Introduction
Dementia is an ongoing public health concern that will affect 153 million people worldwide by 2050 (Nichols et al., Reference Nichols, Steinmetz, Vollset, Fukutaki, Chalek, Abd-Allah and Vos2022). Global efforts to lower the number of future cases are twofold and consist of developing curative treatments as well as identifying behavioral targets for primary prevention and risk reduction. In this regard, research into modifiable risk factors has gained momentum, and it is suggested that a substantial proportion of future dementia cases could theoretically be delayed or prevented by addressing such factors (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee and Cooper2020). Potential targets include the management of health conditions, such as hypertension, obesity, and hearing loss, but also lifestyle-related factors, including physical inactivity, unhealthy diet, or low social contact (Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, Van Boxtel, Schiepers, De Vugt, Sánchez, Anstey and Köhler2015; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee and Cooper2020).
With regard to the latter, research suggests that different aspects of social relationships may be protective against cognitive decline and dementia. For example, studies have shown that people who frequently engage in social leisure activities or are members of clubs/organizations have a lower risk of developing dementia, independent of other dementia risk factors (Almeida-Meza, Steptoe, & Cadar, Reference Almeida-Meza, Steptoe and Cadar2021; Duffner et al., Reference Duffner, Deckers, Cadar, Steptoe, de Vugt and Köhler2022). Similarly, larger social networks, higher quality of social relationships, and lower levels of loneliness and social isolation have been associated with lower dementia risk (Amieva et al., Reference Amieva, Stoykova, Matharan, Helmer, Antonucci and Dartigues2010; Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Zuidersma, Oude Voshaar, Zuidema, van den Heuvel, Stolk and Smidt2015; Samtani et al., Reference Samtani, Mahalingam, Lam, Lipnicki, Lima-Costa, Blay and Brodaty2022). Some of those ‘social relationship constructs’ are ill-defined, based on face validity and highly overlapping, but so far, no studies have addressed this potential overlap psychometrically by studying the existence of overarching latent constructs.
Insight into possible pathways of the association between social relationship factors and dementia risk may help identify the most beneficial ‘social targets’ and is thus of major relevance for the design of future prevention programs and health initiatives. However, relatively few studies have addressed the putative causal structure between different social relationship factors and dementia risk. For instance, while socio-emotional factors, such as depressive symptoms, have been suggested as potential mediators of the association between social relationship factors and dementia risk, no study has formally tested these pathways (John, Patel, Rusted, Richards, & Gaysina, Reference John, Patel, Rusted, Richards and Gaysina2019; Qiao et al., Reference Qiao, Wang, Tang, Zhou, Min, Yin and Li2022). This also includes the possibility of reverse causation, in that impending dementia is the cause rather than the result of changes in social relationships (Bielak & Gow, Reference Bielak and Gow2022; Floud et al., Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Reus, Hofman and Beral2021).
There is thus a need to examine which and how social relationship factors might explain dementia risk. The current study, therefore, aims to identify meaningful constructs of social relationships using factor analysis (research question 1); to examine the association between identified constructs and incident dementia (research question 2), and to determine whether depressive symptoms mediate these associations (research question 3). It also aims to assess the possibility of reverse causation between identified factors and incident dementia (research question 4).
Methods
Study design and participants
Study participants were drawn from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), a multi-center, observational cohort study launched in 2002 (Steptoe, Breeze, Banks, & Nazroo, Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo2013a). Data collection took place in bi-annual waves and included information about health status, socioeconomic position, social well-being, lifestyle, and cognition. Details about the sampling procedure and data collection have been published previously (Steptoe et al., Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo2013a). Participants have been initially selected to be representative of the general English population aged 50 years and older. The current study used data from Wave 2 (2004/2005) as baseline, as this is the first wave where an in-depth assessment of social relationship variables was conducted. Participants were followed up biannually until Wave 9 (2018/2019), resulting in a maximum observational period of 15 years. Of the initial 8780 participants, those with prevalent dementia at baseline (until Wave 2; n = 45) and without follow-up (n = 1199) were excluded. The analytical sample thus included 7536 participants. All participants provided their informed consent and the ELSA data collection protocol was approved by the National Research Ethics Service, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Measures
Dementia ascertainment
Dementia was ascertained by a combination of either self-reported physician-diagnosed dementia (possible/probable Alzheimer's disease or an unspecified subtype) or a score of 3.38 or higher on the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE; Jorm, Reference Jorm1994). In the IQCODE, a close contact person of the participant rates changes (from 1- much improved to 5-much worse) in everyday cognitive functioning over a period of two years, based on 16 questions (e.g., ‘Ability to remember what day/month it is compared to last interview’), responses to which are averaged to arrive at the final rating. The IQCODE has been well-validated across several populations and has shown good psychometric properties with regard to the chosen cutoff. More specifically, the IQCODE has been found to discriminate between people with a clinical diagnosis of dementia and those without with a sensitivity of 0.80 and a specificity of 0.84 (Quinn et al., Reference Quinn, Fearon, Noel-Storr, Young, McShane and Stott2021).
