Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T17:41:40.110Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Establishing best practices in the use of an upgraded airborne teaching laboratory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2023

S. Daniels*
Affiliation:
School of Water, Energy, and Environment, Cranfield University, Cranfield, United Kingdom, MK43 0AL
G. Braithwaite
Affiliation:
School of Aerospace, Transport, and Manufacturing, Cranfield University, MK430A
G. Gratton
Affiliation:
School of Aerospace, Transport, and Manufacturing, Cranfield University, MK430A
*
Corresponding author: S. Daniels; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Since the 1980s National Flying Laboratory Centre has used the Jetstream family of aircraft as a flying classroom, providing university students and developing professionals with real-world exposure to theoretical concepts in the form of practical flight test instruction. Recently the Jetstream was replaced with a newer Saab-340B. The work in this paper presents an experimental analysis of instruction using the Jetstream, compared with known best practices, to inform its replacement process. Flight activities were observed, and participating students (n = 60) were surveyed at four set intervals to establish their mood and interest towards the module. A pen and paper test, comparing what participants learned compared to a controlled group was also administered. While the module was still able to excite, motivate and re-contextualise previously taught information to students, upgrades to the aging technology suite, specifically to support data analysis and briefing was one of the greatest needs from the newer aircraft.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal Aeronautical Society

Nomenclature

CAA

Civil Aviation Authority

CG

Centre of gravity

DME

Distance measuring equipment

EASA

European Union Aviation Safety Agency

G

Gravitational acceleration felt

GPS

Global Positioning System

IRS

Inertial Reference System

ILS

Instrument Landing System

n

Number

NFLC

National Flying Laboratory Centre

RAeS

Royal Aeronautical Society

SD

Standard deviation

TLX

Task load index

1.0 Introduction

Cranfield University’s National Flying Laboratory Centre (NFLC) has, since 1946, operated multi-seat training aircraft, known as flying classrooms. The prime use, alongside duties as a research aircraft, is to provide university students and developing professionals with practical exposure to, experience of, and training in airborne flight test engineering. This instruction is a form of practical work analogous to experimental science education delivered in schools, and serves to fulfil a requirement for Royal Aeronautical Society (RAeS) accredited aerospace engineering courses, which maintains that as flight test is a key aero-engineering specialism dealing with real experiments with real air vehicles, there are inherently indispensable educational qualities to doing real instructional flight test exercises [1].

In 2019, NFLC began the process of replacing its current Jetstream-3102 aircraft with a newer and larger Saab-340B, with the view to enable higher quality teaching and research. This paper concerns lessons on the now retired Jetstream.

The perception that practical work, particularly in the United Kingdom where NFLC mainly operates, is integral to effective science and technology education in general is widely shared amongst teachers and students in addition to the RAeS. Research in flying classroom use also draws conclusions which support it; however, a much larger body of surrounding evidence published over the last 40 years differs, describing a strategy that is difficult and rare to implement effectively [Reference Kirschner and Meester2Reference Abrahams and Millar4].

2.0 Pedagogy of effective flying classrooms

Rather than the airborne environment offsetting the need for good pedagogy, the positive outcomes reported by flying classroom literature are likely due to practices baked into professional aviation regulation and culture [5]. The aviation system of work, originally designed for pilot instruction, also makes good pedagogy for practical work of this type.

The main trait of the aviation training system, and many effective practical work lessons, is adherence to a rehearsed format, composed of: 1) theoretical knowledge instruction and synthesis; followed by 2) pre-flight briefing; 3) exercise; 4) immediate debrief of key events; and 5) deeper analysis and synthesis. Lessons learned from literature, both from the flying classroom and wider pedagogical community, illustrate its value:

Chiefly, practical work itself does not make for consistent learning on its own, but re-enforces it [Reference Harrison6]. Teachers and educators who employ practical work as an alternative teaching strategy to classroom instruction rather than a supplement often find students may do what is expected, but not learn what is expected [Reference Abrahams and Millar4]. This is especially notable for more experiential use-cases, where a student being able to feel, or directly observe theoretical phenomena first-hand is the express aim of the exercise [Reference Kolb7, Reference Brown and Kulik8]. Lewis et al. [Reference Lewis, Potts and Gautrey9] and Stickland and Scanlon [Reference Stickland and Scanlon10] for example, report on flying classroom activities that make use of experiential learning to demonstrate previously learned flight dynamic principles where the timing, depth and relevance of the theoretical instruction provided ahead of the exercise were major considerations. This is reflected in where the curriculum they were used, appearing in the second academic year, building on fundamental material learned in the first. While doing practical work can promote more in depth analysis to understanding introducing new concepts and phenomena [Reference Mccay-peet, Toms and Kelloway11Reference Park, Abrahams and Song12], consistently effective practical work tends to build on knowledge that is consistently explored and taught.

Evidence from general science studies, such as by Jackman et al. [Reference Jackman, Moellenbem and Brabson13], Watson et al. [Reference Watson, Prieto and Dillon14], Hofstein and Lunetta [Reference Hofstein and Lunetta15] and Harrison [Reference Harrison6] show a pattern of practical work being more difficult to learn in due to factors in the environment, such as temperature changes, accelerations, motio and, sounds [Reference Miller16, Reference Haji, Cheung, Woods, Regehr, de Ribaupierre and Dubrowski17]. Many aviation tasks are also sufficiently complex in addition to the environment that careful management and mitigation are essential facets of safe training practice. Both the intrinsic load brought on by task complexity and external load through the environment [Reference Sweller18, Reference Chandler and Sweller19] can be controlled through familiarisation and rehearsal, even simulation in extreme cases [20, Reference Salas, Bowers and Rhodenizer21]. This is the purpose of the pre-flight brief, to mentally allow the student to rehearse the exercise. Crucially, this is not the same as theoretical knowledge instruction. The level of complexity of flying classroom exercises have, in general, shown to be minimal compared to flight crew, not out-pacing level of training required by Part-SPO’s broad requirements. However, the nuances of how students are briefed for specific exercises remain major considerations [Reference Lewis, Potts and Gautrey9, Reference Slingerland, Melkert and Paassen22].

It may be resource constraints driving the removal of post-exercise work [Reference Millar and Wellington23], or the opinion that the exercise alone is sufficient for learning [Reference Kaptan and Timurlenk3], but it is common practice for pedagogy in science education to stop once the exercise has finished [Reference Abrahams24]. Effective practical work does not, instead making use of further academic time, which repeatedly requires the students to recall the exercise over a longer period than the module itself. Committing temporary information gathered in flight from the working memory into long-term memory requires a student to recall it multiple times. The approximate half-life of memory is 24 h [Reference Murre and Dros30], and other activity which draws a student’s attention away – such are the demands of academia – will make this faster [Reference Schacter31]. Hence, a common practice in professional flight test, mirrored in flying classroom lessons is to compile a report detailing what happened, typically within 24 h [Reference Roediger and Karpicke33], before future reference and analysis. Direct comparisons between and further work, and no other intervention shows the former to be far more effective at improving recall and developing knowledge [Reference Roediger and Karpicke33]. Yet, this activity will likely be far longer than the exercise; Slingerland et al. [Reference Slingerland, Melkert and Paassen22] find students will be far less inclined to perform ‘the boring bits’ themselves. In many ways, this portion of a practical work lesson requires as many resources as the exercise itself.

Overt discussions as to an exercise’s ‘affective value’ – educationally beneficial moods and feelings arising from a lesson – are common to other forms of scientific practical work [Reference Braund and Reiss34Reference Simon, Wagner and Killion36], but not most aviation instruction. Practical work tends to be enjoyable [Reference Griffin37], confidence-boosting [Reference Richardson, Abraham and Bond38], interesting [Reference Knogler, Harackiewicz, Gegenfurtner and Lewalter39, Reference Kearney40], and motivating [Reference Cook and Artino41, Reference Hidi and Harackiewicz42]. There is an evidence basis to suggest that each construct aids student achievement (e.g. [Reference Knogler, Harackiewicz, Gegenfurtner and Lewalter39] and has made its way into official policy in some schools, or even used for behaviour control [Reference Abrahams43]. However, each holds little pedagogical value on their own. These facts have been referenced in flying classroom literature and affect treated as an explicit advantage [Reference Slingerland, Melkert and Paassen22, Reference Bromfield and Belberov29], however they refrain from explicitly refer to affect as a policy decision.

2.1 Motivation for study

Community acceptance of how aviation is done, not regulatory or systemic needs, will be the driver for flying classroom operators to continue follow this pedagogy. Compared to pilot instruction there are fewer regulatory or systemic requirements for it. Most, including NFLC, will operate under a separate annex of the operational regulations named CS-Part-SPOFootnote 1 , where legally there are only broad requirements to brief an occupant for their role in the aircraft [44], and operationally one which is not safety critical or requiring the use of unfamiliar equipment. This means other practical or pedagogical factors can yet erode best practice. The Jetstream-3102 was initially sized for the demands of teaching aero-engineering between the 1980s and early 2000s [Reference Lewis, Potts and Gautrey9], which have since evolved along with the scale of operation, prompting NFLC to begin the process of replacing it. An assessment of what strengths its replacement, a larger Saab-340B, needs to re-enforce, and vulnerabilities to address in order to enable best accepted teaching practice for the next 20 years.

2.2 Study objectives

The objective of the work in this paper was to perform an experimentally derived analysis of a representative flying classroom module in practice, to compare it with what is known to be effective aviation, and practical work pedagogy, and from this analysis, draw systemic recommendations which would serve to strengthen its use. This was done in 2019 using NFLC’s previous Jetstream-3102 aircraft, shown in Fig. 1, as part of the learning process leading up to its Saab-340B replacement.

Figure 1. NFLC Jetstream 3102 as used in this research.

2.3 Course selected for study

The course selected for study is one of a series of standard exercises delivered by NFLC, in this case, delivered internally to Postgraduate Aerospace Design Engineering students. Sixty students, all studying an Aerospace Design MSc, agreed to supply data for this work. Of these students, 32 contributed towards the academic trial, 40 contributed survey data, and 10 took part in a post-flight interview. Participation did not affect their academic progress. A pre-study survey indicated a professionally young, global cohort; the average age was 26 (SD = 4.1), representing 19 different nationalities. Eight students reported prior aerospace design or engineering experience, with a mean of 5.8 years (SD = 5.0). The flying experience of the cohort was, on average, low. Six students had been on board in a single Jetstream flight before; very few had any other form of active flying experience. Two, however, held private pilot qualifications.

The objectives of the module were a mixture of procedural knowledge and skills (e.g. Report results using collected flight test data) and declarative knowledge (e.g. explain the practical functions of the primary flying controls). This study mainly analysed changes to higher-order skills. Similar to Trainelli et al. [Reference Trainelli, Rolando, Bonaita and Chimetto25], the demonstrators emphasised that flight test itself was not the aim, but was an incidental means to explore “real” engineering:

“[…] from a teaching perspective there’s nothing like seeing it in practice. It puts theory into context; if you deal purely in theory, then everything is very logical, clean, follows the trend perfectly. One of the things flight test shows –or any experiment, with flight test as an example – is that the real world is not like that. The students feel the real conditions in the aircraft, and they get the variation of that in their data as well.”

Following a week of classroom lectures, in groups of 15, students acted as flight test observers onboard two flights: (1) measuring parameters related to aircraft performance; (2) measuring parameters related to aircraft stability and handling. Students aggregated, analysed, then presented data across all flights. The purpose of these learning objectives was for the students to remember set aerodynamic and flight dynamic knowledge, as well as to demonstrate the robustness of underlying theoretical principles by using real-world data in analysis work.

The Jetstream 3102 had a maximum take-off mass of 7,059kg, and a capacity of 15 students, two pilots flying, with one demonstrator supervising and operating the data collection and display system. The aircraft was fitted with instrumentation allowing for measurement of control surface positions, applied control forces, aircraft attitude in three dimensions, angle-of-attack and angle of sideslip, aircraft body rates of rotation and acceleration, static and differential pressures for airspeed and altitude readings, position using Global Positioning System (GPS) and Inertial Reference System (IRS), and avionics including Distance Measuring Equipment (DME) and an Instrument Landing System (ILS). Data was reduced to meaningful units, which the demonstrator can transmit to seat-mounted displays inside the aircraft for the students to view or use to record data using LABVIEW.

2.4 Study design

A mixed-methods approach was adopted to capture both the qualitative and quantitative characteristics of the course in practice [Reference Cohen, Manion and Morrison45]. A controlled pen-and-paper academic trial was distributed before and after the course to measure the response in student knowledge. The trial required students (n = 17) to complete three questions: 1) and 2) compile two short flight test reports pertaining respectively to performance parameters, and stability and landing parameters using fictional test data; 3) Identify some stability characteristics from printed data traces. Students were briefed that they had 90min and that they could complete the questions in any order. The trial featured a fictional aircraft of a different type to the Jetstream. The results were scored by two independent subject matter experts (SMEs) with flight dynamics and academic grading experience. The marking scheme for each question was a rubric based upon the seven-part paragraph, common in-flight test reporting [Reference Gratton32], and assessed the strength of students analysis, discussion, conclusions, recommendations, relation to real operations, and presentation. To reduce familiarity with the test affecting student scores improving student score, the surface details the trial questions administered post-course were different. A ‘do-nothing’ control group (n = 13) studying the same degree, who had not exposed to the Jetstream course, took part in the trial to determine retest reliability [Reference O’Shea, O’ Connell and Gallagher46]Footnote 2 . The trial was first administered to a pilot study group to check for errors and readability. This trial considered the total learning response across the course as a whole without controlling for extraneous variables, which Bracht and Glass [Reference Bracht and Glass47] suggest that this has more external validity than testing isolated elements, particularly in the context of an educational package.

To determine practical task effectiveness and offer a description of its difficulty, students were timed completing each task and were further asked to complete the NASA TLX self-report scale [Reference Hart and Staveland48] post-flight, detailing six subscales: mental and physical load, effort, success, frustration and pace. TLX is normally used per task; here it was used to explore the effect of each flight as a whole. A total workload rating can be calculated through an average of each raw score, weighted by the student. TLX, however, has not consistently been shown as more, or less, valid considering only raw scores [Reference Hart49]. As a result, total load was not considered to limit unnecessary student engagement.

