Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-zrtmk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-07T01:42:53.767Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of dietary recommendations for reducing free sugar intakes, on free sugar intakes, dietary profiles and anthropometry: a randomised controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Lucy R. Boxall
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Technology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
Emily Arden-Close
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Technology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
Janet James
Affiliation:
Department of Nursing Science, Faculty of Health and Social Sciences, Bournemouth University, Bournemouth, UK
Katherine M. Appleton*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Technology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
*
Corresponding author: Katherine M. Appleton; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Free sugar intakes are currently higher than recommended for health, yet effective strategies for reducing consumption are yet to be elucidated. This work investigated the effects of different dietary recommendations for reducing free sugar (FS) intakes, on relevant outcomes, in UK adults consuming > 5 % of total energy intake (TEI) from FS. Using a randomised controlled parallel-group design, 242 adults received nutrient-based (n 61), nutrient- and food-based (n 60), nutrient-, food- and food-substitution-based (n 63) or no (n 58) recommendations for reducing FS at a single timepoint, with effects assessed for the following 12 weeks. Primary outcomes were FS intakes as a percentage of TEI (%FS) and adherence to the recommendations at week 12. Secondary outcomes included TEI, diet composition, sugar-rich and low-calorie-sweetened food consumption and anthropometry. In intention-to-treat analyses adjusted for baseline measures, %FS reduced in intervention groups (%FSchange = –2·5 to −3·3 %) compared with control (%FSchange = –1·2 %) (smallest B = –0·573, P = 0·03), with effects from week 1 until week 12 and no differences between interventions (largest B = 0·352, P = 0·42). No effects of the interventions were found in dietary profiles, but change in %FS was associated with change in %TEI from non-sugar carbohydrate (B = 0·141, P < 0·01) and from protein (B = –0·171, P = 0·02). Body weight was also lower at week 12 in intervention groups compared with control (B = –0·377, P < 0·05), but associations with %FS were weak. Our findings demonstrate the benefit of dietary recommendations for reducing FS intakes in UK adults. Limited advantages were found for the different dietary recommendations, but variety may offer individual choice.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Free sugar (FS) intakes are associated with a number of poor health outcomes, including increased risk from excess energy intake resulting in increased risk from overweight, obesity and several chronic health conditions(1). As a result, the World Health Organisation (WHO) currently recommends ‘In both adults and children, the intake of free sugars should be reduced to less than 10 % of total energy intake’ ((1), p.4), and ‘A reduction to less than 5 % of total energy intake would provide additional health benefits’ ((1), p.4).

Public health agencies around the world have followed suit(24), but while the recommendations are well-defined, appropriate strategies to achieve adherence to these consumption levels by the general public remain unclear. Of particular note, the recommendations are provided at a nutrient level, i.e. they are based on sugars, while in 1998, the WHO and Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO)(5) recommended that consumer guidelines should be based on foods; the rationale being that consumers are largely unaware of the nutrients in foods; that foods, not nutrients, make up dietary choices and that encouraging change to whole dietary patterns would benefit multiple single nutrient goals and synergies(5). Advice based on nutrients typically focusses on the amount of a nutrient found in foods or beverages, and appropriate targets or limits for consumption. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest some benefit from this type of information in the form of product labels, for consumer understanding(Reference Scapin, Fernandes and Curioni6) and food selection(Reference Cecchini and Warin7,Reference Song, Brown and Tan8) , but impacts on intakes can be limited(Reference Cecchini and Warin7Reference Riesenberg, Peeters and Backholer10). Ninety countries, however, now provide food-based dietary guidelines, and 84 % of these include recommendations specifically on high-sugar foods(Reference Herforth, Arimond and Álvarez-Sánchez11). Adherence to FS recommendations, thus, may be aided by recommendations not only based on the nutrient but also based on the relevant foods.

Sugars and sugar-rich foods are commonly consumed for reasons related to their sweet taste(Reference Beauchamp12,Reference Drewnowski, Mennella and Johnson13) . Substitution with whole fruit, intrinsic milk sugars and low-calorie sweeteners (LCS) may maintain this sweet taste without contributing FS(Reference Drewnowski, Mennella and Johnson13). Recommendations to replace sugars with non-sweet alternatives are also increasing(2,3) . Studies where sugar-rich foods are replaced with low-sugar alternatives demonstrate success from this substitution strategy for reducing FS intakes(Reference Hashem, He and MacGregor14Reference Wise, Nattress and Flammer18). Meta-analyses on product reformulation demonstrate benefits for sugar intakes and body weight (BW), although limited studies are currently available(Reference Hashem, He and MacGregor14). Studies using behavioural substitutions similarly suggest benefits(Reference Piernas, Tate and Wang15Reference Wise, Nattress and Flammer18). Participants in the CHOICE trial showed a 44–54 % reduction in percent energy consumed from FS following encouragement to replace ≥ 2 servings per day of caloric beverages with LCS-sweetened or unsweetened beverages(Reference Piernas, Tate and Wang15,Reference Tate, Turner-McGrievy and Lyons16) . Ebbeling et al. (Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz17) report a 58–65 % reduction in FS intakes following the replacement of ≥ 1 sugar-sweetened beverages with LCS-sweetened or unsweetened beverages. Wise et al. (Reference Wise, Nattress and Flammer18) report a 42–48 % reduction from baseline energy consumed from sugars in participants asked to dilute sugary drinks and replace high-sugar foods with items higher in complex carbohydrates, protein and/or fats.

Reviews of interventions to reduce FS intakes are available(Reference Vargas-Garcia, Evans and Prestwich19,Reference von Philipsborn, Stratil and Burns20) . However, no study as far as we are aware has investigated these differing nutrient-based, food-based and food-substitution-based strategies for reducing FS intakes within the same study. The WHO appeal for the ‘need to evaluate different behavioural-change approaches to promote the reduction of free sugars intake’ in the recommendation guidelines((1), p.20), and discussions on food-based dietary guidelines suggest a need for real-world monitoring and evaluation(5). The WHO further highlights a ‘need for longer term (> 8 weeks) controlled trials of the effect of increasing or decreasing free sugars intake on body weight in free-living individuals’ ((1), p.20). This study investigated the effects of three different dietary recommendations for reducing FS intakes on FS intakes, dietary profiles and anthropometry over 12 weeks. Our primary research purpose was to better understand FS intakes, how to reduce these and the role that different dietary recommendations can play in this. We hypothesised that all dietary recommendations would result in changes in FS intakes v. control.

Methods

Design

Using a randomised controlled trial design, members of the general adult population of the UK consuming > 5 % total energy intake (TEI) from FS were randomised to receive: nutrient-based recommendations (group N); nutrient- and food-based recommendations (group NF); nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations (group NFS) or control (control group) at a single time point, with effects assessed for the following 12 weeks. Given the public health focus of sugar reduction, our study was specifically conducted with the general population, in a public health context. Our primary outcomes were FS intakes as a percentage of TEI (%FS) and adherence to the recommendations at week 12. Secondary outcomes were dietary profiles (daily TEI, diet composition, sugar-rich and LCS-sweetened food consumption), anthropometry (BW, BMI, waist circumference), outcomes related to sweet taste and barriers and facilitators to dietary change at week 12, and %FS and adherence at weeks 1, 2, 4 and 8. This report focuses on the main trial methodology and findings from our primary outcomes and secondary outcomes related to dietary profiles and anthropometry. Methods and findings for the outcomes related to sweet taste and the barriers and facilitators to dietary change will be reported elsewhere.

Approval for the trial was gained from the Research Ethics Committee of Bournemouth University, UK (ID: 30612) on 28 April 2020 with amendments approved on 29 March 2021. The work was undertaken in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1983), the Ethical Guidelines of the British Psychological Society and Bournemouth University’s Research Ethics Code of Practice. The trial was registered on Clinicaltrials.gov (ID: NCT04816955) on 24 March 2021. A full protocol for the study was also published(Reference Boxall, Arden-Close and James21). We adhered to our study registration and published protocol in all respects with a few minor refinements, as given below.

Participants

Volunteers were eligible for inclusion in the trial if they were aged 18–65 years, consuming > 5 %TEI from FS, and were able to provide informed consent and complete all trial measures. Exclusion criteria were pregnancy or breastfeeding; underweight (BMI < 18·5 kg/m2); pre-existing medical conditions affecting swallowing ability, taste and/or smell perception; currently, or within the previous three months, smoking or following a specific dietary programme (e.g. slimming world); pre-existing clinical conditions, including food allergies, diabetes mellitus, eating disorders and Crohn’s disease, leading to the use of external nutritional advice and dietary restrictions.

Sample size equations aimed to test for a 2 % change in %FS from baseline to Week 12 at a power of 80 % for an α of 0·05. Due to a lack of literature on the use of dietary recommendations for reducing FS intakes at trial conception, sample size equations were based on the reported effects of a trial on the use of dietary recommendations for reducing saturated fat intakes(Reference Smith, Kerr and Howie22). The highest standard deviation calculated from these data (sd = 2·4) was used to calculate a required group size of n 46 per trial arm(Reference Whitley and Ball23). This sample size was also assumed to be adequate for our second primary outcome – adherence, as this outcome involved categorising participants based on %FS or change in %FS (see Outcomes). Allowing for a 20 % drop-out rate and unequal recruitment across trial arms, we aimed to recruit 240 individuals.

Potential participants were recruited via personal contacts; University contacts and outlets, including a participant pool; contacts with local groups, e.g. church groups; social media advertising and advertising in local news outlets, public buildings, e.g. libraries and eating establishments. The study was marketed as ‘A study investigating different types of dietary advice’. Potential participants provided electronic written informed consent, then completed eligibility, including a 3-day diet diary to assess FS intakes (see Outcomes). This 3-day diet diary also served as an opportunity to train participants, allow them to gauge the commitment required for the study and ensure they were competent in the diet diary data collection methods prior to their enrolment. Participants were not recruited until they were comfortable with the diet diary data collection methods and the commitment required.

Eligible participants were randomised to one of four trial arms at a ratio of 1:1:1:1 using blocked stratified randomisation, based on gender, BMI and %FS at baseline (block size: 8 participants). An exception was made where participants lived with other participants to avoid contamination between intervention groups. These participants were randomised as a pair or group, based on the stratification of the person making the initial query. Randomisation was undertaken by a researcher with no direct contact with participants (KMA) using a random number generator.

Intervention/Control

There were four trial arms: three arms delivering recommendations for reducing FS intakes and one control arm. The three differing dietary recommendations were nutrient-based, nutrient- and food-based, and nutrient-, food- and food-substitution-based, as below. Nutrient-, food- and food-substitution-based information was investigated in an additive manner to aid consumer understanding. All recommendations used publicly available information as provided by the UK Government or related public health agencies at the time of trial conception(2426). In addition to the specific information provided, all groups were also asked to record their food intakes and given instructions for this.

Group N: nutrient-based recommendations

Nutrient-based recommendations began with the instruction: ‘Your dietary recommendation is to reduce your intake of free sugars to less than 5 % of your total energy intake’. This sentence was followed by one page of nutrient-based information, including the different names for sugars and how to identify the sugar content of foods, e.g. ‘high in sugar – 22·5 g or more of total sugar per 100 g’. Recommendations from the UK government(24) were amended to provide only the nutrient-based information related to sugars.

Group NF: nutrient- and food-based recommendations

These recommendations began with the instruction: ‘Your dietary recommendation is to reduce your intake of free sugars to less than 5 % of your total energy intake. To aid with this, reduce your intake of foods high in free sugars’. Participants were then provided with the same nutrient-based information as for Group N plus four additional pages on foods that are commonly high in FS, with examples of how much sugar is included, e.g. ‘A bowl of sugary breakfast cereal could contribute 70 g of sugar (up to 22 sugar cubes) to your diet over a week’. Recommendations from the UK government(24,25) were amended to provide the nutrient- and food-based information related to sugar and sugar-rich foods.

Group NFS: nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations

These recommendations began with the instruction: ‘Your dietary recommendation is to reduce your intake of free sugars to less than 5 % of your total energy intake. To aid with this, reduce your intake of foods high in free sugars and replace these with low sugar versions’. Participants were then provided with the same nutrient- and food-based information as Group NF, plus five additional pages on low-sugar versions of foods that are usually sugar-rich, and on LCS. This information suggested low-sugar substitutions for high-sugar products, e.g. ‘biscuits – swap for oatcakes, oat biscuits, or unsalted rice cakes’ and provided details on LCS, where they are found and their different uses. Recommendations from the UK government(24,25) were amended to provide the relevant information, and information on LCS was obtained from Diabetes UK(26) to include only the information on LCS, with all references to diabetes removed.

Control group: control

The control group were given no dietary recommendation related to FS, but were only asked ‘to keep an accurate diet diary using the Nutritics software’. This group undertook all trial measures in the same manner as those in the intervention groups. The group was intended to control for the act of dietary recording throughout the study; a behaviour that may increase dietary awareness and impact intakes(27,Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow28) .

Recommendations were delivered to participants at a single time point, as may occur in a public health context. Recommendations were provided in written format in a sealed opaque envelope, alongside an instruction ‘to keep an accurate diet diary using the Nutritics software(29) and a user guide for the Nutritics Libro App (29). This instruction was carefully worded, such that for participants in the control group, this instruction could be construed as a dietary recommendation, with the intention of aiding compliance. All groups received the same instructions regarding the diet diaries, thus all groups received a sealed envelope. On provision of their sealed envelope, all participants were also informed that a dietary recommendation could be anything from simply recording your diet to the provision of specific instructions. Envelopes were packaged to include the same number of pages regardless of intervention or control group through the addition of blank pages, to conceal group allocations from the researcher in contact with participants. Full copies of each intervention, as provided to participants, are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Figure SM1).

Participants were provided with their recommendations following baseline measures. They were not permitted to ask questions, in line with the current scenario for the UK public where dietary recommendations are often provided, e.g. via TV advertisements, without the opportunity to ask questions. An inability to ask questions also ensured that the same information was provided to all participants, maintaining intervention fidelity.

All envelopes containing intervention/control information were identical, sealed and coded by the researcher undertaking the randomisation. The researcher in direct contact with participants (LRB) remained blind to treatment allocation throughout all data collection. Participants were not blinded to condition, but were blinded to the trial aims and to other conditions. To further disguise the purpose of the trial, all participants completed questionnaires on other aspects of their diet alongside those focusing on sugars and sweet foods.

Outcomes

Primary outcomes were %FS and adherence to the recommendations at 12 weeks. Secondary outcomes were dietary profiles (daily TEI, diet composition, food consumption), anthropometry (BW, BMI, waist circumference) at week 12 and %FS and adherence at weeks 1, 2, 4 and 8. Demographic variables, sweet taste sensitivity, sweet-liker status and bitter taste sensitivity were also assessed at baseline, and various attitudes (to sweet foods, towards eating behaviour, for food choice), some dietary knowledge, leisure time physical activity, quality of life and adverse events were assessed at baseline and week 12.

Primary outcomes (baseline, week 12)

% Free sugar intakes

Free sugar intakes, as %TEI, were measured using diet diaries, completed using the Nutritics software platform (research edition, version 5, GB and IE databases) and ‘Libro’ App (29). Dietary intakes at baseline and week 12 were calculated from 3 days of diet diaries comprising of 2 weekdays and 1 weekend day at both time points(Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow28). Diet diaries such as these are recognised as a valid and reliable method for assessing short-term dietary intake in the real world(27,Reference de Castro30Reference Trabulsi and Schoeller32) and were selected here as the best method for assessing intakes and changes to intakes in a comprehensive manner over a 12-week period of free living(27,Reference de Castro30Reference Trabulsi and Schoeller32) . Training with participants was undertaken prior to enrolment into the study, and all subsequently submitted food diaries were checked for likely missing data at the time of submission(27). Portion-size rather than weighed diaries, and handheld portable digital diary entry methods(27,Reference Trabulsi and Schoeller32,Reference Raatz, Scheett and Johnson33) were used to reduce participant burden and minimise any effects as a result of this while maintaining data integrity and validity. Participants had access to all UK and Irish foods, supported by the Nutritics British (GB) and Irish (IE) databases, plus supermarket and brand-specific information, ensuring correct and specific foods could be easily found, with aspects, such as bar code scans for food identification, also available(29). Any issues with dietary recording throughout the trial were addressed the same day by the researcher (blinded to treatment allocation and supported by the Nutritics Support Team), and supplemented with handwritten email, text message or paper recording, where necessary.

FS intakes, as a percentage of TEI were calculated using the Nutritics databases and manufacturer’s information. The Nutritics software is supported by extensive food databases, where food composition data are repeatedly internally validated(29). These databases contain a high number of foods with FS data(29), and where these data were not available from databases, data were sought from manufacturers. Where data on FS could not be obtained, foods were replaced with the closest available food for which FS data were available. The closest available food was selected using nutritional values for energy, carbohydrates and total sugars, by a registered Nutritionist with experience of the UK food supply (blinded to treatment allocation).

Adherence

Adherence was defined as a reduction in FS consumption of ≥ 2 %TEI from baseline, or FS intake at ≤ 5 %TEI, to result in the classification of participants as ‘adherent’ or ‘non-adherent’. Participants were also asked an adherence question: ‘Are you currently following the dietary recommendations you were given?’ Reductions of FS intakes ≥ 2 %TEI and an answer ‘YES’ resulted in a classification of ‘active adherent’, reductions of FS intakes ≥ 2 %TEI and an answer ‘NO’ resulted in a classification of ‘passive adherent’, reductions of FS intakes < 2 %TEI and an answer ‘NO’ resulted in a classification of ‘active non-adherent’ and reductions of FS intakes < 2 %TEI and an answer ‘YES’ resulted in a classification of ‘passive non-adherent’.

Secondary outcomes (baseline, week 12)

Dietary profiles

Daily TEI, diet composition and food consumption were assessed from diet diaries as measures of dietary choice. TEI was summed from all foods and beverages consumed. Diet composition, in terms of macronutrient and select micronutrient consumption, were also calculated to result in measures of %TEI consumed from carbohydrate, protein, fat and saturated fat, and amounts of dietary fibre (g) and Na (mg) consumed. Food consumption was assessed through the identification of high-sugar, medium-sugar and low/no-sugar foods, based on the criteria used for the UK traffic light system(34), where high-sugar foods have > 22·5 g sugar/100 g, medium-sugar foods have 5–22·5 g sugar/100 g and low/no-sugar foods have < 5 g sugar/100 g. LCS-sweetened foods were also classified as any sweet food or beverage described as ‘diet’, ‘low-sugar’, ‘low calorie’ or ‘sugar-free’ that was clearly sweetened with LCS. This description may not capture all foods that include LCS but was intended to capture foods that were likely to have been selected by study participants because they were LCS- rather than sugar-sweetened. Consumption of all food types was measured as number of foods and weight (grams) of food consumed.

Anthropometry

Participant height (m), weight (kg) and waist circumference (cm) were recorded in a fasted state, by a trained researcher, using a portable stadiometer (SECA 213 Height Measure, Germany), digital scales (Tanita Body Composition Analyzer BF-350, Tanita Europe, Germany) and a flexible tape measure (SECA, Germany), respectively.

Secondary outcomes (weeks 1, 2, 4 and 8)

% Free sugar intakes and adherence

Eighteen daily diet diaries, in addition to the three diaries at baseline and at 12 weeks, were undertaken over the 12-week period. These diaries were used to calculate %FS and adherence at weeks 1, 2, 4 and 8, as above.

Additional variables (baseline only)

Demographic information

Direct questioning assessed: gender, age, ethnicity, occupation, education level, income level, diet type (e.g. vegan) and cooking habits.

Sweet taste sensitivity and sweet liker status

Sweet taste sensitivity and sweet liker status were assessed using 10 ml samples of a 1 M aqueous sucrose solution. Participants first reported perceptions of sweet taste intensity using a 100 mm pen and paper version of the general linear magnitude scale(Reference Bartoshuk, Duffy and Fast35) following training in this method(Reference Bartoshuk, Duffy and Fast35,Reference Bartoshuk, Fast and Snyder36) . Liking was then assessed using 100 mm visual analogue scales(Reference Iatridi, Hayes and Yeomans37Reference Trumbo, Appleton and de Graaf39), and ratings were subsequently categorised to describe participants as ‘sweet likers’, ‘those with an inverted U-shaped sweet liking function’ or ‘sweet dislikers’ using published methods(Reference Iatridi, Hayes and Yeomans37). To allow for any situation where the solutions were not appropriate, tests were also conducted using taste papers saturated in the same 1M solution.

Bitter taste sensitivity and bitter taste status

These characteristics were assessed using a taste paper impregnated with 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP). Participants were asked to mark the intensity of the bitter taste using 100 mm general linear magnitude scale, a 100 mm visual analogue scale and a 9-point category scale(Reference Drewnowski, Kristal and Cohen40), and were subsequently classified as ‘non-tasters’, ‘medium-tasters’ or ‘super tasters’ according to published classifications(Reference Bartoshuk, Duffy and Fast35,Reference Bartoshuk, Fast and Snyder36,Reference Drewnowski, Kristal and Cohen40) . A control paper with no impregnation was also tested and rated between the measures for sweet taste and those for bitter taste.

Additional variables (baseline, week 12)

Attitudes towards sugars, sweeteners and sweet foods were assessed using a recently developed questionnaire(Reference Tang, Mars and James41), describing six attitudes towards sugars, sweeteners and sweet-tasting foods, to reflect earlier qualitative work(Reference Tang, Mars and James42).

Attitudes towards eating behaviour: were assessed using the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire(Reference Karlsson, Persson and Sjöström43), to result in scores for ‘cognitive restraint’, ‘uncontrolled eating’ and ‘emotional eating’.

Motives for food choice were assessed using the Food Choice Questionnaire(Reference Steptoe, Pollard and Wardle44); a multi-level measure of nine motives related to food choice.

Knowledge of current UK dietary recommendations were assessed using a single open-ended question requesting participants report all dietary recommendations of which they were aware. Answers were scored where participants were given one point for all correct dietary recommendations reported (total knowledge) and separately, one point for any reference to FS (sugar-related knowledge).

Leisure time physical activity levels were measured using the Godin-Shephard Leisure-time physical activity questionnaire (GSLTPAQ)(Reference Godin45,Reference Godin and Shephard46) .

Quality of life was assessed using the 36-Item Short Form Survey(Reference Ware, Snow and Kosinski47) and scored using standard procedures(Reference Ware, Snow and Kosinski47Reference Jenkinson, Coulter and Wright49), to provide separate scores for mental and physical health.

Adverse events: Participants were asked to report adverse events at any time, regardless of whether they considered these to be associated with the trial or not. To ensure adverse events were comprehensively reported, specific questions on difficulties undertaking the study were also requested at weeks 4, 8 and 12, and adverse events were verified at the study end.

Assessment schedule

An overview of the assessment schedule for all variables is given in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM1). All participants undertook all measures, in the same manner, regardless of study arm. Dietary assessments and all questionnaires, including the questions on adherence and difficulties, were completed via the Nutritics software, using a study-specific Nutritics program. This program, on specific days, requested diary completion and/or provided participants with online links to all study questionnaires, to be completed via an online questionnaire programme(50). All dietary and questionnaire responses were checked by a researcher (blinded to treatment allocation) on submission for completeness. Food intakes that appeared low (less than 5 items recorded) were queried with the participant, confirmed or corrected. Incomplete questionnaires were completed.

Compliance with all trial measures was also enhanced using a bogus pipeline method(Reference Adams, Parkinson and Sanson-Fisher51Reference Strang and Peterson53). Participants were asked to provide a saliva sample at baseline and at trial end, for the supposed purpose of examining salivary enzymes that may vary with dietary change, but in reality, these analyses were not planned nor undertaken. Suggestion of an objective measure to check compliance however has previously been found to increase compliance in participants(Reference Adams, Parkinson and Sanson-Fisher51Reference Strang and Peterson53) and was considered to be a valuable addition to this trial where our primary outcomes were self-reported. Participants were informed on completion of the trial that their saliva samples had not been analysed.

Procedure

The trial was run from Bournemouth University, UK from April 2021 to December 2022. Participants completed all measures throughout the year to avoid seasonal effects, but no participants took part over the Christmas period to avoid poor compliance as a result of festive intakes.

All baseline and Week 12 assessments were conducted in two single test sessions. Sessions were conducted at the University where possible, or in the participant’s home via video-conferencing. The trial was run towards the end of the COVID-19 pandemic and during some related restrictions in the UK (March 2020–July 2021), thus ‘at-home’ test sessions were used if participants were unable or unwilling to come to the University. At-home test sessions may also have opened the trial to participants who would otherwise have been unable to take part, enhancing study inclusivity. Participants were tested in the same location at both baseline and trial end, where possible. All participants completed all measures and in the same manner regardless of their completion of test sessions at the University or ‘at-home’, under the direction of the same researcher, with a few exceptions: Participants who were tested ‘at-home’ did not undertake the solution-based measures of sweet taste sensitivity and sweet liker status and completed their own anthropometric measurements after provision of the necessary equipment, while the trial researcher observed via video conferencing.

All test sessions commenced with participants in a fasted state, at the same time of day at baseline and trial end. The day before testing participants were also asked to consume no alcohol, consume nothing after 22.00 and undertake no heavy exercise.

Individuals were debriefed on exit, or at their original trial end time point if other household members were taking part. During the debrief, participants were asked for their understanding of the trial purpose to investigate the success of our methods to disguise the trial aims and were given the true purpose of the trial. Following the debrief, participants were offered a consultation on their diet by a UK Registered Associate Nutritionist (UK Association for Nutrition), as a thank you for taking part. Participants received no other compensation.

Analyses

Complete data collection was ensured where possible throughout the trial, as above. At trial end, diet diaries were first screened to ensure that all entered foods contributed to composition totals and where data, e.g. grams of FS, were unavailable for certain foods, these foods were replaced in totality with the closest available food with complete data. All data were handled only at the end of data collection, by the researcher responsible for data collection while blinded, to ensure consistency across participants.

Three distinct analyses were specified in advance(Reference Boxall, Arden-Close and James21): (1) Analyses of quantitative data from the population as a whole to investigate FS intakes and the effects of the three different types of dietary recommendation v. control; (2) Analyses of quantitative data to investigate the effects of the dietary recommendations in different population sub-groups and (3) Investigation of qualitative data for barriers and facilitators to success. Analyses 1 and 2 for primary and secondary outcomes as above are reported here. Analyses 1 and 2 for sweet taste outcomes and Analyses 3 will be reported elsewhere. Analyses were conducted on an Intention-to-Treat basis. Missing data at single time points were estimated using multiple imputation(Reference Li and Stuart54,Reference Marshall, Altman and Holder55) . This provided data for 11·7 % diet diaries and 8·7 % questionnaires. All presented results are taken from the pooled analyses.

For analyses 1, multiple regression analyses were used(Reference Howell56Reference Vickers and Altman58). These analyses allowed us to directly address our primary intended research purpose – to better understand FS intakes, how to reduce these and the role that different dietary recommendations can play in these, while also allowing consideration of a number of other factors of likely influence on FS intakes, making maximal use of all available data(Reference Howell56Reference Vickers and Altman58). Regression analyses were considered more appropriate than ANCOVA, considering our primary intended research purpose and the number of factors for consideration(Reference Howell56Reference Vickers and Altman58). Alternative analyses, such as ANOVA or ANCOVA, would also allow test of the recommendations but would not so comprehensively describe FS intakes in our population and the role of the recommendations in these(Reference Howell56Reference Vickers and Altman58). Regression models also allowed us to consider the additive nature of our recommendations in all analyses through the use of the intervention/control group variable as a continuous, rather than a categorical, variable based on the number of recommendations provided(Reference Howell56).

Regression models were conducted for all outcomes, to predict outcomes at Week 12, and change in outcome from baseline to Week 12. Primary regression models included intervention/control group, gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline BW (as randomisation variables), baseline variable of interest, TEI at Week 12 and physical activity at Week 12 (continuous scores), allowing simultaneous adjustment for all these variables. Baseline BW was used in place of baseline BMI for all analyses considering the importance of BW rather than BW in relation to height, i.e. BMI, in our outcomes. Secondary models were also undertaken to include any additional variable that correlated with each outcome when assessed independently, at a significance value of P < 0·01. Analyses of effects at weeks 1, 2, 4 and 8 were only undertaken using secondary models. Select exploratory analyses were also undertaken, only using secondary models. Effects for all continuous variables were investigated using multiple linear regression (enter method), following checks for multi-co-linearity between variables. Effects for adherence were investigated using logistic regression to predict adherence v. non-adherence, again following appropriate checks for multi-co-linearity. Use of the ‘active’ and ‘passive’ descriptors was not discriminatory – almost all participants reported themselves to be following the recommendations that they were given, but subsidiary analyses to investigate differences between those reporting this correctly or mistakenly are given in the Supplementary Materials, following all other analyses.

For analyses 2, the above analyses using secondary models were conducted in females only (no analyses were undertaken in males due to an insufficient sample size), in three sub-groups based on BMI: lean (BMI < 25·01 kg/m2), with overweight (BMI 25·00–30·00 kg/m2), with obesity (BMI > 30·00 kg/m2) and in three sub-groups based on sweet liker status: sweet likers, those with an inverted U-shaped sweet liking function, sweet dislikers.

All analyses were registered in advance as part of our trial registration (Clinicaltrials.gov: NCT04816955) and are provided in our protocol paper(Reference Boxall, Arden-Close and James21). Proposed analyses on the variables described here as ‘additional’ were not undertaken considering the exploratory nature of these analyses and the number of analyses undertaken. Analyses were conducted in SPSS version 28.0. Significance was set at P < 0·05.

Results

Study sample

In total, 1147 individuals registered their interest in the study. Of these, 538 completed consent, and following all screening processes, 242 participants were recruited. Of these, sixty-one participants were randomised to group N, sixty to group NF, sixty-three to group NFS and fifty-eight to the control group. A total of 200 (83 %) participants completed the study and provided data for our primary outcomes at week 12. Flow through the study is detailed in the CONSORT diagram, Figure 1. Characteristics of each intervention/control group based on gender, age, BW, BMI and %FS at baseline are given in Table 1. Full details of the sample are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM2).

Figure 1. CONSORT Diagram demonstrating flow through the study.

Table 1. Participant characteristics (gender, age, body weight, BMI, and %FS) (n or mean (standard deviation)) for the sample as a whole and for each intervention/control group (n 242)

N, nutrient-based recommendations; NF, nutrient- and food-based recommendations; NFS, nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations.

Number of participants who withdrew from the study was 42: 17 participants from the control group, 9 from Group N, 7 from Group NF and 9 participants from Group NFS. Five participants experienced adverse events during the study period, 2 participants in Group NFS and 1 participant in each other group. None of these adverse events were related to the study. When asked at the study end, sixteen individuals thought the study was targeted towards or about sugar intakes, however only one individual thought we were investigating the effects of different types of advice specifically for reducing sugars. This individual was in Group NF.

Of the 242 participants, 179 participants took part in the study by attending the University, 63 participants took part via video conferencing. No systematic differences were found between participants in these groups at baseline, with the exception that participants attending ‘at-home’ rated the sugar-sweetened paper as more sweet (t(241)= 2·53, P = 0·01) and more liked (t(241)= 4·12, P < 0·01) than University attendees, although no differences were found in perceptions of the control paper (largest t(241)= 1·62, P = 0·11). Participants rating liking for both the paper and solution at the University (n 179) demonstrated scores that differed by 3·3 % (solution liking: mean (sd) = 50·4 (25·3) mm, paper liking: mean (sd) = 52·3 (18·4) mm), although the scores were only weakly correlated (r= 0·135, P = 0·07). For categorisation of sweet liker status, all liking scores for participants attending ‘at-home’ were adjusted down by 3·3 %. Scores for sweet taste liking for both measures and in all groups are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM3). Following adjustment and categorisation for sweet liker status, given the absence of other differences between the two groups, the study population was then treated as single sample with an N of 242.

Analyses one

Primary outcomes: % free sugar intakes and adherence (week 12, change from baseline to week 12)

Data for %FS at week 12, adherence at week 12 and change in %FS over the 12-week study period, per intervention group are given in Table 2. Results from primary and secondary regression models are given in Tables 35, with correlation coefficients to identify additional variables for secondary models provided in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM4 and SM5). Both primary outcomes were significantly predicted by the regression models. Lower %FS at week 12 was associated with being in an intervention group v. control (B = –0·573, P = 0·03) and lower %FS at baseline (B = 0·311, P < 0·01). Greater adherence was associated with greater %FS at baseline (B = 0·181, P < 0·01). A greater reduction in %FS over the 12 weeks was associated with intervention v. control (B = 0·610, P = 0·03), greater %FS at baseline (B = 0·631, P < 0·1) and a greater reduction in %TEI from carbohydrate (B = 0·105, P < 0·01). For all three outcomes, regression analyses using secondary models in only the three intervention groups revealed no significant differences between interventions (week 12 (W12) %FS intakes: B = −0·310, P = 0·46; adherence: B = 0·005, P = 0·98; change in %FS: B = 0·352, P = 0·42).

Table 2. Percent free sugar intakes (mean (standard error)) at baseline, week 12, change in percent free sugar intakes over the 12-week study period and adherence at week 12 (n (%), per intervention group (n 242), statistically significant differences between the three intervention groups and control in % free sugar intakes at week 12 and change in % free sugar intakes from baseline to week 12 (P < 0·05), different letters signify significant differences between groups, as gained from regression analyses

N, nutrient-based recommendations; NF, nutrient- and food-based recommendations; NFS, nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations; W12, week 12.

Table 3. Primary and secondary regression models for %FS at week 12*

%FS, free sugar intakes as a percentage of TEI; BL, baseline; BW, body weight; W12, Week 12; TEI, total energy intake; PA, physical activity; %TEI CHO, percent energy consumed from carbohydrate; %TEI Protein, percent energy consumed from protein.

* Primary models investigated effects of intervention/control group (control = 0, N =1, NF = 2, NFS = 3) and included adjustment for gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 TEI and W12 physical activity; secondary models consisted of primary models plus correlating secondary outcomes. Statistically significant effects are given in bold. Correlation coefficients to identify additional variables for secondary models are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM4 and SM5).

Additional variables: W12 %TEI CHO; W12 %TEI Protein.

Table 4. Primary and secondary regression models for adherence data at week 12*

%FS, free sugar intakes as a percentage of TEI; BL, baseline; BW, body weight; W12, Week 12; TEI, total energy intake; PA, physical activity; FCQ, food choice questionnaire; %TEI CHO, percent energy consumed from carbohydrate; %TEI Protein, percent energy consumed from protein.

* Primary models for adherence used binary models – adherent v. non-adherent, to investigate effects of intervention/control group (control = 0, N = 1, NF = 2, NFS = 3) and included adjustment for gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 TEI and W12 physical activity, secondary models consisted of primary models plus correlating secondary outcomes. Statistically significant effects are given in bold. Correlation coefficients to identify additional variables for secondary models are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM4 and SM5).

Additional variables: BL Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ) Natural content factor; W12 %TEI CHO; W12 %TEI Protein. W12 %FS not included due to concerns over multi-co-linearity.

Table 5. Primary and secondary regression models for change in %FS over the 12-week period*

%FS, free sugar intakes as a percentage of TEI; BL, baseline; BW, body weight; W12, Week 12; TEI, total energy intake; PA, physical activity; %TEI CHO, percent energy consumed from carbohydrate.

* Primary models investigated effects of intervention/control group (control = 0, N = 1, NF = 2, NFS = 3) and included adjustment for gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, change (baseline to W12) in TEI and change (baseline to week 12) physical activity, secondary models consisted of primary models plus correlating secondary outcomes. Statistically significant effects are given in bold. Correlation coefficients to identify additional variables for secondary models are given in the Supplementary online Materials (Tables SM4 and SM5).

Additional variables: Baseline %TEI CHO; Change %TEI CHO.

Secondary outcomes: dietary profiles: total energy intake, diet composition and food consumption (week 12, change from baseline to week 12)

Data on TEI, diet composition and high-sugar, medium-sugar, low/no-sugar and LCS-sweetened food consumption are given in Table 6

Table 6. Daily total energy intake (TEI), diet composition (%TEI from carbohydrate (CHO) (%), protein (%), fat (%), saturated fat (%), fibre (g), Na (mg)) and high-sugar, medium-sugar, low/no-sugar and LCS-sweetened food consumption, in number of food items consumed and grams of food consumed/day (mean (standard error)) at baseline, week 12 and change from baseline to week 12, per intervention group (n 242), no statistically significant differences between control and intervention groups

N, nutrient-based recommendations; NF, nutrient- and food-based recommendations; NFS, nutrient-, food- and food-substitution-based recommendations; TEI, total energy intake; W12, Week 12.

Results from the primary and secondary regression models for TEI data at Week 12 and for data on change from baseline to Week 12 are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM6). In all models, TEI at Week 12 and for change from baseline to Week 12 were predicted, where baseline TEI was a significant predictor (smallest B = 0·232, P < 0·01) alongside various measures of diet composition. There were no associations with intervention/control (most significant B = 3·689, P = 0·86).

Regression models for diet composition data at Week 12 and for change from baseline to Week 12 are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM7 and SM8 respectively). All models were statistically significant, with the exception of the secondary model for percent TEI consumed from fat at week 12. In all other models, except the secondary model for Na intake at week 12, baseline consumption was a significant predictor (smallest B = 0·115, P = 0·04) and for all outcomes except those for fat and saturated fat, TEI at week 12 or change in TEI from baseline to TEI was a significant predictor (smallest B = –0·006, P = 0·03). Some other measures of diet composition and some demographic variables were also significant predictors. There were no associations with intervention/control (most significant B = 0·373, P = 0·37).

Regression models for the food consumption data at Week 12 and for data on change from baseline to Week 12 are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM9 and SM10 respectively). Models for all food consumption measures at week 12 (items and grams) were not significant. All models for change in food consumption from baseline to week 12 (items and grams) were significant, where change in food consumption was negatively associated with baseline consumption (smallest B = –0·954, P < 0·01).

Secondary outcomes: anthropometry (week 12, change from baseline to week 12)

Data on BW, BMI and waist circumference over the 12-week period are given in Table 7.

Table 7. Body weight (kg), BMI (kg/m2) and waist circumference (cm) (mean (standard error)) at baseline, week 12, and change from baseline to week 12, per intervention group (n 242), statistically significant differences in body weight between control and the three intervention groups at week 12 (P < 0·05), different letters signify significant differences between groups, as gained from regression analyses

N, nutrient-based recommendations; NF, nutrient- and food-based recommendations; NFS, nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations; W12, Week 12.

Results from the primary and secondary regression models for data at Week 12 and for data on change from baseline to Week 12 are given in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM11 and SM12, respectively). In secondary models, all anthropometry outcomes at Week 12 and for change from baseline to Week 12 were predicted. For all outcomes at Week 12, baseline variable was a significant predictor (smallest B = 0·885, P < 0·01). In addition, intervention/control group was a significant predictor for BW at week 12 (B = –0·377, P = 0·047), where BW was lower in intervention groups compared with control. No significant differences between interventions were found (B = –0·251. P = 0·40). Change in anthropometry over the 12-week period was associated with baseline characteristics, other aspects of anthropometry and some dietary measures. There were no associations with intervention/control (most significant B = 0·283, P = 0·13).

Secondary outcomes: effects in %free sugar intakes and adherence at weeks 1, 2, 4, 8 and 12

Effects in %FS and adherence at different time points are demonstrated in Figures 2 and 3; data and results of the regression analyses are provided in the online Supplementary Materials (Tables SM13SM15). Effects of intervention v. control were found in %FS at all time points (Week 1: B = –0·791, P < 0·01; Week 2: B = –0·921, P < 0·01; Week 4: B = –0·659, P = 0·02; Week 8: B = –0·668, P = 0·01; Week 12: B = –0·573, P = 0·03), and in adherence at weeks 1 (B = 0·418, P < 0·01) and 8 (B = 0·314, P = 0·03). Analyses of only the three intervention groups revealed no differences based on intervention, except in %FS at Week 2 (B = –0·836, P = 0·04), where group NFS had a lower %FS than groups N and NF. %FS and adherence at each time point were also associated with baseline %FS (smallest B = 0·138, P < 0·01), and high correlations were found between %FS at all time points (smallest r= 0·448, P < 0·01).

Figure 2. Time course effects in % free sugar intakes. %FS (mean, standard error) shown at weeks 1, 2, 4, 8 and 12 in Group N (nutrient-based recommendations) (n 61), Group NF (nutrient- and food-based recommendations) (n 60), Group NFS (nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations) (n 63) and the control group (n 58), statistically significant differences between control and all intervention groups at all time points, taking account of gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 total energy intake (TEI), W12 physical activity, W12 %TEI carbohydrate and W12 %TEI Protein (P < 0·05), statistically significant differences between intervention groups N, NF and NFS at week 2, taking account of gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 TEI, W12 physical activity, W12 %TEI carbohydrate and W12 %TEI protein (P < 0·05).

Figure 3. Time course effects in adherence. Adherence (% of n) shown at weeks 1, 2, 4, 8 and 12 in Group N (Nutrient-based recommendations) (n 61), Group NF (nutrient- and food-based recommendations) (n 60), Group NFS (nutrient-, food-, and food-substitution-based recommendations) (n 63) and the control group (n 58), statistically significant differences between control and all intervention groups at weeks 1 and 8, taking account of gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 total energy intake (TEI), W12 physical activity, Baseline Food Choice Natural content scale, W12 %TEI sugars, W12 %TEI carbohydrate and W12 %TEI Protein (P < 0·05).

Exploratory analyses: change in % free sugar intakes (change from baseline to week 12)

Considering our interest in %FS and the association in change in %FS with change in %TEI from carbohydrates, additional analyses investigated change in %FS using all baseline and change variables for diet composition. Results are shown in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM16). A greater reduction in %FS was associated with being in an intervention group v. control (B = 0·605, P = 0·03), greater baseline %FS (B = 0·677, P < 0·01), greater baseline %TEI from protein (B = 0·207, P = 0·04), a greater reduction in %TEI from carbohydrate (B = 0·106, P < 0·01) and an increase in %TEI from protein (B = –0·193, P = 0·01). Considering %FS contributes to %TEI from carbohydrates, analyses were also redone using %TEI from intrinsic sugars and %TEI from non-sugar carbohydrates in place of %TEI from carbohydrates (see also online Supplementary Table SM16). A greater reduction in %FS was associated with intervention v. control (B = 0·537, P = 0·04), greater baseline %FS (B = 0·693, P < 0·01), greater baseline %TEI from protein (B = 0·217, P = 0·02), a greater reduction in %TEI from non-sugar carbohydrate (B = 0·141, P < 0·01) and an increase in %TEI from protein (B = -0·171, P = 0·02).

Considering our interest in the use of nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations to enable change in %FS, exploratory analyses also investigated change in %FS using baseline and change in sugar and LCS-sweetened food consumption. Results are shown in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM16). A greater reduction in %FS was associated with being in an intervention group v. control (B = 0·643, P = 0·03) and greater baseline %FS (B = 0·626, P < 0·01) but not with sugar-sweetened or LCS-sweetened food consumed (largest B = –0·061, P = 0·11).

Exploratory analyses: BW, BMI and waist circumference (week 12)

Considering our interest in anthropometry and the effects of group on BW at week 12, exploratory analyses were also conducted to investigate any changes in diet composition that were associated with BW, BMI and waist circumference. Regression models are provided in the online Supplementary Materials (Table SM17). Limited effects were found. BW at week 12 was associated with age (B = 0·033, P = 0·05) and baseline BW (B = 0·044, P < 0·01), BMI at week 12 was associated only with age (B = 0·012, P = 0·04), and waist circumference was associated with baseline waist circumference (B = 0·081, P = 0·02), baseline %TEI from fat (B = 0·401, P = 0·01) and change in TEI (B = 0·002, P = 0·04). Similar effects were found if %TEI from carbohydrates was separated as %TEI from intrinsic sugars and %TEI from non-sugar carbohydrates (online Supplementary Table SM17).

Analyses two

Analyses for %FS, adherence, TEI, BW, BMI and WC were undertaken using secondary models as defined above, at Week 12 and for change from baseline to Week 12, in the following population sub-groups: females only (n 214); lean individuals (BMI <= 25·00 kg/m2) (n 85); individuals with overweight (BMI 25·01–30·00 kg/m2) (n 85); individuals with obesity (BMI > 30·01 kg/m2) (n 72); sweet likers (n 91); those with an inverted U-shaped sweet liking function (n 90); sweet dislikers (n 61). Full description of the groups, results and commentary are given in the Supplementary Materials (including online Supplementary Tables SM18SM31). Analyses based on correctly v. mistakenly reporting adherence (active adherent (n 126) v. passive non-adherent (n 84)) are also given (online Supplementary Table SM32).

To highlight any differences based on intervention/control, in females only, %FS at Week 12, change in %FS and BW at week 12 were associated with being in an intervention group v. control (smallest B = –0·422, P = 0·04), but no differences between interventions were found. Mean (se) %FS at week 12 for each group was Control = 8·9 (0·8) %, N = 7·8 (0·7) %, NF = 7·6 (0·8) % and NFS = 6·9 (0·6) %. Mean (se) change in %FS for each group was Control = −1·3 (0·8) %, N = −2·7 (0·8) %, NF = −2·9 (0·8) % and NFS = −3·5 (0·7) %. Mean (se) BW at week 12 for each group was Control = 76·2 (2·4) kg, N = 74·0 (2·1) kg, NF = 78·1 (2·4) kg and NFS = 73·0 (1·9) kg, with a mean (se) change in BW from baseline to week 12 for each group of Control = −0·2 (0·4) kg, N = −0·6 (0·4) kg, NF = −1·5 (0·5) kg and NFS = −1·3 (0·5) kg. In participants with overweight, BMI at week 12 was associated with intervention v. control (B = –0·195, P = 0·04), but no effects of intervention type were found. In individuals with obesity, BW at week 12 was associated with intervention v. control (B = 1·036, P = 0·04), but no differences between interventions were found. In those with an inverted U-shaped sweet liking function, effects of intervention v. control were found in BW and BMI at week 12 (least significant B = –0·622, P = 0·02), but no effects of intervention type were found. In sweet dislikers, effects of intervention v. control were found in change in %FS (B = 1·283, P 0·02) with a marginal difference between interventions (B = 1·588, P = 0·052). Mean (se) change in %FS for each group was Control = −1·4 (1·5) %, N = −0·7 (1·2) %, NF = −3·8 (1·7) % and NFS = −3·5 (1·4) %.

Discussion

This randomised controlled trial investigated the effects of nutrient-, nutrient- and food-, and nutrient-, food- and food-substitution-based dietary recommendations v. control for reducing FS intakes, on FS intakes, various measures of diet and anthropometry, in UK adults consuming > 5 % TEI from FS. A total of 242 participants were recruited, with a mean %FS at baseline comparable to that reported in the UK population as a whole(59), and 200 (83 %) participants completed the study. In Intention-to-Treat analyses, adjusted for baseline and select additional measures, we found reduced %FS in all intervention groups compared with control, reductions in %FS at week 1 that remained until week 12, few differences between interventions, some effects in diet composition, but limited effects in measures of food consumption. We also found a lower BW in intervention groups compared with control at week 12, no effects in BMI or waist circumference, no adverse events, and few differences in population sub-groups based on BW or sweet-liker status.

FS intakes were reduced in all intervention groups throughout the study to result in reductions by week 12 of 2·5 % to 3·3 % of TEI. Based on a standard deviation of 4·8 %, as gained from the sample as a whole at baseline, our findings represent moderate effect sizes from 0·52–0·69 sds. These effect sizes are consistent with and larger than the effect sizes reported in meta-analyses of reductions in sugar intake following a range of public health strategies(Reference Vargas-Garcia, Evans and Prestwich19), but smaller than those that have previously been reported with more intensive dietary manipulations(Reference Piernas, Tate and Wang15Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz17). Participants in the CHOICE trial report a reduction in FS as a percentage of TEI of 5·2–5·7 % over a comparable time period(Reference Piernas, Tate and Wang15,Reference Tate, Turner-McGrievy and Lyons16) , and Ebbeling and colleagues report reductions of 7·3–9·9 % TEI over 12 months(Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz17). Participants in the more intensive studies, however, also typically consumed a higher percentage of TEI from FS at the study start (from 14·5 % to 27 % TEI) and greater reductions in those with higher baseline intakes is an effect we also find.

Our effects were achieved using a low-intensity single time point intervention. Effects were found furthermore from week 1 but were seen to reduce over the 12-week period. Our participants were willing volunteers, eager to make a dietary change, thus early effects may be unsurprising. Psychological theories recognise, and numerous studies demonstrate, the importance of motivation for behaviour change(Reference Deci and Ryan60Reference Ng, Ntoumanis and Thøgersen-Ntoumani63), and a potential impact of participant motivation on our findings cannot be denied. Our participants were also monitoring their food intakes throughout the study, and while this was the case for all participants (regardless of intervention/control allocation), various studies also demonstrate the impact of dietary recording for increasing awareness of dietary intakes, to result in dietary change(Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow28,Reference Elfhag and Rössner64Reference Ingels, Misra and Stewart66) . These factors, however, do not diminish the value of our findings. Our trial was designed to investigate effects in a public health setting; thus, while effects may be smaller than those of more intensive interventions, our findings can be considered achievable by the general public, and our interventions would likely have greater reach than can be gained from more intensive protocols.

Few differences were found between interventions. These findings may suggest limited added value to the additional information provided in the food-based and food-substitution-based recommendations, compared with that provided on nutrients. Secondary analyses further reveal no differences between intervention groups in sugar-rich or LCS-sweetened food consumption, as targeted differently for the differing groups. However, while no changes at the food level were found, exploratory analyses on diet composition do reveal associations between a reduced intake of FS, a reduced intake of energy consumed from non-sugar carbohydrates and an increase in energy consumed from protein, with no effects found in energy consumed from fat, or in fibre or Na intakes. These findings suggest that participants were specifically targeting sugars and high carbohydrate foods, by reducing their consumption of food items such as sugar-sweetened beverages, confectionary and desserts, or replacing these with non-sweetened or naturally-sweetened food sources, such as whole fruit and milk-based beverages and desserts such as low-sugar yoghurts. Dietary profiles lower in FS and higher in protein intakes were also found in the CHOICE trial(Reference Piernas, Tate and Wang15,Reference Tate, Turner-McGrievy and Lyons16) and will be beneficial for health from both dietary changes(2,4) . Adverse effects on dietary profiles, e.g., through increases in fat or salt consumption as a result of the replacement of high-sugar foods with high-fat savoury foods, were not found.

The lack of clear effects at the food level suggests that changes in the individual foods consumed were small and varied among participants, but over the diet as a whole and when deconstructed as dietary components resulted in effects that were detected. These findings likely reflect the unconstrained nature of our interventions, where participants in all intervention groups were free to reduce their consumption of high-sugar foods and increase their consumption of non-sweetened or LCS-sweetened foods as they wished whether they were given this advice or not. Indeed, the absence of effects of intervention v. control in all measures of food consumption suggests few differences between groups. These findings suggest also that participants undertook only small changes to their diets and may suggest benefit to retaining the inclusive nature of an intervention by offering participants multiple solutions and free choice. The only difference between interventions in our study demonstrates a greater reduction in %FS at week 2 from the nutrient, food- and food-substitution-based recommendations. These findings may suggest some benefit to recommending food substitutions(Reference Piernas, Tate and Wang15Reference Wise, Nattress and Flammer18), but these recommendations also provided many small behavioural changes that offered choice. Other researchers suggest value from small dietary changes in relation to BW and BW loss(Reference Hill67Reference Rodearmel, Wyatt and Stroebele70), and Stroebele et al. (Reference Stroebele, de Castro and Stuht71) demonstrate value specifically for sugar intakes. The behavioural changes implemented in some sugar reduction studies that have yielded success can also be suggested as small (e.g. replacement of 1 sugar-sweetened beverage per day)(Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz17), and small changes are often preferred for product reformulation to avoid impacts on acceptability(Reference Hashem, He and MacGregor14,Reference MacGregor and Hashem72,Reference Yeung, Gohil and Rangan73) . Choice and autonomy have also been applauded for health behaviour change(Reference Deci and Ryan60Reference Ng, Ntoumanis and Thøgersen-Ntoumani63), and success following the implementation of these ideas has again been documented(Reference Ng, Ntoumanis and Thøgersen-Ntoumani63). Thus, on a preliminary basis, recommendations for dietary change that provide options and alternatives, for small changes to behaviour, may be appropriate at a population level. Many existing public health interventions take this approach(Reference Vargas-Garcia, Evans and Prestwich19,Reference von Philipsborn, Stratil and Burns20,Reference Bradley, Gardner and Rowland74Reference Wrieden and Levy76) , and while evaluations typically state shortcomings as a result of not knowing which elements of an intervention have caused an effect, our findings suggest that this concern may not be justified. Beyond recommendations for reducing FS intakes, our findings may further be relevant to guidelines for reducing or increasing other dietary components. Testing would be required, but various studies suggest poor adherence to dietary guidelines for several aspects of the diet(Reference Brown, Timotijevic and Barnett77Reference Yau, Adams and Monsivais79), and a lack of understanding that would be aided by practical solutions(Reference Brown, Timotijevic and Barnett77). Evidence-based strategies to increase adherence to dietary guidelines would clearly contribute positively towards health.

Effects on BW were also found at week 12, accounting for baseline BW and a range of additional variables, where intervention groups had lower BWs, having lost 0·7–1·4 kg BW over the 12-week period compared with 0·2 kg in the control group. Effect sizes are small but comparable with the effects of other interventions targeting sugar reduction(Reference Hashem, He and MacGregor14,Reference Tate, Turner-McGrievy and Lyons16,Reference Rogers and Appleton80) . Haslem et al. (Reference Hashem, He and MacGregor14) report a BW reduction of 1·04 kg in a meta-analysis of three studies on product reformulation, and we report a BW reduction of 1·06 kg in a meta-analysis of 29 studies comparing sugar with LCS consumption(Reference Rogers and Appleton80). While related to our interventions, however, changes in BW were not associated with FS intakes, or any other dietary measure. These findings may again reflect the small and varied nature of the dietary changes undertaken on an individual basis by members of our study population. Greater changes over the 12 weeks in those with a higher BW at baseline, both in our main analyses and our sub-group analyses, are consistent with the literature(Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz17,Reference Avenell, Broom and Brown81Reference Laviada-Molina, Molina-Segui and Pérez-Gaxiola83) , as are the positive associations between BW at baseline and at week 12(Reference Avenell, Broom and Brown81,Reference Hassapidou, Vlassopoulos and Kalliostra82) .

Limited additional consistent effects were found in our sub-group analyses based on population group. Effects in females reflect those found in the whole sample. The limited effects based on BMI or sweet-liker status are most plausibly explained as a result of an inability to detect small dietary changes against a backdrop of high dietary variety in small samples. The one effect found in sweet dislikers (a greater reduction in %FS in NF and NFS groups) may suggest some changes to food intakes, but sample sizes are now very low. However, the low correlations between liking perceptions for the sweet solution and the impregnated taste paper may also cast doubt on the ‘trait’ status of sweet-liker status, and while the proportions of sweet likers, sweet dislikers and those demonstrating an inverted U-shaped sweet liking function in our study population are comparable to those found in other studies(Reference Jayasinghe, Kruger and Walsh38,Reference Trumbo, Appleton and de Graaf39) , our study population also consumed only around 10 % intake from high-sugar foods and 4 % intake from LCS-sweetened foods at baseline. Further analyses on the perceptions of sweet taste in this study and other outcomes related to sweet taste will be reported elsewhere.

Strengths of our study include adherence to our pre-registered protocol(Reference Boxall, Arden-Close and James21), achievement of our estimated sample size and drop-out rate, resulting in recruitment and inclusion of a large and diverse study sample, and our use of ITT analyses on an imputed data set. Important limitations must also be considered. Firstly, our primary outcome was assessed using diet diaries, which can be subject to misreporting(Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow28Reference Trabulsi and Schoeller32). Food diaries, however, are considered an appropriate method for capturing dietary intakes in a free-living situation(29Reference Trabulsi and Schoeller32), demonstrate validity(29Reference Trabulsi and Schoeller32) and have been found to demonstrate good intra-individual reliability, as is required for investigating short-term changes over time(Reference de Castro30). Reported TEI does decrease over our 12-week assessment period, but weight loss also occurred over the study period, thus a reduction in reported TEI would be expected. All dietary recording was checked at the time of submission, by a researcher blinded to intervention group, difficulties with reporting, including difficulties with digital recording or the necessary software were remedied quickly, our bogus pipeline method and dietary consultations for taking part were intended to encourage accurate reporting, and our control group was designed to control for dietary recording. Furthermore, no group-based differences were found in TEI, while differences were found in our primary outcome (%FS), an assessment adjusted for reported energy intake, rather than an absolute quantity. We can make no comment on the possibility (or not) of sugar-specific misreporting. Considering the weight loss incurred across the study period and the desire by many participants to lose weight as a reason for taking part, standard checks for misreporting(Reference Black84,Reference Tooze, Krebs-Smith and Troiano85) on the select days when reporting was requested, furthermore, are unlikely to be valid. In the absence of a biomarker for sugar intake, we consider our methods to be the most appropriate available for assessing FS intakes in the real world. Second, calculations of FS content were only based on the information available, or calculated from products with known content, but variety between products can exist, and some of this variation will have been lost by our methods(Reference Hashem, He and MacGregor14). As above, however, there will be no systematic bias between groups or over time in our data because all dietary data were cleaned only after data collection, by a researcher blinded to treatment allocation, and foods with missing data were consistently replaced with the same alternative. LCS-sweetened foods were also classified as such, based on product description rather than product ingredient list. This description was used to identify foods that study participants were likely to have selected because they were LCS- rather than sugar-sweetened, based on the details in our recommendations, but this description may not reflect all LCS-sweetened foods and will certainly not reflect all foods that contain LCS. With over 8000 foods consumed as part of the study, and considering our study aims, the methods used were considered the most pragmatic. Again, furthermore, the method was implemented only after data collection, in all participants and over all time points equally, by a researcher blinded to treatment allocation, thus no systematic bias between groups or over time will have been introduced.

Our study sample, while large, was also not powered to detect differences smaller than 2 % TEI, and FS intakes at baseline were only slightly above the 10 % TEI recommendations of the WHO(1), thus differences between interventions may have been difficult to detect. Further study in those of higher FS intakes would be of interest. Our interventions were also not based solely on nutrient information, food-based information or food-substitution-based information, which would have resulted in a purer test of the different types of information, but the interventions were designed for use in a public health context. Our sample was diverse in terms of age, education and occupation, but was dominated by females, thus generalisation to males or the population as a whole is not possible. Females are known to be more interested in diet and health than males(Reference Grymislawska, Puch and Zawada86), thus over recruitment in this group is unsurprising, but females are also still more commonly responsible for dietary purchasing and food provision within the household(Reference Grymislawska, Puch and Zawada86,Reference Flagg, Sen and Kilgore87) (and indeed the majority of the sample reported themselves as ‘the main cook within their household’), thus effects in this group will be of value. Our study must also be considered in context. Our study was advertised as a study of dietary change, to recruit those who might be inclined to comply with our dietary change requests and so provide the best test of our hypothesis. The study thus likely attracted participants who were thinking of and motivated to change their diet, and some of our effects will likely have resulted from this motivation(Reference Deci and Ryan60Reference Ng, Ntoumanis and Thøgersen-Ntoumani63). In this respect, it is probable that the effects of our recommendations in the general population may be smaller than those identified here. This initial desire to change one’s diet may also explain the drop-out rate in the control group. Drop-out was noticeably higher in the control compared with the intervention groups, and some anecdotal comments from participants in this group suggested a dissatisfaction with the dietary recommendations that they were given. We can also make no comment on the length of time over which the effects of our interventions may last, particularly once dietary recording ceased, nor can we offer any suggestion of the value of our recommendations compared with alternative interventions offering dietary advice(Reference Rees, Dyakova and Wilson88), or to alternative types of intervention, such as changes to the environment, e.g. via product reformulation or limiting availability, and changes to public policy, such as increased taxes and legislation requiring reformulation(Reference Vargas-Garcia, Evans and Prestwich19,Reference von Philipsborn, Stratil and Burns20) .

Conclusion

In this randomised controlled trial, we sought to investigate the effects of nutrient-, food- and food-substitution-based dietary recommendations for reducing FS intakes in UK adults consuming > 5 % TEI from FS. The study was conducted in a public health context, where free-living members of a community sample were given recommendations at a single time point, with outcomes assessed over 12 weeks. FS intakes (as a percentage of TEI) reduced in all intervention groups compared with control, from week 1 to remain for 12 weeks. Few differences between interventions were found, and no differences were detected in sugar-rich and LCS-sweetened food consumption, but effects were associated with reductions in non-sugar carbohydrate consumption and increases in protein intake. Reductions in BW were also found, and no adverse events were reported. Our findings demonstrate the benefit from public health recommendations for FS intakes and suggest no universal benefit from any one strategy or another, but instead may suggest benefit from recommendations with multiple options for individual choice.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114525000339

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants who took part. We would also like to express our thanks to Millie Barbrook and Daisy Marsden, Bournemouth University, UK, for help with data collection, to Prof. CC Patterson, Queen’s University Belfast, UK, for statistical advice on the conduct of all analyses, and to Prof France Bellisle, Universite Paris 13, France; Prof Carlo La Vecchia, Universita degli Studi di Milano, Italy; and Prof. Peter Rogers, University of Bristol, UK; for comments on an early draft of the manuscript.

This work was funded by Bournemouth University, UK, and the International Sweeteners Association (ISA), BE. The funders offered limited comment on the trial design and materials but had no role in the finalisation of the trial and played no part in running the trial, interpreting the trial findings or in the decision to publish. The funders have had no direct contact with the PhD student. This trial was completed as part of a PhD studentship, funded by Bournemouth University, UK and The International Sweeteners Association, BE.

K. M. A. has received research funding from Unilever R&D Vlaardingen, NL, a consortium of the American Beverage Association, Arla Foods, Cargill R&D Centre Europe BVBA, DSM-Firmenich SA, International Sweeteners Association, SinoSweet Co., Ltd, Cosun Nutrition Center and Unilever Foods Innovation Centre Wageningen; from ILSI-North America, USA, The Coca Cola Company, USA and Ajinomoto Health and Nutrition North America, Inc., USA; K. M. A. has received speaker’s expenses from the International Sweeteners Association, BE; PepsiCo; ILSI-North America; and EatWell Global. K. M. A. conceived the trial idea; L. R. B. and K. M. A. designed the trial with input from E. A-C. and J. J. K. M. A. conducted all analyses and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors critically reviewed and revised the final version.

Other authors have no conflicts to declare.

Ethics approval was gained from the Research Ethics Committee of Bournemouth University UK (ID: 30612) on 28.04.20 (with amendments approved on 29.03.21). The work was undertaken in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1983), the Ethical Guidelines of the British Psychological Society, and Bournemouth University’s Research Ethics Code of Practice.

Declaration of Generative AI or AI-assisted technologies in the writing process: None used.

Data described in the manuscript will be made available upon request.

References

World Health Organization (2015) Guideline: Sugars Intake for Adults and Children. Geneva: WHO. https://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/healthy-diet (accessed November, 2024).Google Scholar
Health Canada (2018) Dietary Guidelines for Health Professionals and Policy Makers. Health Canada. https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/guidelines/ (accessed November, 2024).Google Scholar
Public Health England (2017) Sugar Reduction: Achieving the 20 % A Technical Report Outlining Progress to Date, Guidelines for Industry, 2015 Baseline Levels in Key Foods and Next Steps. PHE Publications. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5a82e02740f0b62305b94cea/Sugar_reduction_achieving_the_20_.pdf (accessed November, 2024).Google Scholar
U.S. Department of Agriculture & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2020) Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2020–2025, 9th Edition. https://www.dietaryguidelines.gov/sites/default/files/2020–12/Dietary_Guidelines_for_Americans_2020–2025.pdf (accessed November, 2024).Google Scholar
World Health Organisation & Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (1998) Preparation and Use of Food-Based Dietary Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Scapin, T, Fernandes, AC, Curioni, CC, et al. (2021) Influence of sugar label formats on consumer understanding and amount of sugar in food choices: a systematic review and meta-analyses. Nutr Rev 79, 788801. https://doi.org/10.1093/nutrit/nuaa108 Google Scholar
Cecchini, M & Warin, L (2016) Impact of food labelling systems on food choices and eating behaviours: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized studies. Obes Rev 17, 201210. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12364 Google Scholar
Song, J, Brown, MK, Tan, M, et al. (2021) Impact of color-coded and warning nutrition labelling schemes: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. PLoS Med 18, e1003765. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003765 Google Scholar
Hoenink, JC, Stuber, JM, Lakerveld, J, et al. (2021) The effect of on-shelf sugar labeling on beverage sales in the supermarket: a comparative interrupted time series analysis of a natural experiment. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 18, 49. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-021-01114-xGoogle Scholar
Riesenberg, D, Peeters, A, Backholer, K, et al. (2022) Exploring the effects of added sugar labels on food purchasing behaviour in Australian parents: an online randomised controlled trial. PLoS One 17, e0271435. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271435 Google Scholar
Herforth, A, Arimond, M, Álvarez-Sánchez, C, et al. (2019) A global review of food-based dietary guidelines. Adv Nutr 10, 590605. https://doi.org/10.1093/advances/nmy130 Google Scholar
Beauchamp, GK (2016) Why do we like sweet taste: a bitter tale? Physiol Behav 164(Part B), 432437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.007 Google Scholar
Drewnowski, A, Mennella, JA, Johnson, SL, et al. (2012) Sweetness and food preference. J Nutr 142, 1142S1148S. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.111.149575 Google Scholar
Hashem, KM, He, FJ & MacGregor, GA (2019) Effects of product reformulation on sugar intake and health-a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Rev 77, 181196. https://doi.org/10.1093/nutrit/nuy015 Google Scholar
Piernas, C, Tate, DF, Wang, X, et al. (2013) Does diet-beverage intake affect dietary consumption patterns? Results from the Choose Healthy Options Consciously Everyday (CHOICE) randomized clinical trial. Am J Clin Nutr 97, 604611. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.112.048405 Google Scholar
Tate, DF, Turner-McGrievy, G, Lyons, E, et al. (2012) Replacing caloric beverages with water or diet beverages for weight loss in adults: main results of the Choose Healthy Options Consciously Everyday (CHOICE) randomized clinical trial. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 555563. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.111.026278 Google Scholar
Ebbeling, CB, Feldman, HA, Steltz, SK, et al. (2020) Effects of sugar-sweetened, artificially sweetened, and unsweetened beverages on cardiometabolic risk factors, body composition, and sweet taste preference: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Heart Assoc 9, e015668. https://doi.org/10.1161/JAHA.119.015668 Google Scholar
Wise, PM, Nattress, L, Flammer, LJ, et al. (2016) Reduced dietary intake of simple sugars alters perceived sweet taste intensity but not perceived pleasantness. Am J Clin Nutr 103, 5060. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.115.112300 Google Scholar
Vargas-Garcia, EJ, Evans, CEL, Prestwich, A, et al. (2017) Interventions to reduce consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages or increase water intake: evidence from a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 18, 13501363. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12580 Google Scholar
von Philipsborn, P, Stratil, JM, Burns, J, et al. (2019) Environmental interventions to reduce the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages and their effects on health. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2019, issue 6, CD012292.Google Scholar
Boxall, LR, Arden-Close, E, James, J, et al. (2022) Protocol: the effects of nutrient- v. food- v. food-substitution-based dietary recommendations for reducing free sugar intakes, on free sugar intakes, dietary profiles and sweet taste outcomes: a randomized controlled trial. Nutr Health Online. https://doi.org/10.1177/02601060221111234 Google Scholar
Smith, KL, Kerr, DA, Howie, EK, et al. (2015) Do overweight adolescents adhere to dietary intervention messages? Twelve-month detailed dietary outcomes from Curtin University’s activity, food and attitudes program. Nutrients 7, 43634382. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7064363 Google Scholar
Whitley, E & Ball, J (2002) Statistics review 4: sample size calculations. Crit Care 6, 335341. https://doi.org/10.1186/cc1521 Google Scholar
National Health Service (2018) How to Cut Down on Sugar in Your diet – NHS. https://www.nhs.uk/live-well/eat-well/how-to-cut-down-on-sugar-in-your-diet (accessed 12 November 2019).Google Scholar
National Health Service (2019) Top Sources of Added Sugar in our Diet – NHS. https://www.nhs.uk/live-well/eat-well/top-sources-of-added-sugar/ (accessed 26 January 2021).Google Scholar
National Cancer Institute (2021) Learn More about Usual Dietary Intake. Dietary Assessment Primer. https://dietassessmentprimer.cancer.gov/learn/usual.html (accessed 20 January 2021).Google Scholar
Stubbs, RJ, O’Reilly, LM, Whybrow, S, et al. (2014) Measuring the difference between actual and reported food intakes in the context of energy balance under laboratory conditions. Br J Nutr 111, 20322043. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114514000154 Google Scholar
Nutritics (2019) Research Edition. https://www.nutritics.com/en (accessed 06 January 2021).Google Scholar
de Castro, JM (2006) Varying levels of food energy self-reporting are associated with between group but not within subjects differences in food intake. J Nutr 136, 13821388. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/136.5.1382 Google Scholar
Hooson, J, Hutchinson, J, Warthon-Medina, M, et al. (2020) A systematic review of reviews identifying UK validated dietary assessment tools for inclusion on an interactive guided website for researchers: www.nutritools.org. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 60, 1265. https://doi.org/10.1080/10408398.2019.1566207 Google Scholar
Trabulsi, J & Schoeller, DA (2001) Evaluation of dietary assessment instruments against doubly labeled water, a biomarker of habitual energy intake. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 281, E891E889. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpendo.2001.281.5.E891 Google Scholar
Raatz, SK, Scheett, AJ, Johnson, LK, et al. (2015) Validity of electronic diet recording nutrient estimates compared to dietitian analysis of diet records: randomized controlled trial. J Med Int Res 17, e21. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3744 Google Scholar
National Health Service (2022) Food Labels. https://www.nhs.uk/live-well/eat-well/food-guidelines-and-food-labels/how-to-read-food-labels/ (accessed 20 January 2021).Google Scholar
Bartoshuk, LM, Duffy, VB, Fast, K, et al. (2002) Labeled scales (e.g. category, Likert, VAS) and invalid across-group comparisons: what we have learned from genetic variation in taste. Food Qual Pref 14, 125138. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0950-3293(02)00077-0Google Scholar
Bartoshuk, LM, Fast, K & Snyder, DJ (2005) Differences in our sensory worlds: invalid comparisons with labeled scales. Curr Directions Psychol Sci 14, 122125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00346.x Google Scholar
Iatridi, V, Hayes, JE & Yeomans, MR (2019) Quantifying sweet taste liker phenotypes: time for some consistency in the classification criteria. Nutrients 11, 1. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11010129 Google Scholar
Jayasinghe, SN, Kruger, R, Walsh, DCI, et al. (2017) Is sweet taste perception associated with sweet food liking and intake? Nutrients 9, 7. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9070750 Google Scholar
Trumbo, PR, Appleton, KM, de Graaf, K, et al. (2021) Perspective: measuring sweetness in foods, beverages, and diets: toward understanding the role of sweetness in health. Adv Nutr 12, 343354. https://doi.org/10.1093/advances/nmaa151 Google Scholar
Drewnowski, A, Kristal, A & Cohen, J (2001) Genetic taste responses to 6-n-propylthiouracil among adults: a screening tool for epidemiological studies. Chem Senses 26, 483489. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/26.5.483 Google Scholar
Tang, CS, Mars, M, James, J, et al. (2024) Associations between attitudes towards and intakes of sweet-tasting foods, sugar, and sweeteners in the United Kingdom. Appetite 194, 107169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2023.107169 Google Scholar
Tang, CS, Mars, M, James, J, et al. (2021) Sweet Talk: a qualitative study exploring attitudes towards sugars, sweeteners and sweet-tasting foods in the United Kingdom. Foods 10, 1172. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods10061172 Google Scholar
Karlsson, J, Persson, LO, Sjöström, L, et al. (2000) Psychometric properties and factor structure of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men and women. Results from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study. Int J Obes 24, 17151725. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0801442 Google Scholar
Steptoe, A, Pollard, TM & Wardle, J (1995) Development of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite 25, 267284. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1995.0061 Google Scholar
Godin, G (2011) The Godin-Shephard leisure-time physical activity questionnaire. Health Fitness J Can 4, 1822.Google Scholar
Godin, G & Shephard, R (1985) A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Can J Appl Sport Sci 10, 141146.Google Scholar
Ware, JE, Snow, KK, Kosinski, M, et al. (1993) SF-36 Health Survey Manual & Interpretation Guide. Boston, MA: The Health Institute, New England Medical Center.Google Scholar
RAND (2021) 36-Item Short Form Survey (SF-36). https://www.rand.org/health-care/surveys_tools/mos/36-item-short-form/scoring.html (accessed 24 January 2021).Google Scholar
Jenkinson, C, Coulter, A & Wright, L (1993) Short form 36 (SF 36) health survey questionnaire: normative data for adults of working age. BMJ 306(6890), 14371440. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.306.6890.1437 Google Scholar
Qualtrics.XM Qualtrics (2020). www.qualtrics.com (accessed 19 November 2024).Google Scholar
Adams, J, Parkinson, L, Sanson-Fisher, RW, et al. (2008) Enhancing self-report of adolescent smoking: the effects of bogus pipeline and anonymity. Addict Behav 33, 12911296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.004 Google Scholar
Reid, M, Hammersley, R, Duffy, M, et al. (2014) Effects on obese women of the sugar sucrose added to the diet over 28d: a quasi-randomised, single-blind, controlled trial. Br J Nutr 111, 563570. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114513002687 Google Scholar
Strang, E & Peterson, ZD (2020) Use of a bogus pipeline to detect men’s underreporting of sexually aggressive behavior. J Interpersonal Violence 35, 208232. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260516681157 Google Scholar
Li, P & Stuart, EA (2019) Best (but oft-forgotten) practices: missing data methods in randomized controlled nutrition trials. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 504. https://doi.org/10.1093/AJCN/NQY271 Google Scholar
Marshall, A, Altman, DG, Holder, RL, et al. (2009) Combining estimates of interest in prognostic modelling studies after multiple imputation: current practice and guidelines. BMC Med Res Method 9, 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-9-57/TABLES/2 Google Scholar
Howell, DC (1997) Statistical Methods for Psychology, 4th ed. London: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Torgerson, DJ & Torgerson, CJ (2008) Designing Randomised Clinical Trials in the Health, Education and Social Sciences: An Introduction. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Vickers, AJ & Altman, DG (2001) Analysing controlled trials with baseline and follow up measurements. BMJ 323(7321), 11231124.Google Scholar
Public Health England (2020) National Diet and Nutrition Survey Rolling Programme Years 9 to 11 (2016/2017 to 2018/2019). https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5fd23324e90e07662b09d91a/NDNS_UK_Y9–11_report.pdf (accessed 12 September 2023).Google Scholar
Deci, EL & Ryan, RM (1985) Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Deci, EL & Ryan, RM (2000) The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inq 11, 227268.Google Scholar
Hagger, MS, Hardcastle, SJ, Chater, A, et al. (2014) Autonomous and controlled motivational regulations for multiple health-related behaviors: between- and within-participants analyses. Health Psychol Behav Med 2, 565601.Google Scholar
Ng, JY, Ntoumanis, N, Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C, et al. (2012) Self-determination theory applied to health contexts: a meta-analysis. Perspect Psychol Sci 7, 325340. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612447309 Google Scholar
Elfhag, K & Rössner, S (2005) Who succeeds in maintaining weight loss? A conceptual review of factors associated with weight loss maintenance and weight regain. Obes Rev 6, 6785. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2005.00170.x Google Scholar
Hollis, JF, Gullion, CM, Stevens, VJ, et al. (2008) Weight loss maintenance trial research group. Weight loss during the intensive intervention phase of the weight-loss maintenance trial. Am J Prev Med 35, 118126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2008.04.013 Google Scholar
Ingels, JS, Misra, R, Stewart, J, et al. (2017) The effect of adherence to dietary tracking on weight loss: using HLM to model weight loss over time. J Diabetes Res 2017, 6951495. https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/6951495 Google Scholar
Hill, JO (2009) Can a small-changes approach help address the obesity epidemic? A report of the joint task force of the American Society for Nutrition, Institute of Food Technologists, and International Food Information Council. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 477484.Google Scholar
Hills, AP, Byrne, NM, Lindstrom, R, et al. (2013) ‘Small changes’ to diet and physical activity behaviors for weight management. Obes Facts 6, 228238. https://doi.org/10.1159/000345030 Google Scholar
Rodearmel, SJ, Wyatt, HR, Barry, MJ, et al. (2006) A family-based approach to preventing excessive weight gain. Obesity 14, 13921401. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2006.158 Google Scholar
Rodearmel, SJ, Wyatt, HR, Stroebele, N, et al. (2007) Small changes in dietary sugar and physical activity as an approach to preventing excessive weight gain: the America on the Move Family Study. Pediatrics 120, 869879.Google Scholar
Stroebele, N, de Castro, JM, Stuht, J, et al. (2009) A small-changes approach reduces energy intake in free-living humans. J Am Coll Nutr 28, 6368. https://doi.org/10.1080/07315724.2009 Google Scholar
MacGregor, GA & Hashem, KM (2014) Action on sugar-lessons from UK salt reduction programme. Lancet 383, 929931. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60200-2 Google Scholar
Yeung, CHC, Gohil, P, Rangan, AM, et al. (2017) Modelling of the impact of universal added sugar reduction through food reformulation. Sci Rep 7, 17392. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17417-8 Google Scholar
Bradley, J, Gardner, G, Rowland, MK, et al. (2020) Impact of a health marketing campaign on sugars intake by children aged 5–11 years and parental views on reducing children’s consumption. BMC Public Health 20, 331. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-8422-5 Google Scholar
Lamport, DJ, Wu, SY, Drever-Heaps, J, et al. (2021) Can public health interventions change immediate and long-term dietary behaviours? Encouraging evidence from a pilot study of the U.K. Change4Life sugar swaps campaign. Nutrients 14, 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010068 Google Scholar
Wrieden, WL & Levy, LB (2016) ‘Change4Life Smart Swaps‘: quasi-experimental evaluation of a natural experiment. Pub Health Nutr 19, 23882392. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980016000513 Google Scholar
Brown, KA, Timotijevic, L, Barnett, J, et al. (2011) A review of consumer awareness, understanding and use of food-based dietary guidelines. Br J Nutr 106, 1526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114511000250 Google Scholar
Leme, ACB, Hou, S, Fisberg, RM, et al. (2021) Adherence to food-based dietary guidelines: a systemic review of high-income and low-and middle-income countries. Nutrients 13, 1038. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13031038 Google Scholar
Yau, A, Adams, J & Monsivais, P (2019) Time trends in adherence to UK dietary recommendations and associated sociodemographic inequalities, 1986–2012: a repeated cross-sectional analysis. Eur J Clin Nutr 73, 9971005. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41430-018-0347-z Google Scholar
Rogers, PJ & Appleton, KM (2021) The effects of low-calorie sweeteners on energy intake and body weight: a systematic review and meta-analyses of sustained intervention studies. Int J Obes 45, 464478.Google Scholar
Avenell, A, Broom, I, Brown, T, et al. (2004) Systematic review of the long-term effects and economic consequences of treatments for obesity and implications for health improvement. Health Technol Assess 8, 21.Google Scholar
Hassapidou, M, Vlassopoulos, A, Kalliostra, M, et al. (2023) European Association for the study of obesity position statement on medical nutrition therapy for the management of overweight and obesity in adults developed in collaboration with the European federation of the associations of dietitians. Obes Facts 16, 1128. https://doi.org/10.1159/000528083 Google Scholar
Laviada-Molina, H, Molina-Segui, F, Pérez-Gaxiola, G, et al. (2020) Effects of nonnutritive sweeteners on body weight and BMI in diverse clinical contexts: systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 21, e13020.Google Scholar
Black, AE (2000) Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake: basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations. Int J Obes 24, 11191130. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0801376 Google Scholar
Tooze, JA, Krebs-Smith, SM, Troiano, RP, et al. (2012) The accuracy of the Goldberg method for classifying misreporters of energy intake on a food frequency questionnaire and 24-h recalls: comparison with doubly labeled water. Eur J Clin Nutr 66, 569576. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2011.198 Google Scholar
Grymislawska, M, Puch, EA, Zawada, A, et al. (2020) Do nutritional behaviors depend on biological sex and cultural gender? Adv Clin Exp Med 29, 165172. https://doi.org/10.17219/acem/111817 Google Scholar
Flagg, LA, Sen, B, Kilgore, M, et al. (2014) The influence of gender, age, education and household size on meal preparation and food shopping responsibilities. Public Health Nutr 17, 20612070. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980013002267 Google Scholar
Rees, K, Dyakova, M, Wilson, N, et al. (2013) Dietary advice for reducing cardiovascular risk. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2013, issue 12, CD002128. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD002128.pub5 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. CONSORT Diagram demonstrating flow through the study.

Figure 1

Table 1. Participant characteristics (gender, age, body weight, BMI, and %FS) (n or mean (standard deviation)) for the sample as a whole and for each intervention/control group (n 242)

Figure 2

Table 2. Percent free sugar intakes (mean (standard error)) at baseline, week 12, change in percent free sugar intakes over the 12-week study period and adherence at week 12 (n (%), per intervention group (n 242), statistically significant differences between the three intervention groups and control in % free sugar intakes at week 12 and change in % free sugar intakes from baseline to week 12 (P < 0·05), different letters signify significant differences between groups, as gained from regression analyses

Figure 3

Table 3. Primary and secondary regression models for %FS at week 12*

Figure 4

Table 4. Primary and secondary regression models for adherence data at week 12*

Figure 5

Table 5. Primary and secondary regression models for change in %FS over the 12-week period*

Figure 6

Table 6. Daily total energy intake (TEI), diet composition (%TEI from carbohydrate (CHO) (%), protein (%), fat (%), saturated fat (%), fibre (g), Na (mg)) and high-sugar, medium-sugar, low/no-sugar and LCS-sweetened food consumption, in number of food items consumed and grams of food consumed/day (mean (standard error)) at baseline, week 12 and change from baseline to week 12, per intervention group (n 242), no statistically significant differences between control and intervention groups

Figure 7

Table 7. Body weight (kg), BMI (kg/m2) and waist circumference (cm) (mean (standard error)) at baseline, week 12, and change from baseline to week 12, per intervention group (n 242), statistically significant differences in body weight between control and the three intervention groups at week 12 (P < 0·05), different letters signify significant differences between groups, as gained from regression analyses

Figure 8

Figure 2. Time course effects in % free sugar intakes. %FS (mean, standard error) shown at weeks 1, 2, 4, 8 and 12 in Group N (nutrient-based recommendations) (n 61), Group NF (nutrient- and food-based recommendations) (n 60), Group NFS (nutrient-, food- and food substitution-based recommendations) (n 63) and the control group (n 58), statistically significant differences between control and all intervention groups at all time points, taking account of gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 total energy intake (TEI), W12 physical activity, W12 %TEI carbohydrate and W12 %TEI Protein (P < 0·05), statistically significant differences between intervention groups N, NF and NFS at week 2, taking account of gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 TEI, W12 physical activity, W12 %TEI carbohydrate and W12 %TEI protein (P < 0·05).

Figure 9

Figure 3. Time course effects in adherence. Adherence (% of n) shown at weeks 1, 2, 4, 8 and 12 in Group N (Nutrient-based recommendations) (n 61), Group NF (nutrient- and food-based recommendations) (n 60), Group NFS (nutrient-, food-, and food-substitution-based recommendations) (n 63) and the control group (n 58), statistically significant differences between control and all intervention groups at weeks 1 and 8, taking account of gender, age, baseline %FS, baseline body weight, W12 total energy intake (TEI), W12 physical activity, Baseline Food Choice Natural content scale, W12 %TEI sugars, W12 %TEI carbohydrate and W12 %TEI Protein (P < 0·05).

Supplementary material: File

Boxall et al. supplementary material

Boxall et al. supplementary material
Download Boxall et al. supplementary material(File)
File 752.7 KB