., Heyman, G. D., & Lee, K. (2011). Reasoning about modesty among adolescents and adults in China and the U.S. Journal of Adolescence, 34(4), 599608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gino, F., Gu, J., & Zhong, C.-B. (2009). Contagion or restitution? When bad apples can motivate ethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(6), 12991302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.07.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gintis, H., SMITH, E. A., & Bowles, S. (2001). Costly signaling and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 213(1), 103119. https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2001.2406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gneezy, A., & Epley, N. (2014). Worth keeping but not exceeding: Asymmetric consequences of breaking versus exceeding promises. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(7), 796804. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614533134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gneezy, A., Gneezy, U., Riener, G., & Nelson, L. D. (2012). Pay-what-you-want, identity, and self-signaling in markets. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(19), 72367240. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1120893109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). A fine is a price. The Journal of Legal Studies, 29(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1086/468061CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guglielmo, S., & Malle, B. F. (2019). Asymmetric morality: Blame is more differentiated and more extreme than praise. PLOS ONE, 14(3), e0213544. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, M. (2000). ‘It's a wonderful life’: Signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(4), 263282. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00032-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hafenbrädl, S., & Waeger, D. (2019). The business case for CSR: A trump card against hypocrisy? Journal of Business Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.08.043Google Scholar
Halabi, S., Dovidio, J. F., & Nadler, A. (2016). Help that hurts? Perceptions of intergroup assistance. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 53, 6571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2016.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers. Cdp.Sagepub.Com. http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/22/3/186.full.pdf+htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. http://www.pnas.org/content/108/50/19931.shortCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harbaugh, W. T. (1998). What do donations buy?: A model of philanthropy based on prestige and warm glow. Journal of Public Economics, 67(2), 269284. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2727(97)00062-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science, 316(5831), 16221625. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1140738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, R. A., & Norgaard, J. R. (2016). Reputation in the Internet black market: An empirical and theoretical analysis of the Deep Web. Journal of Institutional Economics, 12(3), 515539. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1744137415000454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2017). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2020). The weirdest people in the world: How the West became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous (1st ed.). Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., … Tracer, D. (2005). ‘Economic man’ in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 795815. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466, 29. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029aCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, E., Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children engage in competitive altruism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 176189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Keupp, S., Hare, B., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(1), 6375. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, B., Thoni, C., & Gachter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 13621367. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1153808CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2019). Précis of Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, e169. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X18002145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyman, G., Barner, D., Heumann, J., & Schenck, L. (2014). Children's sensitivity to ulterior motives when evaluating prosocial behavior. Cognitive Science, 38(4), 683700. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyman, G. D., Fu, G., Barner, D., Zhishan, H., Zhou, L., & Lee, K. (2016). Children's evaluation of public and private generosity and its relation to behavior: Evidence from China. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 1630. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyman, G. D., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2007). Evaluating claims people make about themselves: The development of skepticism. Child Development, 78(2), 367375. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01003.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyman, G. D., Itakura, S., & Lee, K. (2011). Japanese and American children's reasoning about accepting credit for prosocial behavior. Social Development, 20(1), 171184. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00578.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hicks, C. M., Liu, D., & Heyman, G. D. (2015). Young children's beliefs about self-disclosure of performance failure and success. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 123135. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirshleifer, J. (1987). On the emotions as guarantors of threats and promises. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M., Hilbe, C., & Nowak, M. A. (2018). The signal-burying game can explain why we obscure positive traits and good deeds. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(6), 397404. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0354-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights, and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7, 346380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M., Yoeli, E., & Nowak, M. A. (2015). Cooperate without looking: Why we care what people think and not just what they do. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(6), 17271732. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1417904112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
House, B. R., Kanngiesser, P., Barrett, H. C., Broesch, T., Cebioglu, S., Crittenden, A. N., … Silk, J. B. (2020a). Universal norm psychology leads to societal diversity in prosocial behaviour and development. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(1), 3644. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0734-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
House, B. R., Kanngiesser, P., Barrett, H. C., Yilmaz, S., Smith, A. M., Sebastian-Enesco, C., … Silk, J. B. (2020b). Social norms and cultural diversity in the development of third-party punishment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1925), 20192794. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2794CrossRefGoogle Scholar
House, B. R., Silk, J. B., Henrich, J., Barrett, H. C., Scelza, B. A., Boyette, A. H., … Laurence, S. (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(36), 1458614591. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1221217110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howe, L. C., & Monin, B. (2017). Healthier than thou? ‘Practicing what you preach’ backfires by increasing anticipated devaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(5), 718735. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imada, H. (2020). Preference for anonymous giving. Letters on Evolutionary Behavioral Science, 11(1), 2226. https://doi.org/10.5178/lebs.2020.76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, K., & Horne, C. (2013). A normative explanation of antisocial punishment. Social Science Research, 42(2), 562570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2012.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, M. O., Rodriguez-Barraquer, T., & Tan, X. (2012). Social capital and social quilts: Network patterns of favor exchange. American Economic Review, 102(5), 18571897. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.102.5.1857CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenni, K., & Loewenstein, G. (1997). Explaining the identifiable victim effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14(3), 235257. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007740225484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnsen, Å. A., & Kvaløy, O. (2016). Does strategic kindness crowd out prosocial behavior? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 132, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.09.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. (2018). Dimensions of altruism: Do evaluations of prosocial behavior track social good or personal sacrifice? (SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 3277444). Social Science Research Network. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3277444Google Scholar
Johnson, T., Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., McElreath, R., & Smirnov, O. (2009). The role of egalitarian motives in altruistic punishment. Economics Letters, 102(3), 192194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2009.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. G. B., & Park, S. Y. (2019). Moral signaling through donations of money and time. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tg9xsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, D., & Linardi, S. (2014). Wallflowers: Experimental evidence of an aversion to standing out. Management Science, 60(7), 17571771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Nowak, M. A., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Uncalculating cooperation is used to signal trustworthiness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(31), 86588663. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601280113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, J. J., Rand, D. G., Arbesman, S., Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2013). Contagion of cooperation in static and fluid social networks. PLOS ONE, 8(6), e66199. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, J. J., Sommers, R., Bloom, P., & Rand, D. G. (2017). Why do we hate hypocrites? Evidence for a theory of false signaling. Psychological Science, 28(3), 356368. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616685771CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanagawa, C., Cross, S. E., & Markus, H. R. (2001). ‘Who am I?’ The cultural psychology of the conceptual self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(1), 90103. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201271008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandori, M. (1992). Social norms and community enforcement. The Review of Economic Studies, 59(1), 6380. https://doi.org/10.2307/2297925?ref=search-gateway:f3a8c4b0a46e82c3f16920b6ecbabe03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawamura, Y., & Kusumi, T. (2020). Altruism does not always lead to a good reputation: A normative explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, 104021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawamura, Y., Ohtsubo, Y., & Kusumi, T. (2020). Effects of cost and benefit of prosocial behavior on reputation. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1948550620929163. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620929163Google Scholar
Kim, T., Zhang, T., & Norton, M. I. (2019). Pettiness in social exchange. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(2), 361373. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000463CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirgios, E. L., Chang, E. H., Levine, E. E., Milkman, K. L., & Kessler, J. B. (2020). Forgoing earned incentives to signal pure motives. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2000065117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2014). The topography of generosity: Asymmetric evaluations of prosocial actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(6), 23662379. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, N., Grossmann, I., Uskul, A. K., Kraus, A. A., & Epley, N. (2015). It pays to be nice, but not really nice: Asymmetric reputations from prosociality across 7 countries. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(4), 10.Google Scholar