A randomised subset of 12 students agreed to take part in a semi-structured debrief to discuss what they remembered from the flight, and what they felt; their comments are shown throughout this work using representative pseudonyms. In this case, the sampling strategy was not based on reaching content saturation, but a practical maximum, based on recommendations given by Onwuegbuzie and Collins [Reference Onwuegbuzie and Collins50]. The affective value of the course was also measured at four different points (n = 48): before the course start, after flight 1, after flight 2 and at module completion. The survey measured four dimensions, identified in other practical education research: Motivation, Interest, Enjoyment and Self-Confidence.

3.0 Results

The first flight took place shortly after a 2-h lecture covering the test plan, explanation of the aircraft systems, and a safety brief; flight time was 26min and students were active for 8min. Comments made regarding each section of the flight are presented in Table 2. Just before the test, students who were sat towards the rear of the aircraft were instructed to move to the front row, moving the centre of gravity to the desired position. The demonstrator changed the seat displays to show the data console and repeated what information the students needed to manually record. The demonstrator froze the screen to allow the students to write down the information displayed. No student appeared to have missed any data or remarked to have struggled with the task; one elected to record extraneous information. Upon completion of data collection, students were returned to their original position for landing. As the instructor needed to fly other groups of students, debrief was limited.

Table 1. Key flying classroom research: conclusions

Table 2. Sample comments from students post-flight, referencing major flight phases

The second flight, the following day, was 44min, where students were actively recording data for 5min and were required to observe for a further 17min. Students appeared to be far more familiar and comfortable with the environment, and visibly less focused on distractions.

Following this, students were prompted to watch flight data displays. The demonstrator also prompted all students to refer to their workbooks, which contained information about the manoeuvres to be flown. Students, experiencing a rapid change in conditions (the “Short Period” mode), freely recalled the facts mentioned in demonstrator’s commentary during debriefing.

The second demonstration was the phugoid mode, a longer period of oscillation where the aircraft exchanges speed and altitude. The demonstrator commented:

“[…] the pilot has released the elevator, feel the aircraft start to pitch nose down”, then, “as the airspeed increases, we generate more lift, once we get beyond 160kts that extra lift will feel as an increase in G, [in the] meantime the aircraft starts to pitch nose up.”

An example of what the students will see is shown in Fig. 2.

Figure 2. (Top) Example data trace of aircraft speed vs altitude during phugoid mode. Reproduced with permission from Ref. [Reference Gratton32].

Recall of facts, such as the relationship between G and airspeed, the instability of the mode was better during debrief compared to other modes. Student feedback tended to concentrate upon the experiential rather than the technical aspects of this.

During Dutch roll (Fig. 3), the pilot made alternate left and right inputs to the rudder to yaw the aircraft back and forth, which also prompts a secondary rolling motion. This is visible to the students out the window as the wingtip tracing a circle around the horizon, and through the flight deck camera as the nose tracing a figure 8. All students during debrief recalled feeling the only the lateral motion, not the prescribed shapes drawn at the nose or wing.

Figure 3. The motion the pilots (top) and students (bottom) see the aircraft trace during Dutch roll mode. Reproduced with permission from Ref. [Reference Gratton32].

During a demonstration of the roll subsidence mode, the damping effect which limits roll rate, was the mode students least freely recalled, and offered little discussion. The data trace onboard the aircraft offered students the best explanation of what was happening, but this was not accessible to them afterwards.

The spiral mode (accelerating and descending with increasing bank angle and G) demonstration was the second mode students most freely discussed. The demonstrator prompted:

“[…] concentrate on what you can feel, rather than what you can see looking out of the window to start with”.

During the mode the demonstrator commented that students are

“[…] probably feeling a little bit of G, now that’s because the airspeed has gone up by 20kts…30kts. You can see 40degrees of bank, which you can see by looking at the screen, or looking outside of the aircraft”.

Student comments on these again concentrated upon the experiential aspects, with emotive terms more often used than engineering ones.

Debriefing for both flights was made by the instructor the following week and covered both flights. The following week, students were asked to report whether they had completed the related coursework. Two had.

3.1 Task loading

Figure 4 shows self-reported task-load index (TLX) scores for each flight. Load and effort increased during the second flight particularly physically, but not unreasonably so. Students reported that they had sufficient spare capacity and desire to do more in each flight, and crucially were able to retain capacity to absorb information.

Figure 4. Comparison of participant (n = 48) mean raw task load index (NASA-TLX) scores (0–20) by flight. ± Standard error shown.