Social relationship items
A set of 36 items pertaining to various aspects of social relationships, including loneliness, was selected for factor analysis from questionnaires administered at Wave 2 based on their use in the operationalization of several social constructs in previous ELSA publications (Duffner et al., Reference Duffner, Deckers, Cadar, Steptoe, de Vugt and Köhler2022; Fancourt, Steptoe, & Cadar, Reference Fancourt, Steptoe and Cadar2020; Khondoker, Rafnsson, Morris, Orrell, & Steptoe, Reference Khondoker, Rafnsson, Morris, Orrell and Steptoe2017; Rafnsson, Orrell, D'Orsi, Hogervorst, & Steptoe, Reference Rafnsson, Orrell, D'Orsi, Hogervorst and Steptoe2020). Table 1 contains an overview of all included items, along with the overarching constructs used in such previous studies. Items were phrased either in a dichotomous (yes/no) fashion or on a Likert scale with four to six response options.
Depressive symptoms
The eight-item version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies–Depression (CES-D) questionnaire was used to assess depressive symptoms. The CES-D assesses affective and somatic symptoms in the general population and has shown excellent psychometric properties (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). Items are rated in a dichotomous manner and then added up, resulting in a theoretical range of 0–8, with higher scores indicating more symptoms related to depression. A detailed overview of the symptoms assessed by the CES-D is included in online Supplementary Table 1.
Demographics and health conditions
Baseline age and gender were used as covariates in all analyses. Furthermore, we incrementally controlled for the socioeconomic and cardiovascular risk factors outlined below. The selection of covariates was based on a pragmatic choice of factors having a known association with both social relationship factors and dementia.
Education. Self-reported levels of education (i.e., the highest educational level achieved) were clustered into high (college/university education), medium (ordinary level or secondary education), and low (no formal education).
Wealth. Household net-wealth was estimated by subtracting open payments and mortgages from all self-reported possessions, including the value of the participant's home, physical wealth, and other assets. Tertiles were subsequently created, representing high, medium, and low wealth.
Coronary heart disease. Self-reported, diagnosed angina pectoris, or myocardial infarction up to Wave 2 were used as measures of coronary heart disease. Both measures were collected retrospectively at the Wave 3 (2006/2007) assessment.
Hypertension. A combination of self-reported, diagnosed hypertension (assessed retrospectively at Wave 3), and mean systolic and diastolic blood pressure, measured at the Wave 2 nurse visit, were used. In line with guidelines of the World Health Organization and the International Society of Hypertension, a mean systolic blood pressure of ⩾140 mm Hg or mean diastolic blood pressure of ⩾90 mm Hg were regarded as indicative of hypertension (Unger et al., Reference Unger, Borghi, Charchar, Khan, Poulter, Prabhakaran and Schutte2020; Whitworth, Reference Whitworth2003).
Diabetes. Self-reported, diagnosed type-1 or type-2 diabetes or high blood glucose levels (assessed at Wave 3) or a blood glycated hemoglobin level of ⩾6.5% (48 mmol/mol) at the Wave 2 nurse visit were used in conjunction for inferring diabetes (World Health Organization, 2011).
Statistical analysis
Baseline differences in demographic characteristics between people with and without dementia were assessed using t-tests or χ2-tests.
Factor analyses
To study the overlap between different social relationship measures and to identify meaningful latent constructs (i.e., data-reduction; research question 1), factor analyses were conducted in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017). For this, we first followed an unrestrained (exploratory factor analysis; EFA) approach, with the goal of identifying a maximum number of social constructs. Based on the results of this EFA, we then conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Unlike EFA, where items are allowed to load across all factors, CFA imposes further restrictions. More specifically, items were initially restrained to load only the factors with the highest factor loadings in EFA. While in a traditional sense, a CFA may imply the replication of findings in a different sample, here we refer to a factor analysis with the imposition of further restrictions in the same sample.
In both EFA and CFA, model fit was assessed through χ2 values, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the non-normed fit index (i.e., Tucker–Lewis Index; TLI). A non-significant χ2 value, RMSEA ⩽0.07 and CFI and TLI ⩾0.90 were used as indicative of acceptable model fit for both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (Hooper et al., Reference Hooper, Coughlan, Mullen, Mullen, Hooper, Coughlan and Mullen2008).
Exploratory factor analysis. After the examination of zero-order correlations, all social relationship items were imported into Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017). Given the expected non-orthogonal (i.e., interrelated) factor structure, an OBLIMIN rotation was chosen. The minimally significant factor loading for an item to be retained was set to |0.4|, reflecting a rather liberal approach in conformity with previous studies (Peterson, Reference Peterson2000). Items below this threshold were dropped iteratively, starting with the lowest-loading item, and loadings were re-examined after removal. Eigenvalues were then examined, and factor solutions with a value of ⩾1.0 were retained for individual inspection. Following a parsimonious approach, the solution with the lowest number of factors that was theoretically sound and had an acceptable model fit was subsequently used for confirmatory factor analysis.
Confirmatory factor analysis. The EFA factor solution was then tested in a CFA. In this CFA, depression was included as a latent factor, comprised of the eight items of the CES-D, to reduce measurement error. With the aim of improving model fit, modification indices were inspected, and cross-loadings or residual correlations between individual items were added wherever this was also theoretically sound. Weighted least square means and variance (WLSMV) adjusted estimators were chosen in light of the individual items being measured on both nominal and ordinal scales. Factor scores were then created and standardized into z-scores (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) for use as predictors in further analyses. In case of missing values for individual items, factor scores were estimated based on all other available information.
Cox proportional hazard regression
To study associations between the social relationship constructs identified by factor analysis and incident dementia (research question 2), Cox-proportional hazard regression analyses (survival analysis) were conducted. More specifically, factor z-scores generated by the CFA were used as main predictors in the survival analyses, resulting in hazard ratios (HRs) and their 95% confidence intervals (CI's). Incident dementia up to Wave 9 was regarded as failure event, and cases were recorded at every wave. Survival time was defined as period from the start of the observation period (i.e., the Wave 2 interview) until the time dementia was reported or until censoring (i.e., the last available interview). The midpoint between the previous and last available Wave was chosen as date of diagnosis. Covariates were added incrementally, starting with a minimally (age and gender) adjusted model (Model 1), with additional adjustments for education (Model 2), wealth (Model 3), and cardiovascular risk factors (hypertension, coronary heart disease, and diabetes; Model 4). The fully adjusted model (Model 4) thus included all socio-demographic and cardiovascular risk factors.
The proportional hazard assumption was assessed both numerically using Schoenfeld residuals (Schoenfeld, Reference Schoenfeld1982) and through visual inspection of clog–log plots. As ELSA participants could also be selected from the same household, we additionally adjusted for household clusters using a sandwich estimator. Furthermore, baseline cross-sectional weights were used to reduce selection bias by back-weighting estimates from the analytical sample to the full ELSA sample. The threshold level for statistical significance was chosen at p ⩽ 0.05 using two-sided tests. Survival analyses were conducted in Stata (StataCorp, 2021; version 17).
Structural equation modeling (mediation analyses)
Next, we examined whether depressive symptoms may explain the association between social relationship factors and incident dementia (research question 3). First, standardized factor scores of the depression factor as derived from CFA were added to Model 4 of the survival analyses as continuous variable, and changes in HRs and their p-values examined. Whenever (partial) mediation was suggested in these additional survival analyses, structural equation modeling was used to decompose associations into total, direct, and indirect (depression-mediated) associations. In these mediation analyses, we controlled for the same covariates as in Model 4 of the survival analyses. The percentage of effect mediation was calculated by dividing the total indirect effect estimates by those of the total effect. Structural equation modeling was conducted in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017).
