Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T22:31:46.574Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Topic, Focus and null subjects in Old French

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2018

Richard Ingham*
Affiliation:
University of Westminster
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Old French subject pronouns (Spro) were omissible if postverbal (Foulet 1928), but not freely so (Vance 1997, Zimmermann 2014). This article addresses their partial omissibility in discourse-syntax terms, following work on partial null subject languages by Holmberg and Nikanne (2002) and Modesto (2008). An observational study of dialogic responses in 13th century prose romances is first reported, finding strong indications of covariation between the Topic/Focus status of an initial non-subject constituent and the expression/omission of post-verbal Spro. A quantitative investigation, in such texts, of preposed discourse-linked anaphoric constituents and preposed intensifiers, taken as diagnostic of Topichood and Focushood respectively, confirmed this analysis. We take null Spro to be available (i) when a null Topic operator targets left-peripheral TopicP, and (ii) with a left-peripheral Focused expression. When a discourse-linked non-subject constituent occupies TopicP, however, Spro must be overt.

Résumé

En ancien français, on pouvait effacer un sujet pronominal en position postverbale (Foulet 1928), sous certaines conditions (Vance 1997, Zimmermann 2014). M'inspirant des recherches de Homberg et Nikanne (2002) et de Modesto (2008) sur les langues partiellement pro-drop, je propose que cet effacement conditionnel s'analyse en termes de syntaxe discursive. L'article présente d'abord une étude de couplets question-réponse recueillis dans des romans en prose du 13e siècle où on peut observer un rapport étroit entre l'effacement d'un sujet pronominal et le statut discursif – soit topique, soit focus – du syntagme phrastique initial. Une analyse quantitative de ces syntagmes en initiale de phrase qui contiennent d'une part une expression anaphorique et d'autre part un intensificateur vient appuyer cette hypothèse et permet de conclure qu'un Spro non exprimé est légitimé en ancien français en présence, soit (i) d'un opérateur nul Topique ciblant un TopP situé dans la périphérie de gauche, soit (ii) d'une expression focalisée. Par ailleurs, lorsqu'un syntagme discursivement lié (autre que le sujet) occupe la position TopP, un sujet pronominal ne peut être effacé.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Canadian Linguistic Association/Association canadienne de linguistique 2018 

1. Introduction

A long-standing problem in Old French syntax is to account for the variability in subject pronoun realisation (see, e.g., Foulet Reference Foulet1935/6, Price Reference Price1973, Roberts Reference Roberts1993, Vance Reference Vance1997, Rinke Reference Rinke, Junghanns and Szucsich2003, Ingham Reference Ingham2005, Prévost Reference Sophie, François and Prévost2011, Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann2014).Footnote 1 As can be seen from the representative set of example data in (1 a–d) below, taken from early 13th century narrative prose, an overt Subject pronoun (henceforth Spro) could appear either before the tensed verb, or after it if a nonsubject constituent stood in first position, whereas a null subject occurred only in the latter eventuality.

  1. (1)

    1. a. Ele  vint  as  deus  rois.

      She  come-pret  to-def.pl  two  king-pl

      ‘She came to the two kings’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 169)

    2. b. Et  por  ce  vos  vint  ele  veoir.

      And  for  this  you-obj  come-pret  she  see-inf

      ‘And therefore she came to see you’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 185)

    3. c. Lors  vint  a  son  cheval.

      Then  come-pret  to  his  horse

      ‘Then s/he came to his/her horse’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 145)

    4. d. *Vint  a  son  cheval.

      come-pret  to  his  horse (unattested)Footnote 2

For Foulet (Reference Foulet1935/6), optional expression of Spro was possible only when Spro, if expressed, would have followed the verb, an analysis generally maintained in subsequent studies. What may have influenced the choice between XVSpro and XVØ has received relatively little attention, however, and forms the issue explored in this article. An exception was Price (Reference Price1973) who noted that after certain clause-initial adverbs such as molt ‘much, very’ Old French subject pronouns were rare to non-existent, whereas after others such as or ‘now’ they were far more common. This suggested a line of enquiry which turns out to be a fruitful one to pursue, as may be seen.

Within contemporary syntactic theory the question has tended to be whether Old French was a null-subject language typologically. Roberts (Reference Roberts1993) and Rinke (Reference Rinke, Junghanns and Szucsich2003) considered that it was, whereas Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2014) argued to the contrary. However, the question should not be seen as a binary choice, since research has demonstrated the existence of partial null-subject languages such as Finnish and Brazilian Portuguese (Holmberg and Nikanne Reference Holmberg, Nikanne and Svenonius2002, Modesto Reference Modesto and Biberauer2008), in which subjects may be left null in restricted contexts.Footnote 3 The observation is that these languages allow definite 3rd person null subjects only when they are coreferential with the subject of a higher clause. This constraint does not apply in consistently null-subject languages. The partial availability of null Spro in Old French suggests it may be amenable to a similar type of analysis.

The null subject parameter has in recent years been remodelled in Minimalist Program terms by expressing the alternative parameter values as whether or not a finite T hosts a [D] feature, intended to represent definiteness. In a full null-subject language such as Italian, the T head will indeed host this D-feature, whereas this will not be the case in non-null-subject languages. Holmberg (Reference Holmberg2010) refines this approach by postulating that the D-feature is valued either by an overt subject or by a null topic in [Spec,CP] which provides a referential index for a null subject. Here Holmberg adopted Frascarelli's (Reference Frascarelli2007) notion of an Aboutness-shift Topic (A-topic), which in turn goes back at least to Grimshaw and Samek-Lodovici (Reference Grimshaw, Samek-Lodovici, Barbosa, Fox, Hagstrom, McGinnis and Pesetsky1998), who required null subjects to be licensed by an antecedent with the discourse status of topic. According to Frascarelli, an A-topic is syntactically represented in CP, either overtly or covertly. We propose here that an analysis along these lines can be applied to Old French in such a way as to explain what influences the (non-)expression of Spro. The null Aboutness Topic will be taken as an operator over a variable in the clausal Subject position, following Modesto's (Reference Modesto and Biberauer2008) analysis of partial null subjects in Brazilian Portuguese.

The structure of the paper is as follows. First, previous accounts of subject pronoun optionality are discussed, followed by an examination of data that shed light on some of the issues raised by previous analyses. An observational survey of how null and overt Spro patterned in dialogic responses in Old French narrative prose is reported, then a quantitative analysis of Old French V2 clauses is conducted, comparing the syntax of clauses featuring intensifying elements and anaphoric expressions in first position. The narrative prose sources used feature a large amount of dialogue in which nonsubject constituents are shown to be fronted for clearly identifiable discourse reasons. These are taken to be clear cases of focusing and topicalisation, respectively, of the first-position constituent. Thereafter an analysis of null subjects as variables bound by a null Topic is proposed as a way of accounting for the data patterns observed. Our analysis is limited to Old French (up to circa 1300), and no claim is made regarding subsequent developments in the language, as the V2 constraint began to be relaxed after the 13th century. The enquiry is restricted to the domain of root clauses, as these constitute the paradigm case of the null subject phenomenon in the Old French period.

2. Null subjects in Old French

The status of null Spro in Old French is generally linked to the validity of the V2 analysis of root clauses accepted by many – though by no means all – researchers (cf. Kaiser Reference Kaiser2002, Elsig and Rinke Reference Elsig, Rinke, Kunstmann and Stein2007). Following Labelle and Hirschbühler (Reference Labelle, Hirschbühler, Batllori, Hernanz, Picallo and Roca2005: 61), we assume that the main clause left periphery normally had the configuration outlined in (2).

  1. (2) [ZP  [FinP  [TP  … ] ] ]

FinP was the position to which the finite verb moved, while ZP represented a discourse-sensitive projection which they considered to host Topic and Focus constituents (see below, however, for a somewhat different approach to Focus). The generally verb-second character of Old French can be handled by assuming that only one constituent moving to the left periphery could traverse [Spec,FinP]. A clause with postverbal Spro, such as (1b), can then be given the analysis in (3), to be further refined below.

  1. (3) (Et) [ZP por ce [FinP vos vint [TP ele  vos vint [VP veoir por ce ]]]]

In a true null-subject language such as Italian, where a subject pronoun is normally left unexpressed, its overt use signals some special discourse effect. Certain authors, especially Vance (Reference Vance1997) and Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2014), have also sought to investigate whether in Old French the expression or omission of Spro is discourse-driven. Vance (Reference Vance1997), focussing on the syntax of the prose romance La Queste del saint Graal, saw the choice between XVØFootnote 4 and XVSpro as depending on the relation between the initial constituent X and the discourse context in which the clause appears. Basing her analysis on Jakobsen's markedness theory, in which the unmarked member of a formal opposition is neutral with respect to the feature in question, she took XVØ as unmarked, with no particular discourse conditioning, but XVS, the marked member, to be discoursally conditioned. She found that it virtually always showed the property [+ deictic linking to the discourse], while XV could, but often did not, show this property. Her findings thus appeared to corroborate only a one-way prediction: that XVØ occurred when the initial X constituent was deictically linked to the discourse. Being unmarked, XVØ was also found when X was new information, as in (4).

  1. (4)

    1. a. Perceval, traie  m’  avez.

      Perceval, betray-ppart  me  have-2pl

      ‘Perceval, you have betrayed me’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 110)

    2. b. Sire, nouveles  vos  apport  molt  merveillleuses.

      Sire, news  you  bring-1pl-pres  very  wonderful

      ‘My Lord, I bring really astonishing news’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 5)

A complication that Vance herself recognised, however, was that the situation as regards the discourse linking factor associated with XVSpro order appeared to change over time. In 12th c. verse it was very common for a discourse-linked X to appear with a null subject, whereas by the 13th c. this was less usual.Footnote 5 In addition, dialogue showed far more use of overt Spro than narrative, a fact she related to the assumption that innovative tendencies are more visible in speech than in writing.Footnote 6

Vance (Reference Vance1997) did consider conditions on the omissibility of Spro, pointing to a strong tendency for Spro to be expressed if a clause-initial element was deictic or anaphoric. However, her analysis was limited to a small number of adverbs such as or ‘now’, which seemed to require a pronominal subject to be overt when deictic, but to allow optionality when an argumentational introducer, with the same function as the English ‘now’ has, in the sentence ‘Now this is a very significant point’. As with Price (Reference Price1973), Vance made lexically specific observations without embedding them in a more general analysis of subject pronoun use or omission.

The realisation or omission of a subject pronoun was also investigated in relation to lexically specific items by Marchello-Nizia (Reference Marchello-Nizia1985), who studied the very common Old French sentential particle si. In our examples below, the subject pronoun il is used when si relates the clause it introduces to the content of the preceding clause, similarly to its Latin etymon sic:

  1. (5) Certes endroit de moi voudroie  ge qu’ il vos en mescheïst,

    surely place of me wish-cond.1sg  I that it you of-it fail-subj.past.3sg

    et  ge  cuit  que  si  fera  il.

    and  I   think-1sg  that  SI  make-fut.3sg  it

    ‘Certainly for my part I wish you will fail, and so I believe you will.’

    (La mort le roi Artu, p. 192)

In (6), however, si appears to have a different function, of reversing the polarity of the previous clause, and this time the subject pronoun is omitted.

  1. (6)

    1. Ele  ne  trouvera  ja  si  bon  chevalier  qui  por  lui  deffendre

      She  neg  find-fut.3sg  ever  so  great  knight  who  for  her  defend

      en  ost  prendre  son  escu.

      in  army  take-inf  his  shield

    2. Certes,  fet  Lancelos,  ge  cuit  quesi fera;

      Surely,  make-pres.3sg  Lancelot  I  think.pres.1sg  that SI make.fut.3sg

      ‘She will never find so good a knight who will take his shield in battle to defend her.

      Assuredly, said Lancelot, I believe that she will.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 94–95)

When si has the sense of ‘nevertheless’, thus activating the previously expressed proposition with which it contrasts the content of its own clause, we again find that the subject pronoun is expressed, as in (7).

  1. (7) Jamés  n’  ere  a  aise  devant  que  je  sache certainement  que

    ever   neg  be-fut.1sg  at  ease  before  that  I  know-subj.1sg certainly  what

    il  senefient  et   si  en    sai      je  une  grant  partie.

    they  mean   and   SI  of-it  know-pres.1sg  I  a   great   part

    ‘I shall never be at ease before I know for sure what they mean, even though I know a lot about them.’ (Lancelot du Lac : La fausse Guenièvre. Tome III, p. 68)

Marchello-Nizia calls this the ‘si adversatif’ use of the particle. However, when the clause introduced by the particle simply moves the discourse forward by introducing new information, as in the last clause in (8), the subject pronoun is not expressed.

  1. (8) Il  ne  porra  croire  en  nule  maniere  que  Mordrés ne

    He  neg  be-able-fut.3sg  believe-inf  in  any  manner   that  M.  neg

    m’  ait   conneüe  charnelment,  por  la  grant  force  qu’  il

    me  have-subj.3sg  know-ppart  bodily       for   the  great  force  that  he

    a     mise   en  moi  avoir;

    have-pres.3sg  put-ppart  in  me   have-inf

    si  sei    veraiement  qu’  il    m’  ocirra.

    SI  know-pres.1sg  truly     that  he   me  kill-fut.3sg

    ‘He won't be able to believe in any way that Mordred took my body thanks to the force he used to possess me, and I know he will kill me.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 217)

Vance (Reference Vance1997) notes that in the prose romance she used as a source, 1st and 2nd person pronouns, which are used in dialogue, were more often overt, whereas 3rd person subjects, most commonly used in narrative, were more often null. This observation was reinforced by Prévost's (Reference Sophie, François and Prévost2011: 174) corpus study of Old French subject pronoun use, in which she observed a higher frequency of omission of 3rd than of 1st person pronouns. Although she did not draw this conclusion, it could be argued that 3rd person referents tend to maintain the existing discourse topic to a greater extent than 1st person referents, so the finding of Prévost (Reference Sophie, François and Prévost2011) appears to be discoursally based rather than relating to a grammatical property of Old French.

Research studies such as these have provided accounts of specific contexts in which subject pronoun omission /realisation could be associated with discourse properties, but a general overview of the question has up to now been lacking. A more recent study of mediaeval French subject pronoun use – Zimmerman (Reference Zimmermann2014) – did, however, consider the issue in broader terms, relating subject pronoun use in V2 contexts to the nature of the clause-initial constituent. For Zimmerman, Old French was not a null subject language as such, but one in which subject pronouns were generally obligatory, save in certain specific contexts (a point which he noted also applies to the modern language). As with earlier generative authors, he posited that a null subject is licensed in a configuration where the finite verb of the clause has raised to C, so that it can govern the position of the null subject (SØ), for instance:

  1. (9) [CP  XP  Vfin   [TP  SØ   … ]]

Zimmermann's account of when Spro was overt or null posited that subjects were null after a pre-verbal XP ‘which receives special emphasis’ and is naturally interpreted as focused information. For Zimmerman (Reference Zimmermann2014: 140) “in declaratives with non-expressed subject pronouns and a verb preceded by a single constituent, this constituent is focalised.” This is the view taken in the present study. However, he went on to formulate the matter in terms we find much less convincing, given in (10).

  1. (10) The pre-verbal constituent in V2 configurations in declaratives in Old and Middle French invariably represents a focus element. (Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann2014: 140)

Apparently referring to all V2 clauses, not only those with null subjects, he thus rejected earlier analyses of the preverbal constituent in Old French V2 clauses as a topic. Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2014: 150–151) furthermore argued that the presence of a topic is not correlated with V-to-C, but that the presence of a focused element is. Referring to our example (11) below (his ex. 32b), he considered the pre-verbal constituent in V-to-C contexts as a focus: “The directly preverbal constituent in [this example among others cited] constitutes a focus element” (Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann2014: 153).

  1. (11) Et  pour  ce  vaut  il  miex,  si  comme  il me semble,

    and  for  this  be-worth-pres.3sg  it  better   so  as  it me seem.pres3.sg

    que  nous  le  façons  occirre   avant   qu’  il   nous  parte    des    mains.

    that  we  him  make-subj.1pl  kill-inf   before  that  he   us   leave-subj3sg  of-the    hands

    ‘And because of this, then, it would be better, so it seems to me, that we have him killed, before he slips from our hands’ (Saint Louis, p. 172)

Appreciating that this stands at variance with his previously stated position, he dismissed such cases as ‘marginal’, however, and maintained his claim that “the presence of a (left peripheral) focus element is principally restricted to declaratives with non-expressed subject pronouns” (Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann2014: 154).

It seems to us, on the contrary, that this case is not at all marginal, but rather represents the ordinary situation in Old French when a preposed constituent is deictic, as indeed Vance's (Reference Vance1997) investigation of or would lead us to suggest. What seems required to resolve this issue, therefore, is a detailed study, taking discourse context into account, of the varying discursive properties of clause-initial constituents and how they related to pronominal subject realisation. The investigation reported here attempts to make up for this apparent gap in our knowledge.

3. Introduction to the study

Researching the relationship between syntax and discourse is far from straightforward in contemporary languages, and when studying historical language states the challenges become much greater. The main problem is, of course, the loss of prosodic information when only written text is available, as prosody very often provides clues to the foregrounding and backgrounding of information (Lambrecht Reference Lambrecht1994, Zubizarreta Reference Zubizarreta1998), as well as to the attitudinal stance taken by the speaker to the propositional content of the discourse. However, the contextual information provided by surviving texts themselves may well furnish sufficient material to determine very plausibly the status of a constituent as denoting focused or topicalised information. This is particularly the case in fictional narrative texts where characters exchange information, especially in dialogic form. Through dialogic exchange, their knowledge ‘slate’ is continually presented as being updated, and the linguistic structuring of that information can be studied so as to relate possible regularities in the positioning of constituents on the one hand with the information status of the content of these constituents on the other. This approach is pursued in the present study.

The crucial concepts in this analysis of information structure are first, Topic – who or what the proposition is presented as being about, understood as given by the discourse context –, and secondly Focus. This can be taken in Lambrecht's (Reference Lambrecht1994) narrow-focus sense, where a non-predicate constituent is focused, as in (12).

  1. (12) Q: I heard your motorcycle broke down. A: My CAR broke down. (Lambrecht Reference Lambrecht1994: 223)

This English example signals Focus by prosodic highlighting (represented orthographically by capital letters), but we will be interested in the extent to which word order modification, specifically movement to the left periphery, may have achieved the same result in Old French, as studied by Labelle and Hirschbühler (Reference Labelle and Hirschbühlerthis volume), who note that the left periphery hosted focused as well as topicalised expressions. In addition, word-order modification may support emphasis (Zubizarreta Reference Zubizarreta1998), and it will be seen below that emphatic focus is a notable characteristic of the Old French left periphery. Corrective uses of argument focus, as in (12), were found to be rare, and are not therefore considered as the central type of Focus to be addressed here.

3.1 The Left Periphery in dialogic text: a preliminary investigation

We approached the issues to be explored in this study by first making an observational examination of dialogic discourse in early 13th c. narrative prose. Numerous texts of this type exist in published sources, though not many of them have been digitised in electronic corpora. As the aim was to gain an impression of the effect of discourse context, close reading of the texts and extraction of data by hand, albeit rather time-consuming, seemed to be of value, which compensated for the ease of data collection normally associated with electronic corpus use. The main point of interest lay in what, in discourse terms, are known as adjacency pairs (Schegloff and Sacks Reference Schegloff and Sacks1973), especially concerning the syntactic form of the second speaker's response to the first speaker's utterance. Such contexts allow the syntax of Information Focus to be observed in responses to Wh-questions: the constituent supplying the requested information is clearly new and the most communicatively salient part of the message.

In the texts studied, Information Focus was normally positioned postverbally, as in (13).

  1. (13)

    1. a. O  volez  vous  aler?  Dame,  fait   il, je voil

      Where  want-pres.2pl  you   go-inf  lady   make-pres.3sg he I want-pres.1sg

      aler  jusqu’  en  ce  bois.

      go-inf  as-far  in  that  wood

      ‘Where do you want to go? Lady, he said, I want to go that wood.’

      (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 396)

    2. b. Sire,  fet   il?  Qui  estes   vos?

      Sir   make-pres.3sg   he   who  be-pres.2pl  you

      Je  sui,    fet     il,  Gauvains  li   niés    lo   roi  Artur.

      I  be-pres.1sg  make-pres.3sg  he  Gawain  the  nephew  the  king  Arthur

      ‘Sir, he said, who are you? I am, he said, G. the nephew of King A.’

      (La queste del saint Graal, p. 155)

However, it could sometimes be found in preverbal position, as in (14).

  1. (14)

    1. Sire,  fait  Gualehoz,  qui  estes  vos?

      Sir,  make-pres.3sg  G.  who  be-pres.2pl  you

    2. Biaus  sire,  uns  chevaliers  sui.

      fair  sir  a  knight  be-pres.1sg

      ‘Sir, said G., who are you? Fair Sir, I am a knight.’ (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 832)

Likewise, new information elicited by imperatives could also appear preverbally, as in (15).

  1. (15)

    1. a.– Mes  or  me  dites  ou  nos  vos  porrons  trover

      But  now  me  tell-imp  where  we  you  be.able-fut.1pl  find-inf

      Quant  nos  vendrons   ceste  part.

      When  we  come-fut.1pl  this  part

      ‘But now tell me where we'll be able to find you when we come that way’

    2. En  ceste  place  meismes,  fet  il,  me  troverez.

      In  this  place  same  make-pres.3sg  he  me  find-fut.2pl

      ‘In this place here, he said, you will find me.’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 30)

    3. b.– Or  me  dites,  fait  il,  lo  non  au  chevalier.

      Now  me  tell-imp  make-pres.3sg  he  the  name  to-the  knight

    4. Desouz  cele  lame,  fait  ele,  de  metal  lo  troveroiz.

      below  this  blade  make-pres.3sg  she  of  metal  it  find-fut.2pl

      ‘Now tell me, he said, the knight's name. – You'll find it underneath this strip of metal’ (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 554)

The significant point in these cases is that when information focus is preverbal, the clause subject was left null. By contrast, when the second speaker in an exchange pair repeated an expression used by the first speaker, a pronominal subject was used, as in (16).

  1. (16)

    1. a.– Biau  sire,  vendrez  vos  a  la  cort  lo  roi  Artur  auvec  moi?

      Fair  sir,  come-fut.2pl  you  to  the  court  the  king  A.  with  me

    2. Auvec  vos  n’  irai  je  pas.

      with  you  neg  go-fut.1sg  I  at-all

      ‘- Sir, will you go to King A.’s court with me? – I won't go with you.’

      (La queste del saint Graal, p.3)

    3. b.– Si  vos  pri  por  Deu  que  vos  me  pardonez  lo  meffait.

      SI  you  ask-pres.1sg  for  God  that  you  me  pardon-pres.2pl  the  misdeed

    4. Certes,  fait  il,  meffait  n’  i  voi  ge  nul.

      Surely  make-pres3.sg  he  misdeeed  neg  there  see-pres.1sg  I  any)

      ‘– I beg you before God to forgive me this misdeed. – Assuredly, he said, I can see no misdeed.’ (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 492)

No cases were found in the response member of the exchange pair where repeated information in first position was accompanied by a null subject.

Preverbal Information Focus seemed rather infrequent, however, so the issue was approached from the other direction, asking what kind of preverbal constituent favoured a null subject. Dialogic texts were manually examined for cases where no subject was expressed, following any type of preposed constituent.Footnote 7 Three prose romances from the first half of the 13th century were used for this purpose.Footnote 8 Contexts were analysed where a V2 main clause with a nonsubject constituent in first position was the second member of any dialogic exchange, not only in question-answer pairs.Footnote 9 Cases of null subjects in these clauses were identified, and the nature of the initial constituent in discourse terms was noted. Twenty-nine examples were obtained, all of which seemed interpretable in terms of some type of focusing, as in (17).

  1. (17)

    1. a. Sire,  fet  Boorz,  por  noiant  i  demorroie.

      Sire,  make-pres.3sg  B.  for  nothing  there  stay-cond.1sg

      ‘Sire, said Boris, I would not stay here for anything.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 82)

    2. b. Par  mon  congié  ne  movrez  hui  mes.

      By  my  authority  neg  move-fut.2pl  today  more

      ‘You will go no further today on my authority.’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 242)

The initial constituent appeared in some cases to be evaluative, as in (18).

  1. (18)

    1. a. Car  mauvesement  vos  estes  contenue  envers  moi.

      For  badly  you  be-pres.2pl  behave-ppart  towards  me

      ‘For you have behaved badly towards me.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 112)

    2. b. Feme,  merveilles  as  fet.

      woman  wonders  have-pres.2sg  do-ppart

      ‘Woman, you have done wonders.’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 542)

In such cases, the initial elements served to intensify the speaker's commitment to the proposition expressed, rather than relate it to the discourse context. They thus seem to be conveying Emphatic Focus, rather than merely the preposing of new information as in (14)–(15). This was particularly clear when the initial constituent was an adverb of intensity (Buridant Reference Buridant2000: 742), supporting Price's (Reference Price1973) observation that adverbs such as molt ‘much’ strongly disfavoured overt subject pronouns when preposed, as in (19).

  1. (19)

    1. a. Moult  m’  avez  bien  paiee  de  ceste  parole.

      Much  me  have-pres.2pl  well  reward-ppart  of  that  word

      ‘You have well rewarded me for saying so.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 10)

    2. b. Messire Gauvains […] dist a Boort: Trop me merveill

      Sire G. say-pret.3sg to B. Much me wonder-pres.1sg

      que messires  Lancelos  n’  a esté  a  ceste  assemblee

      that Sir  Lancelot  neg  have-pres.3sg  be-ppart  at  this  meeting.

      ‘Sir Gawain said to Bohort: I'm very surprised that Sir Lancelot wasn't at this meeting.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 88)

In current syntactic terms, they therefore involved the CP projection, rather than some extraposed position not affecting the syntax of the clause. The interpretation of the adverb moult as modifying a clause-internal element such as paiee in (17b) or dolente in (18), indicates that the adverb has raised out of its base position into CP.

Note that when the clause subject was a nominal, it was always inverted following a preposed adverb, as in (20).

  1. (20) Moult  est  la  reïne  dolente  de  ces  noveles.

    Much  be-pres.3sg  the  queen  sorrowful  of  these  news

    ‘The Queen is very sorrowful at this news.’   (La mort le roi Artu, p. 32)

That is, the syntax of preposed focus constituents obeyed Foulet's generalisation in relation to (1), at least in that inversion took place with a nominal expression, meaning that preposed intensifying adverbs did not belong to his ‘non-inverting’ category of adverbs such as certes.

The next step was to ascertain what kind of initial expression in these dialogic responses typically favoured overt subject pronouns. In accordance with Vance (Reference Vance1997), overt subject pronouns were most commonly found after an anaphoric initial constituent, especially one involving a demonstrative expression such as ce ‘this/that’, as in (21).

  1. (21)

    1. a.– Leissiez  moi  aucuns  enseignes  qui  me  seront  aprés  vostre  mort

      leave-imp  me  some  signs  that  me  be-fut.3pl  after  your  death

      remenbrance.

      memory

    2. Sire,  fait  Josephé,  ce  vos  dirai  je  bien.

      Sir,  make-pres.3sg  J.  that  you  tell-fut.1sg  I  well

      ‘Leave me some tokens that will remind me of you after your death. Sir, said J., indeed I will tell you that.’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 37)

    3. b.– Volez  vos  que  ge  aille  avoc  vos?

      want-pres.2pl  you  that  I  go-subj.pres.1sg  with  you

    4. De  ce  sui  ge  mout  liez,  fait  il.

      Of  that  be-pres1sg  I  much  happy  make-3sg  he

      ‘Would you like me to go with you? – I'd be very happy about that, he said.’ (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 628)

In such cases, null subjects seemed to be avoided. The adverb donc (sometimes spelled dont), in the sense of ‘consequently’, likewise avoided null subjects, as in (22).

  1. (22)

    1. a.– Sire,  me fetes       vos droit de doner     a  la  reïne  si    lonc respit?

      Sire,  me make-pres.2pl  you right of give-inf  to the queen such long wait

    2. Oïl,  fet  li  rois,   ce  sachiez  vos  veraiement.

      Yes  make-pres.3sg  the  king  that  know-subj.2pl  you  truly.

    3. Dont  m’  en  irai  ge,  fet  il.

      So  me  of-it  go-fut.1sg  I  make-pres.3sg  he

      ‘Sire, will you allow me to give the queen such a long wait? Yes, said the king, you can be sure of that. – Then I will go, he said.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p.87)

    4. b.– Je  vos  conois,  fet   li  preudons,  mout  mielz

      I  you  know-pres.1sg  make-pres.3sg  the  good-man  much  better

      que  vos ne  cuidiez.     –Biau  sire,  fet      Gauvains,   donc

      than   you expl  think-pres.2pl  fair   sir    make-pres.3sg  Gawain   then

      me        vos   bien dire,   s’ il   vos plest,       en quoi…

      me  be-able-pres.2pl  you  well say-inf  if it  you please-pres.3sg  in  what

      ‘I know you, the good man said, much better than you think. Good sir, said G. then can you please tell me in what…’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 52)

As an overall impression of the data from this observational study, it can be said that an overt pronominal subject was disfavoured if new and apparently focused information was positioned preverbally. Overt subject pronouns, however, were favoured by old information, as well as by material linking the clause to the preceding discourse anaphorically, through the use of either a demonstrative expression or an adverb expressing a logical relationship, such as donc. In quantitative terms, the results of this exploratory study are not on a scale allowing a conclusive statement to be made on the discourse syntax of Old French. However, they are in line with earlier observations by Marchello-Nizia (Reference Marchello-Nizia1985) and by Vance (Reference Vance1997) in that the distribution of null subjects seems not to have been random, but co-varies with properties of items in the left periphery. Provisionally, they also suggest that the association made by Zimmerman (Reference Zimmermann2014) between null subjects and focusing is correct, though only in the sense of Focus we adopt here, associated with new information and thus excluding deictic/anaphoric expressions (see example (11) above). Vance (Reference Vance1997) already made a convincing case for an association between overt Spro and a preposed deictic constituent, so we can then ask if subject omission and realisation can be distinguished on the basis of whether the discourse role of an initial nonsubject constituent is Focus on the one hand, or Topic, on the other. In the main study reported in section 3.2, frequency data are brought to bear on this question.

3.2 Preverbal anaphoric expressions and intensifiers: a quantitative study

In the previous section, indications were found that two types of initial constituent behaved very differently as regards null subjects: the first where constituents contained an anaphoric expression that could be considered a sentence Topic, and the second involving constituents with what appeared to be a Focus interpretation. The initial survey was not conducted on a quantitative basis, however, and it was held desirable to gain a more authoritative view of the distribution of null and overt pronominal subjects in these contexts. For this purpose, electronic corpus data were sought, particularly Old French prose texts containing substantial amounts of dialogue: prose romances from the early 13th century are known to fit this specification.Footnote 10 Three were selected on the basis that they could readily be searched in electronic versions.Footnote 11 Currently, Old French corpus resources are not tagged for discourse properties, making it a challenge to identify suitable datapoints. Accordingly, lexical items corresponding to the targeted initial constituent types were established as search terms, as follows:

  • To represent initial topicalised expressions, a Prepositional Phrase creating a discourse link with the context, using a form of the demonstratives ce, cel or cest, for example: apres ce, a ceste fois, de cel don, etc.

  • To represent initial focused expressions, an adverb of intensity from among the following lexical set: molt ‘much, very’, tant ‘so much’, asez ‘very’, trop ‘very’, bien ‘really’,Footnote 12 as in (23).

  1. (23)

    1. a. Moult  a  granz  denz  et  dedenz  et  dehors.

      much  have-pres.3sg  large  teeth  and  inside  and  outside

      ‘It has big teeth both inside and outside.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 12)

    2. b. Moult  fu  Lancelos  corrouciez  por  la  mort  de  Gaheriet.

      much  be-past.3sg  L.  anger-ppart  for  the  death  of  G.

      ‘Lancelot was greatly angered over the death of G.’ (La mort le roi Artu, p. 125)

The intensifying meaning conveyed by these expressions allows them to represent the cases of emphatic (though not usually informational) Focus identified in the observational study in 3.1.

Main clauses with a non-subject in first position and the tensed verb in second position were investigated using the search terms specified above. Clauses with an impersonal subject were omitted,Footnote 13 as were those introduced by the conjunction et, so as to exclude conjunction as a possible factor influencing the omission of a pronominal subject.

The results of these searches are shown below. The restrictive criteria imposed on the data have limited the sample size somewhat, but the very different tendencies seen in Table 1 are unmistakable. The expression or omission of Spro overwhelmingly reflects the discourse type of initial constituent.

Table 1: Subject pronoun expression following discourse linked and non-discourse linked constituents, three prose romances from the 13th century

With the intensifiers, taken here as indicating a focus interpretation, Spro was very rarely expressed. They are thus very clearly distinct from clauses with anaphoric initial constituents, such as (24).

  1. (24) De  ceste  chose  est  il  moult  dolans.

    of  this  thing  be-pres.3sg  he  much  grieving

    ‘About this matter, he is very sad.’ (Merlin, roman en prose du 13e siècle, p. 218)

Here, non-expression of a subject pronoun was almost equally rare. The difference in the expression of subject pronouns in these two contexts was found to be highly significant by Fisher's exact test (p = < 0.001, 2-tailed).

It is not being claimed here that the patterns identified by this data sample are exceptionless: discourse linking and focusing are matters where psycholinguistic factors involving the speaker's/writer's stance operate. Especially in a historical context, they may be unrecoverable from the content and context of a solely written documentation. We think, however, that the use in these sample data of items which are strongly identifiable with anaphoricity by demonstrative forms, and focus, by intensifying forms, makes it as clear as can reasonably be expected what the information structure properties of the clause are. The double dissociation between focusing and topicalisation on the one hand, and the expression or omission of the subject pronoun on the other, is not far from complete. The generalisation is thus that null subjects were not normally possible when the initial constituent was discourse-linked. Where the clause-initial constituent was not discourse-linked, however, S was normally left null. Conditions on null S were satisfied, then, as long as no discourse-linked constituent preceded the finite verb. The question that then arises is whether these conditions were structural, or whether the phenomenon can be considered as purely pragmatic.

Theoretical approaches to the problem of subject pronoun expression/omission in Old French have had to find a way of capturing Foulet's generalisation (that null Spro was grammatical when, if overt, it would have appeared postverbally). This is related to the possibility for an initial non-subject constituent to occupy first position, in which case the subject needed to be postverbal. Government and Binding approaches of the 1980s and 90s proposed that null Spro was licensed only when governed by V in C (Roberts Reference Roberts1993), employing the notion of government, but more recent generative treatments following Minimalist Program assumptions have had to find alternative accounts. Holmberg (Reference Holmberg, Kiss and Alexiadou2015) and Biberauer et al. (Reference Biberauer, Holmberg, Roberts, Sheehan, Newmeyer and Preston2013) tie the pre-verbal expression of the subject pronoun, as in (1a) to the verb-second property of Old French, such that the left periphery of a finite matrix clause cannot be left empty. We will follow that assumption here, noting, however, that it still leaves the variable postverbal appearance of Spro to be dealt with.

The link between initial focused phrases and null Spro has not to our knowledge been pointed out in the historical syntax literature on Old French, although Vance (Reference Vance1997) did, as we saw, relate postverbal Spro to an anaphoric initial constituent. A new analysis therefore needs to be developed to take account of these findings, and in what follows we propose to ascribe the null Spro phenomenon in Old French to a property of the language remarked upon in the earlier descriptive literature: topic prominence (Rowlett Reference Rowlett2007: 8). Note that the term ‘topic-prominent’ should be seen as a rough characterisation rather than a formal assignment of Old French to a particular category within a crosslinguistically valid typology (Li and Thompson Reference Li, Thompson and Li1976). The essential characteristic of Old French that is relevant here is its well-known predilection for topic chaining, as in (25).

  1. (25) Et  qant  Phariens  voit     ce,  si saisit      une  hache  qu’ il   avoit

    and when P.    see-pres.3sg this SI seize-pres.3sg an   axe    that he  have- impf.3sg

    en  la  tor  maint  jor  gardee,  si  s'escorce  vers  son  neveu.

    in  the  tower  many  day  keep-ppart.fem  SI  refl-rush  towards  his  nephew

    ‘And when P. sees this, he seizes an axe which he had kept in the tower for a long time, and rushes towards his nephew.’ (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 278)

A new topic is often signalled by placing it to the left of si:

  1. (26) Debonnairetez  est  de  faire  granz  festes  et granz conpaignies…

    Bounteousness  be-pres.3sg  of  make-inf  great  feats  and great companies

    Largesce  si  est  de  doner  a   toz  cels   en  cui  li  don

    Generosity  SI  be-pres.3sg  of  give-inf  to   all  those  in  whom  the  gifts

    puent   estre  bien   emploie.

    be-able-pres.3sg  be-inf  well   employ-ppart

    ‘Bounteousness is making great feasts and occasions. Generosity is giving to all those in whom the gifts can be well used.’ (Lancelot du Lac. Tome I, p. 220)

In the Middle French period, as the language moved towards its modern subject-prominent character, these traits, as well as null Spro and Verb Second, were lost. We therefore propose that Old French null Spro was integrated into the topic-prominent character of the left periphery, as the foot member of a topic chain, with the topic operator as its head, as shown in (27).

  1. (27) (Phariens …. saisit une hache) si s'escorce vers son neveu.

    [TopP OPi [FinP si s'escorce [TP ei vers son neveu ]]]

Motivation for this analysis is presented in the next section.

4. An operator-variable analysis

Addressing the problem of whether Brazilian Portuguese is a pro-drop language, Modesto (Reference Modesto and Biberauer2008) observes that null subjects are licensed only in restricted contexts, specifically in a subordinate clause whose subject is co-referential with that of the main clause.

  1. (28) O  Fecoi  convenceu  a  Dani  que  ei  ganhou  o  campeonato.

    the  Feco  convinced  the  Dani  that    won  the  championship

    ‘Feco convinced Dani that he won the championship.’

Here the understood subject of the subordinate clause can only be coreferential with the matrix subject. Modesto (Reference Modesto and Biberauer2008: 380) proposes that a null subject is a variable bound by a higher subject standing in an Aʹ position, the specifier of a functional category FP to which it has moved, and from which it can bind the subordinate clause subject shown as e in (29).

  1. (29) [FP o Fecoi [TP ti convenceu a Dani [CP que ei ganhou o campeonato ]]]

We believe that this analysis may help to clarify null subject properties in Old French, albeit in V2 contexts, rather than in embedded clauses as in Brazilian Portuguese. Following Holmberg and Nikanne (Reference Holmberg, Nikanne and Svenonius2002) for Finnish, Modesto argues that Brazilian Portuguese is a topic-prominent language, a characterisation which has often been made for Old French (see, e.g., Buridant Reference Buridant2000).Footnote 14 He noted furthermore that in both languages the left periphery preceding the finite verb cannot be left vacant, again a commonality with Old French.

An operator-variable analysis of null subjects is standardly adopted for an undoubted topic-prominent language such as Chinese (Li and Thompson Reference Li, Thompson and Li1976). Importantly for present purposes, the variable can be bound not only by an overt subject, as in Brazilian Portuguese, but also by a null topic operator which serves to maintain the topic identity across a sequence of clauses, as can be seen in (30), from Pu and Pu (Reference Pu and Pu2014: 34).

  1. (30) Duan Quchen  wei-ren  zhen-pai, …  tong  shenmeren  dou neng shuo-

    D.Q.    as-person  honorable in work  with  anyone  all  can  talk-

    shang  hua,  conglaibu  baijiazi.

    a  talk  never  put-on-air

    ‘Duan Quchen was a good leader… he could strike up a conversation with anyone, he never appeared arrogant or remote.’

Chinese subject/topic pronouns are not always omissible, however. In (31), from Pu and Pu (Reference Pu and Pu2014: 40), the omissibility of the subject pronoun ta is seen to depend on whether another constituent is topicalised. In the first clause, (31a), the 3rd-person referent is introduced as the pronoun ta, interpretable from preceding context that has been omitted here. In the last two clauses, (31c) and (31d), the same referent is referred to by a null subject. However, in the intervening clause (31b) the object constituent zhe gushi ‘this story’ is fronted and topicalised, with the result that the subject pronoun ta needs to be overt. Its omission is not acceptable to native speakers consulted.

  1. (31)

    1. a. zuijin  yiduan  shijian  ta  yizhi  zaixie  yige  gushi,

      recent  period  time  he  keep  write  a  story

      ‘Recently, he has been writing a story.’

    2. b. zhe  gushi  ta  gousile  hengjiu

      this  story  he  think.of  very.long

      ‘He has thought about this story for a long time.’

    3. c. izaojiu  xiang  xie,

      long.ago  want  write

      ‘He wanted to write it a long time ago.’

    4. d. ikeshi  yizhi  meiyou  shijian  xie.

      but  all.along  have.no  time  write

      ‘But he never had any time to write it.’

In clauses (31c, d), where a subject pronoun is normally unexpressed, there is no such fronted alternative topic to break the topic chain, which allows the subject to be null.

We suggest that the same approach can be adopted for null subjects in Old French, in which a null topic operator in a left-peripheral Topic phrase licenses a post-verbal null subject, with which it is co-indexed to form a topic chain. The Topic head always bears the formal feature [+ anaphoric], thus embodying the traditional view that Old French V2 clauses were ‘topic-prominent’. This feature is checked by the null Topic operator. Our analysis is first illustrated with the very common topic-chaining construction using the discourse particle si (Marchello-Nizia Reference Marchello-Nizia1985), as in example (32a) below. Under the analysis offered in (32b), a null topic operator can be taken to have moved from the subject position to [Spec,TopP], and is co-referential with the discourse topic established in the preceding clause, as was seen for Chinese in (30) above.

  1. (32)

    1. a. Et  ele  le  regarde,  si  le  conoist.

      and  she  him  look-at-pres.3sg  SI  him  know-pres.3sg

      ‘And she looks at him and recognises him’ (La queste del saint Graal, p. 12)

    2. b. [TopP Opi [FinP si le conoist [TP ti … ]]]

Note that instead of the neutral ‘ZP’ label used by Labelle and Hirschbühler (Reference Labelle, Hirschbühler, Batllori, Hernanz, Picallo and Roca2005) to designate the topmost clausal projection, we have taken this projection to be a Topic Phrase, distinct from the position of preverbal focused elements, which we assume are positioned in [Spec, FinP].Footnote 15 So example (17b) above, reproduced below as (33a), has the structure in (33b).

  1. (33)

    1. a. Par mon congié ne movrez hui mes.

    2. b. [TopP Opi [FinP Par mon congié Vfin ne movrez [TP ti hui mes ]]]

When a deictic/anaphoric constituent is preposed to TopP, however, discourse topic chaining cannot operate, since null-subject operator movement is blocked, as can be seen in example (24) reproduced as (34a), with the structure shown in (34b). With a null subject, the analysis would have been as in (34c).

  1. (34)

    1. a. De ceste chose est il moult dolans.

    2. b. [TopP De ceste chose [FinP Vfin est [TP il moult dolans ]]]Footnote 16

    3. c. [TopP De ceste chose [FinP Vfin est [TP Op moult dolans ]]]

However, in (34c), Op cannot raise to Top, so the structure is ill-formed. Since a null subject required operator movement, the subject pronoun would have had to be overt in this context.

A null-operator-variable analysis thus allows us to explain the key phenomenon under discussion in this study: the difference in subject pronoun realisation between clauses with a preposed Focus element, and those with a Topic element. The structural analysis proposed here accounts for the overwhelming majority of the data discussed in 3.2. The exceptional cases of Spro with preposed focus are presumably benign, in that an overt Spro would also have been available in this context. Exceptional cases of null subjects following a topic are more problematic, however. They may be vestiges of an earlier grammar with different properties that may underlie the freer use of null subjects that seems to be attested in early Old French verse.

5. Summary

This article has sought to explore the discoursal and syntactic conditions under which an Old French subject pronoun remained unexpressed. The view taken here is that Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2014) represented a substantial advance in linking Focus and null S, but did not offer a theoretically satisfactory explanation of the association, in addition to leaving the status of preposed deictic expressions in the left periphery unclear. Whereas for Zimmermann only focused expressions involved V-movement to the left periphery (Fin0 in the approach taken here), we consider that both preposed Topic and Focus elements produce V2 structures, with V moving to Fin0 in both cases. The central claim made here is then that the presence of a fronted anaphoric/deictic topic regularly requires a pronominal subject to be expressed, whereas a fronted Focus element regularly leaves it null.

We have examined data drawn from narrative prose which point to the conclusion that subject pronoun (non-) expression was very strongly related to the discoursal nature of material in the left periphery, at least in root causes – subordinate clauses require separate treatment. When fronted, discourse-linked elements rarely permitted null subjects, and non-discourse-linked elements even more rarely permitted overt pronominal subjects. On a wider and more systematic basis, then, this study has confirmed the proposal of Vance (Reference Vance1997) that after a deictic constituent in the left periphery, a subject pronoun tended to be obligatory. Her contention that subject pronouns were otherwise optional has had to be qualified, however, insofar as fronted elements appearing to have an emphatic focus function overwhelmingly avoided co-occurrence with an overt pronominal subject.

The structural account of this empirical contrast that we have proposed relies on the analysis, by Modesto (Reference Modesto and Biberauer2008) and others, of partial null subject languages, to the effect that in such languages null subjects are co-indexed with a c-commanding Topic constituent. Languages of this type are considered ‘topic-prominent’, a label which seems to suit Old French rather well. On that basis, we have applied to Old French the notion of a topic chain involving a null topic operator, an analysis standardly used for topic-prominent languages such as Chinese. A strength of this analysis is that it accounts straightforwardly for the very common null subject clauses with particle si found in running narrative where the same entity is interpreted as the subject of successive root clauses in the discourse. It is then shown that if the syntactic Topic position is filled by a D-linked nonsubject constituent, a null subject cannot be licensed, as the null operator is blocked from raising to Spec TopP.

The analysis proposed requires preverbal focused elements to front to a different position from that occupied by Topic constituents. Insofar as this attributes to Old French a split left periphery, the availability of two preverbal projections might therefore appear to conflict with the strongly verb-second character of the language. However, V3 sequences that would have been produced by the surface order Top-Foc-VFin were independently ruled out by the bottleneck condition (Holmberg Reference Holmberg, Kiss and Alexiadou2015) on FinP, by which the constituent moved there to check FinP's EPP feature prevented another constituent from moving through it on its way to Top or Focus. Hence V3 constructions in Old French were normally possible only if the first element was in extraposition, as discussed by Skårup (Reference Skårup1975).

The discourse contrast in contexts with pronominal versus null subjects has thus been shown to receive a plausible structural explanation grounded in a current analytic framework that fits the generally accepted character of Old French. However, given the confines of the present article, the empirical findings that motivate the proposed analysis must be regarded as provisional, and further exploration of the information structure properties of the Old French left periphery are clearly called for. The extreme contrast between D-linked fronted constituents and fronted intensifiers in whether they can occur with null subjects leaves little room for doubt that there was a systematic constraint on novel subject to availability. Nevertheless, research into a wider variety of clause-initial constituent types is needed, so as to test the applicability of the analysis proposed here. To do so, corpus resources with the discoursal tagging would clearly be advantageous, though the logistical and analytic challenges involved in creating them should not be underestimated. We hope to have shown in the present study that this would be a worthwhile enterprise, likely to benefit both scholars interested in the discourse pragmatics of historical language varieties, and those concerned more with their syntactic properties. The importance of discourse factors in explicating the syntactic properties of Old French would seem to make highly desirable such a convergence of research interests on the need for corpus resources facilitating both approaches.

Footnotes

1 The following abbreviations are used: A-topic: Aboutness-shift Topic; cond: conditional; def: definite; EPP: Extended Projection Principle; fem: feminine; fut: future; imp: imperative; impf: imperfect; inf: infinitive; neg: negative; obj: object; past: past; pl: plural; ppart: past participle; pres: present; pret: preterite; refl: reflexive; S: subject; sg: singular; Spro: subject pronoun; subj: subjunctive; V2: verb second.

2 This root clause construction is unattested in the 13th c. Old French prose works examined in the present study. Known as V1, it was possible in 12th c. verse, where the verb appears to have been fronted for discourse purposes (see, e.g., Labelle and Hirschbühler Reference Labelle, Hirschbühler, Batllori, Hernanz, Picallo and Roca2005). It seems to have been stylistically marked, and possibly a relic of an archaic construction, by the time of the data analysed here.

3 Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2014) points to the existence in even canonical obligatory-subject languages of apparent null-subject constructions, but these are lexically, not structurally, conditioned, unlike in the cases studied in the research cited here.

4 We have replaced Vance's label ‘C(omplement)’ for the clause-initial constituent by the more neutral term ‘X’.

5 Whether this was an artefact of analysing verse for the 12th and prose for the 13th centuries was not discussed by Vance, but is clearly a possibility in view of the period/text type confound in the data sets used. Unfortunately, the unavailability of freely composed Old French prose narrative before the 13th c. makes this a difficult question to resolve.

6 The present author's comparison of speech-based and written-genre sources in Anglo-Norman (Ingham Reference Ingham2016) obtained the same finding.

7 Instances of preposed ja and onques were omitted, as they were already known to have a special syntactic status: Foulet (Reference Foulet1928) called these adverbs ‘non invertissants’, yet they regularly co-occur with null subjects, in both main and subordinate clauses (Ingham Reference Ingham2005). On the basis of their unusual behaviour, they were set aside from the present investigation.

8 These were La Mort le roi Artu, La Queste del saint Graal, and Roman de Tristan en prose.

9 Initial adverbial clauses were not considered for this purpose, as in the 13th century they were already undergoing change towards a non-V2 grammar lacking postposition of subjects (Donaldson Reference Donaldson2012).

10 The constraints imposed by regular versification make verse texts less suitable for syntax research, especially when a good number of prose texts are available, as they are for the period studied here.

11 These were La Mort le roi Artu, La Queste del saint Graal, searched in the University of Ottawa LFA corpus (Kunstmann Reference Kunstmann2003) and Merlin, roman en prose I, in an electronic version consulted online. The largest Old French electronic corpus, the Base de Français Médiéval < http://txm.bfm-corpus.org> unfortunately contains only one prose romance, making it inadequate for our purpose.

12 See Buridant (Reference Buridant2000: 742). Note that for Old French bien, the equivalent item bien in Spanish is taken as a left-periphery Focus particle by Hernanz (Reference Hernanz, Benincà and Munaro2010).

13 Because the study focused on referential pronouns that could be discourse-linked, impersonal subjects, which could not, were set aside.

14 However, these authors ascribe to Topic-prominent languages a constellation of other properties which Old French did not share, so the commonality is only partial.

15 Mathieu (Reference Mathieu and Arteaga2013: 341) discussed fronted main clause constituents which are not ordinary Topics, but assigns them to a ‘Topic+’ projection. Despite a perhaps infelicitous choice of label, his proposal clearly addressed a distinctive type of preposed element, which may be considered to have the same status as focused constituents in the present analysis. Significantly, the examples he quoted all lacked a postverbal pronominal subject.

16 For expository convenience we ignore cliticisation of il to Vfin.

References

Literary Works Cited

de Joinville, Jean. Histoire de Saint Louis. Ed. de Wailly, Natalis, 1868. Paris: Renouard. [1309]Google Scholar
Lancelot du Lac. Tome I. Ed. Mosès, François, 1991. Le Livre de Poche, coll. “Lettres gothiques”.Google Scholar
Lancelot du Lac. Tome II. Ed. Mosès, François, 1993. Le Livre de Poche, coll. “Lettres gothiques”.Google Scholar
Lancelot du lac : La Fausse Guenièvre. Tome III. Ed. Mosès, François, 1998. Le Livre de Poche, coll. “Lettres gothiques”.Google Scholar
Merlin, roman en prose du 13e siècle. Ed. Paris, Gaston et Ulrich, Jacob, 1886. Paris: Firmin-Didot. Online version at <https://archive.org/stream/merlinromanenpro01robeuoft/merlinromanenpro01robeuoft_djvu.txt>Google Scholar
La Mort le roi Artu. Ed. Frappier, Jean, 1964. Genève: Droz.Google Scholar
La Queste del saint Graal. Ed. Pauphilet, Albert, 1949. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Roman de Tristan en prose. Ed Blanchard, J., 1976. Paris: Klinksieck.Google Scholar

References

Biberauer, Theresa, Holmberg, Anders, Roberts, Ian, and Sheehan, Michelle. 2013. Complexity in comparative syntax: The view from modern parametric theory. In Measuring linguistic complexity, 3ed., ed. Newmeyer, Fritz and Preston, Laurel, 103127. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Buridant, Claude. 2000. Nouvelle grammaire de l'ancien français. Paris: Sedes.Google Scholar
Donaldson, Bryan. 2012. Initial subordinate clauses in Old French: Syntactic variation and the clausal left periphery. Lingua, 122(9): 10211046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elsig, Martin, and Rinke, Esther. 2007. Les adverbes et l'inversion en ancien français. Le Nouveau Corpus d'Amsterdam soumis à l'analyse linguistique. In Le Nouveau Corpus d'Amsterdam: Actes de l'atelier de Lauterbad, ed. Kunstmann, Pierre, and Stein, Achim. Stuttgart: Steiner.Google Scholar
Foulet, Lucien. 1928. Petite syntaxe de l'ancien français. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Foulet, Lucien. 1935/6. L'extension de la forme oblique du pronom personnel en ancien français. Romania 61(243): 257315; 401–463; Romania 62(245): 27–91.Google Scholar
Frascarelli, Mara. 2007. Subjects, topics and the interpretation of referential pro: An interface approach to the linking of (null) pronouns. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 25(4): 691734.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, Jane, and Samek-Lodovici, Vieri. 1998. Optimal subjects and subject universals. In Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax, Ed. Barbosa, Pilar, Fox, Danny, Hagstrom, Paul, McGinnis, Martha, and Pesetsky, David, 193219. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hernanz, Maria Lluïsa. 2010. Assertive bien in Spanish and the left periphery. In Mapping the left periphery, ed. Benincà, Paola and Munaro, Nicola, 1962. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders. 2010. Parameters in minimalist theory: The case of Scandinavian. Theoretical Linguistics 36(1): 148.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders. 2015. Verb second. In Syntax: Theory and analysis. An international handbook, ed. Kiss, Tibor and Alexiadou, Artemis, 242283. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders, and Nikanne, Urpo. 2002. Expletives, subjects, and topics in Finnish. In Subjects, expletives, and the EPP, ed. Svenonius, Peter, 71106. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ingham, Richard. 2005. Adverbs and the syntax of subjects in Old French. Romania 123(489): 99122.Google Scholar
Ingham, Richard. 2016. Investigating language change using Anglo-Norman spoken and written register data. Linguistics 54(2): 381409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, Georg. 2002. Verbstellung und Verbstellungswandel in den romanischen Sprachen [Verb position and verb position change in the Romance languages]. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Labelle, Marie, and Hirschbühler, Paul. (this issue). Topic and Focus in Old French V1 and V2 structures.Google Scholar
Labelle, Marie, and Hirschbühler, Paul. 2005. Changes in the clausal organization and the position of clitics in Old French. In Grammaticalization and parametric variation, ed. Batllori, Montserrat, Hernanz, Maria Lluïsa, Picallo, Carme, and Roca, Francesc, 6071. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lambrecht, Knud. 1994. Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representation of discourse referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Charles N., and Thompson, Sandra. 1976. Subject and topic: A new typology of language. In Subject and topic, ed. Li, Charles N., 457491. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Marchello-Nizia, Christiane. 1985. Dire le vrai: L'adverbe “si” en français médiéval. Geneva: Droz.Google Scholar
Mathieu, Éric. 2013. The left periphery in Old French. In Research in Old French: The state of the art, ed. Arteaga, Deborah, 327350. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Modesto, Marcelo. 2008. Topic prominence and null subjects. In The limits of syntactic variation, ed. Biberauer, Theresa, 375410. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sophie, Prévost. 2011. Expression et position du sujet pronominal en français. In Mémoires de la Société de Linguistique de Paris, Tome XIX: L'Evolution grammaticale à travers les langues romanes, ed. François, Jacques, and Prévost, Sophie, 1333. Available at: <https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00665183/document>.Google Scholar
Price, Granville. 1973. Sur le pronom personnel sujet postposé en ancien français. Revue Romane 8: 226236.Google Scholar
Pu, Ming-Ming, and Pu, Qinghong. 2014. Zero anaphora and topic chain: A cross-linguistic study. International Journal of Linguistics and Communication 2(1): 2744.Google Scholar
Rinke, Esther. 2003. On the licensing of null subjects in Old French. In Syntactic structures and morphological information, ed. Junghanns, Uwe, and Szucsich, Luka, 101127. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Roberts, Ian. 1993. Verbs and diachronic syntax. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Rowlett, Paul. 2007. The syntax of French. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel, and Sacks, Harvey. 1973. Opening up closings. Semiotica 8(4): 289327.Google Scholar
Skårup, Povl. 1975. Les premières zones de la proposition en ancien français. Essai de syntaxe de position. Études Romanes de l'Université de Copenhague. København: Akademisk Forlag.Google Scholar
Vance, Barbara. 1997. Syntactic change in Medieval French: Verb-second and null subjects. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, Michel. 2014. Expletive and referential subject pronouns in Medieval French. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, Maria Luisa. 1998. Prosody, Focus, and Word Order. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: Subject pronoun expression following discourse linked and non-discourse linked constituents, three prose romances from the 13th century