Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-02T18:13:13.347Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Supporting the practical and emotional process of giving-up driving in later life for those living with dementia

Commentary on “Developing the Driving and Dementia Roadmap: A knowledge-to-action process” by Stasiulis et al.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 March 2023

Charles B.A. Musselwhite*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Centre for Ageing and Dementia Research, Aberystwyth University, Aberystwyth, Wales, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentary
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Psychogeriatric Association

Changes that happen everyone as they age (albeit at different rates), including, for example, changes in eyesight or hearing, and changes in cognition (such as attention, overload, and speed of processing), can often combine in later life, making it hard to perform complex tasks, for example, the tasks required to drive a vehicle in a safe manner (see Musselwhite, Reference Musselwhite2017 for a review). Giving-up driving is a not just a cognitive or skill-based decision and is part of a wider social process. Many people are often involved in the decision alongside the older person themselves, including family and friends and health professionals. Advice from health professionals is welcomed, and support of family and friends is a vital protective factor in reducing negative affect of giving-up driving (Musselwhite and Shergold, Reference Musselwhite and Shergold2013). But many people involved in the driver decision process are often reluctant to initiate conversation, offer advice or get involved altogether (see Musselwhite and Shergold, Reference Musselwhite and Shergold2013). Dementia adds a further layer of complexity on the situation; as noted in previous research, people living with dementia and their families lack knowledge of when and how to approach giving-up (LaFrance et al., Reference Lafrance, Dreise, Gouliquer and Poulin2021). Dementia is a term used to cover many different neurodegenerative diseases, but characterized by cognitive and functional impairment, which impact everyday activities. To make matters more complex, dementia is not categorical, and it is generally accepted that cognitive decline is on a continuum from subjective (undiagnosed) through mild cognitive impairment to medically diagnosed dementia (see Pini and Wennberg, Reference Pini and Wennberg2021), meaning diagnosis is not necessarily synonymous with skills, insight and ability to drive (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Liddle, Pachana, Beattie and Mitchell2020). To support the driving cessation process, Stasiulis et al. (Reference Stasiulis2023) present the Driving and Dementia Roadmap (DDR) (see https://www.drivinganddementia.ca/), a toolkit which provides information for people involved in the process of giving-up driving to make informed choices. To be commended, the DDR toolkit is developed systematically, using a Knowledge to Action framework, and is informed by evidence and has been co-developed and tested with people living with dementia and other actors involved in the decision.

Being mobile in later life is linked with satisfaction and quality of life and having to give-up driving is associated with decreases in health and well-being, including feelings of stress and isolation and also increased mortality (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Perkins, Ross and Reynolds2009; Musselwhite and Haddad, Reference Musselwhite and Haddad2010, Reference Musselwhite and Haddad2018; Musselwhite and Shergold, Reference Musselwhite and Shergold2013; Ragland et al., Reference Ragland, Satariano and MacLeod2005; Windsor et al., Reference Windsor, Anstey, Butterworth, Luszcz and Andrews2007). This may in part be due a reduction in activity out of home (Harrison and Ragland, Reference Harrison and Ragland2003) and resulting decreases in physical and social functioning (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Perkins, Ross and Reynolds2009), less frequent health care use for check-ups and chronic care (Arcury et al., Reference Arcury, Gesler, Preisser, Sherman, Spencer and Perin2005), reduced social networks (Mezuk and Rebok, Reference Mezuk and Rebok2008) and social and leisure activities (Marottoli et al., Reference Marottoli, Mendes de Leon, Glass, Williams, Cooney and Berkman2000). In car-dominated societies, giving-up driving is perceived to be something that could cause people to be unable to meet their needs and can cause isolation and loneliness. The issue is amplified in rural areas, where shops and services are further dispersed (Musselwhite, Reference Musselwhite2023). Naismith et al. (Reference Naismith, Howard, Stewart, Pitman and Mueller2022) remind us how a diagnosis of dementia is linked with depression and even attempts at suicide, and although the relationship as to why is not clear-cut, issues surrounding the implications of the diagnosis and labeling are noted, alongside biological or physiological reasons. Giving-up driving alongside a diagnosis of dementia is therefore going to be an extremely emotional and vulnerable time for the individual. Scott et al. (Reference Scott, Liddle, Mitchell, Beattie and Pachana2019) have developed the excellent CarFreeMe program of social support to help older people and those living with dementia normalize the process of giving-up driving and navigate alternatives to the car. The DDR covers these aspects well through supporting older people’s needs beyond driving in the “getting around without driving” section.