Krems, J. A., Williams, K. E., Aktipis, A., & Kenrick, D. T. (2021). Friendship jealousy: One tool for maintaining friendships in the face of third-party threats?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4), 977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krull, D. S., Seger, C. R., & Silvera, D. H. (2008). Smile when you say that: Effects of willingness on dispositional inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 735742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurz, T., Prosser, A. M. B., Rabinovich, A., & O'Neill, S. (2020). Could vegans and lycra cyclists be bad for the planet? Theorizing the role of moralized minority practice identities in processes of societal-level change. Journal of Social Issues, 76(1), 86100. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutlaca, M., Becker, J., & Radke, H. (2020). A hero for the outgroup, a black sheep for the ingroup: Societal perceptions of those who confront discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 88, 103832. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamba, S., & Mace, R. (2011). Demography and ecology drive variation in cooperation across human populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(35), 1442614430. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105186108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, K. F., Campbell, D., & Gaesser, B. (2019). Biased benevolence: The morality of effective altruism [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qzx67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1969). Eating Christmas in the Kalahari. 4.Google Scholar
Leimgruber, K. L., Shaw, A., Santos, L. R., & Olson, K. R. (2012). Young children are more generous when others are aware of their actions. PLOS ONE, 7(10), e48292. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048292CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, E. E., Barasch, A., Rand, D., Berman, J. Z., & Small, D. A. (2018). Signaling emotion and reason in cooperation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(5), 702719. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, N. (2020). Virtue signalling is virtuous. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02653-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y. (2014). The evolution of reputation-based partner-switching behaviors with a cost. Scientific Reports, 4(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep05957Google ScholarPubMed
Lin-Healy, F., & Small, D. A. (2012). Cheapened altruism: Discounting personally affected prosocial actors. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117(2), 269274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.11.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luhrmann, T. M. (2011). Toward an anthropological theory of mind. Suomen Antropologi: Journal of the Finnish Anthropological Society, 36(4), 569.Google Scholar
Lyle, H. F., & Smith, E. A. (2014). The reputational and social network benefits of prosociality in an Andean community. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(13), 48204825. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1318372111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macfarlan, S. J., & Lyle, H. F. (2015). Multiple reputation domains and cooperative behaviour in two Latin American communities. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1683), 20150009. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macfarlan, S. J., Quinlan, R., & Remiker, M. (2013). Cooperative behaviour and prosocial reputation dynamics in a Dominican village. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1761), 20130557. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.0557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macfarlan, S. J., Remiker, M., & Quinlan, R. (2012). Competitive altruism explains labor exchange variation in a Dominican community. Current Anthropology, 53(1), 118124. https://doi.org/10.1086/663700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makov, T., & Newman, G. E. (2016). Economic gains stimulate negative evaluations of corporate sustainability initiatives. Nature Climate Change, 6(9), 844846. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate3033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathew, S., & Boyd, R. (2014). The cost of cowardice: Punitive sentiments towards free riders in Turkana raids. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(1), 5864. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAndrew, F. T., & Perilloux, C. (2012). Is self-sacrificial competitive altruism primarily a male activity? Evolutionary Psychology, 10(1), 147470491201000100. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAuliffe, K., Wrangham, R., Glowacki, L., & Russell, A. F. (2015). When cooperation begets cooperation: The role of key individuals in galvanizing support. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1683), 20150012. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McManus, R. M., Kleiman-Weiner, M., & Young, L. (2020). What we owe to family: The impact of special obligations on moral judgment. Psychological Science, 31(3), 227242. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619900321CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNamara, J. M., & Barta, Z. (2020). Behavioural flexibility and reputation formation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1939), 20201758. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1758CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNamara, J. M., Barta, Z., Fromhage, L., & Houston, A. I. (2008). The coevolution of choosiness and cooperation. Nature, 451(7175), 189192. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melis, A. P., & Semmann, D. (2010). How is human cooperation different? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 26632674. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellström, C., & Johannesson, M. (2008). Crowding out in blood donation: Was Titmuss right? Journal of the European Economic Association, 6(4), 845863. https://doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2008.6.4.845CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melnikoff, D. E., & Bailey, A. H. (2018). Preferences for moral vs. Immoral traits in others are conditional. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(4), E592E600. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1714945115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milinski, M., Semmann, D., & Krambeck, H. (2002a). Donors to charity gain in both indirect reciprocity and political reputation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 269(1494), 881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milinski, M., Semmann, D., & Krambeck, H. (2002b). Reputation helps solve the ‘tragedy of the commons’. Nature, 415(6870), 424426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2005). The development of cynicism. Psychological Science, 16(5), 385390. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01545.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minson, J. A., & Monin, B. (2012). Do-gooder derogation: Disparaging morally motivated minorities to defuse anticipated reproach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(2), 200207. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611415695CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mokos, J., & Scheuring, I. (2019). Altruism, costly signaling, and withholding information in a sport charity campaign. Evolution, Mind and Behaviour, 17(1), 1018. https://doi.org/10.1556/2050.2019.00007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Van Lange, P. A. M., & Balliet, D. (2020). Direct and indirect punishment of norm violations in daily life. Nature Communications, 11(1), 3432. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17286-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monin, B. (2007). Holier than me? Threatening Social Comparison in the Moral Domain. Revue internationale de psychologie sociale, Tome 20(1), 5368.Google Scholar