Assessing the possibility of reverse causation
The possibility of reverse causation (research question 4) was assessed in three ways. First, we added a composite score of baseline cognition as an additional covariate to Model 4 of the survival analyses. This entailed averaging z-scores of tests of memory (delayed recall of a word list), semantic fluency (animal naming), and temporal orientation. Next, we excluded people who developed dementia in the first 24 months after baseline. This step was subsequently repeated for dementia cases of the first 48 months. Finally, the analytical sample was stratified based on follow-up times (⩽5 years v. >5 years), and the main analyses were repeated for each follow-up stratum separately. In view of the resulting reduction in sample sizes and thus statistical power, only changes in HRs (rather than levels of significance) were assessed.
Results
Sample characteristics
At baseline, participants were on average 66.3 years old (s.d. = 9.7) and 55.5% were female. Across a median follow-up time of 11.8 years (IQR = 5.9–13.9 years), 501 people developed dementia (67.9 cases per 10 000 person-years, 95%CI 62.2–74.1 cases). Compared to people who did not develop dementia during the study period, those who did were significantly older (t = 19.1; p < 0.001), more likely to be female (χ2 = 6.3; p = 0.012), had lower education (χ2 = 49.0; p < 0.001) and net-wealth (χ2 = 25.1; p < 0.001), more likely had hypertension (χ2 = 21.4; p < 0.001), coronary heart disease (χ2 = 27.0; p < 0.001) and diabetes (χ2 = 11.5; p = 0.003), and reported more depressive symptoms (t = 5.0; p < 0.001). Table 2 contains a summary of sample characteristics stratified by dementia status. An overview of incident dementia cases per study Wave is presented in online Supplementary Table 2.
a Statistically significant (p ⩽ 0.05).
b Due to rounding, percentages may not add up to 100.
Factor analysis of social relationship items (research question 1)
Step 1 – EFA. There were 58 people with missing information on all social relationship items and were thus excluded from the EFA. Zero-order correlations between individual items ranged from 0 to |.8|. After inspection of eigenvalues >1.0, as well as visual inspection of the scree plot for points of inflexion, the 6-, 7-, 8-, and 9-factor solutions were retained for further scrutiny. Online Supplementary Table 3 contains the fit indices of all examined solutions. One item (‘How often respondent feels in tune with the people around them’) had a factor loading below the threshold of |.4| for all solutions and was not included in further analyses. A 7-factor solution showed adequate model fit (RMSEAEFA = 0.061; CFIEFA = 0.93; TLIEFA = 0.89). Standardized factor loadings for this solution are presented in online Supplementary Table 4. Items relating to meeting up with/speaking on the phone with children and other families cross-loaded on factors reflecting positive experiences of support with the respective relationship type. For contact with friends, this was not the case, but all items pertaining to contact with friends loaded on a single factor. In the subsequent CFA, those items were loaded on only one factor specific to relationship type, in order to aid interpretability.
Step 2 – CFA. The structure of the resulting six-factor model was as follows; Three factors related to items on both frequency of contact and positive experiences of social support with children (factor called: F children), other family (F family), and friends (F friends). Items relating to the engagement in social leisure activities loaded on a fourth factor (F activity). Finally, items pertaining to negative experiences of social support (regardless of relationship type) and feelings of loneliness loaded on factors F neg.exp. and F loneliness, respectively. The model fit of this solution was suboptimal (RMSEA = 0.10; CFI = 0.62; TLI = 0.60). Model modification indices suggested the addition of cross-loadings between items relating to contact frequency between relationship types, as well as between contact frequency and social activity items, incorporation of which significantly improved model fit (RMSEA = 0.04; CFI = 0.93; TLI = 0.92). Depression was then included as additional latent factor using all items of the CES-D (F depression). The item ‘Whether respondent felt lonely much of the time during the past week’ was cross-loaded on both F loneliness and F depression. This final 7-factor (six social factors plus F depression) solution showed good fit (RMSEA = 0.04; CFI = 0.93; TLI = 0.92). Figure 1 contains a simplified visual depiction of the factor structure derived from CFA. Subsequent analyses involving F children, F family and F friends were restricted to those reporting to have children (n = 5881), other family (n = 6285), and friends (n = 6464), respectively.
