Introduction
Impulsivity, broadly defined as the tendency to act on urges or desires without forethought or consideration of potential consequences, is a heritable trait associated with numerous psychiatric disorders, including substance use disorders (SUDs) (Amlung, Vedelago, Acker, Balodis, & MacKillop, Reference Amlung, Vedelago, Acker, Balodis and MacKillop2017; Berg, Latzman, Bliwise, & Lilienfeld, Reference Berg, Latzman, Bliwise and Lilienfeld2015; Ersche, Turton, Pradhan, Bullmore, & Robbins, Reference Ersche, Turton, Pradhan, Bullmore and Robbins2010; Kozak et al., Reference Kozak, Lucatch, Lowe, Balodis, MacKillop and George2019; Lees et al., Reference Lees, Garcia, Debenham, Kirkland, Bryant, Mewton and Squeglia2021; MacKillop et al., Reference MacKillop, Amlung, Few, Ray, Sweet and Munafò2011; Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2019; Vassileva & Conrod, Reference Vassileva and Conrod2019). As the concept of impulsivity has evolved, researchers have recognized that it represents a family of cognitive and behavioral tendencies influenced by partially distinct neurobiological substrates (Levitt, Sanchez-Roige, Palmer, & MacKillop, Reference Levitt, Sanchez-Roige, Palmer and MacKillop2020; Meda et al., Reference Meda, Stevens, Potenza, Pittman, Gueorguieva, Andrews and … Pearlson2009; Miller & Gizer, Reference Miller and Gizer2023; Vassileva & Conrod, Reference Vassileva and Conrod2019), as opposed to a single unitary construct (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Smith, Olausson, Mitchell, Leeman, O’Malley and Sher2010; Evenden, Reference Evenden1999; Griffin, Lynam, & Samuel, Reference Griffin, Lynam and Samuel2018). These different forms can manifest, for instance, through the motor (acting without thinking), non-planning (lack of forethought), choice (preference for immediate rewards), risk-taking (engaging in potentially harmful activities), or attentional (difficulty focusing) impulsivity. To thoroughly assess this multi-dimensionality, several psychological instruments have been developed, including self-reported questionnaires such as the Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P) (Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey, & Karyadi, Reference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi2014; Whiteside, Lynam, Miller, & Reynolds, Reference Whiteside, Lynam, Miller and Reynolds2005), the Barratt Impulsiveness scale (BIS) (Barratt, Reference Barratt1994), and the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ) (Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999).
Impulsivity is a well-established risk factor for SUDs and is known to influence several stages of vulnerability, including initial substance use, regular use without dependence (‘normative use’; Kearns, Gunn, Stevens, Berey, & Metrik, Reference Kearns, Gunn, Stevens, Berey and Metrik2022; Shin, Hong, & Jeon, Reference Shin, Hong and Jeon2012; Wasserman, Mathias, Hill-Kapturczak, Karns-Wright, & Dougherty, Reference Wasserman, Mathias, Hill-Kapturczak, Karns-Wright and Dougherty2020; Wasserman et al., Reference Wasserman, Shaw-Meadow, Moon, Karns-Wright, Mathias, Hill-Kapturczak and Dougherty2021 ), as well as the progression towards compulsive and problematic use (Lee, Hoppenbrouwers, & Franken, Reference Lee, Hoppenbrouwers and Franken2019; Petker, Ferro, Van Ameringen, Murphy, & MacKillop, Reference Petker, Ferro, Van Ameringen, Murphy and MacKillop2021; Poulton & Hester, Reference Poulton and Hester2020; Verdejo-Garcia & Albein-Urios, Reference Verdejo-Garcia and Albein-Urios2021), and treatment outcomes (Athamneh et al., Reference Athamneh, DeHart, Pope, Mellis, Snider, Kaplan and Bickel2019, Reference Athamneh, Freitas Lemos, Basso, Tomlinson, Craft, Stein and Bickel2022; Heinz, Bui, Thomas, & Blonigen, Reference Heinz, Bui, Thomas and Blonigen2015; E. E. Levitt et al., Reference Levitt, Sousa, Costello, LaBelle, Rush and MacKillop2023; Loree, Lundahl, & Ledgerwood, Reference Loree, Lundahl and Ledgerwood2015). In addition, these epidemiological studies have suggested that specific impulsivity facets have unique relationships with these different stages of substance use vulnerability. For instance, sensation seeking is often associated with substance use initiation, whereas other facets, such as negative urgency, lack of premeditation, and delay discounting, are associated with substance use-related problems or poorer treatment outcomes (Hershberger, Um, & Cyders, Reference Hershberger, Um and Cyders2017; Hildebrandt, Dieterich, & Endrass, Reference Hildebrandt, Dieterich and Endrass2021; Kearns et al., Reference Kearns, Gunn, Stevens, Berey and Metrik2022; Kräplin et al., Reference Kräplin, Höfler, Pooseh, Wolff, Krönke, Goschke and … Smolka2020; MacKillop et al., Reference MacKillop, Amlung, Few, Ray, Sweet and Munafò2011; Petker et al., Reference Petker, Ferro, Van Ameringen, Murphy and MacKillop2021; Stamates & Lau-Barraco, Reference Stamates and Lau-Barraco2017; Stautz & Cooper, Reference Stautz and Cooper2013; Tran, Teese, & Gill, Reference Tran, Teese and Gill2018). A deeper knowledge of these differences could lead to more effective strategies for treating and preventing SUDs by targeting specific dimensions of impulsivity. However, these prior phenotypic studies often face methodological challenges, such as small sample sizes, variability in instrument measurement, and difficulty controlling for potential environmental biases, which can limit the generalizability and interpretability of the findings (Friedman & Gustavson, Reference Friedman and Gustavson2022; Sanchez-Roige & Palmer, Reference Sanchez-Roige and Palmer2020).
Genetic studies can partially overcome some of the limitations of phenotypic studies by examining the relationship between impulsivity and substance use vulnerability using independent cohorts, which partially controls for potential environmental confounds (Miller & Gizer, Reference Miller and Gizer2024; Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Fontanillas, Elson, Gray, de Wit, MacKillop and Palmer2019; Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023). For more than two decades, family and twin studies have shown that the relationships between impulsivity and substance use behaviors are largely due to an underlying shared genetic liability (Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, Reference Kendler, Prescott, Myers and Neale2003; Slutske et al., Reference Slutske, Heath, Madden, Bucholz, Statham and Martin2002; Tarter, Kirisci, Habeych, Reynolds, & Vanyukov, Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Habeych, Reynolds and Vanyukov2004). Recent multivariate techniques, such as genomic structural equation modeling (genomic SEM), can combine data from multiple correlated phenotypes derived from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) and model the latent genetic factor structure of impulsivity and substance use behaviors (Grotzinger et al., Reference Grotzinger, Rhemtulla, de Vlaming, Ritchie, Mallard, Hill and … Tucker-Drob2019). Using GWAS of several impulsivity traits (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023; Sanchez-Roige, Fontanillas, et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Fontanillas, Elson, Gray, de Wit, MacKillop and Palmer2019), in a previous study we applied genomic SEM to model the genetic architecture of impulsivity facets and corroborated that impulsivity is multidimensional (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020, Reference Gustavson, Morrison, Mallard, Jennings, Fontanillas, Elson and …Sanchez-Roige2024; Mallard et al., Reference Mallard, Tubbs, Jennings, Zhang, Gustavson, Grotzinger and … Sanchez-Roige2023). While most impulsivity facets were positively genetically correlated with one another, others, such as sensation seeking and delay discounting, were divergent (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020, Reference Gustavson, Morrison, Mallard, Jennings, Fontanillas, Elson and …Sanchez-Roige2024; Mallard et al., Reference Mallard, Tubbs, Jennings, Zhang, Gustavson, Grotzinger and … Sanchez-Roige2023). We also used genomic SEM to model the latent structure of SUDs, and showed that the genetic architecture of normative use and SUDs are distinct (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Johnson, Colbert, Polimanti, Zhou, Walters and … Agrawal2022, Reference Hatoum, Colbert, Johnson, Huggett, Deak, Pathak and Agrawal2023; Mallard et al., Reference Mallard, Savage, Johnson, Huang, Edwards, Hottenga and Sanchez-Roige2022). Furthermore, a recent genomic SEM study identified a stronger genetic overlap between sensation seeking and alcohol consumption compared to alcohol use disorder (Miller & Gizer, Reference Miller and Gizer2024). However, the extent to which other impulsivity facets are differentially genetically related to aspects of substance use involvement has not been systematically explored.
Here we build upon these genomic SEM findings to model the genetic architecture of impulsivity facets and substance use behaviors. We leveraged access to well-powered GWAS summary statistics (N = 79,729–903,147) to examine the latent genetic relationship between nine impulsivity facets, captured by five single-item variables and one latent factor based on our prior work - these included negative and positive urgency (inability to resist temptations while experiencing positive or negative affect), lack of premeditation (tendency to act without planning or self-control), lack of perseverance (inability to persist on difficult tasks), sensation seeking (tendency to enjoy risky situations) and delay discounting (preference for smaller immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards)- and seven substance use-related measures, captured by two factors based on our prior work (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Johnson, Colbert, Polimanti, Zhou, Walters and … Agrawal2022, Reference Hatoum, Colbert, Johnson, Huggett, Deak, Pathak and Agrawal2023), - these included normative substance use (SU; based on lifetime and quantity/frequency use) and SUD. Based on prior findings, we hypothesized that specific impulsivity facets would be differentially associated with SU and SUD factors, providing a more comprehensive understanding of the etiology of substance use vulnerability.
Methods
GWAS data
Overview. We used GWAS data derived from individuals of European ancestry, predicted based on genetic similarity (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering & Medicine, 2023). Our study was limited to this group because similar impulsivity GWAS data are unavailable for other groups. Table 1 provides an overview of the GWAS datasets used.
Table 1. Summary of GWAS datasets for impulsivity, substance use, and SUDs

Abbreviations: UPPS-P, Impulsive Behavior Scale; BIS, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; MCQ, Monetary Choice Questionnaire.
Impulsivity. We used summary statistics from our latest impulsivity GWAS (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023). These included five measures from the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale (Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey, & Karyadi, Reference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi2014; Whiteside, Lynam, Miller, & Reynolds, Reference Whiteside, Lynam, Miller and Reynolds2005) and three from the BIS scales (Barratt, Reference Barratt1994) in a cohort comprising up to 133,517 consented research participants from 23andMe, Inc. These data sets have been extensively described elsewhere (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023).
Delay discounting. We used summary statistics from our latest GWAS of delay discounting (Thorpe et al., Reference Thorpe, Fontanillas, Pham, Meredith, Jennings, Courchesne-Krak and … Sanchez-Roige2024), which included 134,935 consented research participants from 23andMe, Inc. Participants completed the 27-item MCQ (Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999). More information about this dataset can be found in Thorpe et al. (Reference Thorpe, Cupertino, Pakala, Fontanillas, Jennings, Yang and Sanchez-Roige2024).
Substance use. We used summary statistics from three normative substance use GWAS: drinks per week (N = 666,978; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Wang, Chen, Jang, Liu, Wang and Vrieze2022), smoking initiation (N = 805,431; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Wang, Chen, Jang, Liu, Wang and Vrieze2022), and lifetime cannabis use (N = 184,765; Pasman et al., Reference Pasman, Verweij, Gerring, Stringer, Sanchez-Roige, Treur and Vink2018).
Substance use disorders. We used summary statistics from four SUDs GWAS: problematic alcohol use (PAU) (N = 903,147; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Kember, Deak, Xu, Toikumo, Yuan and Gelernter2023), tobacco use disorder (TUD) (N = 495,471; Toikumo et al., Reference Toikumo, Jennings, Pham, Lee, Mallard, Bianchi and Sanchez-Roige2024), cannabis use disorder (CUD) (N = 886,025; Levey et al., Reference Levey, Galimberti, Deak, Wendt, Bhattacharya, Koller and Gelernter2023) and opioid use disorder (OUD) (N = 302,585; Kember et al., Reference Kember, Vickers-Smith, Xu, Toikumo, Niarchou, Zhou and Kranzler2022).
Data analysis
All analyses were conducted in R (Version 4.1.1; R Core Team, 2022). We used the genomicSEM package (Version 0.0.4; Grotzinger et al., Reference Grotzinger, Rhemtulla, de Vlaming, Ritchie, Mallard, Hill and … Tucker-Drob2019), which applies SEM methods to GWAS summary statistics. Genomic SEM leverages linkage disequilibrium score regression (LDSC) (Bulik-Sullivan et al., Reference Bulik-Sullivan, Loh, Finucane, Ripke, Yang and … Neale2015) to generate a genetic correlation matrix between traits from GWAS summary statistics. Genomic SEM adjusts for potential sample overlap by estimating a sampling covariance matrix that indexes the precision of the estimates as well as the extent to which the sampling dependencies of the estimates are associated (Grotzinger et al., Reference Grotzinger, Rhemtulla, de Vlaming, Ritchie, Mallard, Hill and … Tucker-Drob2019).
Structural equation models are fit to the data using genomic SEM, which draws on functionality from the lavaan R package (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). We used standard European reference panels and parameter settings, and the diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) estimation method. Model fit was determined based on fit indexes less sensitive to sample size, including the comparative fit index (CFI) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Good-fitting models are expected to have CFI higher than .95 (.90 for acceptable fit), SRMR smaller than .08, and smaller AIC values than competing nested models (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1998). In addition, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was calculated manually according to Shi, Lee, and Maydeu-Olivares (Reference Shi, Lee and Maydeu-Olivares2019) using the sample size of the smallest GWAS included in our study (N = 133,517), with RMSEA values lower than 0.05 indicating a good fit. The RMSEA of the null model was 0.048 [0.044, 0.052]. Because RMSEA is sensitive to large sample sizes, such as those used in this study, it is likely to approach zero even with large χ2 values (Chen, Curran, Bollen, Kirby, & Paxton, Reference Chen, Curran, Bollen, Kirby and Paxton2008). Therefore, we focused on other statistics (CFI and SRMR) to evaluate model fit (Grotzinger et al., Reference Grotzinger, Rhemtulla, de Vlaming, Ritchie, Mallard, Hill and … Tucker-Drob2019). The significance of individual parameter estimates was established with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) and FDR corrected p-value.
Model-fitting approach
First, we separately fitted confirmatory factor models of impulsivity and substance use-related measures. For impulsivity, we fitted a confirmatory model using GWAS data from the UPPS-P (five traits) and the BIS (three traits) subscales and delay discounting (Table 1, Supplementary Figure 1a), based on our prior work (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020, Reference Gustavson, Morrison, Mallard, Jennings, Fontanillas, Elson and …Sanchez-Roige2024) and informed by psychology and psychometric theories of the UPPS-P model (Carver & Johnson, Reference Carver and Johnson2018; Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey, & Karyadi, Reference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi2014; Whiteside, Lynam, Miller, & Reynolds, Reference Whiteside, Lynam, Miller and Reynolds2005). This model included the three BIS subscales and the UPPS-P lack of premeditation subscale as indices of a lack of premeditation factor (Figure 2, Supplementary Figure 1a). We also fitted a confirmatory two-factor model comprising the SU and SUD factors, using GWAS data from seven substance use-related measures, based on prior work by Hatoum et al. (Reference Hatoum, Johnson, Colbert, Polimanti, Zhou, Walters and … Agrawal2022) and Karlsson Linnér et al. (Reference Karlsson Linnér, Mallard, Barr, Sanchez-Roige, Madole, Driver and Dick2021) (Table 1, Supplementary Figure 1b). Notably, our SU factor is composed of indices of lifetime use as well as quantity/frequency of use. Given the lack of GWAS for lifetime alcohol use, we used another well-established GWAS of normative alcohol use based on quantity/frequency measures, drinks per week. Given the high genetic correlation between drinks per week and smoking/cannabis lifetime use measures (Figure 1), we proceeded to fit these measures into a single latent factor. Additional measures of normative substance use, such as cigarettes per day, were excluded due to low correlation with other substance use measures (Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Wang, Chen, Jang, Liu, Wang and Vrieze2022), consistent with prior work (Brick et al., Reference Brick, Benca-Bachman, Johnson, Gustavson, Carper and Palmer2023; Horwitz, Zorina-Lichtenwalter, Gustavson, Grotzinger, & Stallings, Reference Horwitz, Zorina-Lichtenwalter, Gustavson, Grotzinger and Stallings2024; Linnér et al., Reference Linnér, Mallard, Barr, Sanchez-Roige, Madole, Driver and Dick2021). Our SUD factor replicated the addiction-risk-factor model by Hatoum et al. (Reference Hatoum, Johnson, Colbert, Polimanti, Zhou, Walters and … Agrawal2022, Reference Hatoum, Colbert, Johnson, Huggett, Deak, Pathak and Agrawal2023), replacing problematic tobacco use for TUD (Toikumo et al., Reference Toikumo, Jennings, Pham, Lee, Mallard, Bianchi and Sanchez-Roige2024), which was unavailable at the time of their analyses. Finally, we constructed a final model in which all factors (impulsivity facets, SU factor and SUD factor) were fitted simultaneously. We fitted two versions of this model: (a) a correlated factors model where we estimated the genetic correlations between all impulsivity, SU, and SUD factors, and (b) a multiple regression model, where impulsivity was regressed on SU and SUD factors, with the latent factors treated as correlated outcomes. Within the correlated factor model, to test whether impulsivity facets were differentially correlated with the SU and SUD factors, we calculated χ2 difference tests comparing a model in which the covariances of the SU and SUD factors to the impulsivity facet were constrained to be equal. The χ2 and degrees of freedom differences across these two models were used to compute the p-value. The multiple regression model was used to quantify whether genetic associations between impulsivity and SU were independent of genetic influences on SUD (and vice-versa). To account for multiple tests, we applied an FDR correction to all p-values.

Figure 1. Genetic correlation (rg) matrix between all study variables. The size and color of the circle indicates the strength of the correlation. Non-significant genetic correlations are left blank. rg and 95% confidence intervals can be found in Supplementary Table 1. UPPS-P = Impulsive Behavior Scale; BIS = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale.

Figure 2. Path diagram for the final model of impulsivity, substance use (SU), and substance use disorder (SUD) traits. Observed indicators are represented by squares and latent factors are represented by circles. Single-headed arrows indicate factor loading, and double-headed arrows indicate correlations. Orange arrows represent genetic correlations significantly different between impulsivity and SU versus SUD, and green arrows represent genetic correlations of similar magnitude between impulsivity and SU or SUD. All values indicate standardized parameter estimates. 95% confidence intervals and p-values can be found in Supplementary Table 2.
Results
Figure 1 shows the genetic correlation matrix (rg) among all study variables. Genetic correlations between impulsivity facets and substance use involvement traits were broadly significant and positive, ranging from rg = 0.11 to rg = 0.80, with a few exceptions (Supplementary Table 1). Delay discounting was negatively genetically correlated with sensation seeking, lack of perseverance, and lifetime cannabis use (rg range = −0.1 to −0.16).
The impulsivity confirmatory model, which was based on prior work from (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020), showed an acceptable fit (χ2(17) = 441.49, CFI = 0.938, SRMR = 0.074, RMSEA = 0.014 [0.012, 0.015]) (Supplementary Figure 1a). This model included five single-item variables (i.e. negative urgency, positive urgency, lack of perseverance, sensation seeking, and delay discounting), and one latent factor comprising the three BIS subscales and lack of premeditation. The two-factor substance use model, which captured the shared genetic variation of drinks per week, smoking initiation, and lifetime cannabis use into a SU factor, and the shared genetic variation of PAU, TUD, CUD and OUD into a SUD factor, also showed an acceptable fit (χ2(10) = 135.19, CFI = 0.978, SRMR = 0.058, RMSEA = 0.008 [0.007, 0.009]). As expected, the SU and SUD factors exhibited a strong positive genetic correlation, indicating significant shared genetic influences (rG = 0.77, 95% CI = [0.73, 0.81]). Because we expected that measures related to the same substance (e.g. drinks per week and PAU) would be especially correlated with each other, we also included residual correlations between these measures in our model. The residual correlation between drinks per week and PAU was moderate (rG = 0.42, 95% CI = [0.34, 0.49]), while the residual correlations between smoking initiation and TUD, and lifetime cannabis use and CUD, were relatively low (rG = 0.30, 95% CI = [0.24, 0.35] and 0.10, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.18], respectively).
Our final model, displayed in Figure 2, combined the impulsivity, SU and SUD factors (χ2(78) = 2884.55, CFI = 0.924, SRMR = 0.074, RMSEA = 0.016 [0.015, 0.018]). The SU and SUD factors were most strongly genetically correlated with lack of premeditation (SU rG = 0.50, 95% CI = [0.42, 0.58]; SUD rG = 0.46, 95% CI = [0.4, 0.52]), and least strongly genetically correlated with lack of perseverance (SU rG = 0.10, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.17]; SUD rG = 0.05, 95% CI = [−0.01, 0.11], p = n.s.). Sensation seeking was significantly more strongly genetically correlated with the SU factor than with the SUD factor (rG = 0.27, 95% CI = [0.2, 0.35] versus rG = 0.10, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.16], respectively, Δχ2 (1) = 123, p = 1.4e-28). On the contrary, delay discounting was significantly more strongly genetically correlated with the SUD factor than with the SU factor (rG = 0.31, 95% CI = [0.26, 0.37] versus rG = 0.21, 95% CI = [0.15, 0.27], respectively, Δχ2 (1) = 28.9, p = 7.63e-08). For all other impulsivity facets, genetic correlations with the SU and SUD factors were not significantly different from one another.
Given the strong genetic correlation between the SU and SUD factors, we performed a specificity analysis via a multiple regression model to test whether these factors were independently associated with impulsivity facets. This final model had the same fit as the correlated factor model (from Figure 2), and the results are described in Figure 3 and Supplementary Table 3. We observed distinct patterns of associations for most of the impulsivity facets with the SU and SUD factors. Controlling for SUD, the SU factor was positively genetically associated with positive urgency (beta (β) = 0.33, 95% CI = [0.15, 0.51], p = 6.21e-04), lack of premeditation (β = 0.42, 95% CI = [0.2, 0.65], p = 3.58e-04), and sensation seeking (β = 0.48, 95% CI = [0.31, 0.65], p = 4.30e-08). Controlling for SU, the SUD factor was positively genetically associated with negative urgency (β = 0.33, 95% CI = [0.19, 0.46], p = 4.74e-06), lack of premeditation (β = 0.21, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.4], p = 3.15e-02) and delay discounting (β = 0.36, 95% CI = [0.24, 0.48], p = 8.51e-09), and negatively genetically associated with sensation seeking (β = −0.27, 95% CI = [−0.42, −0.12], p = 7.35e-04).

Figure 3. Standardized genetic associations between SU and SUD (controlling for one another), and the impulsivity facets. Estimates, 95% confidence intervals, and p-values for these regressions can be found in Supplementary Table 3. Significant associations are indicated with a filled-in circle.
Discussion
A deeper understanding of the relationship between impulsivity facets and stages of substance use vulnerability can facilitate the development of better prevention, diagnosis, and treatment strategies for SUDs. In this study, we used GWAS data and genomic SEM techniques to model the genetic architecture of nine impulsivity facets and seven measures related to substance use and SUDs. Virtually all facets of impulsivity were genetically correlated with both the SU and SUD factors, but the magnitude of these correlations varied across different impulsivity facets. Our findings provide evidence that specific impulsivity facets confer risk for distinct stages of substance use involvement and emphasize the need to consider the multi-dimensional nature of impulsivity in SUD-related research.
As expected, the SU and SUD factors were strongly genetically correlated but not at unity, consistent with our prior work (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Johnson, Colbert, Polimanti, Zhou, Walters and … Agrawal2022, Reference Hatoum, Colbert, Johnson, Huggett, Deak, Pathak and Agrawal2023). This supports the idea that while normative use and SUDs are related, they have different genetic architectures, which may also contribute to differences in their relationships with other traits or behaviors (Dick, Meyers, Rose, Kaprio, & Kendler, Reference Dick, Meyers, Rose, Kaprio and Kendler2011; Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Zhou, Kember, Vickers Smith, Justice, Damrauer and … Gelernter2019; Levey et al., Reference Levey, Galimberti, Deak, Wendt, Bhattacharya, Koller and Gelernter2023; Mallard et al., Reference Mallard, Savage, Johnson, Huang, Edwards, Hottenga and Sanchez-Roige2022; Mallard & Sanchez-Roige, Reference Mallard and Sanchez-Roige2021; Polimanti et al., Reference Polimanti, Walters, Johnson, McClintick, Adkins, Adkins and Gelernter2020; Sanchez-Roige, Kember, & Agrawal, Reference Sanchez-Roige, Kember and Agrawal2022; Sanchez-Roige, Palmer, et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Palmer, Fontanillas, Elson, Adams and … Clarke2019). For example, some impulsivity facets were most strongly linked to aspects of normative use. In particular, sensation seeking showed a stronger genetic correlation with the SU than the SUD factor. Delay discounting, however, showed a stronger genetic correlation with the SUD than the SU factor. Meanwhile, negative and positive urgency, lack of premeditation, and lack of perseverance showed similar genetic correlations with both the SU and SUD factors. Most of these results held in our regression model, which accounted for the genetic overlap between the SU and SUD factors. Our findings further highlight impulsivity facets as independent phenotypic and genetic constructs, in some cases only moderately correlated with each other (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Franz, Kremen, Carver, Corley, Hewitt and Friedman2019, Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020; Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023; Thorpe et al., Reference Thorpe, Cupertino, Pakala, Fontanillas, Jennings, Yang and Sanchez-Roige2024), that capture specific biological processes relevant to normative and disordered substance use (Miller & Gizer, Reference Miller and Gizer2024; Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2019; Poulton & Hester, Reference Poulton and Hester2020).
Sensation seeking, defined as the tendency to engage in highly stimulating behaviors, was more strongly genetically correlated with the SU factor compared to the SUD factor, which was further supported by our regression model. Indeed, among all impulsivity facets, sensation seeking exhibited the strongest genetic association with the SU factor. Our results contribute to a substantial body of evidence demonstrating that sensation seeking is primarily associated with aspects of initiation and experimental substance use (Birkley & Smith, Reference Birkley and Smith2011; Fischer, Smith, & Cyders, Reference Fischer, Smith and Cyders2008; Griffin et al., Reference Griffin, Lynam and Samuel2018; Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Dieterich and Endrass2021; Stautz & Cooper, Reference Stautz and Cooper2013). After controlling for SU, we also observed a significant negative genetic association between sensation seeking and the SUD factor, which aligns with previous phenotypic (Courtney et al., Reference Courtney, Arellano, Barkley-Levenson, Gálvan, Poldrack, Mackillop and … Ray2012) and genetic (Miller & Gizer, Reference Miller and Gizer2024) studies and suggests that the influence of sensation seeking on SUDs may primarily manifest through increased normative substance use. It will be important for future studies to examine how the unique variance in sensation seeking might be protective over SUDs. For instance, it may reflect the adaptive and functional aspects of sensation seeking, which may be at times advantageous (Gullo & Dawe, Reference Gullo and Dawe2008). This is supported by prior phenotypic studies reporting positive associations with psychological well-being and mental health status (Ravert & Donnellan, Reference Ravert and Donnellan2021) and negative associations with alcohol problems (Courtney et al., Reference Courtney, Arellano, Barkley-Levenson, Gálvan, Poldrack, Mackillop and … Ray2012).
Lack of perseverance, which reflects the cognitive difficulties in maintaining effort over an extended period, showed a unique but weak genetic correlation with the SU factor, but not with the SUD factor. Meanwhile, in the regression model, lack of perseverance showed no significant association with either factor. These results align with prior phenotypic studies (Fischer & Smith, Reference Fischer and Smith2008; Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Dieterich and Endrass2021; Kearns et al., Reference Kearns, Gunn, Stevens, Berey and Metrik2022; Magid & Colder, Reference Magid and Colder2007; Stamates & Lau-Barraco, Reference Stamates and Lau-Barraco2017) and our prior GWAS (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023; Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Fontanillas, Elson, Gray, de Wit, MacKillop and Palmer2019), suggesting that lack of perseverance may have a distinct genetic architecture and weaker genetic associations with substance use-related outcomes compared to other impulsivity facets (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020, Reference Gustavson, Morrison, Mallard, Jennings, Fontanillas, Elson and …Sanchez-Roige2024).
Conversely, delay discounting showed a significantly stronger genetic correlation with the SUD factor compared to the SU factor, and as observed in the regression model, the strongest genetic association with the SUD factor among all impulsivity facets. This finding aligns with clinical- and population-based phenotypic studies, where devaluation of delayed rewards appears most relevant to problematic substance use over normative use (Courtney et al., Reference Courtney, Arellano, Barkley-Levenson, Gálvan, Poldrack, Mackillop and … Ray2012; Fröhner et al., Reference Fröhner, Ripke, Jurk, Li, Banaschewski and Bokde2022; Kräplin et al., Reference Kräplin, Höfler, Pooseh, Wolff, Krönke, Goschke and … Smolka2020; Murphy & Garavan, Reference Murphy and Garavan2011; Stamates & Lau-Barraco, Reference Stamates and Lau-Barraco2017).
Positive and negative urgency showed equal genetic correlations with the SU and SUD factors, however, this was not consistent in our regression model. While positive urgency was uniquely associated with the SU factor, negative urgency was uniquely associated with the SUD factor. This specificity aligns with theoretical models proposing that positive urgency, driven by enhanced sensitivity to rewards, may promote initial experimentation and escalation in use, whereas negative urgency, reflecting difficulties regulating responses to negative emotions, may specifically contribute to compulsive use via a process of negative reinforcement during withdrawal (Cyders & Smith, Reference Cyders and Smith2008; Koob & Le Moal, Reference Koob and Le Moal2001; Koob & Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2010; Smith & Cyders, Reference Smith and Cyders2016). Prior epidemiological evidence on this specificity is inconsistent. Positive urgency has been previously associated with both normative (Kaiser, Bonsu, Charnigo, Milich, & Lynam, Reference Kaiser, Bonsu, Charnigo, Milich and Lynam2016; Tomko, Prisciandaro, Falls, & Magid, Reference Tomko, Prisciandaro, Falls and Magid2016; Zapolski, Cyders, & Smith, Reference Zapolski, Cyders and Smith2009) and problematic substance use (Coskunpinar, Dir, & Cyders, Reference Coskunpinar, Dir and Cyders2013; Stautz & Cooper, Reference Stautz and Cooper2014; Tran et al., Reference Tran, Teese and Gill2018), whereas negative urgency has been primarily implicated in SUDs severity and related problems (Adams, Kaiser, Lynam, Charnigo, & Milich, Reference Adams, Kaiser, Lynam, Charnigo and Milich2012; Coskunpinar et al., Reference Coskunpinar, Dir and Cyders2013; Fischer & Smith, Reference Fischer and Smith2008; Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Dieterich and Endrass2021; Kearns et al., Reference Kearns, Gunn, Stevens, Berey and Metrik2022; Magid & Colder, Reference Magid and Colder2007; Murphy & Mackillop, Reference Murphy and Mackillop2012; Stamates & Lau-Barraco, Reference Stamates and Lau-Barraco2017; Stautz & Cooper, Reference Stautz and Cooper2013; Tomko et al., Reference Tomko, Prisciandaro, Falls and Magid2016; Verdejo-Garcia & Albein-Urios, Reference Verdejo-Garcia and Albein-Urios2021), supporting our results. Despite the high genetic correlation between positive and negative urgency, our results suggest distinct genetic relationships with stages of substance use vulnerability. This finding reinforces the notion that these impulsivity facets should be considered separate constructs (Billieux et al., Reference Billieux, Heeren, Rochat, Maurage, Bayard, Bet and Baggio2021; Carver & Johnson, Reference Carver and Johnson2018).
Lastly, lack of premeditation, a facet characterized by low executive control and difficulties with prospective thinking and planning, showed the strongest genetic correlation with both the SU and SUD factors. Our regression model suggests that lack of premeditation has independent genetic contributions to both the SU, which aligns with prior phenotypic studies (Fischer & Smith, Reference Fischer and Smith2008; Griffin & Trull, Reference Griffin and Trull2021; McCabe, Louie, & King, Reference McCabe, Louie and King2015; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Hong and Jeon2012; Shin, Chung, & Jeon, Reference Shi, Lee and Maydeu-Olivares2019), and the SUD factor, where prior findings have been mixed (Fischer & Smith, Reference Fischer and Smith2008; Hershberger et al., Reference Hershberger, Um and Cyders2017; Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Dieterich and Endrass2021; Magid & Colder, Reference Magid and Colder2007; McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Louie and King2015; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Hong and Jeon2012).
Our findings should be interpreted in the context of the following limitations. First, GWAS were only conducted in individuals of genetically-inferred European ancestry; while we have no specific reason to believe these findings are specific to Europeans, future analyses should diversify genetic analyses as larger non-European samples become available. Additionally, all impulsivity GWAS were conducted in a cohort of 23andMe participants who were generally older and had higher socioeconomic status than the general population (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023). These factors may introduce biases due to gene-environment correlations (Abdellaoui, Dolan, Verweij, & Nivard, Reference Abdellaoui, Dolan, Verweij and Nivard2022; Thorpe et al., Reference Thorpe, Fontanillas, Pham, Meredith, Jennings, Courchesne-Krak and … Sanchez-Roige2024) and reduce the generalizability of the results. Furthermore, genomic SEM allows modeling of the genetic architecture between the traits studied, but it does not infer causality or directionality. Impulsivity facets can represent both risk factors as well as consequences of substance use (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Bonsu, Charnigo, Milich and Lynam2016; Verdejo-García, Lawrence, & Clark, Reference Verdejo-García, Lawrence and Clark2008), as seen in longitudinal, family-based, and clinical studies where individuals with increased impulsive behaviors have a higher susceptibility to substance use and SUDs (Verdejo-Garcia & Albein-Urios, Reference Verdejo-Garcia and Albein-Urios2021). In addition, chronic substance use can lead to brain alterations that can further exacerbate impulsivity (de Wit, Reference de Wit2009). However, the 23andMe cohort used in the impulsivity GWAS data reported relatively low levels of drug use, suggesting that impulsivity due to recent drug use is unlikely to be a major confounder in our study (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Jennings, Thorpe, Mallari, van der Werf, Bianchi and Palmer2023). Furthermore, the SU and SUD factors exhibited a strong genetic correlation, complicating the interpretation of the regression model due to collinearity issues. Despite this challenge, it remains crucial to distinguish between normative use and SUDs, even if the unique variance is small. This differentiation will inform more effective prevention and intervention strategies aimed at preventing normative use from progressing to a SUD. Additionally, it is noteworthy that some of our results differ from those reported in previous phenotypic studies. However, genetic studies and phenotypic observations can sometimes diverge due to factors like gene-environment interplay, sampling error, and confounding factors not accounted for in the analysis (e.g. socioeconomic status, peer influences, or availability of substances) (Sodini, Kemper, Wray, & Trzaskowski, Reference Sodini, Kemper, Wray and Trzaskowski2018). Finally, further studies are needed to explore to what extent other related traits, such as externalizing behaviors, or cognitive measures, such as educational attainment, mediate some of the associations observed in our study.
In conclusion, our study provides novel insights into the complex genetic relationships between distinct facets of impulsivity and different stages of substance use involvement. Our findings highlight both similarities and differences in the genetic contributions of various impulsivity domains to aspects of normative substance use and SUDs. These results reinforce the importance of studying impulsivity as a multidimensional construct when examining its role in SUDs vulnerability. Future work should further explore the underlying biological mechanisms of individual facets of impulsivity, many of which are amenable to studies in non-human animals. The insights from this study can also be leveraged to inform targeted prevention and intervention efforts tailored to individual impulsivity profiles and substance use risk.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000145.
Data availability
Summary statistics for the SU and SUD GWAS were publicly available. Data from 23andMe are available upon request (see https://research.23andme.com/dataset-access/). The R data files containing the genomic SEM matrices and an R markdown with the analysis scripts and corresponding outputs are available at the following link https://osf.io/ba8dy/?view_only=acacd8c2eae844e88b8b4510a7d59973, which allows for replication and analyses of competing models without obtaining the source data.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the research participants and employees of 23andMe for making this work possible.
The following members of the 23andMe Research Team contributed to this study:
Stella Aslibekyan, Adam Auton, Elizabeth Babalola, Robert K. Bell, Jessica Bielenberg, Ninad S. Chaudhary, Zayn Cochinwala, Sayantan Das, Emily DelloRusso, Payam Dibaeinia, Sarah L. Elson, Nicholas Eriksson, Chris Eijsbouts, Teresa Filshtein, Pierre Fontanillas, Davide Foletti, Will Freyman, Zach Fuller, Julie M. Granka, Chris German, Éadaoin Harney, Alejandro Hernandez, Barry Hicks, David A. Hinds, M. Reza Jabalameli, Ethan M. Jewett, Yunxuan Jiang, Sotiris Karagounis, Lucy Kaufmann, Matt Kmiecik, Katelyn Kukar, Alan Kwong, Keng-Han Lin, Yanyu Liang, Bianca A. Llamas, Aly Khan, Steven J. Micheletti, Matthew H. McIntyre, Meghan E. Moreno, Priyanka Nandakumar, Dominique T. Nguyen, Jared O’Connell, Steve Pitts, G. David Poznik, Alexandra Reynoso, Shubham Saini, Morgan Schumacher, Leah Selcer, Anjali J. Shastri, Jingchunzi Shi, Suyash Shringarpure, Keaton Stagaman, Teague Sterling, Qiaojuan Jane Su, Joyce Y. Tung, Susana A. Tat, Vinh Tran, Xin Wang, Wei Wang, Catherine H. Weldon, Amy L. Williams, and Peter Wilton.
Funding statement
L.V.R., S.S.R, A.A.P., and D.E.G. are funded through the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA DP1DA054394, P30DA060810, P50DA037844 and R01DA059804). A.A.P. and S.S.R. are funded through the Tobacco-Related Disease Research Program (T32IR5226 and 28IR-0070). T.T.M. is supported by funds from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; K08MH135343) and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA; AA030083). A.D.G. is supported by NIMH Grant (R01MH120219).
Competing interest
S.E., P.F., and 23andMe Research Team are employed by and hold stock or stock options in 23andMe, Inc. All other authors report no competing interests.