Monin, B., Sawyer, P. J., & Marquez, M. J. (2008). The rejection of moral rebels: Resenting those who do the right thing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 7693. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.76CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montealegre, A., Bush, L., Moss, D., Pizarro, D., & Jimenez-Leal, W. (2020). Does maximizing good make people look bad? [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2zbaxCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moussaoui, L. S., Herren-Vichet, J., Baldelli, S., Waldvogel, S., & Desrichard, O. (2019). Examining if sending text messages to thank blood donors for saving lives is a ‘great idea’? Evidence from a randomized field experiment. Journal of Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing, 31(5), 486506. https://doi.org/10.1080/10495142.2018.1526753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadler, A., & Halabi, S. (2006). Intergroup helping as status relations: Effects of status stability, identification, and type of help on receptivity to high-status group's help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 97110. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelissen, R. M. A. (2008). The price you pay: Cost-dependent reputation effects of altruistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 242248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, G. E., & Cain, D. M. (2014). Tainted altruism. Psychological Science, 25, 648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, G. E., & Jeremy Shen, Y. (2012). The counterintuitive effects of thank-you gifts on charitable giving. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(5), 973983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2012.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nikiforakis, N., & Engelmann, D. (2011). Altruistic punishment and the threat of feuds. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 78(3), 319332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.01.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohtsuki, H., & Iwasa, Y. (2006). The leading eight: Social norms that can maintain cooperation by indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239(4), 435444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olivola, C. Y., & Shafir, E. (2013). The martyrdom effect: When pain and effort increase prosocial contributions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(1), 91105. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.767CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostrom, E., Burger, J., Field, C., Norgaard, R., & Policansky, D. (1999). Revisiting the commons: Local lessons, global challenges. Science (New York), 284(5412), 278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parks, C. D., & Stone, A. B. (2010). The desire to expel unselfish members from the group. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 303310. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peacey, M. W., & Sanders, M. (2014). Masked heroes: Endogenous anonymity in charitable giving. University of Bristol: Centre for Market and Public Organisation.Google Scholar
Penn, D. J. (2003). The evolutionary roots of our environmental problems: Toward a Darwinian ecology. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 78(3), 275301. https://doi.org/10.1086/377051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, T., Rutte, C., Killingback, T., Taborsky, M., & Bonhoeffer, S. (2005). Evolution of cooperation by generalized reciprocity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 272(1568), 11151120. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2988Google ScholarPubMed
Piazza, J., & Bering, J. (2008). Concerns about reputation via gossip promote generous allocations in an economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(3), 172178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pizarro, D., Uhlmann, E., & Salovey, P. (2003). Asymmetry in judgments of moral blame and praise: The role of perceived metadesires. Psychological Science, 14(3), 267272. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.03433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pleasant, A., & Barclay, P. (2018). Why hate the good guy? Antisocial punishment of high cooperators is greater when people compete to be chosen. Psychological Science, 29(6), 868876. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617752642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, E. A., & Ready, E. (2018). Building bigness: Reputation, prominence, and social capital in rural South India. American Anthropologist, 120(3), 444459. https://doi.org/10.1111/aman.13100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Przepiorka, W., Norbutas, L., & Corten, R. (2017). Order without law: Reputation promotes cooperation in a cryptomarket for illegal drugs. European Sociological Review, 33(6), 752764. https://doi.org/10.1093/esr/jcx072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quillien, T. (2019). Universal modesty in signal-burying games. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1906), 20190985. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0985CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, N. J. (2014). Hidden altruism in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 10, 20130884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, N. J., & Aitken, D. (2011). Uncertainty, rationality and cooperation in the context of climate change. Climatic Change, 108(1–2), 4755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-010-0014-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raihani, N. J., & Barclay, P. (2016). Exploring the trade-off between quality and fairness in human partner choice. Royal Society Open Science, 3(11), 160510. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, N. J., & Bshary, R. (2015). Why humans might help strangers. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 2531. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, N. J., & Bshary, R. (2019). Punishment: One tool, many uses. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 1. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raihani, N. J., & Hart, T. (2010). Free-riders promote free-riding in a real-world setting. Oikos. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2010.18279.x/pdfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raihani, N. J., & Smith, S. (2015). Competitive helping in online giving. Current Biology, 25(9), 11831186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 413425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, L. I., Zeglen, K. N., & Schmidt, K. L. (2012). Facial expressions as honest signals of cooperative intent in a one-shot anonymous Prisoner's Dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(3), 200209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resnick, P., Zeckhauser, R., Swanson, J., & Lockwood, K. (2006). The value of reputation on eBay: A controlled experiment. Experimental Economics, 9(2), 79101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-006-4309-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G. (1998). Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 265(1394), 427431. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G. (2005). Cooperation through interdependence. Animal Behaviour, 70(4), 901908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G. (2015). Partner choice drives the evolution of cooperation via indirect reciprocity. PLoS ONE, 10(6), e0129442. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, G. (2020). Honest signaling of cooperative intentions. Behavioral Ecology. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/araa035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvadori, E., Blazsekova, T., Volein, A., Karap, Z., Tatone, D., Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2015). Probing the strength of infants’ preference for helpers over hinderers: Two replication attempts of Hamlin and Wynn (2011). PLOS ONE, 10(11), e0140570. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140570CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samu, F., Számadó, S., & Takács, K. (2020). Scarce and directly beneficial reputations support cooperation. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 11486. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-68123-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savary, J., & Goldsmith, K. (2020). Unobserved altruism: How self-signaling motivations and social benefits shape willingness to donate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 26(3), 538550. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000261Google ScholarPubMed
Savary, J., Li, C. X., & Newman, G. E. (2020). Exalted purchases or tainted donations? Self-signaling and the evaluation of charitable incentives. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 30(4), 671679. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlingloff, L., Csibra, G., & Tatone, D. (2020). Do 15-month-old infants prefer helpers? A replication of Hamlin et al. (2007). Royal Society Open Science, 7(4), 191795. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191795CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, J. F., Bahrami-Rad, D., Beauchamp, J. P., & Henrich, J. (2019). The Church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science, 366(6466). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau5141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semmann, D., Krambeck, H., & Milinski, M. (2004). Strategic investment in reputation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56(3), 248252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sen, S., Du, S., & Bhattacharya, C. (2016). Corporate social responsibility: A consumer psychology perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 7075. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.12.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shackelford, T. K., & Buss, D. M. (1996). Betrayal in mateships, friendships, and coalitions. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 11511164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, A., & Olson, K. (2015). Whose idea is it anyway? The importance of reputation in acknowledgement. Developmental Science, 18(3), 502509. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, L. (2011). Monetary rewards, image concern, and intrinsic motivation: Evidence from a survey on blood donation. Working Paper, 26.Google Scholar
Siegel, J. Z., Crockett, M. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2017). Inferences about moral character moderate the impact of consequences on blame and praise. Cognition, 167, 201211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siem, B., & Stürmer, S. (2018). Attribution of egoistic versus altruistic motives to acts of helping. Social Psychology, 50(1), 5366. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silk, J. B. (2003). Cooperation without counting: The puzzle of friendship. In Hammerstein, P. (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp. 3754). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Silver, I., Newman, G., & Small, D. A. (2021). Inauthenticity aversion: Moral reactance toward tainted actors, actions, and objects. Consumer Psychology Review, 4(1), 7082. https://doi.org/10.1002/arcp.1064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silver, I. M., & Shaw, A. (2018). Pint-sized public relations: The development of reputation management. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 277279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, B., White, K., & Laran, J. (2018). When public recognition for charitable giving backfires: The role of independent self-construal. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(6), 12571273. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucx101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M., & Hoffman, M. (2021). Commitment and impersonation: A reputation-based theory of principled behavior. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ua57rCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, D. A., & Cryder, C. (2016). Prosocial consumer behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 107111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. A., & Bird, R. L. B. (2000). Turtle hunting and tombstone opening: Public generosity as costly signaling. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(4), 245261. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00031-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. A., Bird, R. B., & Bird, D. W. (2003). The benefits of costly signaling: Meriam turtle hunters. Behavioral Ecology, 14(1), 116126. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/14.1.116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparkman, G., & Attari, S. Z. (2020a). Credibility, communication, and climate change: How lifestyle inconsistency and do-gooder derogation impact decarbonization advocacy. Energy Research & Social Science, 59, 101290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erss.2019.101290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparkman, G., & Attari, S. Z. (2020b). Credibility, communication, and climate change: How lifestyle inconsistency and do-gooder derogation impact decarbonization advocacy. Energy Research & Social Science, 59, 101290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erss.2019.101290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359393. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Srna, S., Barasch, A., & Small, D. (2017). When modesty pays. ACR North American Advances, NA-45. https://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/1024799/volumes/v45/NA-45Google Scholar
Stouten, J., van Dijke, M., Mayer, D. M., De Cremer, D., & Euwema, M. C. (2013). Can a leader be seen as too ethical? The curvilinear effects of ethical leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 24(5), 680695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sylwester, K., Herrmann, B., & Bryson, J. (2013). Homo Homini Lupus? Explaining antisocial punishment. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 6, 167188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sylwester, K., & Roberts, G. (2010). Cooperators benefit through reputation-based partner choice in economic games. Biology Letters, 6(5), 659662. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2010.0209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sylwester, K., & Roberts, G. (2013). Reputation-based partner choice is an effective alternative to indirect reciprocity in solving social dilemmas. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 201206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tasimi, A., Dominguez, A., & Wynn, K. (2015). Do-gooder derogation in children: The social costs of generosity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tognetti, A., Berticat, C., Raymond, M., & Faurie, C. (2012). Sexual selection of human cooperative behaviour: An experimental study in rural Senegal. PLoS ONE, 7, e44403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torelli, C. J., Monga, A. B., & Kaikati, A. M. (2012). Doing poorly by doing good: Corporate social responsibility and brand concepts. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(5), 948963. https://doi.org/10.1086/660851CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Vugt, M. (2009). Averting the tragedy of the commons: Using social psychological science to protect the environment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 169173. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01630.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Vugt, M., & Hardy, C. L. (2009). Cooperation for reputation: Wasteful contributions as costly signals in public goods. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. http://gpi.sagepub.com/content/early/2009/11/13/1368430209342258.full.pdfGoogle Scholar
Visserman, M. L., Righetti, F., Impett, E. A., Keltner, D., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2018). It's the motive that counts: Perceived sacrifice motives and gratitude in romantic relationships. Emotion, 18(5), 625637. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Rueden, C. R., & Jaeggi, A. V. (2016). Men's status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(39), 1082410829. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1606800113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Rueden, C. R., Redhead, D., O'Gorman, R., Kaplan, H., & Gurven, M. (2019). The dynamics of men's cooperation and social status in a small-scale society. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1908), 20191367. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.1367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, X., & Tong, L. (2015). Hide the light or let it shine? Examining the factors influencing the effect of publicizing donations on donors’ happiness. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 32(4), 418424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijresmar.2015.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., Sebastián-Enesco, C., Benjamin, N. E., & Pieloch, K. A. (2019). Pay to play: Children's emerging ability to use acts of generosity for selfish ends. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 188, 104675. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, A., Michels, C., Burgmer, P., Mussweiler, T., Ockenfels, A., & Hofmann, W. (2020). Trust in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Hashimoto, H., Cook, K. S., Kiyonari, T., Shinada, M., Mifune, N., … Li, Y. (2012). Modesty in self-presentation: A comparison between the USA and Japan: Modesty in self-presentation. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 15(1), 6068. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X.2011.01362.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoeli, E., Hoffman, M., Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Powering up with indirect reciprocity in a large-scale field experiment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(Supplement 2), 1042410429. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1301210110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, Y., Gürhan-Canli, Z., & Schwarz, N. (2006). The effect of corporate social responsibility (CSR) activities on companies with bad reputations. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16(4), 377390. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp1604_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuan, M., Wu, J., & Kou, Y. (2018). Donors’ social class and their prosocial reputation. Social Psychology, 49(4), 205218. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yudkin, D. A., Prosser, A. M. B., & Crockett, M. J. (2019). Actions speak louder than outcomes in judgments of prosocial behavior. Emotion, 19(7), 11381147. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000514CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zane, D. M., Irwin, J. R., & Reczek, R. W. (2016). Do less ethical consumers denigrate more ethical consumers? The effect of willful ignorance on judgments of others. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(3), 337349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar