Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T08:14:46.750Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonological characteristics of novel gesture production in children with developmental language disorder: Longitudinal findings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2021

Laiah Factor*
Affiliation:
Callier Center for Communication Disorders, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson, TX, USA
Lisa Goffman
Affiliation:
Callier Center for Communication Disorders, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson, TX, USA
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Children with developmental language disorder (DLD; aka specific language impairment) are characterized based on deficits in language, especially morphosyntax, in the absence of other explanatory conditions. However, deficits in speech production, as well as fine and gross motor skill, have also been observed, implicating both the linguistic and motor systems. Situated at the intersection of these domains, and providing insight into both, is manual gesture. In the current work, we asked whether children with DLD showed phonological deficits in the production of novel gestures and whether gesture production at 4 years of age is related to language and motor outcomes two years later. Twenty-eight children (14 with DLD) participated in a two-year longitudinal novel gesture production study. At the first and final time points, language and fine motor skills were measured and gestures were analyzed for phonological feature accuracy, including handshape, path, and orientation. Results indicated that, while early deficits in phonological accuracy did not persist for children with DLD, all children struggled with orientation while handshape was the most accurate. Early handshape and orientation accuracy were also predictive of later language skill, but only for the children with DLD. Theoretical and clinical implications of these findings are discussed.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Developmental language disorder (DLD; also known as specific language impairment) affects approximately 7% of young children and is characterized as a deficit in language that is not explained by another disorder, such as hearing impairment, intellectual impairment, or autism (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Snowling and Thompson2017; Leonard, Reference Leonard2014; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Records, Buckwalter, Zhang, Smith and O’Brien1997; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Records and Zhang1996). In the current work, DLD and SLI are used synonymously because the children in the DLD group met the inclusionary criteria for both DLD and SLI (see the method section below). Research concerning children with DLD has classically focused on language processing and production, particularly morphosyntax (Leonard, Reference Leonard2014), but phonology (e.g., Alt & Plante, Reference Alt and Plante2006; Benham et al., Reference Benham, Goffman and Schweickert2018; Dollaghan & Campbell, Reference Dollaghan and Campbell1998; Gray, Reference Gray2005) and semantics (e.g., Kan & Windsor, Reference Kan and Windsor2010; McGregor et al. 2002; Reference McGregor, Oleson, Bahnsen and Duff2013b) have also been implicated. It has become apparent that children with DLD also show extra-linguistic deficits, such as in working memory (e.g., Archibald & Gathercole, Reference Archibald and Gathercole2006; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Leitão, Claessen and Boyes2020; Lum et al., Reference Lum, Conti-Ramsden, Page and Ullman2012) and motor skill (e.g., Brumbach & Goffman, Reference Brumbach and Goffman2014; Hill, Reference Hill2001; Sanjeevan et al., Reference Sanjeevan, Rosenbaum, Miller, van Hell, Weiss and Mainela-Arnold2015; Vuolo et al., Reference Vuolo, Goffman and Zelaznik2017). Such cross-domain impairments suggest that the underlying mechanisms associated with DLD may not be specific to language.

The central issue addressed in the present work is whether the deficits attested in children with DLD are specific to spoken language, or if they manifest in, and can be explored through other modalities—in the present case, manual gesture. Gesture, like speech, is tightly linked to language (McNeill, Reference McNeill1985), incorporating meaning and form via motor implementation. However, unlike speech, gestures are often iconic and thus not arbitrarily linked to their meaning (McNeill, Reference McNeill1992). While gestures often show transparency in form-to-meaning mapping, their alignment to spoken and signed language (Goldin-Meadow & Brentari, Reference Goldin-Meadow and Brentari2017) affords the analysis of the conceptual and motor implementation components of phonological form.

Gestures are defined as abstract actions that do not physically change the environment, but influence behavior and cognition via communication (Goldin-Meadow, Reference Goldin-Meadow2015; McNeill, Reference McNeill1985; Reference McNeill1992). In joining meaning to form, gestures can be assessed for both their conceptual and phonological components. As in speech, where constituent phonological features such as voicing or place of articulation can be subjected to fine-grained analysis, gesture can be broken down by phonological features, such as handshape, path, location, and orientation (Brentari, Reference Brentari1998; Brentari et al., Reference Brentari, Coppola, Cho and Senghas2017; Cheek et al., Reference Cheek, Cormier, Repp and Meier2001; Liddell & Johnson, Reference Liddell and Johnson1989; Pettenati & Stefanini, Reference Pettenati and Stefanini2010; Sandler, Reference Sandler1989). In iconic (i.e., representational) gesture, handshape often reflects object characteristics, thus serving a noun-like function. The path of the hands often expresses functional information (Capone & McGregor, Reference Capone and McGregor2005; McNeill, Reference McNeill1985), including the trajectory and manner of the movement, capturing the verb-like aspects of gesture (e.g., Kita & Özyürek, Reference Kita and Özyürek2003; Mumford & Kita, Reference Mumford and Kita2014; see also Goldin-Meadow et al., Reference Goldin-Meadow, McNeill and Singleton1996). Location of the hands in reference to the body and the orientation of the hands to each other provide additional spatial information about the referent (see Brentari, Reference Brentari1998 for further discussion of orientation in sign). The fine-grained analysis of these phonological features in iconic gestures is the focus of the current work. The integration of meaning and form, as in spoken and signed language, is a central characteristic of gesture. That is, gesture provides a window into the intersection of language and motor systems in both typical and atypical development.

Spoken word production in DLD

Many researchers have shown that there are form deficits in children with DLD that affect both morphosyntax (Goffman & Leonard, Reference Goffman and Leonard2000; Leonard, Reference Leonard2014; Redmond & Rice, Reference Redmond and Rice2001; Rice & Wexler, Reference Rice and Wexler1996) and phonology (Benham et al., Reference Benham, Goffman and Schweickert2018; Goffman, Reference Goffman1999; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Leitão, Claessen and Boyes2019). Both children and adults with DLD have difficulty producing novel word forms, likely driven in part by deficits in phonological sequencing (Benham et al., Reference Benham, Goffman and Schweickert2018; Heisler et al., Reference Heisler, Goffman and Younger2010) and in encoding (e.g., Gathercole, Reference Gathercole2006; Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, Karpicke, Deevy, Weber, Christ, Haebig, Souto, Kueser and Krok2019; McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Licandro, Arenas, Eden, Stiles, Bean and Walker2013a; Reference McGregor, Arbisi-Kelm, Eden and Oleson2020). One of the most robust findings within the DLD literature is that, during the preschool and school-aged years, these children show deficits in nonword repetition tasks, with accuracy decreasing as word length increases (Deevy et al., Reference Deevy, Weil, Leonard and Goffman2010; Dollaghan & Campbell, Reference Dollaghan and Campbell1998; Ellis Weismer et al., Reference Ellis Weismer, Tomblin, Zhang, Buckwalter, Chynoweth and Jones2000; Graf Estes et al., Reference Graf Estes, Evans and Else-Quest2007; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Leitão, Claessen and Boyes2019). Preschool-aged children with DLD frequently have co-occurring speech sound deficits, as indicated by impaired performance on standardized articulation tests (Alt & Suddarth, Reference Alt and Suddarth2012; Leonard, Reference Leonard2014; Shriberg et al., Reference Shriberg, Tomblin and McSweeny1999; Vuolo & Goffman, Reference Vuolo and Goffman2018). While articulation deficits may diminish in the school years (Shriberg et al., Reference Shriberg, Gruber and Kwiatkowski1994), it appears that deficits in encoding new word forms (McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Licandro, Arenas, Eden, Stiles, Bean and Walker2013a; Reference McGregor, Arbisi-Kelm, Eden and Oleson2020) and in phonological sequencing (Benham & Goffman, Reference Benham and Goffman2021) persist into the school years and beyond. In the present work, we ask whether these same deficits that are documented in speech production are also observed in the mapping of manual phonological form to novel gestures and whether these deficits continue from preschool into the early school years.

Gesture production in children with DLD

While gesture appears to facilitate communication and lexical acquisition in children with DLD (e.g., Ellis Weismer & Hesketh, Reference Ellis Weismer and Hesketh1993; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Alibali and McNeil2001; Mainela-Arnold et al., Reference Mainela-Arnold, Alibali, Hostetter and Evans2014; Vogt & Kauschke, Reference Vogt and Kauschke2017), there is some indication that, as in spoken words, there may be deficits in phonological form (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Norbury and Alcock2016; Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017; cf. Botting et al., Reference Botting, Riches, Gaynor and Morgan2010). In studies of elicited and imitated single gesture production, children with DLD and typical development (TD) were asked to produce iconic gestures representing familiar objects (Botting et al., Reference Botting, Riches, Gaynor and Morgan2010; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Norbury and Alcock2016) and/or actions (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017). School-aged children with DLD were less accurate in their gesture productions (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Norbury and Alcock2016; Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017; cf. Botting et al., Reference Botting, Riches, Gaynor and Morgan2010). For example, Wray and her colleagues reported decrements in gesture form accuracy in children with DLD when gestures were rated for overall accuracy on a 5-point Likert scale (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Norbury and Alcock2016) and when gesture productions were scored based on a combination of form components (the hands’ shape, movement, and location; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017). In addition, Hill and colleagues (Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998) identified form errors associated with hand orientation and positioning in relation to the body in the elicited and imitated gestures of children with DLD. This pattern of error type suggests that the form deficits observed in the gestures of children with DLD may stem from phonological feature errors. However, the form errors reported in previous studies were analyzed as a motor rather than a linguistic skill; phonological features of sign language have generally not been considered. For example, the gesture analysis framework employed by Hill and her colleagues (Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998) was derived from studies of adult apraxia. This analytic framework assumes a neuromotor, as opposed to phonological, source of the gesture errors produced by the children with DLD. Only a subset of the error types reported by Hill and her colleagues (Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998) can be linked to deficits in phonological form—for example, the “external configuration” error, in which the child did not accurately represent the distance between the hand and the body, can be interpreted as a location error, but could also reflect a motor-based deficit. Other error types were judged as apraxic errors such as clumsiness or delayed imitation.

Wray and her colleagues (Reference Wray, Norbury and Alcock2016; Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017) report general form assessments, but do not apply a fine-grained analysis of individual phonological features. For example, Wray and her colleagues (Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017) developed a holistic coding of accuracy that incorporated the phonological features of shape, path, and location, but did not differentiate their contributions. Their finding that children with DLD produced gestures less accurately than their TD peers suggests that form deficits may be phonological in nature. However, global measures of accuracy do not reveal the nature of the phonological deficit. It is also of note that Wray and her colleagues (Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017) report a fine motor deficit in the children with DLD, leaving open the possibility that errors in gesture form may be tied to the motor system.

Gesture production, like speech, is phonological but also requires fine motor skill. Children with DLD have documented deficits in gross and fine motor skill, as evidenced by relatively weak performance on standardized tests compared to TD peers (Brumbach & Goffman Reference Brumbach and Goffman2014; Hill, Reference Hill2001; Sanjeevan & Mainela-Arnold, Reference Sanjeevan and Mainela-Arnold2019; Sanjeevan et al., Reference Sanjeevan, Rosenbaum, Miller, van Hell, Weiss and Mainela-Arnold2015; Zelaznik & Goffman, Reference Zelaznik and Goffman2010). However, more specific components of motor skill may be implicated, with both preschool- and school-aged children with DLD showing sequencing and coordination deficits in the manual domain on such tasks as bimanual clapping (Vuolo et al., Reference Vuolo, Goffman and Zelaznik2017), serial reaction time tasks (Hsu & Bishop, Reference Hsu and Bishop2014; Lum et al., Reference Lum, Conti-Ramsden, Morgan and Ullman2014; Tomblin et al., Reference Tomblin, Mainela-Arnold and Zhang2007), and sequential handshape imitation (Marton, Reference Marton2009; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017). These findings, when considered in conjunction with the body of work showing motor implementation deficits in children with DLD, suggest that phonological errors in the speech and gestures of children with DLD may arise from a specific motor implementation impairment connected to sequenced or coordinated movement across multiple effectors (Vuolo et al., Reference Vuolo, Goffman and Zelaznik2017).

Current study

In the current work, we assessed the longitudinal progression of form aspects of gesture learning from preschool into the early school years. We also indexed fine motor and language development across this same developmental time period. A major objective was to determine the presence and persistence of early observed form-based deficits in the gestural domain.

The first theme of this work addressed the presence and developmental time course of phonological deficits in gesture production in young children with DLD. Children with DLD and TD participated in a three-time point (two year) longitudinal study beginning at age 4 to 5 years and continuing until 6 to 7 years of age. Children were tested annually in a task in which they were taught novel gestures in a word learning paradigm (described below). We asked whether the documented deficits in gesture production in young children with DLD (e.g., Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017) can be described phonologically and whether they persist into the early school years, mirroring phonological deficits in spoken language (e.g., Benham & Goffman, Reference Benham and Goffman2021; McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Licandro, Arenas, Eden, Stiles, Bean and Walker2013a). The presence of phonological deficits in the novel gesture productions of children with DLD would bolster claims of cross-modal influences that are not specifically tied to speech production. Conversely, if we observe no evidence of phonological deficits in gesture production in children with DLD, then, consistent with findings reported by Botting and colleagues (Reference Botting, Riches, Gaynor and Morgan2010), we can infer that errors are driven by more general motor deficits (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Norbury and Alcock2016; Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017). We are particularly interested in the developmental time course of phonological deficits in gesture production, since it is documented that form deficits in spoken language persist into later childhood (Benham & Goffman, Reference Benham and Goffman2021) and adulthood (McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Licandro, Arenas, Eden, Stiles, Bean and Walker2013a; Reference McGregor, Arbisi-Kelm, Eden and Oleson2020). Persistence of deficits in phonological aspects of gesture production from the preschool into the school years would provide evidence of a protracted cross-modal phonological impairment associated with DLD. However, it may be that phonological deficits are not fully cross-modal, with deficits in gesture production resolving over time.

A second theme centered on the phonological features of handshape, path, and orientation. While gesture and speech are tightly aligned within the linguistic system, the phonology of gesture differs from that of speech in that iconic gesture form is non-arbitrarily linked to its meaning (e.g., two hands in fists moving back and forth synchronously represent a swing or the act of swinging). The phonological features of handshape (tied to nouns) and path (tied to verbs) may be especially iconic (e.g., Capone & McGregor, Reference Capone and McGregor2005). We asked whether specific phonological features, including handshape, path, and orientation, showed deficits that extended into the school years.

Gestures, like speech, are of particular interest in typical and atypical development because they are thought to relate closely to both language and fine motor performance. The final theme addressed in this work is the relationship of both language and fine motor skill with gesture production. We asked whether early phonological feature accuracy in gesture was predictive of later language and/or fine motor outcomes. Children with DLD are defined by their language deficit, yet gross and fine motor skill are also frequently implicated in the disorder (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Snowling and Thompson2017; Vuolo et al., Reference Vuolo, Goffman and Zelaznik2017). Gesture is, by definition, both motoric and linguistic. Thus, we asked whether early phonological accuracy in gesture production predicted either language or fine motor outcomes two years later.

Method

Participants

The children in this study participated in a series of experiments across a three-time point (two year) span focused on the relationship between language and action in children with DLD. Because we were interested in phonological aspects of gesture when children are 4 to 5 years old and the relationship to outcomes two years later, the current study concerns the first and final time points. Twenty-eight children (14 with DLD and 14 with TD) are included in the current analyses and represent a longitudinal subset of a larger time point one sample (n = 55, 34 with DLD and 21 with TD). These 28 children were the only children from the larger time point one sample to return at time point three to complete the novel gesture production task (described below) as well as the language and motor outcome measures. Independent sample t-tests confirmed that the 28 children who comprised the longitudinal sample did not significantly differ from the full time point one sample in performance on the standardized measures of language, cognition, and motor skill. The presence or severity of language impairment did not determine continued participation in the longitudinal study (Table 1).

Table 1. Group comparisons, means, and standard deviations for standardized tests at time point one

Note. TD: Typical Development; DLD: Developmental Language Disorder; SPELT-P2/SPELT-3: Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool 2 or Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool 3; BBTOP: Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology; Nonverbal intelligence in the full sample was calculated using standard scores from the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale-3 (n = 49) and the Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (n = 6); nonverbal intelligence in the longitudinal sample was calculated using standard scores from the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale-3 (n = 28); MABC-2: Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2.

a TD vs DLD p-values

b Full Sample vs Longitudinal Sample p-values

The 14 children with DLD in the longitudinal cohort had a mean age of 4 years, 10 months (SD of 0;5 months; 6 females; 2 left-handed) at the study outset, as did the 14 children who were TD (M age of 4;10; SD of 0;5 months; 7 females; 2 left-handed). Upon return at time point three, the children with DLD had a mean age of 7 years, 7 months (SD of 0;6 months), and those with TD had a mean age of 7 years, 4 months (SD of 0;5 months). The children with DLD were recruited from a clinical research program for children with DLD at Purdue University. These children all received intervention during this clinical program that focused on literacy and vocabulary, not word form or gesture. Other aspects of therapy history are unknown. The children with TD were also recruited from the greater West Lafayette, Indiana area. They did not receive any speech or language interventions, and no history of a speech-language disorder was reported by their parents/legal guardians. Approval from the Institutional Review Board of Purdue University was obtained prior to the start of the study; the parents or legal guardians of the children provided written informed consent, and the children provided both written and verbal assent that was presented using developmentally appropriate language. The University of Texas at Dallas Institutional Review Board approved the data analyses included in the present study.

Inclusionary criteria

At study entry at time point one, all of the children performed within the typical range in nonverbal intelligence as measured by the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale—Third Edition (CMMS-3; Burgemeister et al., Reference Burgemeister, Blum and Lorge1972). All of the children also passed a pure tone hearing screening (pure tones presented bilaterally at 20 dB HL at 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz), as well as a structural oral motor screener using the Robbins and Klee (Reference Robbins and Klee1987) protocol. Finally, all children were rated as having minimal-to-no symptoms of autism spectrum disorder on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale—Second Edition (Schopler et al., Reference Schopler, Van Bourgondien, Wellman and Love2010), and no history of neurological pathologies (e.g., seizures and head injuries) was reported by the parents.

DLD criteria for time point one

As is required for the diagnosis, the children included in the DLD group showed impairments in language abilities in accordance with the exclusionary criteria specified in Leonard (Reference Leonard2014). A child was considered to have a language impairment if they received a standard score of 87 or below on the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test—Preschool 2 (SPELT-P2; Dawson, et al., Reference Dawson, Stout, Eyer, Tattersall, Fonkalsrud and Croley2005), as this cutoff has been shown to be a sensitive (96%) and specific (95%) measure of DLD status (Greenslade et al., Reference Greenslade, Plante and Vance2009). Children with DLD all participated in a summer clinical research program for which the SPELT-P2 served as the diagnostic measure. For the TD children, who did not participate in the summer program, the SPELT-3 (Dawson et al., Reference Dawson, Stout and Eyer2003) was implemented. A standard score of 85 or below was used as the cutoff point. This cut-off point has been found to have 71.9% sensitivity and 100% specificity for detecting DLD according to Perona and colleagues (Reference Perona, Plante and Vance2005). The lowest standard score for the TD group was 90.

DLD status was further confirmed by a finite verb morphology composite score (comprised of percent correct production of regular past –ed, third-person singular –s, and copula and auxiliary forms of is, are, and am) that fell greater than 1.25 standard deviations below the mean based on normative data from the local West Lafayette, Indiana area (Goffman & Leonard, Reference Goffman and Leonard2000; Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, Miller and Gerber1999). Notably, the children with TD obtained typical finite verb morphology composite scores.

These results, as well as performance on the Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology (BBTOP; Bankson & Bernthal, Reference Bankson and Bernthal1990), are shown in Table 1. As is permitted for a diagnosis of DLD, performance on the BBTOP was free to vary (Leonard, Reference Leonard2014). The performance of the TD children on all measures was within expected levels.

Longitudinal measures

Language and motor ability were tracked across time points one and three for all of the children. At time point three, language ability was assessed using the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—4th Edition (CELF-4; Semel et al., Reference Semel, Wiig and Secord2003). This battery consists of four subtests: Concepts and Following Directions, Word Structure, Formulated Sentences, and Recalling Sentences. Scaled scores from each of the subtests were used to calculate a Core Language Score. Performance on the CELF-4 at time point three was free to vary; it was the initial diagnosis at time point one that was used to classify children with DLD.

Fine and gross motor skills were assessed using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children—2nd Edition (MABC-2; Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Sugden and Barnett2007). At time point one, children completed the 3- to 6-year-old age band, and at time point three, the 7- to 10-year-old age band. There are three areas assessed: Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance. Standard scores from each of the subtests were used to calculate a Subtest Score and a Total Test Score. These scores were free to vary at both time points one and three as performance on the MABC-2 is not a part of the inclusionary-exclusionary criteria, and motor deficits are often identified in children with DLD (e.g., Hill, Reference Hill2001; Vuolo et al., Reference Vuolo, Goffman and Zelaznik2017).

To summarize, at time point one, all of the children participated in a battery of assessments as shown in Figure 1. When the children returned at time point three, their language and motor skills were once again assessed now as outcome measures. At both time points, the children completed the experimental gesture task which is described in the next section.

Figure 1. Experimental Protocol and Gesture Stimuli Presentation Schematic with Stimuli Exemplars and Prompts. CARS-2: Childhood Autism Rating Scale-2; CMMS-3: Columbia Mental Maturity Scale-3; SPELT-P2/SPELT-3: Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool 2 or Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool 3; FVM: Finite Verb Morphology; BBTOP: Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology; MABC-2: Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2; CELF-4: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4.

Experimental stimuli & procedure

The children were presented with four novel gestures and, in some cases, corresponding novel objects (unusual objects found at kitchen supply stores). The novel gestures were constrained to a set of four in this learning study because multiple productions (14 per gesture) were elicited and it was necessary to maintain a manageable number of productions for preschool-aged children. The novel gestures were constructed in accordance with American Sign Language phonotactic rules regarding handshape, path, and orientation (Brentari, Reference Brentari1998; Liddell & Johnson, Reference Liddell and Johnson1989; Sandler, Reference Sandler1989) and were designed to be iconic, reflecting the attributes and affordances of the paired novel object (Capone & McGregor, Reference Capone and McGregor2005). The handshapes used in the novel gestures were drawn from those that are early developing in sign language (e.g., Boyes Braem, Reference Boyes Braem, Volterra and Erting1990; Marentette & Mayberry, Reference Marentette, Mayberry, Chamberlain, Morford and Mayberry2000). The paths of the novel gestures were also designed to be simple single syllable movements as opposed to more complex multi-directional or repeated movements (e.g., an analogous speech production would be /ba/ versus /baba/ or /bapεf/). Table 2 shows pictures and descriptions of the novel gestures, as well as pictures of the novel objects. The study protocol, stimuli, and de-identified raw data can be accessed at https://osf.io/fr64e/.

Table 2. Novel gestures and object referents

Note. Handshapes are described based on the ASL fingerspelling alphabet

Each novel gesture was recorded separately with and without the corresponding novel object referent in view. During the presentation of the stimuli, half of the novel gestures appeared with their referent and half appeared without their referent. The current work focuses on the phonological, rather than lexical, component of gesture production, and, in an analysis of the referent manipulation within the full time point one sample that is in preparation, no effects of referent were observed (p = .108); therefore, referential status was counterbalanced, but not analyzed. The videos were presented on a computer monitor display during data collection in a child friendly manner by a woman wearing a black shirt and seated against a dark gray background.

The children were seated in a child-sized chair at a table approximately 51cm in height and told that they were going to learn an alien language in which people talk with their hands. The children were video-recorded using a Panasonic HDC-HS700 camera for later phonological coding. Hand motions were also recorded, though not analyzed here.

The novel gestures were yoked into pairs and split into two blocks. Order of the novel gestures within a block was quasi-random (a gesture was never presented more than twice in a row). Each block was divided into three phases (Heisler et al., Reference Heisler, Goffman and Younger2010; see Figure 1 for a schematic of the stimuli presentation across experimental phases):

Pretest Phase: The child was presented with a video for two novel gestures (no object referents were included) and was instructed to watch and then imitate the gesture (7 times each).

Exposure Phase: The child was passively exposed to the same two novel gestures; one of the gestures was presented without a referent, and the other was presented with its referent.

Posttest Phase: Same as pretest.

Following the posttest phase of each block, comprehension and production probes were administered in a fixed order (comprehension then production):

Comprehension Probe: The four novel objects were presented to the child. Two of the novel objects corresponded to the novel gestures presented during the experimental block, while the other two objects (foils) had not been shown to the child during the experimental block. The child was shown a video-recording of the woman producing one of the novel gestures that the child had been exposed to during the experimental block, and the child was instructed to find the object that corresponded to the gesture. The child’s response was binarily coded as correct (1 point) or incorrect (0 points).

Production Probe: The child was presented with a picture of one of the novel objects and was asked to produce the corresponding gesture. The coding of the child’s response is described in the next section.

Behavioral data analysis

Gesture coding

Gestures were analyzed for phonological accuracy in the pre- and posttest phases, as well as in the production probe. Gesture productions were coded for accuracy based on the phonological features of handshape, path, and orientation. Each phonological feature was coded on a three-point scale: two points were assigned if the feature was produced completely accurately (e.g., target handshape: closed fist/a-hand, child production: closed fist/a-hand), one point if aspects of the target feature were present in the child’s production but some errors were made (e.g., target handshape: closed fist/a-hand, child production: cupped hand; in this example, the child has demonstrated partial knowledge of the target by bending their fingers), and zero points if the feature was completely inaccurate (e.g., target handshape: closed fist/a-hand, child production: flat hand). A maximum of six points per gesture production were possible (i.e., two points for handshape, two points for path, and two points for orientation). All pretest, posttest, and production probe productions were coded for phonological feature accuracy at the time points one and three; interrater reliability was established for time point one with 87.1% agreement and time point three with 90.2% agreement between the first author and two trained research assistants.

Analyses

Descriptive statistics are included to establish the linguistic and motor developmental profiles of the DLD and TD groups at time points one and three. Performance on the language measures was compared at time point one using an independent t-test while a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used at time point three to compare between-group performance on the composite language and subtest scores of the CELF-4. Separate analyses were used because, as appropriate for age, two different standardized language measures were administered across time points (the SPELT-P2/3 at time point one and the CELF-4 at time point three). Because the same standardized battery was used to index motor ability at each time point, a 2x3 (language group x MABC-2 subtest) mixed ANOVA with repeated measures was conducted to compare between-group (TD or DLD) performance (dependent variable) on the three subtests of the MABC-2 (within-subjects variable) at the first and final time points.

The children’s performance on the comprehension and production learning probes was also analyzed. Two 2x2 mixed ANOVAs were used. In each analysis, the effect of language group membership (between-subjects variable: TD or DLD) and time (within-subjects variable) on comprehension and production probe performance (percent correct), respectively, was assessed.

Phonological deficits over time

The first and second themes addressed a) whether phonological deficits in the production of gesture were found when children were preschool-aged and persisted over time, and b) whether phonological feature accuracy differed within and across time points. These themes were addressed with a 2x3x2 (language group x phonological feature x practice) mixed ANOVA with repeated measures. This analysis assessed phonological accuracy (dependent variable) between language groups (TD or DLD), as well as across phonological features (handshape, path, and orientation) and practice (pretest and posttest) as a function of time (time points 1 and 3). Pairwise analyses were controlled for Type I error using a Bonferroni correction.

Phonological accuracy in relation to language and fine motor outcomes

The final theme of this work assessed the longitudinal relationship between phonological accuracy in novel gesture production at time point one and language and fine motor outcome measures at time point three. This was done using a series of hierarchical regressions for each language group. The average time point one accuracy scores for each of the phonological features (handshape, path, and orientation), collapsed across pretest and posttest, were entered as predictor variables. The CELF-4 Recalling Sentences subtest scores at time point three were entered as the language outcome variable because this task has been found to be both sensitive and specific to language impairment status (Archibald & Joanisse, Reference Archibald and Joanisse2009; Conti-Ramsden et al., Reference Conti-Ramsden, Botting and Faragher2001). Recalling sentences tasks have also been shown to specifically reflect language skill, and not short-term memory or processing speed, in preschool-aged children with and without DLD (Klem et al., Reference Klem, Melby-Lervåg, Hagtvet, Halaas Lyster, Gustafsson and Hulme2015; Plym et al., Reference Plym, Lahti-Nuuttila, Smolander, Arkkila and Laasonen2021). Likewise, fine motor skill intuitively is aligned with the production of gesture, and children with DLD have previously been reported to demonstrate deficits in fine motor ability, including manual sequencing and coordination (Marton, Reference Marton2009; Sanjeevan & Mainela-Arnold, Reference Sanjeevan and Mainela-Arnold2019; Vuolo et al., Reference Vuolo, Goffman and Zelaznik2017; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017). Therefore, the MABC-2 Manual Dexterity subtest scores were entered as the fine motor outcome variable.

Finally, because nonverbal ability may influence the production of gesture, we included an analysis of nonverbal scores. The time point one CMMS-3 scores were entered as a covariate in all of the hierarchical regressions. As noted in the inclusionary criteria section above, all of the children, regardless of language ability, scored within the typical range on the CMMS-3 with the lowest score being a 94 (Table 1). Nonetheless, to account for the potential influence of nonverbal ability, this covariate was included.

Results

Performance on the standardized language and motor measures

Language ability of the children with DLD was compared to their TD peers to assess performance at time point one and at time point three. Recall that children were assigned to the DLD group based on performance at time point one, and time point three was free to vary. Therefore, as expected, a between-language group comparison of the time point one language scores confirmed that the language ability of the TD and DLD groups was significantly different, t(26) = 8.78, p = .000 (TD: M = 109.07, SD = 11.63; DLD: M = 74.07, SD = 9.33). At time point three, the children with a diagnosis of DLD at age 4 to 5 years (time point one) still showed language scores significantly lower than the TD group as measured by both the CELF-4 core language score, F(1,24) = 16.50, p = .000, ηp 2 = 0.30 (TD: M = 108.79, SD = 12.46; DLD: M = 89.00, SD = 12.29) and the CELF-4 recalling sentences subtest score, F(1,26) = 8.10, p = .006, ηp 2 = 0.20 (TD: M = 10.42, SD = 3.48; DLD: M = 7.14, SD = 2.18); see Table 3 for all CELF-4 subtest between-group comparisons. However, it is important to note that at time point three, only three of the fourteen children in the DLD group had a CELF-4 core language score greater than one standard deviation below the mean. Six of the fourteen children with DLD scored greater than one standard deviation below the mean on the recalling sentences subtest. Based on this measure, language disorder did not persist for several of these children.

Table 3. CELF-4 subtest group comparisons for the longitudinal cohort at time point three

Note. The DLD group sample size for the Concepts and Following Directions subtest, and subsequently the Core Language Score, is n = 12 because two participants did not complete the Concepts and Following Directions subtest.

The three subtests of the MABC-2 (Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance) were used to index the children’s fine and gross motor skills at the first and final time points. Overall, there was a main effect of language group, F(1,26) = 4.24, p = .05, ηp 2 = 0.14, with the TD group demonstrating higher motor subtest scores, but no main effect of time, F(1,26) = 1.70, p = .204, ηp 2 = 0.06. Additionally, there was a group by subtest interaction, F(2,52) = 3.52, p = .037, ηp 2 = 0.12. TD children scored higher on the Balance subtest (M = 11.82, SD = 2.64) than did the children with DLD (M = 8.96, SD = 3.56), p = .004. Though not statistically significant, the TD children also scored higher on the Manual Dexterity subtest (M = 10.25, SD = 2.27) in comparison to the children with DLD (M = 8.39, SD = 3.22), p = .056. The groups did not significantly differ on the Aiming and Catching subtest, p = .90. Table 4 shows the group comparison of means, standard deviations, and score ranges for the MABC-2 total test scores and subtests at each time point.

Table 4. MABC-2 longitudinal cohort group means and standard deviations between time points

Although the group comparison of the Manual Dexterity subtest, which indexes fine motor skill, was not statistically significant, the DLD group was notably more variable in their fine motor performance than their TD peers. At time point one, all but one TD child scored within expected levels on the fine motor tasks that comprise the Manual Dexterity measure, while over a third of the children with DLD (5 out of 14) scored one standard deviation below the mean. When the children returned at time point three, all of the TD children scored within the typical range of the Manual Dexterity subtest, while 4 of the 14 children with DLD scored below one standard deviation of the mean.

Performance on experimental tasks

Learning probes

Comprehension and production probes were administered following the posttest phase. All children performed at similar levels on both the comprehension probe, F(1,26) = 0.00, p = 1.00, ηp 2 = 0.00 (TD: M percent correct = 82%, SD = 39%; DLD: M percent correct = 82%, SD = 39%), and production probe, F(1,26) = 1.77, p = .195, ηp 2 = 0.06 (TD: M percent correct = 72%, SD = 26%; DLD: M percent correct = 66%, SD = 27%). There was a significant improvement in performance for all children on the comprehension probe, F(1, 26) = 14.60, p = .001, ηp 2 = 0.36, as well as on the production probe from time point one to three, F(1,26) = 61.13, p = .000, ηp 2 = 0.70, as shown in Table 5. No interaction effects between group and time for either probe were found.

Table 5. Longitudinal cohort learning probes mean percent correct and standard deviations

Note. There is no range reported for the comprehension probe because comprehension responses were indexed using a binary system of correct (1pt) or incorrect (0pts).

Phonological deficits over time

We began by asking a) whether phonological deficits in gesture production were observed at time point one and, if so, whether they persisted to time point three, and b) which, if any, phonological features showed group differences or relative weaknesses at each time point.

There was a main effect of language group: the children with TD demonstrated higher phonological accuracy (M = 1.62, SD = 0.28) compared to the children with DLD (M = 1.50, SD = 0.33), F(1,26) = 9.2, p = .005, ηp 2 = 0.26. There was also a main effect of time, F(1, 26) = 56.4, p = .000, ηp 2 = 0.68; all of the children significantly improved in phonological accuracy from time point one (M = 1.46, SD = 0.32) to time point three (M = 1.66, SD = 0.27). The children were not equally accurate across phonological features, F(2,52) = 34.0, p = .000, ηp 2 = 0.57. Handshape (M = 1.67, SD = 0.20) and path (M = 1.66, SD = 0.33) were both more accurate than orientation (M = 1.35, SD = 0.28), p = .000, but were similarly accurate to one another, p = 1.00. There was no main effect of short-term practice (pretest to posttest), F(1,26) = .004, p = .949, ηp 2 = .000.

There were two significant interactions: phonological feature accuracy by pretest-posttest, F(2,52) = 4.5, p = .015, ηp 2 = 0.15, and phonological feature accuracy by time point, F(2,52) = 5.1, p = .009, ηp 2 = 0.16. Both interactions reflect the relative weakness of orientation accuracy. Specifically, both before and after the exposure phase, orientation was significantly less accurate than handshape, p = .000, and path, p = .000. Orientation was also the only feature to not significantly improve from time point one to three, p = .09; both handshape, p = .003, and path, p = .000, demonstrated significant longitudinal gains; see Figures 2 and 3.

Figure 2. Mean Phonological Accuracy by Feature Across Pre- and Posttest and Between Groups with Standard Error. The maximum overall phonological accuracy score is six points. When accuracy scores are separated by phonological feature (shape, path, and orientation), a maximum of two points per feature is possible.

Figure 3. Mean Phonological Accuracy by Feature Across Time Points and Between Groups with Standard Error. The maximum overall phonological accuracy score is six points. When accuracy scores are separated by phonological feature (shape, path, and orientation), a maximum of two points per feature is possible.

Phonological accuracy in relation to language and fine motor outcomes

We also examined the predictive relationship between preschool-aged gesture phonological feature accuracy and school-aged language and fine motor outcomes. Using a series of planned hierarchical regressions, each group’s time point one phonological feature accuracy scores averaged across pretest and posttest (due to the null main effect of short-term practice) were entered separately as the predictor variables while the time point three CELF-4 Recalling Sentences scores and the MABC-2 Manual Dexterity scores were entered as the outcome variables, respectively. Nonverbal ability at time point one, as indexed by the CMMS-3, was entered as a covariate for all regressions.

For the children with DLD, the hierarchical regression analysis of time point three language ability demonstrated a combined significant effect across all three predictor variables (time point one handshape, path, and orientation accuracy) as well as the nonverbal covariate in the final model, F(4,9) = 6.02, p = .012, R2 = .72 (adjusted R2 = .61); see Table 6. An examination of the individual predictor variables indicated that time point one handshape accuracy was the largest significant predictor (ß = .608, t = 3.05, p = .014), followed by time point one orientation accuracy (ß = .450, t = 2.45, p = .037), and nonverbal ability (ß = .411, t = 2.28, p = .049). Path accuracy at time point one failed to reach significance at all model stages (p = .445). As shown in Table 6, time point one handshape accuracy was a significant predictor at every model stage and the covariate of nonverbal ability did not reach significance until the final model when time point one orientation was entered. It is also of note that the standardized ß coefficients for time one orientation accuracy and nonverbal ability are comparable, indicating that both variables contribute to the final model to a similar degree. However, there is no evidence of multicollinearity between time point one orientation accuracy (variance inflation factor = 1.11, tolerance = .90) and nonverbal ability (variance inflation factor = 1.08, tolerance = .93). Conversely, no significant relationship between phonological feature accuracy, nonverbal ability, and later language ability was found for the TD children, F(4,9) = 0.52, p = .721, R2 = .19 (adjusted R2 = .00), as shown in Figure 4.

Table 6. Hierarchical analysis for the DLD group CELF-4 recalling sentences time point three language outcome (n=14)

Note. *p ≤ .05; ** p ≤ .01; *** p ≤ .001.

Figure 4. Time Point One Significant Predictors for Time Point Three Outcome CELF-4 Recalling Sentences Scores. a) Time point one mean handshape phonological accuracy; b) time point one mean orientation phonological accuracy; c) time point one nonverbal CMMS-3 scores.

Turning to the time point three MABC-2 Manual Dexterity scores, the hierarchical regression analyses failed to yield a significant predictive relationship between time point one phonological feature accuracy, nonverbal ability, and time point three fine motor skill for both children with DLD, F(4,9) = 0.83, p = .537, R2 = .27 (adjusted R2 = .00), and children with TD, F(4,9) = 1.70, p = .233, R2 = .43 (adjusted R2 = .18). As an example, Figure 5 displays the null relationship between time point one handshape accuracy and time point three MABC-2 Manual Dexterity scores for the TD and DLD groups; a similar pattern of results was found for path and orientation. Note that at time point three, four of the children with DLD demonstrated a fine motor impairment; however, this did not relate to earlier phonological gesture accuracy.

Figure 5. Time Point One Mean Handshape Phonological Accuracy and Time Point Three MABC-2 Manual Dexterity Scores.

Summary of results

Analysis of the children’s phonological accuracy showed that, while children with DLD are generally less accurate than their TD peers, all of the children improved from time points one to three. The phonological deficits observed in the gestures of the children with DLD when they were preschool-aged did not persist. Further examination revealed differential degrees of accuracy between phonological features: handshape and path were more accurate than orientation; only orientation failed to improve in accuracy in response to practice and time. Finally, handshape accuracy, orientation accuracy, and nonverbal ability at time point one were found to be positively linked to later time point three language ability, but only for children with DLD. No longitudinal links between early phonological accuracy, nonverbal ability, and later fine motor ability were found.

Discussion

In the current work, we explored whether and how phonological features in novel gesture production predict language and motor outcomes in children with DLD. Gestures, like speech, recruit both the linguistic and motor systems; however, unlike speech, the form-to-meaning mapping found in iconic gestures is transparent and non-arbitrary. This pattern of overlap and divergence in speech and gesture allowed us to explore three themes regarding the conceptual and motor systems’ contributions to gesture phonological production accuracy in children with TD and DLD, including developmental change over time.

Theme 1: Phonological deficits over time

First, we asked whether there were deficits in the phonological accuracy of gestures in preschool-aged children with DLD, as well as whether these deficits persisted into the early school years. The presence and persistence of these phonological errors into the early school years would indicate that the form-based deficits documented in the production of novel spoken language cross to the manual modality. While children with DLD did demonstrate a phonological deficit in gesture, it appeared to resolve in the production of simple iconic gestures. All of the children, whether DLD or TD at study entry, demonstrated significant gains in overall phonological gesture accuracy between the preschool- and school-aged years, with the children in the DLD group converging on typical levels of accuracy by time point three.

The initially weak performance and positive developmental gains in phonological accuracy observed for children with DLD cannot be attributed to deficits in the conceptual encoding of the novel gestures. Analysis of the learning probes at each time point indicates that the form-based production deficits observed when the children with DLD were in preschool were not connected to their ability to receptively and expressively map the novel gestures to their referents. In fact, there were no group differences in the form-to-meaning mapping accuracy on the comprehension and production probes at either time point. This mirrors previous research showing that word form deficits are central in children (Benham et al., Reference Benham, Goffman and Schweickert2018) and adults with DLD (McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Licandro, Arenas, Eden, Stiles, Bean and Walker2013a; Reference McGregor, Arbisi-Kelm, Eden and Oleson2020). It appears that relative phonological weakness in the production of simple iconic gestures decreases over time for children with DLD and TD alike.

The convergence to typical levels in phonological gesture accuracy differs from the pattern observed in novel word form production in speech, which has shown deficits that persist into adulthood (e.g., McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Licandro, Arenas, Eden, Stiles, Bean and Walker2013a). This dissociation between gesture and spoken word form could be due in part to the nature of the form-to-meaning connection in speech versus gesture. While the meaning of an iconic gesture is inherent in its form, this same intrinsic relationship between meaning and form is not obligated in speech (though iconicity may be found in both speech and sign; for example, Goldin-Meadow & Brentari, Reference Goldin-Meadow and Brentari2017; Perniss & Vigliocco, Reference Perniss and Vigliocco2014). As such, the link between the conceptual and phonological levels may lead to more phonologically accurate gesture production.

Theme 2: Phonological feature accuracy

While the children’s overall phonological gesture accuracy increased across time points, the degree of improvement in individual phonological features was not equivalent. The phonological features of handshape and path were produced more accurately by all of the children in comparison to hand orientation. Orientation, unlike handshape and path, failed to show significant gains in accuracy in response to practice (pretest to posttest), as well as maturation (time points one to three). The unexpected persistence of weakness in orientation, independent of language ability, signals the presence of phonological markedness in iconic gestures; handshape, as well as path, may be more perceptually salient or semantically rich in comparison to orientation. Orientation may draw upon different conceptual constructs in comparison to handshape and path.

Other work has shown that specific features of gestures may influence learning. When paired with speech, handshape in iconic gesture is especially facilitative of novel spoken word learning in both typical toddlers (Capone & McGregor, Reference Capone and McGregor2005), as well as toddlers classified as late talkers (Capone Singleton & Anderson, Reference Capone Singleton and Anderson2020). It is hypothesized that this is because handshape, more so than path, emphasizes salient key conceptual features of the referent that disambiguate it from other possible referents. For example, the action of drinking from both a cup and a mug can be represented by lifting the hand to the lips; it is the handshape that differentiates between these two objects (a cupped hand versus a closed fist). In this way, handshape carries a high level of saliency because it adds specificity to the abstract action of gesturing (Goldin-Meadow, Reference Goldin-Meadow2015), thus laying the concrete representational foundation that characterizes iconic gestures.

In contrast, hand orientation in gesture may be more conceptually abstract and more closely aligned to visuospatial processing and mental imagery (e.g., Frick et al., Reference Frick, Daum, Walser and Mast2009; Levine et al., Reference Levine, Goldin-Meadow, Carlson and Hemani-Lopez2018; Shepard & Metzler, Reference Shepard and Metzler1971), both of which undergo developmental changes during early childhood (e.g., Dukette & Stiles, Reference Dukette and Stiles2001; Frick et al., Reference Frick, Hansen and Newcombe2013). When conveying the (dynamic) positioning of a referent in space via gesture, one is engaging in a form of mental imagery wherein visual, haptic, and proprioceptive knowledge of the affordances of one’s own body as well as an object’s affordances are drawn upon (e.g., Amorim et al., Reference Amorim, Isableu and Jarraya2006; Barsalou, Reference Barsalou2008). This sensorimotor knowledge is reflected in the orienting of the palms in reference to each other, the body, and the interlocutor.

While mental imagery and visuospatial processing were not directly measured in the current research, it is perhaps unsurprising that all of the children, and the children with DLD to a slightly greater degree, struggled with hand orientation. The production of novel iconic gestures presumably taps multiple representational levels—handshape and path reflect concrete conceptual elements of the referent and are tied to the word classes of nouns (handshape) and verbs (path; e.g., Capone & McGregor, Reference Capone and McGregor2005). Orientation in gesture does not have a direct word class analogue and often serves as a modifier by relaying additional information about the spatial positioning of the referent. For example, whether a cupped hand representing a glass is upright or tilted to palm down communicates two different states of the glass and its contents. The spatial positioning of the hand may have changed, but what did not change was the fact the handshape represented a glass.

When producing the novel gestures included in the current study, the children appeared to be sensitive to the phonological features that conveyed the majority of the key semantic information, leaving orientation and the spatial information it conveys to vary. In leaving orientation to vary, the children were still preserving the core of the gesture form—the shape of the object (the noun) and its movement/function (the verb). The visuospatial information conveyed by orientation was conceptually, and perhaps perceptually, peripheral to the core form-to-meaning link in this gestured word learning task. As such, the effect of practice and time on orientation accuracy was diminished. This, in conjunction with the ongoing development of mental imagery abilities, may have led to the persistent weakness in orientation that was observed for all of the children.

Theme 3. Phonological accuracy in relation to language and fine motor outcomes

Finally, we assessed whether the production of handshape, path, and orientation during the preschool years related to later language and fine motor outcomes. Handshape and orientation accuracy, as well as nonverbal ability at time point one when the children were preschool-aged were positively predictive of later language ability, but only for the children with DLD. None of the phonological features nor nonverbal ability at time point one were linked to later language skill for the TD children. There were no relationships in either group between phonological feature accuracy or nonverbal ability during the preschool years and later fine motor development. These findings suggest that phonological factors, rather than fine motor ability (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bishop and Nimmo-Smith1998; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017), drive the form deficits observed in gesture production in children with DLD.

Gesture as a part of the language system

The connection between phonological gesture accuracy and language, rather than fine motor outcomes, aligns with unified system theories of gesture, language, and cognition (e.g., Goldin-Meadow, Reference Goldin-Meadow, Amsel and Byrnes2002; McNeill, 1992; Reference McNeill2005). These theories argue that gesture exists within an interconnected conceptual network. Within this network, gesture augments and combines with language to convey meaning. The longitudinal relationship between gesture phonology and language skill fits into this theoretical framework by reinforcing the shared conceptual underpinnings underlying gesture and language, as opposed to gesture functioning as a separate representational act (e.g., Krauss et al., Reference Krauss, Chen, Gottesman and McNeill2000). Of course, phonological accuracy in gesture is derived in part from fine motor skill. However, the novel iconic gestures used in this study were simple productions that incorporated early developing handshapes and were produced in isolation. These gestures presumably required fewer motor demands than those that are embedded in a series of contiguous gestures or co-produced with speech. Gesture production accuracy has been linked to manual dexterity regardless of language impairment when multiple iconic gestures were concatenated or multiple effectors were recruited (e.g., including both the hands and the feet when producing a “ladder” gesture; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017).

Because phonological accuracy was assessed over numerous gesture productions, the influence of fine motor ability may have decreased as the children practiced the novel gestures. It is possible that manual dexterity would be found to affect phonological accuracy during initial stages of novel gesture fast-mapping when cognitive demand is high. In the spoken domain, higher cognitive demand has been shown to increase articulation variability in children with DLD, whereas repetition decreased variability (Saletta et al., Reference Saletta, Goffman, Ward and Oleson2018). Repetition over time has also been shown to decrease spoken word form errors (e.g., Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, Deevy, Karpicke, Christ and Kueser2020). As such, the motor and linguistic systems differentially influence speech production at different stages of learning. While gesture phonology appears to be tightly linked to the linguistic system, the contribution of fine motor ability during initial stages of gesture production remains open to investigation.

Gesture phonology, nonverbal ability, and DLD

The finding that time point one gesture accuracy, specifically handshape and orientation accuracy, was tied to the later language ability of the children with DLD and not their TD peers may provide particular insight into the nature of DLD. If the phonological accuracy of gesture is simply a reflection of language skill, then it would be expected that it would be tied to language gains for all of the children; however, this was not the case. Both children with DLD and their TD peers demonstrated varying degrees of handshape and orientation accuracy at time point one, as well as a range of language performance scores at time point three (Figure 4), yet the longitudinal effect of handshape and orientation accuracy (to a lesser degree) on later language skill was only found for the children with DLD.

In one account of DLD, the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis (Ullman et al., Reference Ullman, Earle, Walenski and Janacsek2020; Ullman & Pierpont, Reference Ullman and Pierpont2005), it is hypothesized that the declarative system is relatively spared and may act as a compensatory mechanism for an impaired procedural system. The spared declarative system is responsible for word learning in isolation, including the binding of form and meaning (e.g., Ullman & Pierpont, Reference Ullman and Pierpont2005). Handshape may tap into this preserved conceptual system more so than other phonological features. As discussed above, handshape plays a core role in disambiguating key distinguishing physical characteristics of a referent, thus capitalizing on conceptual knowledge and facilitating the connection between meaning and form. The present finding that handshape positively predicts later language skill, but only for children with DLD, supports that the conceptual system of children with DLD is preserved and provides a compensatory mechanism for language, thus representing a source of resiliency.

In addition to handshape, early orientation accuracy and nonverbal ability were also found to relate to improved language skill in the children with DLD when they were school-aged. As discussed above, orientation may be connected to more conceptually abstract mechanisms such as mental imagery and visuospatial processing. Both mental imagery and visuospatial processing represent cognitive mechanisms that support nonverbal problem solving and reasoning (i.e., fluid intelligence; Gillam et al., Reference Gillam, Montgomery, Evans and Gillam2019). Indices of nonverbal reasoning have been linked to nonverbal hand position imitation tasks in preschool-aged children with DLD, but not their TD peers (Plym et al., Reference Plym, Lahti-Nuuttila, Smolander, Arkkila and Laasonen2021), a pattern that was found in the current study as well. It is possible that these tasks tap into a general nonverbal processing mechanism for children with DLD. While dynamic nonverbal visuospatial tasks have been identified as an area of weakness in DLD (e.g., Gillam et al. Reference Gillam, Montgomery, Evans and Gillam2019; Savich, Reference Savich1984; Ullman et al., Reference Ullman, Earle, Walenski and Janacsek2020; Ullman & Pierpont, Reference Ullman and Pierpont2005), it is possible that the children with DLD who demonstrated better orientation accuracy or nonverbal ability in the current work were able to recruit an underlying nonverbal processing mechanism such as sustained visuospatial attention (e.g., Smolak et al., Reference Smolak, McGregor, Arbisi-Kelm and Eden2020) to support their impaired language system, allowing for greater language gains longitudinally. This interpretation of the positive longitudinal relationship between early orientation accuracy, nonverbal ability, and later language ability in children with DLD is speculative and requires further investigation.

The pattern of the regression analysis results for the children with DLD suggests that there are areas of underlying resiliency that may fulfill compensatory or facilitatory roles for language development within an impaired language system. The origin of this resiliency may lie in the conceptual system as suggested by the relationship between handshape accuracy and language, as well as domain-general mechanisms such as visuospatial processing. The origin and nature of this resiliency as well as how it may contribute to improved language ability in children with DLD require further investigation; however, these findings help to further characterize the nature of the cognitive mechanisms that give rise to the pattern of strengths and weaknesses associated with DLD.

A parallel relationship between early gesture accuracy or nonverbal ability and later language skill was not found for the TD children. This null result cannot be attributed to all of the TD children clustering around a similar language score when school-aged; children with TD demonstrated a range of language skills (Figure 4). Nonetheless, these children had typically developed language systems. Gesture has been shown to foreshadow linguistic development in typically developing toddlers (e.g., Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Iverson and Goldin-Meadow2005). However, by the preschool years, gesture assumes a supplementary role with language (signed or spoken) carrying the burden of communication; gesture may no longer provide direct insight into language development. Conversely, when language is impaired, the role of gesture as a primary communication facilitator persists (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Alibali and McNeil2001; Lavelli & Majorano, Reference Lavelli2016; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Saunders, McGuire, Cousins and Norbury2017). In this way, gesture, specifically handshape and orientation, appears to continue to provide insight into the language systems of children with DLD, including perhaps serving as an indicator of sources of resiliency.

Future directions, limitations, and conclusions

The connection between early phonological gesture accuracy and later language skill in DLD provides many possible avenues of further inquiry. Key is continuing to identify preserved underlying cognitive mechanisms, as these may serve as points of strength for children with DLD. It is possible that the connection between gesture and conceptual knowledge extends beyond a compensatory relationship and may offer a means to promoting improvements in language ability for children with DLD. Future work utilizing larger sample sizes than in the present work, as well as novel methodologies, should continue to investigate the gesture-language link in both imitated and spontaneous gesture.

Also requiring further investigation are the divergent cognitive mechanisms that underlie the phonological features of gesture. We have put forth one possible explanation as to why orientation is generally harder for children independent of language ability, as well as why it still may be related to language development in DLD; however, this phonological feature is understudied in both children and adults.

Likewise, the investigation of handshape and its tie to concepts and vocabulary acquisition holds promise for language impaired populations, but this line of inquiry is still in the early stages. The current study did not directly measure the general cognitive mechanisms that may relate to the different phonological features of gesture. To better understand the connections between gesture, language, and broader cognition, an important next step is to unpack the cognitive mechanisms that underlie gesture features.

Finally, while preschool-aged children with DLD were less phonologically accurate during novel gesture production than their TD peers, this deficit did not persist into the early school years. The DLD group’s gesture phonological accuracy ultimately converged to typical levels. We theorized that this resolution of the gesture phonological deficit was due to the role of iconicity in the form-to-meaning link. However, the novel gestures were designed to be simple in form and this simplicity cannot be ruled out as a contributing factor. Future work that incorporates more complex gestures designed to tax the language and motor systems is required to better understand the scope and duration of cross-modal phonological deficits in DLD.

In sum, we have shown that phonological deficits occur cross-modally in the gestures of preschool-aged children with DLD. However, these phonological deficits in gesture do not persist. Fine-grained analyses revealed differential levels of phonological accuracy within and across time points. Handshape and path were highly accurate, while orientation accuracy was comparatively lower and did not improve over time. Finally, we explored the longitudinal relations between early phonological accuracy and later language and fine motor skills. Early handshape accuracy, orientation accuracy, and nonverbal ability were found to be positively predicative of later language skill, but only for children with DLD. Taken together, this study has shown that the phonological systems of children with and without DLD can be explored cross-modally and that the phonological features of handshape and orientation provide insight into the underlying linguistic system. While it is important to not lose sight of gesture’s holistic nature and how this contributes to communication, more fine-grained analyses of gesture’s components may aid in furthering our understanding of gesture as a window to both language and cognition within both typical and atypical development.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grants R01DC04826 and R01DC016813 awarded to Lisa Goffman. The authors thank Mitch Barna, Lakin Brown, Ilana Feld, and Paulina Simon for their contributions to this project.

Competing Interests

The authors declare none.

References

Alt, M. & Plante, E. (2006). Factors that influence lexical and semantic fast mapping of young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 941954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alt, M. & Suddarth, R. (2012). Learning novel words: Detail and vulnerability of initial representations for children with specific language impairment and typically developing peers. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45, 8497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amorim, M., Isableu, B., & Jarraya, M. (2006). Embodied spatial transformations: “Body analogy” for the mental rotation of objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 327347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archibald, L. M. D. & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M. D. & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. Journal of Speech, Language, Hearing Research, 52(4), 899914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bankson, N. W. & Bernthal, J. E. (1990). Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology-Second Edition (BBTOP-2). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benham, S. & Goffman, L. (2021). A longitudinal study of the phonological organisation of novel word forms in children with developmental language disorder. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, ahead-of-print, 1-12. https://doi-org.libproxy.utdallas.edu/10.1080/17549507.2021.1975816 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benham, S., Goffman, L., & Schweickert, R. (2018). An application of network science to phonological sequence learning in children with development language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 22752291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, & the CATALISE-2 consortium (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 10681080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., Riches, N., Gaynor, M., & Morgan, G. (2010). Gesture production and comprehension in children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyes Braem, P. (1990). Acquisition of the handshape in American Sign Language: A preliminary analysis. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 107127). Washington, D.C.: Gallaudet University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brentari, D. (1998). A prosodic model of sign language phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brentari, D., Coppola, C., Cho, P. W., & Senghas, A. (2017). Handshape complexity as a precursor to phonology: Variation, emergence, and acquisition. Language Acquisition, 24(4), 283306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brumbach, A. C. D. & Goffman, L. (2014). Interaction of language processing and motor skill in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(1), 158171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgemeister, B. B., Blum, L. H., Lorge, I. (1972). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale—Third Edition (CMMS-3). San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Capone Singleton, N. & Anderson, L. (2020). Making object shape explicit for toddlers with late language emergence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 749763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capone, N. C. & McGregor, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers’ word retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 14681480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheek, A., Cormier, K., Repp, A., & Meier, R. P. (2001). Prelinguistic gesture predicts mastery and error in the production of early signs. Language, 77(2), 292323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, J., Stout, C., & Eyer, J. (2003). Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test- Third Edition (SPELT-3). DeKalb, IL: Janelle Publications.Google Scholar
Dawson, J., Stout, C., Eyer, J., Tattersall, P., Fonkalsrud, M., Croley, M. (2005). Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool Second Edition (SPELT-P2). DeKalb, IL: Janelle Publications.Google Scholar
Deevy, P., Weil, L. W., Leonard, L. B., Goffman, L. (2010). Extending use of the NRT to preschool-age children with and without specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 277288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollaghan, C. & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 11361146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dukette, D. & Stiles, J. (2001). The effects of stimulus density in children’s analysis of hierarchical patterns. Developmental Science, 4(2), 233251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S. & Hesketh, L. J. (1993). The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 10131025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., Chynoweth, J. G., Jones, M. (2000) nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 865878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2001). Divergence of verbal expression and embodied knowledge: Evidence from speech and gesture in children with specific language impairment. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 309331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, A., Daum, M. M., Walser, S., & Mast, F. W. (2009). Motor processes in children’s mental rotation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10(1–2), 1840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, A., Hansen, M. A., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). Development of mental rotation in 3-to- 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 28, 386399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R. B., Montgomery, J. W., Evans, J. L., & Gillam, S. L. (2019). Cognitive predictors of sentence comprehension in children with and without developmental language disorder: Implications for assessment and treatment. International Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 21(3), 240251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goffman, L. & Leonard, J. (2000). Growth of language skills in preschool children with specific language impairment: Implications for assessment and intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9(2), 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, L. (1999) Prosodic influences on speech production in children with specific language impairment and speech deficits: Kinematic, acoustic, and transcription evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 14991517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Brentari, D. (2017). Gesture, sign, and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, 160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002). From thought to hand: Structured and unstructured communication outside of conventional language. In Amsel, E. & Byrnes, J. P. (Eds.), Language, literacy, and cognitive development: The development and consequences of symbolic communication (pp. 127156). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). From action to abstraction: Gesture as a mechanism of change. Developmental Review, 38, 167184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., McNeill, D., & Singleton, J. (1996). Silence is liberating: Removing the handcuffs on grammatical expressions in the manual modality. Psychological Review, 103(1), 3455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., & Else-Quest, N. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: A meta- analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(1), 177195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, S. (2005). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Effect of phonological or semantic cues. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 14521467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenslade, K. J., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2009). The diagnostic accuracy and construct validity of the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test—Preschool: Second Edition. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 150160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heisler, L., Goffman, L., & Younger, B. (2010). Lexical and articulatory interactions in children’s language production. Developmental Science, 13(5), 722730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A., & Barnett, A. (2007). Movement Assessment Battery for Children—Second Edition (MABC-2). London, UK: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, E. L., Bishop, D.V. M., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Representational gestures in developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment: Error-types and the reliability of ratings. Human Movement Science, 17, 655678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. J. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Sequence-specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17(3), 352365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Boyes, M. (2019). Fast mapping short and long words: examining the influence of phonological short-term memory and receptive vocabulary in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Communication Disorders, 79, 1123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Boyes, M. (2020). Working, declarative, and procedural memory in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 117. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00135 Google ScholarPubMed
Kan, P. F. & Windsor, J. (2010). Word learning in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 739756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 1632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klem, M., Melby-Lervåg, M., Hagtvet, B., Halaas Lyster, S., Gustafsson, J., Hulme, C. (2015). Sentence repetition is a measure of children’s language skills rather than working memory limitations. Developmental Science, 18(1), 146154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krauss, R., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and Gesture (pp. 261283). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavelli, M. & Majorano (2016). Spontaneous gesture production and lexical abilities in children with specific language impairment in a naming task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 784796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment and their contribution to the study of language development. Journal of Child Language, 41(1), 3847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P., Karpicke, J. D., Christ, S. L., & Kueser, J. B. (2020). After initial retrieval practice, more retrieval produces better retention than more study in the word learning of children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 27632776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Karpicke, J., Deevy, P., Weber, C., Christ, S., Haebig, E., Souto, S., Kueser, J. B., & Krok, W. (2019). Retrieval-based word learning in young typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder I: The benefits of repeated retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62, 932943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Miller, C., & Gerber, E. (1999). Grammatical morphology and the lexicon in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(3), 678689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S. C., Goldin-Meadow, S., Carlson, M. T., & Hemani-Lopez, N. (2018). Mental transformation skill in young children: The role of concrete and abstract motor training. Cognitive Science, 42, 12071228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, S. K. & Johnson, R. E. (1989). American sign language: The phonological base. Sign Language Studies, 64, 195278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A. T., & Ullman, M. T. (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48, 11381154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mainela-Arnold, E., Alibali, M. W., Hostetter, A. B., Evans, J. L. (2014). Gesture-speech integration in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 761770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marentette, P. & Mayberry, R. I. (2000). Principles for an emerging phonological system: A case study of early ASL acquisition. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J., & Mayberry, R. (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 7190). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Marton, K. (2009). Imitation of body postures and hand movements in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., Arbisi-Kelm, T., Eden, N., & Oleson, J. (2020). The word learning profile of adults with developmental language disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 5, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., Licandro, U., Arenas, R., Eden, N., Stiles, D., Bean, A., & Walker, E. (2013a). Why words are hard for adults with developmental language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(6), 18451856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 9981014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., Oleson, J., Bahnsen, A., & Duff, D. (2013b). Children with developmental language impairment have vocabulary deficits characterized by limited breadth and depth. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 48(3), 307319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92(3), 350371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumford, K. H. & Kita, S. (2014). Children use gesture to interpret novel verb meanings. Child Development, 85(3), 11811189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perniss, P. & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perona, K., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2005). Diagnostic accuracy of the structured photographic Expressive language test: Third edition (SPELT-3). Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36(2), 103115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettenati, P. & Stefanini, S. (2010). Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language, 37, 887911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plym, J., Lahti-Nuuttila, P., Smolander, S., Arkkila, E., & Laasonen, M., (2021). Structure of cognitive functions in monolingual preschool children with typical development and children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 64, 31403158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redmond, S. M. & Rice, M. L. (2001). Detection of irregular verb violations by children with and without SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 655669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L. & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 12391257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, J. & Klee, T. (1987). Clinical assessment of oropharyngeal motor development in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 271277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saletta, M., Goffman, L., Ward, C., & Oleson, J. (2018). Influence of language load on speech motor skill in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 675689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandler, W. (1989). Phonological representation of the sign: Linearity and nonlinearity in American sign language. Providence, RI: Foris Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanjeevan, T. & Mainela-Arnold, E. (2019). Characterizing the motor skills in children with specific language impairment. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 71, 4255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanjeevan, T., Rosenbaum, D. A., Miller, C., van Hell, J. G., Weiss, D. J., & Mainela-Arnold, E. (2015). Motor issues in specific language impairment: A window into the underlying impairment. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2, 228236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savich, P. A. (1984). Anticipatory imagery ability in normal and language-disabled children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 494501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schopler, E., Van Bourgondien, M. E., Wellman, G. J., & Love, S. R. (2010). Childhood Autism Rating Scale—Second Edition (CARS-2). Torrance, CA: WPS Publishing.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Fourth Edition (CELF-4). Toronto, Canada: The Psychological Corporation/A Harcourt Assessment Company.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriberg, L. D., Gruber, F. A., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1994). Developmental phonological disorders III: Long-term speech-sound normalization. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 37(5), 11511177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriberg, L. D., Tomblin, J. B., & McSweeny, J. (1999). Prevalence pf speech delay in 6-year- old children and comorbidity with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 14611481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolak, E., McGregor, K. K., Arbisi-Kelm, T., & Eden, N. (2020). Sustained attention in developmental language disorder and its relation to working memory and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 40964108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X (2007). Procedural learning and adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Language Learning and Development, 3(4), 269293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 12451260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N., Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(6), 12841294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T. & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T., Earle, F. S., Walenski, M., & Janacsek, K. (2020). The neurocognition of developmental disorders of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 389417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogt, S. & Kauschke, C. (2017). Observing iconic gestures enhances word learning in typically developing children and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 44, 14581484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuolo, J. & Goffman, L. (2018). Language skill mediates the relationship between language load and articulatory variability in children with language and speech sound disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 30103022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuolo, J., Goffman, L., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2017). Deficits in coordinative bimanual timing precision in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 393405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, C., Norbury, C. F., & Alcock, K. (2016). Gestural abilities of children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(2), 164182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, C., Saunders, N., McGuire, R., Cousins, G., Norbury, C. F. (2017). Gesture production in language impairment: It’s quality, not quantity, that matters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 969982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelaznik, H. N. & Goffman, L. (2010). Motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 283293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Group comparisons, means, and standard deviations for standardized tests at time point one

Figure 1

Figure 1. Experimental Protocol and Gesture Stimuli Presentation Schematic with Stimuli Exemplars and Prompts. CARS-2: Childhood Autism Rating Scale-2; CMMS-3: Columbia Mental Maturity Scale-3; SPELT-P2/SPELT-3: Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool 2 or Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-Preschool 3; FVM: Finite Verb Morphology; BBTOP: Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology; MABC-2: Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2; CELF-4: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4.

Figure 2

Table 2. Novel gestures and object referents

Figure 3

Table 3. CELF-4 subtest group comparisons for the longitudinal cohort at time point three

Figure 4

Table 4. MABC-2 longitudinal cohort group means and standard deviations between time points

Figure 5

Table 5. Longitudinal cohort learning probes mean percent correct and standard deviations

Figure 6

Figure 2. Mean Phonological Accuracy by Feature Across Pre- and Posttest and Between Groups with Standard Error. The maximum overall phonological accuracy score is six points. When accuracy scores are separated by phonological feature (shape, path, and orientation), a maximum of two points per feature is possible.

Figure 7

Figure 3. Mean Phonological Accuracy by Feature Across Time Points and Between Groups with Standard Error. The maximum overall phonological accuracy score is six points. When accuracy scores are separated by phonological feature (shape, path, and orientation), a maximum of two points per feature is possible.

Figure 8

Table 6. Hierarchical analysis for the DLD group CELF-4 recalling sentences time point three language outcome (n=14)

Figure 9

Figure 4. Time Point One Significant Predictors for Time Point Three Outcome CELF-4 Recalling Sentences Scores. a) Time point one mean handshape phonological accuracy; b) time point one mean orientation phonological accuracy; c) time point one nonverbal CMMS-3 scores.

Figure 10

Figure 5. Time Point One Mean Handshape Phonological Accuracy and Time Point Three MABC-2 Manual Dexterity Scores.