3.2 Educational effectiveness

Students were administered the pre-test 2h prior to the start of the module; mean score was 4.18, (SD = 3.57), and 6.38 (SD = 6.05) for the control (n = 13) and intervention groups (n = 17), respectively. The post-test was administered 2h after module completion, with a mean score of 5.90 (SD = 5.63), 13.25 (SD = 9.80) for the control and intervention groups, respectively. An initial one-way ANOVA was performed, comparing the post-test scores of the intervention group and the control group, controlling for pre-test scores. No statistically significant differences were observed (F = 3.874, p = 0.061, 1-β = 0.472), indicating the exercises were not effective.

However, further analysis of individual questions shows a greater change than their combined sum represents. Firstly, the control group showed statistically significant practice effects, but for Q1 only (Wilcoxon signed rank test [Reference Wilcoxon51], z = 2.036, p = 0.042). Repeat analysis, excluding the results of Q1, show that the intervention group made a small but significant improvement compared to the control group (Wilcoxon sign rank test, z = 2.661, p = 0.008). These are shown in Table 3. Students made the largest improvement to initial identification of the problem they were presented with, but were not able yet to apply it to reasonable conclusions.

Table 3. Comparison of average test scores for the intervention group across rubric dimensions for aircraft handling and aircraft dynamic mode questions

3.3 Affective valueFootnote 3

Some moods and feelings generated by the module are educationally beneficial. Figure 5 shows the change in different dimensions of positive affect throughout the module. All measures peak completing the second flight and sharply decrease once the practical elements are finished, which suggests it is a short-term advantage and not a long-term outcome. One student commented that this reinforced a desire to enter the flight test professions; evidence from other authors suggests, however, that this may be temporary [Reference Abrahams43]. The value of affect in practical education like this is as a mechanism to hold a student’s attention enough to complete the supporting task or assignment.

Figure 5. Student’s (n = 48) self-report of four dimensions of affect, interest, motivation, enjoyment and self-efficacy. ± Standard error shown.

3.4 Experiential value

When left to freely recall specific facts about the flights, students mentioned the more physically demanding experiences. They valued being able to feel aircraft motion during dynamic mode demonstrations, which they felt helped to contextualise and visualise theoretical knowledge they already held. These experiences, coincidentally, were related to the controlled trial questions less sensitive to practice effects, suggesting rote learning would be a less-effective method. For example, one student suggested that the numerical data they recorded was not the main benefit compared to feeling what those numbers mean in action. A second had mixed feelings about the flight, describing it simultaneously as “amazing” and “so uncomfortable”, but felt they could now attach judgement to numbers associated with dynamic modes. A third discussed that being on board the aircraft was important, because “my imagination would not have been sufficient”.

Prior work by Lewis et al. [Reference Lewis, Potts and Gautrey9] indicated students feel that experience of direct aircraft operation, particularly the role of the pilot, would also be beneficial to experience, and this view was repeated here. The avionics suite onboard the Jetstream-3201 does not allow for much integration of this students noted that they lacked situational awareness of the flights as a whole in a way that can be aided by modernisation in the cabin (e.g. moving maps). By comparison, the commentary and guidance that were given during dynamic demonstrations were considered to greatly enhance the experiential value of the flight, and when coupled with the visual aids on the data screens shows the potential of such an upgrade.

3.5 Limitations

There are two primary limitations to this study. First, a modestly powered test (1-β = 0.46) was achieved, which falls below commonly accepted metric of statistical confidence (1-β ≥ 0.8) but is reasonable for this type of exploratory research [Reference Cohen, Manion and Morrison45]. Because of this, the tests performed were not sensitive enough to reveal whether the observed results were statistically significant; however, the supporting qualitative data gathered suggesting it could be pedagogically meaningful. Second, only the acute affective and experiential value of the module was recorded; its impact on students in later professional life was not explored. However, from prior evidence drawn from related studies, a reasonable hypothesis is that affect, and recall of the related experiences will tend to diminish over time [Reference Roediger and Karpicke33, Reference Abrahams43].

4.0 Discussion and conclusions

In broad, these findings resonate with other flying classroom literature, counter to the wider body of practical work knowledge. Chiefly, that well-implemented flying classroom instruction continues to commute a narrow but indispensable benefit in aero-engineering education. Students ended the module with a measurable improvement in their ability to identify different aircraft dynamic modes and were beginning to calculate the effects that longitudinal centre of gravity (CG) has on stability using real non-idealised experimental data, a chief aim of the course. The flight test exercises themselves promoted interest, self-confidence, motivation and enjoyment among students, and the first-hand experience of relevant aircraft manoeuvres engendered the impression that their current understanding of aero-engineering knowledge had been deepened, consistent with what aspects students improved the most at during the controlled trial.

The results here, however, re-enforce how sensitive to disturbances in procedure practical work of this nature is, emphasising the need to keep the facility up-to-date to cope with modern teaching demands. The two major vulnerabilities discovered were largely indicative of problems related to the aging Jetstream-3201 and its equipment, that had become increasingly challenging for instructors to compensate for.

Firstly, a passenger capacity of 15 is now too small to handle the volume of students on modern aero-engineering courses, requiring more flights than optimal, accelerating aircraft maintenance intervals and pressuring staff time which would otherwise be available to provide further teaching support. The pedagogical effect was borne out here, where scheduling limits imposed a longer than ideal gap between theoretical knowledge instruction and the exercise, and again between the exercise and debrief. This affected recall of theoretical knowledge and key facts regarding the flight once they were finished. The instructor also could not maximise the positive affective and experiential values the exercise commutes, coinciding with lower coursework completion rates after a week.

Secondly, even though task loading indices showed students felt they had spare capacity, an initial first flight was essential in developing a capacity and situational awareness in the following exercises. Students would likely develop this capacity naturally if exposed to more flights, and NFLC offer courses that see students fly up to five times. Many clients, however, as in the example studied here, select two, or even one flight, and the capabilities of the data suites onboard the Jetstream-3201 were no longer sufficient to extract more performance from students in that timespan. Upgrades were relatively cost-prohibitive to install into that airframe.

Targeted use of technology may bridge this gap. Specific reference is made to students’ situational awareness during flights, a problem which technology fitted to other training aircraft suites has greatly improved. Students recall and debriefing would likely have benefited from being able to immediately download and review data traces from their flights.

5.0 Recommendations

The recommendations found as they relate to designing and implementing flying classroom activities in general to stem from this study are:

  • Well-implemented flying classroom exercises can be highly effective. However, as in other branches of aviation, well-crafted initial ‘air experience flights’ permit familiarisation with the airborne environment and improve students’ capacity to learn effectively.

  • Airborne effectiveness is contingent on good ground-based pedagogy. Flying classroom activity will provide best learning benefits where the remembering and reflection process is strengthened by encouraged by debriefing students as soon as practicable.

  • Affect generated by flight activities may be best exploited by using it to encourage students to engage with the necessary, but less interesting academic work, understanding it is a short-lived effect.

  • Repeating this research once operations and teaching using the Saab-340B has matured, to directly compare and contrast the changes made will offer further strength to flying classroom literature.

Specifically, hypothesised points for optimising teaching and learning on the Saab-340B or other similar airborne teaching environments:

  • The ability to fly more students allows for greater operational flexibility in larger courses (legal limits notwithstanding). The effect it will have on good pedagogy should be noticeable.

  • Prioritise student situational awareness throughout the course; upgraded instrumentation should assist with this.

  • Maximise the ability of any new instrumentation or avionics suites to enable students to use more examples of real-world data they will, or have, personally experienced as an aid to theoretical knowledge instruction, briefing and debriefing.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the staff of NFLC and their students for their support in this work, particularly Professor Nicholas Lawson, now at the University of Sydney.

Footnotes

1 Part-SPO was created in 2014 by the European Union Aviation Safety Agency (EASA), responsible for regulating UK aviation while part of the EU. Responsibility transitioned back to the Civil Aviation Authority (CAA) in January 2021, but retained Part-SPO, along with a number of other EU regulations, to maintain broad alignment with the bloc.

2 See supplementary material at 10.17862/cranfield.rd.11559018

3 The three authors of this paper all had this learning experience as students, all entered aeronautics, and one specifically as a flight test specialist, so are ourselves a biased sample.

References

Royal Aeronautical Society. Accreditation Handbook [Online], 2018. Verfügbar unter: http://aerosociety.com/Assets/Docs/1_ACC_Handbook_V_16_1-8_April_2016.pdf.Google Scholar
Kirschner, P.A. and Meester, M.A.M. The laboratory in higher science education: Problems, premises and objectives, High Edu. (Dordr.), 1988, 17, (1), pp 8198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaptan, K. and Timurlenk, O. Challenges for science education, Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. [Online], 2012, 51, pp 763771. Verfügbar unter: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1877042812033757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrahams, I. and Millar, R. Does Practical Work Really Work? A study of the effectiveness of practical work as a teaching and learning method in school science, Int. J. Sci. Edu. [Online], 2008, 30, (14), pp 19451969. Verfügbar unter: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09500690701749305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Federal Aviation Administration. Aviation Instructor’s Handbook, 2023. https://www.faa.gov/regulations_policies/ handbooks_manuals/aviation/aviation_instructors_handbook.Google Scholar
Harrison, M. Making practical work work: using discussion to enhance pupils’ understanding of physics, Res. Sci. Technol. Edu. [Online], 2016, 34, (3), pp 290306. Verfügbar unter: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02635143.2016.1173668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, D.A. Experiential Learning: Experience as a Source of Learning and Development, 2nd Aufl. Pearson Education. 2015, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Brown, R. and Kulik, J. Flashbulb memories, Cognition [Online], 1977, 5, (1), 7399. Verfügbar unter: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/001002777790018X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, O., Potts, J. and Gautrey, J. Flight testing in aircraft design teaching: implementation and impact on student experience, in: Inspire to Succeed: Transforming Teaching and Learning in STEM [Online]. Nottingham, 2016. Verfügbar unter: https://www.heacademy.ac.uk/download/session-124-dr-oliver-lewis-dr-jonathon-potts-and-dr-jim-gautrey.Google Scholar
Stickland, M.T. and Scanlon, T.J. A novel method for the provision of flight experience and flight testing for undergraduate aeronautical engineers at the University of Strathclyde, Aeronaut. J. [Online], 2004, 108, (1084), pp 315318. Verfügbar unter: https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0001924000000178/type/journal_article.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mccay-peet, L., Toms, E.G. and Kelloway, E.K. Examination of relationships among serendipity, the environment, and individual differences, Inf. Process. Manag. [Online], 2015, 51, (4), 391412. Verfügbar unter: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2015.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, J., Abrahams, I. and Song, J. Unintended knowledge learnt in primary science practical lessons, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 2016, 38, (16), pp 25282549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackman, L.E., Moellenbem, W.P. and Brabson, G.D. Evaluation of three instructional methods for teaching general chemistry, J. Chem. Educ., 1987, 64, (9), 794796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, R., Prieto, T. and Dillon, J.S. The effect of practical work on students’ understanding of combustion, J. Res. Sci. Teach. [Online], 1995, 32, (5), 487502. Verfügbar unter: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/tea.3660320506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstein, A. and Lunetta, V.N. The role of the laboratory in science teaching: neglected aspects of research, Rev. Educ. Res. [Online], 1982, 52, (2), 201. Verfügbar unter: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0034-6543%28198222%2952%3A2%3C201%3ATROTLI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-F&origin=crossref.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychol. Rev., 1955, 101, (2), pp 343352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haji, F.A., Cheung, J.J.H., Woods, N., Regehr, G., de Ribaupierre, S. and Dubrowski, A. Thrive or overload? The effect of task complexity on novices’ simulation-based learning, Med. Educ., 2016, 50, (9), pp 955968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design, Learn Instr., 1994, 4, (4), 295312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandler, P. and Sweller, J. The split attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction, Br. J. Educ. Psychol., 1992, 62, (2), 233246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EHEST. Helicopter Flight Instructor Guide [Online]. EASA, 2018. Verfügbar unter: https://www.easa.europa.eu/ document-library/general-publications/ehest-helicopter-flight-instructor-guide.Google Scholar
Salas, E., Bowers, C.A. and Rhodenizer, L. It is not how much you have but how you use it: toward a rational use of simulation to support aviation training, Int. J. Aviat. Psychol., 1998, 8, (3), pp 197208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slingerland, R., Melkert, J. and Paassen, D.M. Flight testing as an enhancement of understanding aerodynamics and flight mechanics, 43rd AIAA Aerospace Sciences Meeting and Exhibit, 2005. Reno, S. 1–8.10.2514/6.2005-1074CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millar, R. Rhetoric and reality: what practical work in science education is really for, in Wellington, J. Herausgeber (Ed), Practical Work in School Science: Which Way Now, Routledge, 1998, London.Google Scholar
Abrahams, I. An unrealistic image of science, Sch. Sci. Rev. [Online], 2007, 88, pp 119122. Verfügbar unter: http://eprints.ioe.ac.uk/259/.Google Scholar
Trainelli, L., Rolando, A., Bonaita, G. and Chimetto, P. Experiences in academic flight testing education, Aircraft Eng. Aerospace Technol. [Online], 2013, 86, (1), 5666. Verfügbar unter: http://www.emeraldinsight.com/ doi/10.1108/AEAT-10-2012-0178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orio, J.J.F., Blanco, R.G. and Aragon, J.B.T. Low cost flight test instrumentation for education purposes, 44th International Symposium of the Society of Flight Test Engineers, 2013. Texas, S. 1–14.Google Scholar
Padfield, G.D. Flight handling qualities, Aeronaut. J., 2006, 110, (1104), pp 7384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muratore, J., Moonan, W. and Young, J. From kneeboards to mobile computers – achieving higher quality flight test engineering education at lower cost, 41st Society of Flight Test Engineers Annual Symposium, 2010. Washington D.C., S. 1–15.Google Scholar
Bromfield, M.A. and Belberov, A. The flying classroom – a cost effective integrated approach to learning and teaching flight dynamics, Eur. J. Eng. Educ. [Online], 2017, 42, (6), 11741195. Verfügbar unter: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/03043797.2016.1270903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murre, J.M.J. and Dros, J. Replication and analysis of Ebbinghaus’ forgetting curve, PLoS One, 2015, 10, (7), pp 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D.L. The seven sins of memory: insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience, American Psychologist [Online], 1999, 54, (3), pp 182203. Verfügbar unter: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G.B. Initial Airworthiness: Determining the Acceptability of New Airborne Systems, 2nd Aufl. Initial Airworthiness (2nd ed), Springer, 2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H.L. and Karpicke, J.D. Test-enhanced learning: taking memory tests improves long-term retention, Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, (3), 249255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braund, M. and Reiss, M. Towards a more authentic science curriculum: the contribution of out-of-school learning, Int. J. Sci. Educ. [Online], 2006, 28, (12), 13731388. Verfügbar unter: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500690500498419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taskinen, P.H., Schütte, K. and Prenzel, M. Adolescents’ motivation to select an academic science-related career: the role of school factors, individual interest, and science self-concept, Educ. Res. Eval., 2013, 19, (8), pp 717733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, R.M., Wagner, A. and Killion, B. Gender and choosing a STEM major in college: femininity, masculinity, chilly climate, and occupational values, J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2017, 54, (3), pp 299323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, J. Research on students and museums: looking more closely at the students in school groups, Sci. Educ. 2004, 88, (S1), S59S70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M., Abraham, C. and Bond, R. Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis, Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, (2), pp 353387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knogler, M., Harackiewicz, J.M., Gegenfurtner, A. and Lewalter, D. How situational is situational interest? Investigating the longitudinal structure of situational interest, Contemp. Educ. Psychol. [Online], 2015, 43, pp 3950. Verfügbar unter: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.08.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kearney, C. Efforts to Increase Students’ Interest in Pursuing Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics Studies and Careers. National Measures taken by 30 Countries – 2015 Report [Online]. Brussels: European Schoolnet; 2016. 96 S. Verfügbar unter: http://www.voced.edu.au/content/ngv51728.Google Scholar
Cook, D.A. and Artino, A.R. Motivation to learn: an overview of contemporary theories, Med. Educ. [Online], 2016, 50, (10), pp 9971014. Verfügbar unter: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/medu.13074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hidi, S. and Harackiewicz, J.M. Motivating the academically unmotivated: a critical issue for the 21st century, Rev. Educ. Res. [Online], 2000, 70, (2), pp 151179. Verfügbar unter: http://rer.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.3102/00346543070002151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrahams, I. Does practical work really motivate? A study of the affective value of practical work in secondary school science, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 2009, 31, (17), pp 23352353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, L., Manion, L. and Morrison, K. Research Methods in Education, 6th Aufl. The Handbook of Psychology – Methods 2 Specific Methods, vol. 2, Routledge, 2007, Abingdon. 274–315 S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Shea, D., O’ Connell, B.H. and Gallagher, S. Randomised controlled trials in WOHP interventions: a review and guidelines for use, Appl. Psychol. [Online], 2016, 65, (2), pp 190222. Verfügbar unter: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/apps.12053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracht, G.H. and Glass, G.V. The external validity of experiments, Am. Educ. Res. J. [Online], 1968, 5, (4), pp 437474. Verfügbar unter: http://aer.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.3102/00028312005004437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, S.G. and Staveland, L.E. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): results of empirical and theoretical research, Adv. Psychol., 1988, 52, (C), pp 139183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, S.G. NASA-task load index (NASA-TLX); 20 years later, Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. [Online], 2006, 904908. Verfügbar unter: https://humansystems.arc.nasa.gov/groups/TLX/downloads/HFES_2006_Paper.pdf.Google Scholar
Onwuegbuzie, A.J. and Collins, K.M.T. A typology of mixed methods sampling designs in social science research, Qual. Rep. [Online], 2007, 12, (2), pp 281316. Verfügbar unter: http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR12-2/onwuegbuzie2.pdf.Google Scholar
Wilcoxon, F. Individual comparisons by ranking methods, Biom. Bull. [Online], 1945, 1. Verfügbar unter: http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. NFLC Jetstream 3102 as used in this research.

Figure 1

Table 1. Key flying classroom research: conclusions

Figure 2

Table 2. Sample comments from students post-flight, referencing major flight phases

Figure 3

Figure 2. (Top) Example data trace of aircraft speed vs altitude during phugoid mode. Reproduced with permission from Ref. [32].

Figure 4

Figure 3. The motion the pilots (top) and students (bottom) see the aircraft trace during Dutch roll mode. Reproduced with permission from Ref. [32].

Figure 5

Figure 4. Comparison of participant (n = 48) mean raw task load index (NASA-TLX) scores (0–20) by flight. ± Standard error shown.

Figure 6

Table 3. Comparison of average test scores for the intervention group across rubric dimensions for aircraft handling and aircraft dynamic mode questions

Figure 7

Figure 5. Student’s (n = 48) self-report of four dimensions of affect, interest, motivation, enjoyment and self-efficacy. ± Standard error shown.