Further, the car can be seen to meet psychological needs of independence, freedom and status and can help ensure roles (Musselwhite and Haddad, Reference Musselwhite and Haddad2010, Reference Musselwhite and Haddad2018). In societies where freedom and independence are championed the potential loss of a driving licence can be seen to be hugely detrimental on well-being and make the decision to stop driving less rational. Many older people in individualistic cultures, like the United Kingdom for example, do not like to have to rely on others, such as family, friends or neighbors, to support their mobility and may go to extreme lengths to keep driving themselves to avoid dependency on others (Murray and Musselwhite, Reference Murray and Musselwhite2019, Musselwhite and Shergold, Reference Musselwhite and Shergold2013). The normalness of the car in car-centric societies is also hugely influential, with strong cultural and social norms of driving very hard to operate against, feeling unusual or for the first time “feeling old” as a result of not being able to do what the dominant culture expects people to do in society (Musselwhite and Haddad, Reference Musselwhite and Haddad2010, Reference Musselwhite and Haddad2018).

Older people and especially people living with dementia may lack insight into their driving capability. This means they may not be aware they need to stop driving (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Liddle, Mitchell, Beattie and Pachana2019) and hence support is needed and indeed often welcomed, especially from healthcare professionals. However, research suggests driving is not an issue often discussed in primary care; a synthesis of 18 studies suggested many primary care physicians feel uncomfortable and lack confidence in judging whether someone would be fit to drive, in the context of possible cognitive impairment (Sinnott et al., Reference Sinnott, Foley, Forsyth, McLoughlin, Horgan and Bradley2018). Reasons for this include lack of familiarity with legal requirements, potential to damage the doctor–patient relationship and impact on the patient’s quality of life, health and well-being. Studies conclude that primary care physicians and general practitioners would like education and training to help them make decisions about fitness to drive (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Liddle, Mitchell, Beattie and Pachana2019; Sinnott et al., Reference Sinnott, Foley, Forsyth, McLoughlin, Horgan and Bradley2018). In addition, people living with dementia would really like more support but also crucially more agency surrounding decisions to stop driving (Sinnott et al., Reference Sinnott, Foley, Forsyth, McLoughlin, Horgan and Bradley2018). In order for this to happen, people need more insight into their driving ability. The DDR with its focus on helping people recognize when it might be a good idea to stop driving is very useful in this regard and it does this not just for the older person themselves, but for friends and family and also healthcare workers. The DDR picks up well the key reasons why people lack insight into their own driving which are only made worse with cognitive impairments and dementia:

  • Older people compensate well for changes effecting driving performance. When people have made changes to their driver behavior, these are largely done in a small, incremental manner and may be part of an automatic unconscious process, where the person themselves may not have insight and have noticed the changes they have made. Hence, older people do not have insight into how they have adapted and how the adaptation has changed how they are performing, and whether they need to make more drastic change or stop driving altogether.

  • The lack of detecting feedback from the driving process. There is a disconnect between a person’s ability and skill and their performance. Driving as a skill often has little to no feedback for the user to learn or act upon. Roads have been designed in a way to reduce collisions and injuries, creating a relatively forgiving environment where errors and misjudgments often go unpunished. Drivers can drive relatively poorly and receive no negative feedback, for example through not crashing, and not even experiencing a near-miss collision. Older drivers may therefore be very unaware that they have been driving in a dangerous manner, and have not learnt to adapt or change from feedback.

  • Compensation. Older people are often able to compensate for driving when the task demand is too high, but these are often situations where older people may not have to drive anymore. For example, older people may not have to drive in busy traffic, having retired from work, hence they may not be in situations of high demand where they may be more likely to notice their skills are deteriorating. They are also able to pick and choose routes and when to travel, reducing the demand on their skills and allowing them to compensate for changes in ability without having to give-up driving, but also reducing insight into the need to stop driving altogether.

There is tentative evidence that training and education surrounding driving can have a positive effect on driver performance, though this may not be long lived, and there is a lack of evidence around which training and education work most effectively, and whether it actually converts into reducing collisions (see Musselwhite, Reference Musselwhite2017 for review). However, there is evidence that those who access training and education voluntarily are the “worried well”, that is, they are performing the task well (Musselwhite, Reference Musselwhite2017). Some of the most over confident or the least aware drivers do not access training voluntarily. The need to encourage insight into behavior and discussions with family members and healthcare professionals taking the lead are needed and are covered in the DDR.

Countries that offer more stringent process for carrying on driving in later life, including places that have on-road testing, cognitive or medical screening, have no fewer collisions or injuries on the road than countries that have relatively relaxed measures (e.g. self-declaration at 70 years, and then every 3 years as is the case in the United Kingdom) (see Mitchell, Reference Mitchell and Musselwhite2017). In the absence of tests that clearly demarcate someone as safe or unsafe as a driver, there needs to be support for initiating insight. This is obviously hard to do with cognitive decline and dementia that may affect people’s insight. The social issues surrounding driving, the loss of freedom, independence and the feeling of being out of touch with society make it hugely emotional.

Overall, then there is no panacea to support giving-up driving. It is moreover a process that has to be “smoothed” through encouraging insight, helping the older person or person living with dementia make the decision to stop themselves, with the practical and emotional support of family and friends. The DDR toolkit reflects the social process, encouraging the initiation of conversations among family and from healthcare professionals and emphasizes the importance of having insight into driving. The next stages for the DDR will be to evaluate its effectiveness, and allowing it to adapt to changes and developments in our understanding cognitive impairment and dementia, alongside changes in design and the use of vehicles; for example in the future will a person living with dementia have similar issues giving up being the “driver” of a more automated vehicle?

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Arcury, T. A., Gesler, W. M., Preisser, J. S., Sherman, J., Spencer, J. and Perin, J. (2005). The effects of geography and spatial behavior on health care utilization among the residents of a rural region. Health Services Research, 40, 135156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J. D., Perkins, M., Ross, L. A. and Reynolds, S. L. (2009). Driving status and three-year mortality among community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64, 300305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, A. and Ragland, D. R. (2003). Consequences of driving reduction or cessation for older adults. Transportation Research Record, 1843, 96104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lafrance, M. N., Dreise, E., Gouliquer, L. and Poulin, C. (2021). We’re not doing it to be nasty. Canadian Journal on Aging/La Revue canadienne du vieillissement, 41, 18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980820000409.Google Scholar
Marottoli, R. A., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Glass, T. A., Williams, C. S., Cooney, L. M. and Berkman, L. F. (2000). Consequences of driving cessation: decreased out-of-home activity levels. Journals of Gerontology: Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55B, 334340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mezuk, B. and Rebok, G. W. (2008). Social integration and social support among older adults following driving cessation. Journal of Gerontolology Social Science, 63B, 298303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, K. (2017). Are older people safe drivers on the roads, testing and training? In: Musselwhite, C. (Ed.), Transport, Travel and Later Life (Transport and Sustainability, Vol. 10) (pp. 3763). Bingley: Emerald Publishing Limited. https://doi.org/10.1108/S2044-994120170000010014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, A. and Musselwhite, C. B. A. (2019). Older peoples’ experiences of informal support after giving up driving. Research in Transportation Business & Management, 30, 100367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musselwhite, C. B. A. (2017). Assessment of computer-based training packages to improve the safety of older people’s driver behaviour. Transport Planning & Technology, 40, 6479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musselwhite, C. B. A. (2023). Rurality, sustainability and health. Journal of Transport and Health, 28, 101571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musselwhite, C. B. A. and Haddad, H. (2010). Mobility, accessibility and quality of later life. Quality in Ageing and Older Adults, 11, 2537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musselwhite, C. B. A. and Haddad, H. (2018). Older people’s travel and mobility needs: a reflection of a hierarchical model 10 years on. Quality in Ageing and Older Adults, 19, 87105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musselwhite, C. B. A. and Shergold, I. (2013). Examining the process of driving cessation in later life. European Journal of Ageing, 10, 89100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naismith, H., Howard, R., Stewart, R., Pitman, A. and Mueller, C. (2022). Suicidal ideation in dementia: associations with neuropsychiatric symptoms and subtype diagnosis. International Psychogeriatrics, 34, 399406. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610222000126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pini, L. and Wennberg, A. (2021). Does, one size fit all, in subjective cognitive decline? International Psychogeriatrics, 33, 651653. https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161022000335X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ragland, D. R., Satariano, W. A. and MacLeod, K. E. (2005). Driving cessation and increased depressive symptoms. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 60, 399403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, T. L., Liddle, J., Mitchell, G., Beattie, E. and Pachana, N. (2019). Implementation and evaluation of a driving cessation intervention to improve community mobility and wellbeing outcomes for people living with dementia: study protocol of the, CarFreeMe, for people with dementia program. BMC Geriatrics, 19, 66. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-019-1074-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, T. L., Liddle, J., Pachana, N. A., Beattie, E. and Mitchell, G. K. (2020). Managing the transition to non-driving in patients with dementia in primary care settings: facilitators and barriers reported by primary care physicians. International Psychogeriatrics, 32, 14191428. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610218002326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinnott, C., Foley, T., Forsyth, J., McLoughlin, K., Horgan, L. and Bradley, C. P. (2018). Consultations on driving in people with cognitive impairment in primary care: a scoping review of the evidence. PLoS ONE, 13, e0205580. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stasiulis, E. et al. (2023). Developing the driving and dementia roadmap: a knowledge-to-action process. International Psychogeriatrics, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610222001235.Google ScholarPubMed
Windsor, T. D., Anstey, K. J., Butterworth, P., Luszcz, M. A. and Andrews, G. R. (2007). The role of perceived control in explaining depressive symptoms associated with driving cessation in a longitudinal study. The Gerontologist, 47, 215223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed