Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T06:47:48.327Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is grammatical tone item-based or process-based?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2023

Hannah Sande*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article considers the question of what constitutes item-based morphology, with a specific look at grammatical tone. Numerous case studies of grammatical tone are examined in light of the debate on whether morphology is item-based or process-based. In each case, tonal alternations are an exponent, sometimes the sole exponent, of some grammatical feature. Two of the case studies are examples of grammatical tone that can straightforwardly be analysed as involving concatenated morphophonological forms; however, in other cases, the grammatical tone cannot be reduced to the concatenation of a tonal affix or phonological feature with some stem. The latter type cannot straightforwardly be analysed as item-based, but if still phonologically predictable and productive, is not satisfactorily analysed as suppletive. This article suggests a set of diagnostics that can be used to determine whether a given phenomenon is best analysed as item-based, process-based or suppletive. Then, an analysis is presented in Cophonologies by Phase (CbP), where morphosyntactic features can be mapped not only to underlying phonological items, but also to morpheme-specific constraint weight adjustments. CbP allows for what may have been traditionally called item-based and process-based morphology to co-exist in a single framework.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

There is a long-standing debate in the phonological literature about where phonological exceptionality is located in the grammar. If a given morpheme triggers or undergoes some phonological process not found elsewhere in the language, that exceptionality could be analysed as due to some exceptional underlying representation. Alternatively, the presence of a particular morpheme could trigger an exceptional rule, process or construction-specific phonological grammar. The former are often referred to as item-based and the latter as process-based accounts. There is a prominent claim in the literature that all morphology is item-based (see e.g. Bye & Svenonius Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012). In such a view, all exceptional phonology must be accounted for through differences in underlying representations of distinct morphemes,Footnote 1 and there is no morpheme-specific phonological grammar. Other approaches assume, on the basis of the existence of morphological deletion or reordering of segments as opposed to the addition of material, that all phonology is process-based (Anderson Reference Anderson1992). Other proponents of the existence of process-based morphophonology do not deny the existence of item-based phonology, but allow for the presence of both item- and process-based morphophonology in the same model. One recurring issue in this debate is the question of where to draw the line between item-based and process-based morphology.

This article considers a number of cases of grammatical tone, or morpheme-specific tonal alternations. Grammatical tone is morpheme-specific by definition, which makes it a challenge to model using regular, fully general phonology applying to underlying phonological forms. Some of the cases considered can straightforwardly be analysed as item-based, in the traditional sense, whereby the addition of phonological underlying forms, plus the application of the general phonology of the language, results in the correct surface forms. Others are not phonologically natural or productive, and are best analysed as involving suppletive allomorphs that differ in tone. The final two case studies show that not all morphophonology is straightforwardly analysable as item-based; however, this categorisation into item-based versus process-based depends on the definition of what qualifies as an item. These case studies raise questions of how we define an item, and inform the status of the debate on whether all morphology is item-based.

The Cophonologies by Phase (CbP) model, in which vocabulary items can consist of an item-based component and/or a process-based component, is shown to be able to account for both item- and process-based morphophonology. CbP is, in a sense, item-based, in that morphosyntactic features are mapped to listed phonological information. However, the listed phonological information may contain an item (underlying phonological form) or a process (a morpheme-specific constraint weighting); in this way, traditional notions of item-based and process-based phonology are combined in a single model. Instances of grammatical tone, which are construction-specific by nature, especially benefit from the expanded vocabulary items of CbP, and this article walks through how cases along the item-to-process continuum can all be modelled in this single framework. Various predictions of this model are shown to be borne out in grammatical tone processes across languages, and diagnostics are proposed for determining which instances of grammatical tone are best analysed as introducing underlying items versus morpheme-specific phonological grammars.

§1.1 discusses the item-versus-process debate, and compares the predictions of existing item-based accounts with those that allow for process-based morphophonology. §1.2 provides background on tonal systems, focusing on grammatical tone. §1.3 presents a road map to this article.

1.1 Background on the item-versus-process debate

This section summarises the debate about whether phonological alternations specific to a morphosyntactic context should be analysed as triggered by an underlying phonological item (Lieber Reference Lieber1992; Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Trommer2012; Bye & Svenonius Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012; Trommer & Zimmermann Reference Trommer and Zimmermann2014), or whether a morpheme or morphosyntactic context can trigger a phonological process, rule or constraint (Hockett Reference Hockett1954; Anderson Reference Anderson1992; Sande Reference Sande2018). Adopting Hockett’s terminology, in an item-and-arrangement model, all morphology is item-based. In a recent implementation of a purely item-based account, Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012: 495) state that they ‘have found no cases in which process morphology is needed, suggesting that in fact there is no non-concatenative morphology: all morphology is concatenative, in a very straightforward sense’. Any apparent non-catenative effect or process is, for Bye & Svenonius, the result of concatenation and phonological interpretation of underlying items. That is, each set of morphosyntactic features is mapped to one or more underlying phonological forms or items, which are concatenated and subject to the general phonological computation of the language, resulting in surface forms. In item-and-arrangement approaches, henceforth referred to as simply item-based, any morpheme-specific phonological alternation or phonotactic restriction must be due to differences in underlying representations. In short, item-based accounts, as Hockett (Reference Hockett1954: 212) states, are concerned with ‘things and the arrangements in which those things occur’. A suppletive analysis, in which a single set of morphosyntactic features is associated with multiple suppletive forms sensitive to context, can be considered a subtype of item-based analysis, since differences in surface forms are attributed to different underlying items.

On the other hand, a process-based analysis (an item-and-process model, in Hockett’s terms) allows for morphosyntactic features to trigger phonological processes through morpheme- or construction-specific rules or constraint rankings. There are many types of process-based accounts, some of which assume that all morphology is process-based (see e.g. Anderson Reference Anderson1992). Most process-based accounts, however, allow for morphemes to be additive (item-based) or to trigger processes. These approaches include, among others, Indexed Constraint Theory, which allows for item-based morphemes as well as morpheme-specific constraints that result in morpheme-specific phonological processes (Itô & Mester Reference Itô, Mester, Kager, Hulst and Zonneveld1999; Pater Reference Pater and Parker2009), and Cophonology Theory, which allows for item-based morphemes as well as morpheme-specific or construction-specific phonological grammars (Orgun Reference Orgun1996; Anttila Reference Anttila2002; Inkelas & Zoll Reference Inkelas and Zoll2005, Reference Inkelas and Zoll2007). See Uchihara & Cano (Reference Uchihara and Cano2020: §1.2) for a recent overview of process-based approaches, and an example implementation of a mixed item-and-process approach. Approaches that allow for morpheme-specific processes are in stark contrast to those which assume that all morphology is item-based, such as Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012), mentioned above.Footnote 2

Hockett (Reference Hockett1954) points out that process-based approaches likely pre-date item-based ones; many early descriptions of Native American languages contain wording consistent with a process-based approach (Harris Reference Harris1944; Haas Reference Haas and Osgood1946). However, item-and-arrangement approaches were perhaps formalised earlier (e.g. in The Sound Pattern of English; Chomsky & Halle Reference Chomsky and Halle1968).

Canonical arguments against item-and-arrangement approaches come from morphology that does not obviously involve a one-to-one, additive, form-to-meaning mapping: zero-realisation, subtractive morphology, portmanteau morphs, ablaut or umlaut, morphologically motivated metathesis; and from cases where it is not obvious how a single underlying item could result in the many possible surface allomorphs of a given morpheme: chain shifts, reduplication, etc. (see Hockett Reference Hockett1954 and Anderson Reference Anderson1992: Chapter 3). Anderson (Reference Anderson1992) points out that while additive morphology can be stated as a process (X $\rightarrow $ X+affix), phenomena such as morpheme-specific metathesis and chain shifts cannot be analysed as item-based (p. 72) without ‘trivializing the problem’ (p. 67). On the basis of these arguments, Anderson rejects classical item-based morphemes and treats all morphology as rule-governed relations. Others have taken this statement as a challenge, attempting to account for apparently non–item-based phenomena in item-based ways. For example, Trommer & Zimmermann (Reference Trommer and Zimmermann2014) analyse subtractive morphology through the addition of an underlying morphological item, namely a defective mora.

As Inkelas (Reference Inkelas2014: 64) states, ‘Subtractive morphology has served as the strongest argument that morphological constructions are, at least in some cases, processual, in the sense that they cannot be analysed by means of the addition of a morpheme’. Trommer & Zimmermann (Reference Trommer and Zimmermann2014) and Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2017) provide an analysis of subtractive morphology that relies on defective underlying autosegmental representations: affixing a defective mora results in leaving an underlying segment or syllable unpronounced on the surface. They argue that the fact that there are additive representations which, with the right inventory of phonological constraints, can result in an optimal form with less surface structure than input structure, supports the view that all morphology can be analysed as additive (or more specifically concatenative). The idea is that if subtraction can be additive, all other morphology is analysable as additive as well.

Canonical arguments against item-and-process approaches have to do with worries that they are too powerful; however, as Hockett (Reference Hockett1954) & Anderson (Reference Anderson1992) point out, these fears may have more to do with the fact that a specific formalism of process-based accounts is (or was, at the time) lacking. If one limits the types of processes possible in an item-and-process account, such an approach can be as restrictive as an item-and-arrangement account (see also computational work showing that item-based and process-based accounts are computationally equivalent (Karttunen Reference Karttunen and Gelbukh2003; Roark & Sproat Reference Roark and Sproat2007), or in fact that some process-based approaches are computationally simpler than item-based approaches (Dolatian et al. Reference Dolatian, Rawski and Heinz2021; Rawski et al. Reference Rawski, Dolatian, Heinz and Raimy2023)).

Perhaps one reason this debate has been so long-lived in the theoretical literature is that there are some morphological phenomena in human language that clearly seem best analysed as item-based (i.e. those where a meaning maps to a single (additive) form in the same way across a paradigm), and there are others that are difficult to model in an item-based way under reasonably standard assumptions about underlying forms, and are more straightforwardly analysed as processes: chain shifts, metathesis, ablaut/umlaut, etc. This article follows a recent line of work adopting the view that morphology need not be fully item-based or fully process-based. For example, Uchihara & Cano (Reference Uchihara and Cano2020: 810) assume that ‘productive and automatic’ alternations are derived through items subject to phonological manipulation, while also allowing for the existence of morphologically specific phonological processes. Here, I formalise the co-existence of item-based and process-based morphology in CbP (Sande Reference Sande2019; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020). Like item-based accounts, CbP assumes there are lists of morphemes in the lexicon, but it allows for morphemes to be associated with both items (underlying phonological forms) and processes. CbP centres the morpheme, which includes both item- and process-based components, as the molecular unit of word-formation; thus, in CbP, the question is not whether all morphology is item-based or process-based, but rather which morphological realisations are due to the presence of an underlying phonological form (item, $\mathcal {F}$ ) versus a morpheme-specific phonological grammar (process, $\mathcal {R}$ ). CbP is introduced in more detail in §4.1.

1.2 Background on grammatical tone

Tonal systems are common but understudied (Welmers Reference Welmers1959; Hyman Reference Hyman, Goldsmith, Riggle and Alan2011; McPherson Reference McPherson2019). Tonal phenomena have many insights to offer the theoretical literature, since as Hyman (Reference Hyman, Goldsmith, Riggle and Alan2011) points out, tone systems have properties not found in other phonological systems. Hyman (Reference Hyman, Goldsmith, Riggle and Alan2011: 198) finds ‘that tone can do everything that segmental and metrical phonology can do, but that the reverse is not true’.

Of particular relevance to this article is grammatical tone. In a thorough recent overview, Rolle (Reference Rolle2018: 1) defines grammatical tone as ‘a tonological operation that is not general across the phonological grammar, and is restricted to the context of a specific morpheme or construction, or a natural class of morphemes or constructions’. I adopt Rolle’s definition of grammatical tone here. Tonological operations include at least the addition, deletion or replacement of tone; tonal assimilation or dissimilation; tonal polarity; tonal spreading and tonal shift. Grammatical tonological operations are those triggered in a particular morphosyntactic context, rather than those that are generally applied in a particular phonologically determined context. For recent examples of work on grammatical tone in particular languages or language families, see Trommer (Reference Trommer2011), Cruz (Reference Cruz2011), McKendry (Reference McKendry2013), Campbell (Reference Campbell2014), McPherson (Reference McPherson2014, Reference McPherson, DiCanio, Malins, Good, Michelson, Jaeger and Keily2016, Reference McPherson2019), Harry & Hyman (Reference Harry and Hyman2014), Odden & Bickmore (Reference Odden and Bickmore2014), Villard (Reference Villard2015), McIntosh (Reference McIntosh2015), Sullivant (Reference Sullivant2015), Marlo et al. (Reference Marlo, Mwita and Paster2015), Kim (Reference Kim, Panacar and Léonard2016), Palancar & Léonard (Reference Palancar and Léonard2016), Zimmermann (Reference Zimmermann2016), Kubozono (Reference Kubozono2016), McPherson & Heath (Reference McPherson and Heath2016), Sande (Reference Sande2018), Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021), Meyase (Reference Meyase2021) and Rodriguez (Reference Rodriguez2021).

There have been numerous approaches to grammatical tone in the literature, ranging from purely concatenative, item-based analyses (e.g. Hayes Reference Hayes1986 on morpheme-specific tone spreading in autosegmental phonology) to process-based analyses (e.g. Sande Reference Sande2018 on Guébie scalar tone shift in a cophonologies account). In a mixed approach, Rolle (Reference Rolle2018) analyses grammatical tone as involving underlying floating tones, or items. However, he combines this item-based approach with the process-based approach of Cophonology Theory, where morpheme-specific or construction-specific phonological grammars determine whether the tone of the base is overwritten by the floating grammatical tones or not.

The nature of tone, as discussed by Yip (Reference Yip2002) and Hyman (Reference Hyman, Goldsmith, Riggle and Alan2011), including its ability to dock non-locally to its morphological host, its ability to spread over many syllables and even many words and its sometimes paradigmatic nature, leads to numerous grammatical tone patterns that appear difficult to analyse in an item-based way. This makes tone an excellent testing ground for item-based versus process-based models of morphophonology. This article categorises grammatical tone processes as concatenative, or item-based, on the one hand, or as non-concatenative, or process-based, on the other. In CbP, this distinction boils down to which morphological realisations are due to the presence of some underlying phonological form ( $\mathcal {F}$ ) versus a morpheme-specific constraint weight readjustment ( $\mathcal {R}$ ). This categorisation is based on a set of diagnostics developed in §2 and throughout the article.

1.3 Road map

§2 discusses cases of grammatical tone that have been or can be straightforwardly analysed as item-based. §3 discusses cases of grammatical tone that are not easily analysed as item-based in the traditional sense; in fact, both case studies considered in this section have been specifically argued to not involve underlying morphophonological items. §4 presents an analysis in CbP in which morphemes can be associated with underlying phonological forms (traditional items) or morpheme-specific phonological grammars or cophonologies (processes) in a unified way. One example each of apparently item-based and of apparently process-based grammatical tones are modelled in CbP. §5 discusses the implications of the data and analysis presented here and compares the proposed account with alternatives. §6 concludes.

2. Item-based grammatical tone

This section presents a number of case studies of grammatical tone that can be straightforwardly analysed as item-based. These include cases of clearly additive tone, where the addition of some tonal exponent to the tonal melody of a stem corresponds to the addition of the meaning of a morphosyntactic feature across a paradigm. Also included in this section are cases of grammatical tone that are best analysed as involving suppletive allomorphy, where a segmentally identical but tonally distinct and not phonologically predictable allomorph is present in a small subset of morphosyntactic contexts. Suppletion is characterised here as an item-based account because, like other item-based analyses, different underlying representations result in distinct surface forms.

Additive grammatical tone is often analysed as a morpheme whose underlying form is a floating tone, which docks to the segmental material of another morpheme. Docking or association of tones to tone-bearing units (TBUs) takes place during the phonological computation. The floating tone may be the sole realisation of some morpheme, as we will see in Guébie in §2.2, or it may be accompanied by changes or additions to the segmental structure as in some tense/aspect paradigms in Ebira in §2.1.

Suppletive allomorphs are those that cannot be derived from a single underlying form in a phonologically predictable way (Spencer Reference Spencer1991; Mel’čuk Reference Mel’čuk1994; Bredemann Reference Bredemann2022). Rolle (Reference Rolle2018: 25–26) identifies a set of traits of tonal suppletive allomorphs.Footnote 3 First, a morpheme has two or more underlying allomorphs that are tonally distinct but segmentally identical. Second, the choice between these allomorphs is determined by a systematic factor in the linguistic environment. And lastly, other morphemes that appear in the same environment do not show the same tonal alternation. The last point is key in differentiating completely phonologically predictable and phonologically derivable tonal alternations from unproductive suppletive alternations. See also Iosad (Reference Iosad2014) on diagnostics for additive versus suppletive morphophonology.

Cases of tone sandhi or tone replacement, when they apply only to a small subset of morphemes and are not phonologically regular, fall into the category of suppletive grammatical tone.Footnote 4 We examine one such case from Standard Mandarin Chinese in §2.3. For an analysis of suppletive tone sandhi in an African language, see McPherson (Reference McPherson2019) on Seenku (Mande, Burkina Faso), and for a suppletive analysis of tonal paradigms in an American language, see Kim (this volume) on Amuzgo. The Ebira and Guébie case studies in §2.1 and §2.2 also involve suppletion in certain corners of the paradigm.

Throughout this article, the diagnostics in (1) are used to determine whether a grammatical tone pattern involves an additive underlying phonological item, listed suppletive allomorphs or morpheme-specific phonology. If a morpheme has a consistent realisation across a paradigm (the same segment(s) or suprasegment(s) can be pointed to as realising a morpheme in all cells of the paradigm where that meaning is present), if it is phonologically derivable through natural phonological rules or constraints and if it adheres to the general phonological rules or constraints of a language, it is best analysed through the addition of an underlying item. If a morpheme surfaces with inconsistent realisation across a paradigm, and is neither phonologically derivable nor subject to the general phonology of the language, it is likely best analysed as suppletive (cf. Rolle’s diagnostics for suppletion, above). If a grammatical meaning is phonologically derivable and predictable, but not by the general phonology of the language, and is not necessarily realised consistently across a paradigm, it is best analysed as due to a morpheme-specific phonological grammar or process. Process-based grammatical tone is discussed further in §3.

In other words, for any alternation, we can ask 1) whether it can be attributed to the addition of a single item (consistent realisation), 2) whether it can be derived by a single phonological process (modelled as rules or constraints) and 3) whether it is derivable by the general phonology of the language. If the answer to all of these questions is no, the alternation is best analysed as suppletive. If the answer to all of these questions is yes, the alternation is likely best analysed as item-based. If, however, the alternation cannot be derived via the regular phonology of the language (i.e. no to question 3), but is phonological derivable and predictable (yes to question 2), it is likely best analysed as a morpheme-specific process.

Note that whether a particular alternation is phonologically derivable may depend on one’s assumptions about possible underlying representations, rules or constraints. Here, I assume that any process that is phonologically natural and can be accounted for by rules or constraints independently needed in human language is phonologically derivable.

There are many cases of clearly additive or suppletive grammatical tone that are not discussed here. The case studies selected in this section are meant to be representative of grammatical tone that can be straightforwardly analysed as item-based, that is, as additive or suppletive.

2.1 Ebira subject + TAMP forms

In Ebira (Benue-Congo, Nigeria), tense, aspect, mood and polarity are exponed through vowel length, vowel quality and tonal alternations on subject markers (Rolle Reference Rolle2022) in what are referred to as stamp (subject, tense, aspect, mood and polarity) paradigms (Anderson Reference Anderson2011, Reference Anderson, Mietzner and Storch2015, Reference Anderson, Payne, Pacchiarotti and Bosire2016; Garvin et al. Reference Garvin, Russell and Sande2022). The first person stamp paradigm in Ebira is shown in (2), from Scholz (Reference Scholz1976: 53–54, 65–66, 107), cited by Rolle (Reference Rolle2022). While Rolle uses a dot under a vowel to denote [ $-$ ATR], I have translated his notation into the IPA. There are four surface tone heights in Ebira: super-high (S), high (H), mid (M) and low (L). Vowels unmarked for tone are mid-toned. High tones are marked with an acute accent and low tones with a grave accent. Super-high tones are marked with two acute accents on a single vowel. Vowel length is not underlyingly contrastive, but long and super-long vowels appear in derived contexts.

Stamp markers in Ebira agree with verbal roots in ATR. The verbal root itself does not alternate with tamp categories, as shown for the verb ‘come’ in (2). In analysing the stamp morphs in Ebira, Rolle shows that we need not say that every person marker has a suppletive set of 11 different lexically listed allomorphs sensitive to tamp context. Rather, there is predictability in the stamp paradigms such that each person feature and each tamp feature can be analysed as contributing a distinct underlying phonological item, and these phonological items interact in predictable ways to determine the output form in each cell of the paradigm. For example, in the habitual form, all stamp markers are mid-toned with a long vowel, whereas in the completive, they are all HL-toned with a short non-high vowel (the [ $-$ ATR] counterpart of [e] is [a], so the vowel can be low in the context of a [ $-$ ATR] verb). [ $+$ ATR] forms are given in (3).Footnote 5

Rolle analyses the person markers as having the underlying forms in (4), each linked to a single mora, and with no underlying tone. Capital symbols represent vowels underspecified for ATR features, since ATR on person markers is determined by the ATR value of the verbal root.

Habitual stamp morphs are consistently realised as long vowels. Thus, the habitual morpheme is analysed as a mid tone linked to a mora, which associates with the vowel of the person marker, lengthening the vowel. The completive is consistently realised with a HL tone melody and a non-high vowel, and is analysed as an underlying floating HL tone and a floating [A] segment. The floating HL docks to the mora of the person marker, and the floating A segment interacts with the vowel quality of the person marker’s underlying vowel to result in a surface non-high vowel. Similarly, the remaining stamp paradigms are attributed to combinations of underlying items interacting in predictable ways.

It is worth mentioning that certain corners of the stamp paradigm are phonologically unpredictable and thus must be analysed as involving suppletion. For example, in the subjunctive, the first- and second-person stamp markers surface with a mid tone, but the third-person ones surface with a low tone. Thus, the subjunctive does not have a consistent realisation across the paradigm. Rolle analyses this as due to suppletive subjunctive allomorphs sensitive to person. The third-person allomorph has a floating low tone, and the elsewhere allomorph a floating mid tone. The [ $+$ ATR] subjunctive paradigm is given in (5).

To summarise, the Ebira completive, habitual and person-marking morphemes are analysed as additive, but the subjunctive as suppletive. The table in (6) considers the Ebira facts based on the diagnostics presented in (1).

Recall that the Ebira subjunctive person markers are realised with a low tone in some contexts, but a mid tone in others. While it is in principle possible to derive a low tone from a mid tone, it is not clear what the phonological motivation would be in the context of only the third-person subjunctive morphemes, since neither the third person nor the subjunctive is otherwise marked with a low tone, and there is otherwise no difference in phonological context. Additionally, mid tones do not surface as low in the relevant phonological context in general in the language. Thus, the completive, habitual and person marking morphemes match the diagnostics for item-based, additive morphology, whereas the subjunctive in Ebira is best analysed as suppletive.

The habitual, completive and subjunctive are three of many stamp paradigms in Ebira analysed by Rolle (Reference Rolle2022). Throughout the Ebira paradigms, he analyses long vowels in stamp morphs as the result of the underlying person marker plus a tamp morpheme that contributes another mora. The change in vowel quality to a mid (or low) vowel is proposed to be due to the addition of a non-high [A] vowel. Changes in tone across the paradigm are due to floating or linked tones that originate with tamp morphemes and associate with the mora of the person marker. With a few exceptions that are analysed as involving suppletion, such as the third-person subjunctive forms discussed above, stamp markers in Ebira can be straightforwardly analysed as combinations of underlying person markers (4) plus tamp morphs that contribute moras, floating tones and additional segmental material. No morpheme-specific or exceptional phonological processes apply, since a single set of functions for associating underlying forms can derive the correct set of surface forms across contexts.

2.2 Guébie negation as adding a high tone

In Guébie, a Kru language spoken in Southwest Côte d’Ivoire, negation is exponed by a high tone on the subject noun phrase. The data presented here were collected by the author in collaboration with the Guébie community of Gnagbodougnoa, Côte d’Ivoire, between 2013 and 2022.

Guébie has four contrastive tone heights labelled 1–4, of which 4 is the highest. All morphemes, with the exception of the definite suffix (Sande Reference Sande2017), are associated with an underlying tone melody, and surface with that tone melody unless some phonological or grammatical tonal alternation applies. Basic clausal word order is SAuxOV or SVO. There are many grammatical tone processes in the language. This section focuses on grammatical tone in negative contexts, and §3.2 focuses on grammatical tone in imperfective contexts, which is realised as a scalar tone shift.

Non-pronominal subjects surface with their default lexical tone melodies in positive perfective contexts. In negative contexts, negation is realised by the addition of a high tone to the end of the tonal melody of the subject. The subject’s lexical tone melody is not overwritten; it still surfaces, but is followed by an additional level H tone. When the subject already has a H tone, the result is vowel lengthening to realise the subject’s high tone as well as the high tone of the negative morpheme. A lack of lengthening would result in no difference between positive and negative contexts. In (7), each possible tone of the final word of a non-pronominal subject is listed in its positive and negative forms.

The addition of a final high tone in negative contexts results in a surface tone melody predictable from the general tonal association principles of the language, with tones of stems and their affixes associated with TBUs from left to right, potentially creating contour tones at the right edge of the word. Thus, the realisation of the negative is consistent across the paradigm, is phonologically derivable and is sensitive to the general phonology (tone association principles) of the language.

The final high tone is added to the end of the tone melody of the final word of the subject. This could be a bare noun, as in (8a) and (8b), or even a relative clause marker if there is a relative clause in subject position as in (8c) and (8d). In most cases, the result is a contour tone on the final syllable of the subject.

When the final word of the subject ends in a high tone, the final vowel of the word is lengthened to host the tone of the negative morpheme, as in (9). Otherwise, the vowel is not lengthened, as in (8b) and (8d). If the vowel were not lengthened in negative contexts after a tone 4, there would be no difference between positive and negative contexts.

In all cases, we see the addition of a high tone to the end of the perfective tonal melody in negative contexts. This alternation can be analysed as the negative contributing a floating H tone, which docks to the right edge of the subject. The general tone association principles of the language, which associate tones with TBUs in a one-to-one manner from left to right, also seem to apply in negative contexts with non-pronominal subjects.

When the subject is a pronoun, we see an irregular correlation between positive and negative tones on the pronominal subject. We cannot assume that the same high-tone item present in the context of a non-pronominal subject is the exponent of negation in pronominal contexts, since we do not see the same tonal interactions. If the same tonal association rules or constraints as in (7) applied in (10), we would expect the surface form of the 1sg pronoun to have a 44 tone melody, the 2sg pronoun to have a 24 melody, and the 3sg pronoun to have a 34 melody. This is not what we see. Instead, there is no regular tonal change between the positive and negative forms of pronouns, and negative forms of pronouns are best analysed through suppletive allomorphy.

For example, when there is a third-person singular human subject pronoun, it surfaces with tone 3 in positive contexts like (11a) but tone 24 in negative contexts like (11b). The tone difference on the verb in (11) is due to a separate grammatical tone process in positive imperfective contexts, discussed further in §3.2; it is unrelated to the negative tone pattern.

To summarise, in non-pronominal contexts, a high tone (tone 4) is transparently added to the end of the subject’s tone melody in negative contexts. This can be analysed as a high-tone affix or floating tone. However, when the subject is a pronoun, there is no way to derive the surface tone patterns from an underlying pronominal tone plus a negative high tone. In fact, the pronominal tone paradigm does not show any consistent tonal difference between positive and negative forms across persons; thus, the negative morpheme cannot be analysed as additive in pronominal contexts, and must instead be suppletive. Much like stamp forms in Ebira, we see that negation in Guébie can be analysed as item-based, with a systematic association of underlying forms to derive the surface forms, in most cases. However, in a corner of the paradigm, namely, when the subject is a pronoun, negation must be analysed as suppletive. This pattern is summarised in (12).

Comparing the Guébie facts, summarised in (12) with the diagnostics in (1), we see that the Guébie negation pattern in non-pronominal contexts matches the diagnostics for item-based morphology, whereas negation with pronominal subjects matches the diagnostics for suppletion.

2.3 Mandarin tonal allomorphy

This section considers Standard Mandarin Chinese morpheme-specific tonal alternations (Chang Reference Chang1992; Chen Reference Chen2002; Wang Reference Wang2014; Yang Reference Yang2015; Rolle Reference Rolle2018). For the remainder of this section, I refer to Standard Mandarin Chinese as simply Mandarin. Mandarin is a tonal language, where many lexical items are distinguished from each other based on their tonal patterns. There are four distinct tonal melodies in Mandarin, given in three different notation schemes in (13).

Henceforth, I use the numerical system (the middle column in (13)) to notate the underlying and surface tone patterns in Mandarin. There is a tone replacement process that applies to exactly four morphemes in the language. These four words surface with a 35 tone melody before a morpheme with a 53 tone melody, as in (14).

These morphemes surface with their lexical tones when preceding melodies other than 53, as in (15a). They surface as 35 before 53 melodies as in (15b). Other segmentally and tonally identical morphemes do not surface with a 35 melody in the same phonological contexts, as with /bu $^{53}$ / ‘division’ in (15c), which is segmentally and tonally identical to /bu $^{53}$ / ‘not’ (Chen Reference Chen2002: 22).

This process cannot be described as phonologically or morphosyntactically regular, but is instead limited to the four morphemes in (14). Because this process is not generally applicable or phonologically predictable, it is best analysed as a case of suppletive allomorphy in which the four morphemes in (14) each have two listed allomorphs, one with tone 35 and the other with tone 53 or 55. The 35 allomorph is selected in the context of a following 53 melody, but the default allomorph is selected in all other contexts. (16) summarises the Mandarin pattern in terms of the diagnostics in (1).

Rather than assuming that these four morphemes have different underlying forms from other 55- or 53-toned morphemes, which result in their distinct behaviour before tone-53 morphemes, the canonical analysis instead posits suppletive allomorphy. However, suppletive analyses are similar to other item-based analyses in that the different surface forms are attributable to differences in underlying representation.

2.4 Summary of item-based grammatical tone

Almost all surface forms in the Ebira stamp and Guébie negation paradigms can be straightforwardly analysed as combinations of underlying phonological items subject to the general phonotactic and phonological principles of the language. In Ebira, there is a consistent contribution of each person/number across tense/aspect/mood paradigms (e.g. a high round vowel in second person forms), and there is a consistent realisation of the habitual and completive morphemes across the person/number paradigm (e.g. the addition of a mid tone and extra vowel length, analysed as a mora, in habitual forms). In Guébie, any time the subject of a sentence is not a pronoun, negation corresponds to the addition of a high tone at the end of the subject. A suppletive or morpheme-specific account of these instances of grammatical tone would not be informative. There is no morpheme-specific phonology at work; rather, the general phonology of the language is sufficient. The phonological alternations across the relevant paradigms are predictable based on the addition of some underlying item plus concatenation and general phonology.

The suppletive patterns we have seen here all involve phonologically exceptional tonal alternations sensitive to context that cannot be phonologically derived in a unified way. In Mandarin, four morphemes surface with a 35 tone melody before 53 tones, but no other morphemes, even phonologically identical ones, also show this alternation. In Ebira, most of the stamp paradigms are straightforwardly analysed as additive floating tones, length (moras) and vowel features. However, third-person forms show exceptional tonal melodies in subjunctive contexts, such that multiple suppletive subjunctive morphemes are proposed, one of which appears in third-person contexts, and the other of which surfaces elsewhere. Similarly, in Guébie, negation typically adds a high tone to the end of the subject. However, in the case of pronominal subjects, negation is not consistently realised, and suppletive positive and negative subject pronouns which differ only in tone are proposed. All such cases are analysed as tonal allomorphy, in which each alternating morpheme has multiple tonally distinct allomorphs whose presence is sensitive to morphosyntactic and/or phonological context.

Because a suppletive account assumes that distinct underlying forms are responsible for the differences between surface forms, such accounts constitute a type of item-based analyses.

There are many other cases of grammatical tone analysed in the literature as item-based. Not represented in the case studies presented here are American tone languages; for an example of an item-based analysis of an American tonal language, see Uchihara & Cano (Reference Uchihara and Cano2020) on Tlapanec.

While this section has considered cases of grammatical tone that are straightforwardly additive or suppletive (per the diagnostics in (1)), not all instances of grammatical tone clearly fit into one of these two categories. Specifically, some cases of grammatical tone are phonologically predictable and derivable, but they involve tonal alternations that are not part of the regular phonology of the language, or that are not obviously the result of adding an underlying item. The following section considers two such cases, and determines that such phenomena cannot be considered item-based.

3. Morpheme-specific grammatical tone processes

This section considers cases of grammatical tone that are neither obviously additive nor best analysed as suppletive; they are not item-based. These instances of grammatical tone fall under the label of ‘process morphology’: they seem to involve an easily describable, phonologically predictable and phonologically derivable process; however, that process cannot obviously be attributed to the presence of some underlying item, nor is it a regular phonological alternation that applies equally in all morphosyntactic contexts in the language. According to Anderson (Reference Anderson1992: 59), one set of problematic cases for item-based morphology are ‘those in which there does not seem to be any plausible analysis of some morphological element in terms of an isolable, identifiable “affix” that could constitute the phonological shape of the morpheme’. It is exactly these cases that we turn to in this section.

The case studies presented here include across-the-board tonal polarity on nominative modifiers in Kipsigis (Nilo-Saharan) and scalar tone shift in Guébie (Kru). Such phenomena have been modelled using morpheme-specific constraints or multiple phonological grammars within a single language (see Sande Reference Sande2018 and Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020 on Guébie, and Kouneli & Nie Reference Kouneli and Nie2021 on Kipsigis).

3.1 Across-the-board tonal polarity in Kipsigis

This section presents data from Kipsigis, a Kalenjin (Southern Nilotic) language spoken by about 2 million speakers in and around Kenya. The data presented here come from Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) and additional discussion with Kouneli & Nie. They thank 12 Kipsigis speakers in their article, including 2 in the United States and 10 in Kenya. This section presents the tonal facts of the language, focusing on tonal polarity in nominative modifiers, then discusses why a traditional item-based approach to Kipsigis tonal polarity is not ideal. §4.3 presents a novel process-based analysis of the Kipsigis facts.

Let us begin with some relevant phonological background on Kipsigis. There are two contrastive tone heights: H and L. HL contours are attested on heavy syllables, which include those with long vowels or short vowels plus a sonorant coda: kárâ:rán, ‘beautiful.sg’. When one morpheme ends in a vowel and the next begins with a vowel, hiatus is resolved through coalescence. LH tones never surface on a single syllable, although sequences of L and H are permitted on successive syllables. When a LH tone would otherwise surface in hiatus contexts, there is a regular flattening of LH to H, as in (17c) and (17d).

One final regular tonal process in the language is found when two H tones would otherwise be expected to associate with the same surface syllable. This sequence of underlying H tones instead surfaces as a falling HL contour, as in (18).

Now, we turn to the grammatical tone process of interest to us here: across-the-board tonal polarity. Modifiers agree in number and case with the nouns they modify. The nominative form of a modifier surfaces with exactly the opposite tones from its oblique form. An /H/ in the oblique form of the modifier surfaces as L in the nominative, and an /L/ surfaces as H in the nominative, as in (19).

All TBUs predictably surface with the opposite tone in nominative contexts than in oblique contexts. When a HL contour undergoes tonal polarity, it surfaces as H, rather than LH, as in (20). This is expected given the ban on LH surface tones and regular flattening of LH to H, discussed above and shown in (17).

Note that the nominative forms in (19e) and (20a) are tonally identical (L.H.L), but their corresponding oblique forms differ (H.L.H vs. H.HL.H). Nominative forms are predictable given oblique forms, but not vice versa. Additionally, a greater range of tonal melodies is attested in oblique forms, whereas only a subset of these is attested in nominative forms (HL contours never surface in nominative contexts). These two pieces of evidence support the claim that the nominative form is derived from the oblique form.Footnote 6

As we saw in (20), a HL contour in oblique contexts surfaces as H, not LH in nominative contexts. This could be thought of as a two-step process in a rule-based approach. First, /HL/ undergoes tonal polarity, becoming LH. Then regular LH flattening applies, resulting in H: HL $\rightarrow $ LH $\rightarrow $ H.

With the exception of six adjectives that follow a tonal subpattern (HL.L $\rightarrow $ L.H), all modifiers undergo predictable tonal polarity in nominative contexts. These six adjectives are also exceptional in other ways, such as plural formation (Kouneli & Nie Reference Kouneli and Nie2021).

Apart from these six exceptions and the regular effects of LH flattening, tones in nominative forms are consistently the exact opposites of their counterparts in oblique contexts. This tonal change is the only marker of nominative case on modifiers; there is no additional segmental change or affix. This tonal polarity process in Kipsigis differs from previously attested cases of tonal polarity in that it affects all tones in the modifier, not just one edge. That is, it applies across the board.

An item-based approach to Kipsigis tonal polarity would require that the polarity process be triggered by the addition of some underlying phonological form, with any additional phonological changes following from the regular phonology of the language. A process-based approach, on the other hand, allows for morphemes to be associated with processes rather than (or in addition to) underlying forms. The remainder of this section considers and rules out an item-based analysis of Kipsigis tonal polarity, and then §4.3 presents a possible process-based analysis set in CbP.

Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) make a number of arguments against an item-based account. First, a floating tone or tonal feature at one edge of the nominative modifier could be proposed to have a cascading effect, resulting in each successive H becoming L and vice versa. The motivation for this kind of analysis could be something like the Obligatory Contour Principle (OCP), which prevents multiple identical consecutive tones. This approach is appealing because previous instances of tonal polarity in the literature, such as in Mende (Mande; Leben Reference Leben1973, Reference Leben, Bowler, Duncan, Major and Torrence2019) and Kɔnni (Gur; Cahill Reference Cahill, Githiora, Littlefield and Manfredi2003), have been analysed as OCP effects. However, the Kipsigis facts differ from these previous cases in one crucial respect: Kipsigis nominative modifiers show across-the-board polarity, whereas in Mende & Kɔnni, only one tone at the edge of the word is affected.

There are also a number of language-specific problems with this item-based OCP analysis. The first is that if we assume a floating H or L tone in the context of nominative modifiers, we would have to say that H or L has a polarising effect only in this context, and not elsewhere in the language. For example, the plural suffix surfaces as L after both H and L tones, including on nominative modifiers. The secondary suffix surfaces as H no matter whether it follows a H or L tone. Recall from (18) that two underlying Hs on the same syllable surface as a HL falling tone through a regular tonal process in Kipsigis. This /H-H/ $\rightarrow $ HL process is different from the alternation we see between oblique and nominative contexts, where H $\rightarrow $ L, L $\rightarrow $ H and HL $\rightarrow $ H. Even if we allow for a morpheme-specific cascading OCP effect, we still do not accurately account for the Kipsigis facts, since sequences of consecutive HH and LL are possible, and in fact are regularly attested, on nominative modifiers. For example, consider [tòróːr-éːn], L.H.H, ‘tall-pl.nom’, repeated from (20b).Footnote 7

An alternative possible item-based analysis considered by Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) posits that there are multiple allomorphs (H and L) of the nominative agreement morpheme on modifiers, and that one allomorph attaches to every TBU of a nominative modifier, replacing the lexical tone. This analysis is problematic for a number of reasons, including that we would lose the generalisation that this is a phonologically predictable process. Instead, phonologically conditioned allomorphy would coincidentally insert a morpheme with the opposite tone of every TBU of the modifier. The allomorphy account complicates the lexicon by adding two suppletive allomorphs of the nominative morpheme, when in fact its realisation is always phonologically predictable. Another issue with this analysis is the question of how to generate an exponent of the nominative agreement marker on each TBU of each modifier; this would result in unnecessary extensive multiple exponence, in which the number of exponents of nominative case on the modifier would be equal to the number of TBUs of the modifier. I know of no other cases where the phonological structure of a morpheme determines the number of exponents of another morpheme.

A typical argument in favour of item-based accounts is that they are more restrictive than item-and-process accounts. Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) show that an item-based account would require a constraint or set of morpheme-specific readjustment rules specific to that context. This would be just as unrestrictive as an account without an underlying item. Thus, the restrictiveness argument cannot distinguish between the two approaches, and a process-based account is more economical than an item-based one. To summarise their argument, if an item-based account would still require a morpheme-specific phonological rule or constraint (a process), a process-based account without an underlying item is preferable on economic grounds.

One possible analysis involving both an underlying item and a context-specific phonological rule or process is the following, suggested by Nicholas Rolle (p.c.). A floating tone is inserted at the right edge of a nominative modifier (H after L, L after H), causing each successive tone to shift one slot to the left on the tonal tier. This would cause every TBU that was previously associated with a L tone to instead be associated with a H and vice versa; however, this shifting process would, again, have to be specific to the context of nominative modifiers. Thus, the analysis requires both an underlying item and a morpheme-specific process.

Considering the Kipsigis facts in light of the diagnostics in (1), we see that indeed Kipsigis nominative modifiers show the behaviour we would expect from morpheme-specific phonology, and not from item-based or suppletive morphology, as summarised in (21). There is no consistent segment or suprasegment that can be pointed to as exponing nominative agreement on modifiers across a paradigm, and general phonological principles of the language cannot account for why tonal polarity applies in this context. However, polarity is phonologically describable, is predictable and can be analysed as phonologically natural.

Based on the arguments presented here against an item-based account of Kipsigis tonal polarity, I assume, following Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021), that a process-based approach to this phenomenon is optimal. Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) do not adopt a specific item-and-process approach, so I propose one in §4.3. First, let us consider an additional example of non-item-based grammatical tone in §3.2.

3.2 Scalar tone shift in Guébie

Guébie is an Eastern Kru language spoken in southwest Côte d’Ivoire. The data presented here were collected by the author between 2013 and 2022. This section describes the basics of a grammatical tone pattern in Guébie; for a full-fledged description and discussion of possible analyses of this pattern, see Sande (Reference Sande2017, Reference Sande2018) and Sande et al. (Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020).

As introduced in §2.2, Guébie has four contrastive tone heights labelled 1–4, where 4 is high. In specific grammatical contexts, such as the one described here, a super-high tone, tone 5, can also surface. A tone 5 is derived from an underlying high tone, tone 4, plus a grammatical tone raising process. Contrastive underlying tone melodies include levels 1, 2, 3 and 4 and contours 41, 42, 31, 32, 23 and 24. All possible sequences of two-level tones, and almost all possible sequences of three-level tones, are possible in derived contexts (e.g. in the context of the imperfective scalar shift described here, or the negative contexts discussed in §2.2).

Basic clausal word order is SAuxOV or SVO. Nothing can intervene between the subject and auxiliary in SAuxOV clauses, or between the subject and verb in SVO clauses. Lexical and grammatical tones are rampant. This section focuses on grammatical tone in imperfective contexts, which is realised as a scalar tone shift.

Tone is the sole marker of imperfective aspect in Guébie. A given verb shows the same tone melody in all contexts other than the positive imperfective, as illustrated in (22) (Sande Reference Sande2017, Reference Sande2018).

On imperfective verbs, which surface only in SVO contexts, the tone is one step lower than elsewhere, as in (23).

When the verbal tone melody contains a sequence of tones in a contour, or multiple syllables with different tone heights, only the first tone level of the verbal melody lowers in imperfective contexts. The scalar shift of the first verbal tone level is summarised in (24).

When the tone of a verb is already low, we do not see lowering to super-low (tone 0). We also do not see neutralisation between perfective and imperfective contexts. Instead, the scalar tone shift affects the final tone of the subject by raising it one step, as in (25).

Subject tone raising before low-toned verbs occurs even when the result is a super-high tone, as in (26).

The subject raising process is summarised in (27).

To summarise the full Guébie imperfective scalar tone shift pattern, the first tone height of a verb surfaces one step lower in the imperfective than elsewhere, unless the verb is already low, in which case the final subject tone raises one step. Both verb tone lowering and subject tone raising result in the tonal interval between subject and verb increasing by one.

As with Kipsigis tonal polarity, we can ask whether an item-based analysis of Guébie scalar tone shift is possible. Following Sande (Reference Sande2018) and Sande et al. (Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020), I conclude that no item-based account of the Guébie facts is simpler than a process-based account. No matter which featural affix or floating tone is posited as the underlying representation of the Guébie imperfective morpheme, rules or constraints are still needed to derive a scalar tone shift seen only in the imperfective context.

One plausible candidate for an underlying form of the imperfective might be a floating 41 (HL) tone. The low second portion could have a lowering effect on the verb, whereas the high initial portion could have a raising effect on the subject. If we posit that this item sits between the subject and the verb, the phonological grammar must be able to account for why the low portion of the 41 floating tone attaches to the right by default, and the high portion only attaches to the left when the verb is underlyingly low (tone 1). Crucially, neither tone level 1 nor tone level 4, nor the very common 41 contour tone, has a scalar effect elsewhere in the grammar (e.g. recall the negative floating high tone (tone 4) described in §2.2, which does not have a scalar effect, but adds a 4 to the end of the subject tone melody), so we must explain why associating a 1 or 4 with a nearby tone results in a scalar effect in imperfective contexts only. If we had chosen any other floating tone for the imperfective, we would still need to posit the same set of imperfective-specific rules or constraints to result in a scalar shift. That is, no single underlying tone better predicts when and where these scalar effects occur than any other underlying form. And, as Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) argue for Kipsigis, since the item-based account still requires morpheme-specific phonology or morpheme-specific processes, a process-based account without an underlying item is simpler.

An alternative possible UR for the imperfective morpheme is a tonal feature. Proposed features for four-tone systems share the property that the mid tones (levels 2 and 3) have no features in common (Yip Reference Yip1980; Clements Reference Clements and Dihoff1983; Pulleyblank Reference Pulleyblank1986; Bao Reference Bao1999; Snider Reference Snider1999). See, for example, the features proposed by Yip (Reference Yip1980) & Pulleyblank (Reference Pulleyblank1986) in (28).

A major problem with a feature-based account is that no single feature change can result in the scalar effect in which $4\rightarrow 3$ , $2\rightarrow 1$ and $3\rightarrow 2$ . The shifts from tone 4 to 3 and from 2 to 1 could involve a single featural change, namely changing a [ $+$ High/Raised] feature to [ $-$ High/Raised]. However, the same featural change cannot be responsible for the shift from tone 3 to 2, because tones 3 and 2 differ in two features, and the High/Raised change would have to be from $-$ to $+$ rather than the reverse.

The failure of binary features to account for scalar phenomena, and other grammatical tone phenomena in general, has long been understood (Contreras Reference Contreras1969; Hyman Reference Hyman, Goldsmith, Hume and Leo Wetzels2010). Tonal features seem to be a useful analytical tool in occasional circumstances (McPherson Reference McPherson, DiCanio, Malins, Good, Michelson, Jaeger and Keily2016; Meyase Reference Meyase2021), but they cannot account for the scalar tone shift in Guébie.

In addition to the two possible underlying representations discussed here, Sande (Reference Sande2018) rules out an additional item-based account in which tonal subfeatures combine to result in different surface tones. No combination of subfeatures consistently results in the correct optimal surface form, and a morpheme-specific set of phonological principles is still needed to derive the correct patterns. As Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) conclude for Kipsigis, if we need both morpheme-specific phonology and an underlying representation in an ostensibly item-based account, such an account is less economical than a process-based account with morpheme-specific phonology but no underlying representation. Sande (Reference Sande2018) presents a morpheme-specific analysis of these facts in Cophonology Theory, which is reinterpreted in CbP by Sande et al. (Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020). These existing analyses are very similar to the analysis to be presented for Kipsigis in §4.3. The result is a tonal process, here a scalar tone shift, that applies only in imperfective contexts.

The imperfective triggers a morpheme-specific process, consistent with the diagnostics in (1), as shown in (29). There is no consistent segmental or suprasegmental realisation of the imperfective morpheme across the paradigm, and a scalar shift is not part of the general phonology of the language, although the alternation is phonological predictable and derivable.

3.3 Summary of morpheme-specific grammatical tone processes

In this section, we have seen cases of grammatical tone that are phonologically describable and derivable, but that cannot be analysed as involving language-wide general phonological alternations, no matter what is proposed as the relevant underlying form. While these cases could be analysed as suppletive, we would lose the generalisation that they are phonologically predictable and derivable. Additionally, a suppletive analysis of Kipsigis tonal polarity would necessarily involve two suppletive allomorphs of all possible nominal modifiers, one of which would coincidentally be the tonal opposite of the other. Likewise, for a suppletive account of the Guébie imperfective, all verbs as well as all possible subject-final morphemes—both of which are open classes—would need to have multiple listed allomorphs that are coincidentally all tonally related in a consistent way. In both cases, a suppletive analysis loses a phonological generalisation and greatly expands the list of necessary underlying forms. Instead, the phonological predictability and derivability of these two grammatical tone processes suggests a phonological analysis, although that process must be specific to the morphological context at hand.

The case studies presented here are by no means the only instances of grammatical tone that are problematic for item-based accounts and suggest the need for morpheme-specific processes. For an example from an American tone system, Rodriguez (Reference Rodriguez2021) proposes a morpheme-specific account, couched in Cophonology Theory, to model tonal lowering in particular morphosyntactic contexts in Copala Triqui (Otomanguean, Mexico). Many Bantu languages also have interacting grammatical tone patterns, some of which can be accounted for in an additive, item-based way, but some of which show morpheme-specific effects that are argued to be best analysed through cophonologies. For example, see Downing (Reference Downing2003) on Chichewa final-H versus penult-H grammatical tone patterns.

One recurring argument against an item-based account of the grammatical tone patterns in this section was that any item-based account of these phenomena would require a novel underlying form and/or would require a morpheme-specific rule or constraint in addition to the proposed underlying item. In both cases, a morpheme-specific process is argued to be more economical. Thus, on economic grounds, we can add to our diagnostics of item- versus process-based morphophonology that if an item-based account would require a novel underlying form or would still require a morpheme-specific rule or constraint, a process-based account is preferable.

§4 presents a model that can account for item-based and process-based grammatical tone in a single, unified way.

4. A Cophonologies by Phase account of item-based and process-based grammatical tone

We have examined cases of grammatical tone that can be analysed as item-based, suppletive or process-based. Diagnostics for determining whether a given phenomenon involves items, suppletion or processes have been proposed. Once diagnosed, the question remains how to best analyse each type of morphophonology. This section presents an analysis in CbP which allows for a unified approach to all three types of morphophonology diagnosed here.

4.1 Background on Cophonologies by Phase

CbP is a framework of the morphosyntax/phonology interface that combines Distributed Morphology operations, such as late insertion of vocabulary items, with phonological evaluation by means of weighted constraints. The crucial component of CbP for our purposes is an enriched notion of vocabulary items or lexical representations (Sande & Jenks Reference Sande and Jenks2018; Sande Reference Sande2019; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020). Traditional Distributed Morphology-style vocabulary items pair a set of morphosyntactic features with their respective phonological underlying representation. These items can be sensitive to phonological or morphosyntactic context. For example, in (30), the plural suffix in English can be sensitive to the specific roots it attaches to. Only suppletive, phonologically non-derivable allomorphs are listed, whereas the phonological component can manipulate these suppletive allomorphs to derive additional allomorphs (e.g. we need not list /-s/ or /-əz/ as separate plural vocabulary items in English, since they are phonologically derived from /-z/).

In CbP, each vocabulary item contains not only a phonological underlying form, but three phonological components, listed in (31).

$\mathcal {F}$ is much like the underlying phonological form of a vocabulary item in traditional Distributed Morphology. $\mathcal {P}$ is a prosodic subcategorisation frame that can specify whether a morpheme is a prefix or suffix, and can require that a morpheme attach to (or inside of) a specific prosodic category. The details of $\mathcal {P}$ are not particularly relevant for this article. $\mathcal {R}$ , the morpheme-specific constraint-weight readjustment, allows for morpheme-specific phonology to apply within the cycle of phonology in which the triggering morpheme is introduced.

Any of these three phonological components of the vocabulary item can be null for a given morpheme. For example, focusing on $\mathcal {F}$ and $\mathcal {R}$ , there are four logical possibilities. A concatenative morpheme without a morpheme-specific phonological process has only a specified $\mathcal {F}$ . A concatenative morpheme plus a morpheme-specific process (often called morphologically conditioned phonology) is specified for both $\mathcal {F}$ and $\mathcal {R}$ . A non-concatenative morphological process that does not also have an affixal component is only specified for $\mathcal {R}$ . And, a null morpheme would be specified for neither $\mathcal {F}$ nor $\mathcal {R}$ .

Inasmuch as there are listed morphemes or vocabulary items in which morphosyntactic features map to phonological content, this framework has much in common with item-based accounts, which necessarily assume listed phonological content of morphemes. Traditional items correspond to the $\mathcal {F}$ component in CbP vocabulary items, and traditional processes correspond to the $\mathcal {R}$ component. A morpheme can be associated with an item $\mathcal {F}$ , a process $\mathcal {R}$ , neither or both.

The domain in which morpheme-specific constraint-weight adjustments apply is determined by syntactic phases. Phases are chunks of syntactic structure whose contents become inaccessible to further syntactic manipulation once the phase is complete (Chomsky Reference Chomsky and Kenstowicz2001, Reference Chomsky, Freiden, Otero and Zubizarreta2008). At phase boundaries, spell-out applies, including phonological operations (Ishihara Reference Ishihara2003, Reference Ishihara2007; Kratzer & Selkirk Reference Kratzer and Selkirk2007; Pak Reference Pak2008; Kahnemuyipour Reference Kahnemuyipour2009; Jenks & Rose Reference Jenks and Rose2015; Deal & Wolf Reference Deal, Wolf, Gribanova and Shih2017; Sande Reference Sande2017; Kastner Reference Kastner2019). Which morphemes introduce phase boundaries is the topic of much discussion in the syntactic literature. The specifics of phasehood are not particularly important for this article, so I leave out a detailed discussion here. For our purposes, the syntactic heads Voice, D, and C will be considered phase heads (Chomsky Reference Chomsky and Kenstowicz2001, Reference Chomsky, Freiden, Otero and Zubizarreta2008).

Previous work in CbP has shown that it can model a wide range of facts at the morphology–phonology interface, many of which are difficult to account for in other frameworks. These include category-specific phonology (Sande & Jenks Reference Sande and Jenks2018; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020), cross-word (phrasal) morpheme-specific phonology (Sande & Jenks Reference Sande and Jenks2018; Sande Reference Sande2019), morpheme-specific phonology affecting a sub-word domain (Sande Reference Sande2019, Reference Sande2020), inside-out effects, where a lower morpheme within a phase domain triggers an alternation on a higher morpheme within the same domain (Sande Reference Sande2019; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020), multiple interacting morpheme-specific effects within the same syntactic domain (Sande Reference Sande2020), apparent prosodic-word level recursion (Miller & Sande Reference Miller and Sande2021) and morpheme-specific effects that target a prosodic constituent (Jenks Reference Jenks2018). Additionally, CbP makes strong predictions about the locality of morpheme-specific phonological processes: morpheme-specific phonology should only affect morphemes introduced in the same phase as—or hierarchically lower phases than—the triggering morpheme. We do not expect to see a morpheme-specific phonological process affecting morphemes introduced in higher syntactic phases.

For arguments in favour of CbP over other approaches that allow for morpheme-specific constraints or constraint rankings, such as Indexed Constraint Theory, see Sande (Reference Sande2019) and Sande et al. (Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020).

In the remainder of this section, I show a further benefit of CbP: the expanded vocabulary items of CbP provide a single framework in which morphological patterns traditionally treated as item-based and those traditionally treated as process-based can both be analysed. Since there are existing item-based analyses of the Ebira and Mandarin item-based tone patterns presented in §2, which are easily translatable into CbP analyses (the previously proposed items are $\mathcal {F}$ forms in CbP, with no morpheme-specific $\mathcal {R}$ ), in §4.2, I present an analysis of the apparently item-based Guébie negative tone pattern (§2.2), which has not previously been analysed in the literature. In §4.3, I provide the first full-fledged analysis of the Kipsigis tonal polarity facts, showing that a process-based phenomenon can also be modelled in CbP. A similar process-based analysis of the Guébie imperfective scalar tone shift in CbP can be found in Sande et al. (Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020).

4.2 Accounting for item-based grammatical tone

This section presents the first analysis of Guébie negative tone. Recall that in non-pronominal contexts, negation is consistently realised as the addition of a high tone at the end of the subject. In general in the language, tones can be straightforwardly analysed as associating with TBUs one-to-one from left to right; contour tones and consecutive TBUs with the same level tone only appear at the right edge of a word. The negative tone pattern, then, is subject to the regular tone association mechanism of the language.

Full noun-phrase subjects are first spelled out in a separate phase, headed by the determiner D. Thus, if subjects were associated with a particular cophonology, that morpheme-specific cophonology will no longer be active at the point in the derivation when the C phase, which includes both the subject and negative morpheme, is spelled out. The negative vocabulary item is given in (32). It contains a floating high tone (tone 4) item $\mathcal {F}$ , not associated with segmental content. The prosodic subcategorisation $\mathcal {P}$ specifies that the tonal item is prosodified with the preceding prosodic word (in other words, it is a suffixal tone). There is no morpheme-specific $\mathcal {R}$ , since the tonal association process in negative contexts is identical to tonal association elsewhere in the language, so only the default, general phonology of Guébie is needed to account for the negative tone pattern.Footnote 8

The phonological constraint weights ensure that underlying tones all associate with TBUs, and they do so in a one-to-one manner from left to right. In the input in (33), since the subject has already been spelled out, its tones are already associated (tones 3 and 1 both associated with the single TBU of the subject). The verb and negative have not yet been spelled out and so their tones are not yet associated with TBUs.

Associate assigns a violation to any tone not associated with a TBU. *Toneless assigns a violation for any TBU not associated with a tone. *ComplexContour assigns a violation to more than two tones associated with a single TBU.Footnote 9 The specific choice of constraints here is not crucial to the point of this section; any set of constraints that results in exhaustive, one-to-one, left-to-right tone-to-TBU association could derive the correct surface forms for Guébie. Crucially, though, item-based grammatical tone can be accounted for in CbP through morphemes associated with phonological suprasegmental content in $\mathcal {F}$ .

This is a MaxEnt-Harmonic Grammar tableau (Goldwater & Johnson Reference Goldwater, Johnson, Spenader, Eriksson and Dahl2003; Hayes & Wilson Reference Hayes and Wilson2008). Harmony scores (shown in the last column under the heading H) are calculated by multiplying the weight of each constraint by the number of times it is violated, and summing the products. The candidate with the lowest harmony score surfaces most frequently (in this case, and throughout this article, categorically). The index (☞) is used to indicate the categorically best candidate.

Candidate (33b) is ruled out because the negative tone 4 is not associated, incurring a violation of Associate. Candidate (33c) is ruled out because the tone-4 melody of the verb has not associated with both syllables of the verb, leaving one of the syllables toneless, incurring a violation of *Toneless. All three candidates contain contour tones on the subject, but candidates (33a) and (33c) contain complex contours, with three tones associated with a single TBU. Despite violating *ComplexContour, candidate (33a) is preferred over the alternatives.

Recall that negation does not show a consistent realisation in the context of a pronominal subject. The form of a negative pronominal subject is not predictable given the positive form plus a tone-4 negative morpheme. Instead, §2.2 argued for a suppletive account of negative pronouns, with different listed forms for positive and negative pronominal subjects. A suppletive account can be implemented in CbP by assuming distinct positive and negative vocabulary items associated with each person/number pronominal subject. Proposed vocabulary items for the first- and second-person singular forms are given in (34); other pronouns have similar suppletive forms in positive and negative contexts.

All grammatical tones previously analysed as item-based (as opposed to process-based) can be reanalysed straightforwardly in CbP by assuming vocabulary items in which morphosyntactic features are mapped to phonological content containing an $\mathcal {F}$ component but no $\mathcal {R}$ component. Suppletive grammatical tone can be analysed using different listed morphemes with distinct phonological content. A key innovation of CbP, however, is that the phonological content of a morpheme can also contain a morpheme-specific phonological grammar, implemented through adjustments to the default constraint weights in the phonology of the language, which can account for morpheme-specific processes not triggered by traditional items, as shown in §4.3.

4.3 Accounting for process-based grammatical tone

This section presents the first full-fledged analysis of across-the-board tonal polarity in Kipsigis. Not only does this analysis serve as an example of how process-based grammatical tone can be modelled in CbP, but it also shows the way forward for a morpheme-specific constraint-weighting analysis of polarity more generally.

A constraint-based analysis of tonal polarity in Kipsigis requires tonal faithfulness (35a) and anti-faithfulness constraints (35b).

In the default phonological grammar of the language, the weights of these two constraints are such that Id-Tone is always stronger than *Id-Tone, and unless another markedness constraint rules out faithfulness, the tonally faithful candidate is optimal. To account for the regular LH flattening process, the markedness constraint in (36) is needed.

This constraint must have a very high weight throughout the phonology of Kipsigis, since it is never violated. Default weights of these three constraints, determined using the MaxEnt Grammar Tool (Wilson & George Reference Wilson and George2008), are given in (37). Weights are rounded up to the nearest whole number.

As shown in (38), the vocabulary items for roots in Kipsigis contain an $\mathcal {F}$ , namely their underlying tonal and segmental form, but they are not specified for a morpheme-specific constraint weight adjustment $\mathcal {R}$ . For modifiers, the underlying form is assumed to be identical to the oblique form.

Segmental affixes, such as the plural in (39), have an $\mathcal {F}$ and are specified as suffixes by $\mathcal {P}$ .

The oblique case marker in Kipsigis has no segmental or tonal effect, nor does it correspond to any morpheme-specific phonology. It is null, and all phonological components of its corresponding vocabulary item are also null.Footnote 11

The nominative case marker (more likely a nominative concord marker) has no segmental or suprasegmental underlying phonological form, but is associated with tonal polarity, motivated by the constraint weight adjustments of $\mathcal {R}$ . This vocabulary item is shown in (41).

When the root ‘tall’ is present, and agrees with a plural oblique noun, the vocabulary items in (38)–(40) are inserted. None of these items is associated with a constraint-weight readjustment, so the default phonological grammar applies as in (42).

In nominative contexts, the same root and plural vocabulary items are inserted, this time along with the nominative item in (41). The $\mathcal {R}$ of (41) adds 16 to the default weight of *Id-Tone and subtracts 8 from the default weight of Id-Tone, resulting in a reversal of strength of these two constraints. The result is tonal polarity rather than tonal identity, as in (43). The only nominative context in which we do not see exact tonal polarity from input to output is when there is a HL syllable in the input, as in the second syllable of ‘tall’ in (43). In this case, the polar result would be a LH contour, which is ruled out by the *LH constraint. Candidate (43c) is categorically the best output candidate.Footnote 12

In both grammars, a LH contour is ruled out by the highly weighted *LH constraint. In the default grammar, the weight of Id-Tone overpowers that of the antifaithfulness constraint, resulting in tonal faithfulness as in (42). Within the spell-out domain containing the nominalising agreement morpheme, the tonal antifaithfulness requirement is stronger than tonal faithfulness, resulting in polarity as in (43).

The articulated vocabulary items of CbP allow for item-based morphemes, non-item-based morphology, null morphemes and morphemes that both have a concatenative component and trigger a process, as set out in (44) (Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020). This seems to be the desired cross-linguistic result, since we not only see concatenative morphemes (the Kipsigis plural), null morphemes (the oblique) and morphologically conditioned phonology across languages (see e.g. the discussion of Chichewa in §4.4), but also morphemes that are associated with a process but have no concatenative component, such as tonal polarity on nominative adjectives in Kipsigis.

4.4 Summarising CbP analyses of grammatical tone

This section has shown how a CbP account can be used to model traditionally additive item-based grammatical tone, suppletive grammatical tone and process-based grammatical tone through morphemes associated with item-based components as well as morpheme-specific constraint weight readjustments. In §4.2, we saw that Guébie negative item-based tone can be analysed in CbP with a morpheme associated with a floating low tone. The exceptional negative pronouns are analysed as suppletive, with different listed allomorphs sensitive to morphosyntactic context. In §4.3, we saw that CbP can also account for grammatical tone processes, such as across-the-board tonal polarity. The nominative case morpheme on modifiers in Kipsigis does not add any (supra-)segmental content, but is associated with a listed morpheme-specific constraint-weight adjustment. The Guébie negative morpheme has an item-based $\mathcal {F}$ component and no morpheme-specific grammar $\mathcal {R}$ , while the Kipsigis nominative case morpheme on modifiers triggers a morpheme-specific grammar $\mathcal {R}$ but is not associated with an item $\mathcal {F}$ . Other cases of grammatical tone show the need for both $\mathcal {F}$ and $\mathcal {R}$ components associated with a single morpheme.

For example, in Chichewa (Bantu), some tense/aspect/mood contexts are exponed by both a segmental prefix and a H tone associated with the penult of the stem (the negative imperative, e.g., as in (45a)), while others correspond to a H associated with the vowel immediately following the prefix (such as the recent past, exemplified in (45b); Hyman Reference Hyman, Palancar and Léonard2016, Reference Hyman, Kobozono and Giriko2018; Downing & Mtenje Reference Downing and Mtenje2017).

Both the recent past and negative imperative morphemes are exponed by a prefix as well as the addition of a high tone on the stem. They could both be analysed as involving additive items consisting of a segmental prefix plus a floating high tone. However, the position of the high tone on the stem varies with morphosyntactic context. One solution is a morpheme-specific phonological constraint ranking or weighting, which derives the appropriate tone docking sites in each morphosyntactic context. In CbP, the vocabulary item for the recent past, then, would involve an item $\mathcal {F}$ /na H/, with a floating high tone, as well as an $\mathcal {R}$ that ensures the phonological grammar will choose a candidate where a H tone docks to the immediately following TBU. The vocabulary item for the negative imperative would also involve an item $\mathcal {F}$ with a segmental component and floating H, but the $\mathcal {R}$ would ensure that the floating H docks to the penult.

The Bantu verb stem, or the domain of inflectional tone in Chichewa, has been argued to correspond to a syntactic phase domain (Cheng & Downing Reference Cheng and Downing2016). Thus, the morpheme-specific grammatical tone docking patterns could be analysed as morpheme-specific phonological constraint weights in CbP that place H tone in the correct morpheme-specific surface position within the relevant syntactic phase-delimited domain.Footnote 13 See Hyman & Mtenje (Reference Hyman, Mtenje, Kager, Hulst and Zonneveld1999), Downing & Mtenje (Reference Downing and Mtenje2017) and Hyman (Reference Hyman, Kobozono and Giriko2018) for more on grammatical tone patterns in Chichewa.

In allowing for process-based and item-based components of the same listed morphemes, CbP provides a means of analysing so-called ‘secondary exponence’ phenomena (cf. Haugen Reference Haugen, Siddiqi and Harley2016), in which both an affixal and a process-based component expone the same morphosyntactic features. (Inkelas (Reference Inkelas2014) calls this ‘morphologically conditioned phonology’.) The Chichewa case exemplifies how morphemes might be associated with both a phonological underlying form (a traditional item) $\mathcal {F}$ and a phonological constraint weight adjustment (a traditional morpheme-specific process) $\mathcal {R}$ in a single lexical entry or vocabulary item. Morpheme-specific phonological processes often co-occur with overt segmental or suprasegmental affixes, which CbP predicts in a straightforward way: morphemes can be associated with an underlying form $\mathcal {F}$ as in the Guébie negative, a constraint reweighting $\mathcal {R}$ as in Kipsigis tonal polarity, or both as in Chichewa.

The CbP analysis raises the question of what qualifies as an item-based model. CbP differs from some purely process-based accounts which posit no underlying morphemes, only processes associated with constructions or morphosyntactic contexts (see e.g. Anderson Reference Anderson1992). In CbP, there is a list of mappings of morphosyntactic features to phonological content, as is true of many item-based accounts. The difference from previous purely item-based accounts, though, such as Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012), is that in CbP the phonological content of a morpheme can contain not just an underlying phonological item, but also a morpheme-specific constraint weight readjustment, which can result in morpheme-specific or construction-specific phonological processes. Thus, in the sense that there are listed morphemes, CbP is like previous item-based accounts, but in the sense that it allows for morpheme-specific phonological grammars, CbP differs from existing item-based accounts, and in particular from those that claim that all morphology involves the addition of some underlying phonological form. Instead, CbP allows for process-based morphophonology through morpheme-specific constraint weight adjustments. The predictions of the independent but co-existing $\mathcal {F}$ and $\mathcal {R}$ components of CbP seem to be borne out among the grammatical tone patterns of the world’s languages, as in (44).

5. Discussion

This section points out potential problems for and specific implications of the data, diagnostics and analyses presented throughout this article. §5.1 discusses cases that have been analysed as both item-based and process-based in the literature. These cases seem to be ambiguously item- or process-based, but the diagnostics presented in (1) can help to distinguish between analytic possibilities. §5.2 discusses an alternative analysis not yet considered, which involves item-based morphology that can be sensitive to different phonological grammars at different morphophonological levels or strata. Such analyses are shown to be unable to account for the process-based phenomena described here. §5.3 discusses the implications of the analysis presented here for the syntax–phonology interface.

5.1 Ambiguous cases of grammatical tone

The case studies presented throughout this article are fairly straightforwardly analysed as either item-based or process-based, and existing analyses tend to agree that the cases presented in §2 are additive, item-based instances of grammatical tone, while the case studies presented in §3 involve morpheme-specific phonological processes. However, certain cases of grammatical tone have been previously analysed as both item-based and process-based. One such example comes from Kuria, a Bantu language spoken in and around Tanzania.

In Kuria, the placement of the first high tone on the verbal stem is determined by the aspect of the clause (Marlo et al. Reference Marlo, Mwita and Paster2015). The high tone then spreads to the penultimate mora of the domain in a tone-spreading process that is very general and consistent in the language. The position of the first high tone on the verb stem is sometimes analysed as due morpheme-specific processes. In this analysis, different tense/aspect contexts trigger different phonological grammars, and a floating high tone associates with the first, second, third or fourth mora of the verb stem (cf. Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020). However, there are also item-based analyses of the Kuria tense/aspect-dependent high-tone placement. One such analysis, proposed by Rolle & Lionnet (Reference Rolle, Lionnet, Baek, Takahashi and Yeung2020), involves a novel type of underlying representation, phantom planes of prosodic structure. In their approach, an underlying item can be associated with so-called phantom structure, which is then mapped onto surface structure, perhaps the root or stem. Under this analysis, the Kuria inceptive morpheme is associated with an item that has the segmental content [ra] followed by four phantom moras, where a H tone is associated with the fourth of the phantom moras. These four phantom moras are then mapped onto the verb stem, and a H tone surfaces on the fourth mora of the stem. In order to account for this tonal phenomenon in an item-based way, then, a new type of very powerful representational tool has been proposed, requiring the phonological component to have the ability to interpret phantom prosodic structure. Until phantom structure can be shown to be restricted, not too powerful, and useful in accounting for other types of phenomena across languages, it seems that a process-based account of Kuria morpheme-specific tone docking is preferable.

Referring back to the diagnostics proposed in §2 and §4.4, the properties of Kuria TAM-specific tone assignment seems to match up most closely with the properties expected of a process-based analysis. Given the stem and tense/aspect context, the tonal morphophonology in Kuria is predictable and phonologically derivable, though not based on language-general phonological principles. Additionally, an item-based analysis of the Kuria facts requires a novel type of item to be proposed (phantom planes), along with constraints or mechanisms in the phonological grammar that can operate on those novel items. Thus, given the diagnostics in (1) and §4.4, which apply to Kuria as shown in (46), I propose that Kuria is best analysed as a morpheme-specific process.

In Kuria, we see a phonologically derivable process that is not general to the phonology of the language, like other morpheme-specific processes discussed in §3. However, Kuria, unlike the other cases of process morphology discussed here, shows a consistent realisation across paradigms: H tone beginning on the fourth mora of the verb stem in all inceptive contexts. Perhaps its consistent realisation across a paradigm is what makes the Kuria case ambiguous between item-based and process-based. I propose that whether a morpheme has a consistent realisation across a paradigm should not determine whether it is analysed as an item or as a process. Item-based morphology always has a consistent realisation across a paradigm, whereas process-based morphology might have a consistent realisation across the paradigm. This criterion does, however, help to distinguish item-based morphophonological alternations from suppletive morphology (see §2). The key diagnostic for item- versus process-based morphology, then, is whether the pattern is phonologically derivable and phonologically general (item-based), or phonologically derivable but not general (process-based). An updated diagnostic table is provided in (47), where process morphology may have a consistent realisation across a paradigm as in Kuria, or it may not, as in Guébie and Kipsigis.

Other potentially ambiguous cases come from highly paradigmatic tonal systems such as those of Oto-Manguean languages, including Amuzgo (Kim Reference Kim, Panacar and Léonard2016; Palancar Reference Palancar2021) & varieties of Mixtec (McKendry Reference McKendry2013; Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann, Gallagher, Gouskova and Yin2018). It is not clear that there is a single right answer to whether complex paradigmatic tonal systems should be analysed as item-based, suppletive or process-based; all such analyses exist in the literature. I recommend that analysts consider the diagnostics presented in this article, as well as economy, and whether new types of items would need to be proposed to derive such paradigmatic tone as item-based as opposed to suppletive or process-based.

5.2 Items plus strata

So far, this article has considered item-based analyses, in which all morphology involves adding some item, and analyses that allow morphemes or constructions to trigger some morpheme-specific process, or be subject to a morpheme-specific grammar or cophonology. There is an alternative, hybrid type of analysis in which all morphology is assumed to be item-based, but morphemes are associated with different levels or strata that are subject to different phonological rules or constraint rankings. Such analyses include item-based implementations of Stratal OT (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero1999; Kiparsky Reference Kiparsky, Vaux and Nevins2008) such as Generalised Non-Linear Affixation (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Trommer2012; Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann2017).

Stratal approaches assume that there are fixed phonological domains such as stems and words, which can be subject to different phonological grammars. Within a particular domain, all morphemes are subject to the same phonological constraints. The question arises, then, whether morpheme-specific processes can be reanalysed as stratal differences. If so, perhaps a purely item-based analysis can be maintained, as long as stratal differences in phonology are permitted.

Level-ordering or stratal analyses (Kiparsky Reference Kiparsky1982, Reference Kiparsky, Elert, Johansson and Strangert1984, Reference Kiparsky2000, Reference Kiparsky, Vaux and Nevins2008; Mohanan Reference Mohanan1982; Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero1999, Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Trommer2012) assume that inner morphemes, those that surface closer to the root, are attached first. Roots plus inner morphemes are subject to stem-level, or level-1, phonological rules or constraints. Then, word-level affixes are attached, and the stem plus word-level affixes are evaluated by word-level phonological rules or constraints, which can differ in principled ways from the stem-level grammar. Words are evaluated separately before being evaluated together in strings by a fully general, non–morpheme-specific phrase-level or post-lexical grammar. This type of model makes a number of predictions: 1) all morphemes introduced at the stem level should be subject to the same phonological grammar; 2) all morphemes introduced at the word level should be subject to the same phonological grammar and 3) since multiple words are not present when stem- and word-level (exceptional) phonology applies, exceptional phonology should not cross word boundaries, and across-word phonology should be fully regular.

In the process-based case studies discussed here, the exceptional phonology is always associated with a very particular morphosyntactic context, such as nominative case on nominal modifiers in Kipsigis, or positive imperfective aspect in Guébie. These exceptional phonological processes do not apply to all stems, or to all words, but only in the presence of one particular morphosyntactic feature bundle. A stratal analysis would then require a stratum specific to a single morpheme, defeating the purpose of generalising over morphemes within a given stratum. Additionally, in Guébie, the imperfective tone shift affects both subjects and verbs; it crosses word boundaries. Stratal approaches assume that morpheme-specific or exceptional phonology will only apply within stems or words, and that cross-word phrasal phonology is fully general, so it is unclear how a cross-word morpheme-specific process like imperfective scalar tone shift could be modelled in a stratal account. For both reasons, stratal phonology does not seem to be able to account for the morpheme-specific behaviour of the process-based grammatical tone processes presented here.

Outside of tonal phonology, segmental processes and the behaviour of stress in other languages also suggest that morpheme-specific phonology is needed instead of, or perhaps in addition to, stratal phonology (Sande Reference Sande2019; Dąbkowski Reference Dąbkowski2021, to appear; Felice Reference Felice2022). That said, phenomena traditionally analysed as involving phonological strata can be reanalysed in CbP as syntactic phase-heads associated with morpheme-specific phonologies $\mathcal {R}$ , such that different subdomains are subject to different phonological grammars (Sande Reference Sande2019).

5.3 The phonology–syntax interface

One reason stated for assuming that all morphology is item-based is to simplify the syntax–phonology interface such that the only operation that refers to both morphosyntactic and phonological information is vocabulary insertion. For example, Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012: 428) write: ‘Morphology, we argue, may be reduced entirely to the function that spells out the syntactic tree by choosing and inserting phonologically contentful lexical items’. This limits the power of any one module of grammar, and maintains strict modularity between syntax and phonology (Inkelas Reference Inkelas1990; Scheer Reference Scheer, Block-Trojnar and Bloch-Rozmej2012). Strict modularity is a desirable property of grammatical architecture: ‘From a standpoint of theoretical economy and restrictiveness, it is clear that a theory is preferable where the different modules of the grammar do not have direct access to all information of other modules[…]. The challenge is hence to account for morpheme-specific phonology in a theory where the phonology has no direct access to specific morphological information’ (Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann2017: 17).

CbP is consistent with the view that the only operation that has access to morphosyntactic and phonological primitives is vocabulary insertion. Before vocabulary insertion, the only primitives present are morphosyntactic features and trees, so operations that apply before vocabulary insertion can only refer to morphosyntactic information. Vocabulary insertion maps morphosyntactic feature bundles to phonological content, such that after vocabulary insertion, the only information present is phonological. The differences between the assumptions of CbP and the assumptions of, say, Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012) come down to the assumptions about what kind of phonological information is present in vocabulary items. For Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012), the operation of vocabulary insertion replaces morphosyntactic feature content with phonological underlying forms and perhaps subcategorisation frames. In CbP, in addition to the two components assumed by Bye & Svenonius (Reference Bye, Svenonius and Trommer2012), there may also be constraint-weight readjustments associated with vocabulary items. The phonological module does not have access to any morphosyntactic information in CbP, only the phonological content of vocabulary items. The key novelty of CbP is the addition of the $\mathcal {R}$ morpheme-specific constraint weight component that drives morpheme-specific processes.

6. Conclusion

Here, we have seen that grammatical tone is sometimes straightforwardly analysed as additive, whereas in other cases, it is best analysed as a morpheme-specific phonological process such as across-the-board tonal polarity or scalar tone shift. CbP provides a model in which item-based and process-based phonologies coexist in morphological items which map morphosyntactic features to phonological content. The phonological content may contain an additive underlying form and/or a morpheme-specific cophonology which triggers a process. The data and model presented here bring into question the dichotomy between item-based and process-based analyses. In CbP, both item-based and process-based morphology are possible, either separately, as in the purely additive case of Guébie negative tone or the purely process-based case of Kipsigis tonal polarity, or the two may co-occur as realisations of a single morpheme, as in Chichewa morpheme-specific H-tone placement. All instances of morphology in CbP involve morphosyntactic features mapped to listed phonological content; however, that phonological content may contain item-base and/or process-based components.

One question that arises in CbP is how to determine whether a morpheme is best analysed as involving a process $\mathcal {R}$ or an item $\mathcal {F}$ . The diagnostics in (47), as well as the discussion in §2.4 and §4.4, are meant to serve as a guide. To elaborate on these diagnostics, process-based tonal morphology, or grammatical tone, is best analysed as involving some $\mathcal {R}$ in CbP if it involves an easily describable process that is phonologically predictable and derivable given some stem and phonological grammar, but that phonological process is not part of the general phonology of the language. In these cases, no single underlying form predicts the optimal output across cells in a paradigm, and a process-based analysis is preferable. Unlike item-based grammatical tone, the tonal alternations in morpheme-specific processes are not subject to the general phonological tonal behaviour of the language. Unlike suppletive grammatical tone, morpheme-specific grammatical tone processes are phonologically derivable and predictable. Item-based tone ( $\mathcal {F}$ ), on the other hand, involves a single, consistent, phonologically derivable realisation across paradigms, and can be derived through fully general phonological principles of the language. If an item-based analysis would require the proposal of a novel type of item, or would still require a morpheme-specific rule or constraint, a process-based account is preferred to an item-based one for reasons of economy. Suppletive analyses should be seen as a last resort, when alternations are not phonologically derivable, natural or predictable.

By including morpheme-specific constraint weight adjustments in our vocabulary items, morphemes can be associated with phonological items, processes, neither or both. The case studies here have shown that we need to be able to associate morphosyntactic features with processes in addition to items, motivating the $\mathcal {R}$ portion of CbP vocabulary items.

The case studies discussed here all involve grammatical tone. Tonal phenomena are particularly dramatic in their ability to cross word boundaries (such as the Guébie imperfective), affect multiple segments and morphemes within a word (such as in Kipsigis) and interact in complex ways. These across-the-board and long-distance phenomena make the non–item-based nature of tonal processes particularly easy to spot, but the proposed model does not rule out item-based grammatical tone, which was argued to be the best analysis of Guébie negation and Ebira stamp morphology in §2, or process-based segmental or metrical phonology. For examples of morpheme-specific segmental processes analysed in CbP, see Sande (Reference Sande2019, Reference Sande2020) and Felice (Reference Felice2022).

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Maria Kouneli and Yining Nie for sharing their work on Kipsigis, and to Yuni Kim for teaching me about the complex morphophonology of Amuzgo. Thanks also to Peter Jenks and Sharon Inkelas, who have been key collaborators in developing the formalism of CbP, and to Taylor Miller and Yuni Kim for helping to explore the consequences of the framework. I am ever grateful to Katherine Russell for being an excellent companion in fieldwork and co-analyst of the Guébie language. Innumerable thanks to the Guébie community and Guébie documentation team. Any mistakes are the author’s.

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Footnotes

1 This account could be combined with a stratal account, where there are distinct domain-specific phonological grammars, for example, in Stratal OT plus Generalised Non-Linear Affixation, as proposed by Bermúdez-Otero (Reference Bermúdez-Otero1999, Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Trommer2012), and as discussed further in §5.

2 A third type of approach not discussed in detail in the article are Word-and-Paradigm approaches (Matthews Reference Matthews1965; Blevins Reference Blevins2006, Reference Blevins2016), where inflectional classes are the basic units of grammar, whereas roots and affixes are epiphenomenal. Like process-based approaches, Word-and-Paradigm approaches are motivated by a lack of isomorphism between form and meaning, or a lack of consistent morphemes (Carstairs-McCarthy Reference Carstairs-McCarthy1983, Reference Carstairs-McCarthy1994; Stump Reference Stump2001, Reference Stump2015; Finkel & Stump Reference Finkel and Stump2007; Corbett Reference Corbett, Montermini, Boy and Teng2009; Ackerman & Malouf Reference Ackerman and Malouf2013; Booij Reference Booij2018).

3 Rolle (Reference Rolle2018) does not consider suppletive tonal allomorphy to be an instance of grammatical tone, since he considers all grammatical tone to be phonologically derived, and suppletive allomorphs are, by definition, not phonologically derived.

4 Phonologically regular, productive tone sandhi is arguably not a case of grammatical tone, but is purely phonologically conditioned. For a phonological analysis of tone sandhi, see, for example, Mortensen (Reference Mortensen2006).

5 Abbreviations used throughout this article: sec = secondary, obl = oblique, th = theme, poss = possessive, rel = relativiser, neg = negative, sg = singular, pl = plural, pfv = perfective, ipfv = imperfective, nom = nominative, acc = accusative, fut = future, agt = agentive, 1/2/3 = persons, cpl = completive, incpl = incompletive, CbP = Cophonologies by Phase.

6 Broader facts about the language also support the claim that the nominative is derived from the oblique form. Nilotic languages show marked nominative case (König Reference König2006), nouns have a predictable tone melody in the nominative and numerals take an affix in the nominative but not the oblique (Kouneli Reference Kouneli2019; Kouneli & Nie Reference Kouneli and Nie2021).

7 For additional evidence showing the existence of an OCP effect without cascading in Kipsigis, see Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021: Appendix A1) on tone patterns in the contexts of oblique modifiers introduced by a demonstrative, and Kouneli (Reference Kouneli2019) on tone in the context of a relativiser and other tone sandhi patterns in the language.

8 There is a suppletive, segmental negative morpheme in a particular tense/aspect context when an object enclitic is present. Otherwise, any time there is a non-pronominal subject, the negative morpheme in (32) appears.

9 While not shown here, when the subject is longer than one syllable, a constraint like Associate(L-R) ensures one-to-one left-to-right directional association of tones with TBUs. Any contour tone or sequence of identical level tones not at the right edge will incur a violation of this constraint.

10 These (anti)faithfulness constraints are defined gradiently, not existentially: a single candidate can incur multiple violations (Mortensen 2006; contra Alderete 2001).

11 Note that there is a third tonal form of modifiers, which Kouneli & Nie (Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) refer to as the predicative form. There may be reason to believe that the surface predicate form, rather than the oblique form, is (closest to) the underlying tonal form of a given modifier. The nominative seems to be built on the oblique form, although the best analysis may be that the oblique form is first built from the underlying (predicative-like) form, and the nominative is then built from the oblique form. This would involve an extra step in the CbP derivation, and would involve introducing a third tonal form of each modifier. For simplicity, I assume that the oblique is the underlying form, which does not change the relevant pieces of the analysis presented here.

12 Among DP elements that inflect for nominative case, tonal polarity is attested among adjectives, demonstratives and possessives, while nouns and numerals follow different nominative formation patterns. The assumption in the CbP analysis is that nouns have previously been spelled out as part of the n phase, and therefore are subject to stronger faithfulness constraints than the modifiers. There is independent evidence in the language that numerals have distinct structural properties from other modifiers (Kouneli Reference Kouneli2019).

13 Rolle (p.c.) analyses the difference between the recent past and negative imperative tone docking facts as due to a representational difference that assumes phantom planes of structure in the input, which are mapped to the prosodic structure of other morphemes in the output. The theoretical consequences of assuming phantom planes of structure in representations are not yet well understood, but may provide a purely representational means of differentiating between the tone docking sites across contexts in Chichewa.

References

Ackerman, Farrell & Malouf, Robert (2013). Morphological organization: the low conditional entropy conjecture. Lg 89. 429464.Google Scholar
Alderete, John D. (2001). Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonology 18. 201253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2011). Auxiliary verb constructions in the languages of Africa. Studies in African Linguistics 40. 1409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2015). STAMP morphs in Central Sudanic languages. In Mietzner, Angelika & Storch, Anne (eds.) Nilo-Saharan: models and descriptions. Köln: Rüdiger Köppe. 151167.Google Scholar
Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2016). STAMP morphs in the Macro-Sudan Belt. In Payne, Doris L., Pacchiarotti, Sara & Bosire, Mokaya (eds.) Diversity in African languages. Berlin: Language Science Press. 513539.Google Scholar
Anderson, Stephen R. (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anttila, Arto (2002). Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. NLLT 20. 142.Google Scholar
Bao, Zhiming (1999). The structure of tone. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (1999). Constraint interaction in language change: quantity in English and Germanic. PhD dissertation, University of Manchester.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2012). The architecture of grammar and the division of labour in exponence. In Trommer, Jochen (ed.) The morphology and phonology of exponence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, James P. (2006). Word-based morphology. JL 42. 531573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, James P. (2016). Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booij, Geert (2018). The construction of words: advances in Construction Morphology. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bredemann, Sebastian (2022). The role of phonology at morphological spell-out. PhD dissertation, Goethe University Frankfurt.Google Scholar
Bye, Patrik & Svenonius, Peter (2012). Nonconcatenative morphology as epiphenomenon. In Trommer, Jochen (ed.) The morphology and phonology of exponence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 427495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahill, Mike (2003). Tonal polarity in Knni. In Githiora, Chege, Littlefield, Heather & Manfredi, Victor (eds.) Kinyĩra njĩra! Step firmly on the pathway! Number 5 in Trends in African Linguistics. Lawrenceville, NJ: Africa World Press. 5362.Google Scholar
Campbell, Eric William (2014). Aspects of the phonology and morphology of Zenzontepec Chatino, a Zapotecan language of Oaxaca, Mexico. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Carstairs-McCarthy, Andrew (1983). Paradigm economy. JL 19. 115128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carstairs-McCarthy, Andrew (1994). Inflection classes, gender, and the Principle of Contrast. Lg 70. 737788.Google Scholar
Chang, Mei-Chih Laura (1992). A prosodic account of tone, stress, and tone sandhi in Chinese languages. PhD dissertation, University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Chen, Matthew Y. (2002). Tone sandhi: patterns across Chinese dialects. Number 92 in Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen & Downing, Laura J. (2016). Phasal syntax = cyclic phonology? Syntax 19. 156191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam (2001). Derivation by phase. In Kenstowicz, Michael (ed.) Ken Hale: a life in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 152.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam (2008). On phases. In Freiden, Robert, Otero, Carlos P. & Zubizarreta, Maria Luisa (eds.) Foundational issues in linguistic theory: essays in honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 133166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1983). The hierarchical representation of tone features. In Dihoff, Ivan R. (ed.) Current approaches to African linguistics, volume 1. Dordrecht: Foris. 145176.Google Scholar
Contreras, Heles (1969). Simplicity, descriptive adequacy, and binary features. Lg 45. 18.Google Scholar
Corbett, Greville G. (2009). Canonical inflectional classes. In Montermini, Fabio, Boy, Gilles & Teng, Jesse (eds.) Selected proceedings of the 6th Décembrettes: morphology in Bordeaux. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. 111.Google Scholar
Cruz, Emiliana (2011). Phonology, tone and the functions of tone in San Juan Quiahije Chatino. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Dąbkowski, Maksymilian (2021). Dominance is non-representational: evidence from A’ingae verbal stress. Phonology 38. 611650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbkowski, Maksymilian (to appear). Two grammars of A’ingae glottalization: a case for Cophonologies by Phase. To appear in NLLT.Google Scholar
Deal, Amy Rose & Wolf, Matthew (2017). Outwards-sensitive phonologically conditioned allomorphy in Nez Perce. In Gribanova, Vera & Shih, Stephanie S. (eds.) The morphosyntax–phonology connection: locality and directionality at the interface. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolatian, Hossep, Rawski, Jonathan & Heinz, Jeffrey (2021). Strong generative capacity of morphological processes. Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics 4. 228243.Google Scholar
Downing, Laura J. (2003). Compounding and tonal non-transfer in Bantu languages. Phonology 20. 142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downing, Laura J. & Mtenje, Al (2017). The phonology of Chichewa. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Embick, David & Noyer, Rolf (2007). Distributed Morphology and the syntax–morphology interface. In Ramchand, Gillian & Reiss, Charles (eds.) The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 289324.Google Scholar
Felice, Lydia (2022). Cyclicity and linearity in Distributed Morphology: the view from Icelandic, Gã, and Kabyle. PhD dissertation, Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Finkel, Raphael & Stump, Gregory (2007). Principal parts and morphological typology. Morphology 17. 3975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garvin, Karee, Russell, Katherine & Sande, Hannah (2022). There is no unified formal analysis of STAMP morphemes. Ms, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Goldwater, Sharon & Johnson, Mark (2003). Learning OT constraint rankings using a Maximum Entropy model. In Spenader, Jennifer, Eriksson, Anders & Dahl, Östen (eds.) Proceedings of the Stockholm Workshop on Variation Within Optimality Theory. Stockholm: Stockholm University. 111120.Google Scholar
Haas, Mary (1946). A grammatical sketch of Tunica. In Osgood, Cornelius (ed.) Linguistic structures of Native America. New York: Viking Fund. 337366.Google Scholar
Harris, Zellig S. (1944). Yokuts structure and Newman’s grammar. IJAL 10. 196211.Google Scholar
Harry, Otelemate & Hyman, Larry M. (2014). Phrasal construction tonology: the case of Kalabari. Studies in Language 38. 649689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haugen, Jason D. (2016). Readjustment: rejected? In Siddiqi, Daniel & Harley, Heidi (eds.) Morphological metatheory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 303342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1986). Assimilation as spreading in Toba Batak. LI 17. 467499.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce & Wilson, Colin (2008). A Maximum Entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning. LI 39. 379440.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F. (1954). Two models of grammatical description. Word 10. 210234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2010). Do tones have features? In Goldsmith, John A., Hume, Elizabeth & Leo Wetzels, W. (eds.) Tones and features: phonetic and phonological perspectives. Berlin: De Gruyter. 5080.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2011). Tone: is it different? In Goldsmith, John, Riggle, Jason & Alan, Yu (eds.) The handbook of phonological theory, 2nd edition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. 197239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2016). Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: who wins? In Palancar, Enrique L. & Léonard, Jean Léo (eds.) Tone and inflection: new facts and new perspectives. Berlin: De Gruyter. 1539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2018). Towards a typology of postlexical tonal neutralizations. In Kobozono, Haruo & Giriko, Mikio (eds.) Tonal change and neutralization. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. 726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. & Mtenje, Al (1999). Prosodic morphology and tone: the case of Chichewa. In Kager, René, van der Hulst, Harry & Zonneveld, Wim (eds.) The prosody–morphology interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 90133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon (1990). Prosodic constituency in the lexicon. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon (2014). The interplay of morphology and phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon & Zoll, Cheryl (2005). Reduplication: doubling in morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon & Zoll, Cheryl (2007). Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned phonology. Linguistics 45. 133171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iosad, Pavel (2014). The phonology and morphosyntax of Breton mutation. Lingue e linguaggio 13. 2342.Google Scholar
Ishihara, Shinichiro (2003). Intonation and interface conditions. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Ishihara, Shinichiro (2007). Major phrase, focus intonation, multiple spell-out (MaP, FI, MSO). The Linguistic Review 24. 137167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itô, Junko & Mester, Armin (1999). Realignment. In Kager, René, van der Hulst, Harry & Zonneveld, Wim (eds.) The prosody–morphology interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 188217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenks, Peter (2018). The composition of the Moro verb. Paper presented at the Princeton Symposium on Syntactic Theory (PSST).Google Scholar
Jenks, Peter & Rose, Sharon (2015). Mobile object markers in Moro: the role of tone. Lg 91. 269307.Google Scholar
Kahnemuyipour, Arsalan (2009). The syntax of sentential stress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karttunen, Lauri (2003). Computing with realizational morphology. In Gelbukh, Alexander (ed.) Computational linguistics and intelligent text processing. Berlin: Springer. 203214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastner, Itamar (2019). Templatic morphology as an emergent property. NLLT 37. 571619.Google Scholar
Kim, Yuni (2016). Tonal overwriting and inflectional exponence in Amuzgo. In Panacar, Enrique L. & Léonard, Jean Léo (eds.) Tone and inflection. Berlin: De Gruyter. 199224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (1982). Lexical morphology and phonology. In Linguistics in the morning calm: selected papers from SICOL-1981. Seoul: Hanshin. 391.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (1984). On the lexical phonology of Icelandic. In Elert, Claes-Christian, Johansson, Iréne & Strangert, Eva (eds.) Nordic prosody III: papers from a symposium. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell. 135164.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (2000). Opacity and cyclicity. The Linguistic Review 17. 351367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (2008). Fenno-Swedish quantity: contrast in Stratal OT. In Vaux, Bert & Nevins, Andrew (eds.) Rules, constraints, and phonological phenomena. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 185219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
König, Christa (2006). Marked nominative in Africa. Studies in Language 30. 655732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kouneli, Maria (2019). The syntax of number and modification: an investigation of the Kipsigis DP. PhD dissertation, New York University.Google Scholar
Kouneli, Maria & Nie, Yining (2021). Across-the-board tonal polarity in Kipsigis: implications for the morphology–phonology interface. Lg 97. e111e138.Google Scholar
Kratzer, Angelika & Selkirk, Elisabeth (2007). Phase theory and prosodic spellout: the case of verbs. The Linguistic Review 24. 93135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kubozono, Haruo (2016). Diversity of pitch accent systems in Koshikijima Japanese. Gengo Kenkyu (Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan) 150. 131.Google Scholar
Leben, William (1973). Suprasegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Leben, William R. (2019). Tone and length in Mende. In Bowler, Margit, Duncan, Philip T., Major, Travis & Torrence, Harold (eds.) Schuhschrift: papers in honor of Russell Schuh. Los Angeles, CA: eScholarship Publishing, University of California. 7787.Google Scholar
Lieber, Rochelle (1992). Deconstructing morphology: word formation in syntactic theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Marlo, Michael R., Mwita, Leonard Chacha & Paster, Mary (2015). Problems in Kuria H tone assignment. NLLT 33. 251265.Google Scholar
Matthews, Peter H. (1965). The inflectional component of a word-and-paradigm grammar. JL 1. 139171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McIntosh, Justin Daniel (2015). Aspects of phonology and morphology of Teotepec Eastern Chatino. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
McKendry, Inga (2013). Tonal association, prominence and prosodic structure in South-Eastern Nochixtlán Mixtec. PhD dissertation, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2014). Replacive grammatical tone in the Dogon languages. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2016). Cyclic spell-out and the interaction of Seenku tonal processes. In DiCanio, Christian, Malins, Jeffrey, Good, Jeff, Michelson, Karin, Jaeger, Jeri & Keily, Holly (eds.) Tonal aspects of languages 2016. International Speech Communication Association. 4347.Google Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2019). Seenku argument-head tone sandhi: allomorph selection in a cyclic grammar. Glossa 4. 22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, Laura & Heath, Jeffrey (2016). Phrasal grammatical tone in the Dogon languages. NLLT 34. 593639.Google Scholar
Mel’čuk, Igor (1994). Suppletion: toward a logical analysis of the concept. Studies in Language 18. 339410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyase, Savio M. (2021). Polarity in a four-level tone language: tone features in Tenyidie. Phonology 38. 123146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Taylor L. & Sande, Hannah (2021). Is word-level recursion actually recursion? Lg 6. 100.Google Scholar
Mohanan, Karuvannur Puthanveettil (1982). Lexical phonology. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Mortensen, David Roland (2006). Logical and substantive scales in phonology. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Odden, David & Bickmore, Lee (2014). Melodic tone in Bantu: overview. Africana Linguistica 20. 313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orgun, Cemil Orhan (1996). Sign-based morphology and phonology with special attention to Optimality Theory. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Pak, Marjorie (2008). The postsyntactic derivation and its phonological reflexes. PhD dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Palancar, Enrique L. (2021). Paradigmatic structure in the tonal inflection of Amuzgo. Morphology 31. 4582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palancar, Enrique L. & Léonard, Jean Léo (2016). Tone and inflection: new facts and new perspectives. Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pater, Joe (2009). Morpheme-specific phonology: constraint indexation and inconsistency resolution. In Parker, Steve (ed.) Phonological argumentation: essays on evidence and motivation. London: Equinox. 123154.Google Scholar
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1986). Tone in lexical phonology. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawski, Jonathan, Dolatian, Hossep, Heinz, Jeffrey & Raimy, Eric (2023). Regular and polyregular theories of reduplication. Glossa 8. 38 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roark, Brian & Sproat, Richard (2007). Computational approaches to morphology and syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, Jamillah Shantel (2021). The phonology and syntax of grammatical tone in Copala Triqui. PhD dissertation, State University of New York at Albany.Google Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas (2018). Grammatical tone: typology, theory, and functional load. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas (2022). Unpacking portmanteaux: non-linear morphology in the Ebira STAMP system. Studies in African Linguistics 51. 140179.Google Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas & Lionnet, Florian (2020). Phantom structure: a representational account of floating tone association. In Baek, Hyunah, Takahashi, Chikako & Yeung, Alex Hong-Lun (eds.) Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. 10 pp.Google Scholar
Sande, Hannah (2017). Distributing morphologically conditioned phonology: three case studies from Guébie. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Sande, Hannah (2018). Cross-word morphologically conditioned scalar tone shift in Guébie. Morphology 28. 253295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sande, Hannah (2019). A unified account of conditioned phonological alternations: evidence from Guébie. Lg 95. 456497.Google Scholar
Sande, Hannah (2020). Morphologically conditioned phonology with two triggers. Phonology 37. 453493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sande, Hannah & Jenks, Peter (2018). Cophonologies by phase. NELS 48. 3952.Google Scholar
Sande, Hannah, Jenks, Peter & Inkelas, Sharon (2020). Cophonologies by ph(r)ase. NLLT 38. 12111261.Google Scholar
Scheer, Tobias (2012). Chunk definition in phonology: prosodic constituency vs. phase structure. In Block-Trojnar, Maria & Bloch-Rozmej, Anna (eds.) Modules and interfaces. Lublin: KUL. 221253.Google Scholar
Scholz, Hans-Jürgen (1976). Igbira phonology. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Snider, Keith L. (1999). The geometry and features of tone. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Spencer, Andrew (1991). Morphological theory: an introduction to word structure in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stump, Gregory (2001). Inflectional morphology: a theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stump, Gregory (2015). Inflectional paradigms: content and form at the syntax–morphology interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivant, John Ryan (2015). The phonology and inflectional morphology of Cháknyá, Tataltepec de Valdés Chatino, a Zapotecan language. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Trommer, Jochen (2011). Phonological aspects of Western Nilotic mutation morphology. PhD dissertation, Unversität Leipzig.Google Scholar
Trommer, Jochen & Zimmermann, Eva (2014). Generalised mora affixation and quantity-manipulating morphology. Phonology 31. 463510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uchihara, Hiroto & Cano, Gregorio Tiburcio (2020). A phonological account of Tlapanec (Mè’phàà) tonal alternation. JL 56. 807863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villard, Stéphanie (2015). The phonology and morphology of Zacatepec Eastern Chatino. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Wang, C.-M. Bonnie (2014). The prosody–syntax interaction in the “yi-bu-qi-ba” rule: a morphologically conditioned tone change in Mandarin Chinese. Master’s thesis, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.Google Scholar
Welmers, Wm. E. (1959). Tonemics, morphotonemics, and tonal morphemes. General Linguistics 4. 19.Google Scholar
Wilson, Colin & George, Benjamin (2008). Maxent grammar tool. Software package. Available at https://linguistics.ucla.edu/people/hayes/MaxentGrammarTool/.Google Scholar
Yang, Bei (2015). Perception and production of Mandarin tones by native speakers and L2 learners. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, Moira (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, Moira Jean (1980). The tonal phonology of Chinese. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, Eva (2016). The power of a single representation: morphological tone and allomorphy. Morphology 26. 269294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, Eva (2017). Morphological length and prosodically defective morphemes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, Eva (2018). Being exceptional is being weak: tonal exceptions in San Miguel el Grande Mixtec. In Gallagher, Gillian, Gouskova, Maria & Yin, Sora Heng (eds.) Proceedings of the 2017 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. 12 pp.Google Scholar