Associations between social relationship constructs and incident dementia (research question 2)
Multivariable Cox regression then studied the relation between the social relationship factors and incident dementia (Table 3). Higher z-scores of F children were associated with lower dementia risk (Model 4: HR = 0.88; 95%CI 0.79–0.98; p = 0.021), as were higher z-scores of F activity (Model 4: HR = 0.84; 95%CI 0.74–0.94; p < 0.001). In contrast, higher z-scores of F loneliness (Model 4; HR = 1.13; 95%CI 1.03–1.25; p = 0.011) and F neg.exp. (Model 4: HR = 1.10; 95%CI 1.00–1.20; p = 0.047) were associated with higher dementia risk. Neither of the other two social factors significantly predicted dementia risk in Model 4.
CI, confidence interval; HR, hazard ratio.
a Sample restricted to those reporting to have children;
b Sample restricted to those reporting to have other family;
c Sample restricted to those reporting to have friends;
d p ⩽ 0.01;
e p ⩽ 0.05.
Model 1 = age + gender; Model 2 = model 1 + education; Model 3 = model 2 + net-wealth; Model 4 (main model) = model 3 + heart disease, hypertension and diabetes; Model 5 = model 4 + F depression.
Mediation by depressive symptoms (research question 3)
Structural equation modeling identified significant indirect effects (mediation) by depressive symptoms (F depression) for F children (40.7% mediation; HR = 0.82; p = 0.006), F activity (50.7% mediation; HR = 0.84; p = 0.010), F neg.exp. (47.6% mediation; HR = 1.16; p = 0.009), and F loneliness (78.8% mediation; HR = 1.49; p = 0.016; online Supplementary Figures 1-4).
In the opposite direction (i.e., using the individual factor scores as mediators), no significant indirect (mediated) effects of depressive symptoms via F children, F activity, F neg.exp., or F loneliness on incident dementia were observed (results not shown).
The possibility of reverse causation (research question 4)
The results observed in the Cox proportional hazard regression remained virtually unchanged after adding baseline cognition and excluding incident dementia cases of the first 24 and 48 months. When assessing those with ⩽5 and >5 years of follow-up separately, associations remained directionally similar, though significant only for those with >5 years of follow-up for z-scores of F activity. In the analyses only including those with ⩽5 years of follow-up, associations were not statistically significant and attenuated for z-scores of F children and F loneliness. For z-scores of F activity, an inverse association was found (HR = 1.13) in those with ⩽5 years of follow-up. Detailed results of analyses for assessing the possibility of reverse causation can be found in Table 4.
Abbreviations: HR, hazard ratio; FU, follow-up.
a Model 4;
b Sample restricted to those reporting to have children;
c Statistically significant (p ⩽ 0.05);
d Sample restricted to those reporting to have other family;
e Sample restricted to those reporting to have friends.
Discussion
In this study, we examined the presence of different social relationship constructs and their associations with incident dementia across 15 years of follow-up in the general English population. Factor analysis suggested six distinct social relationship factors. Higher scores on factors pertaining to the frequency and the quality of contact with children, social leisure activity engagement, as well as lower scores on factors concerning negative experiences of social support and loneliness were associated with lower dementia risk. These associations were independent of age, gender, education, wealth, and cardiovascular risk factors. Furthermore, for all four factors, associations were partly mediated by depressive symptoms. In additional analyses aiming to assess the possibility of reverse causation, associations remained largely unchanged.
Social relationship factors and dementia risk
The finding that the quality and quantity of contact with the respondent's children (as opposed to other family and friends) was predictive for incident dementia aligns with some studies suggesting the potential protective role of some structural aspects of intergenerational contact for cognition and dementia risk (Fratiglioni, Wang, Ericsson, Maytan, & Winblad, Reference Fratiglioni, Wang, Ericsson, Maytan and Winblad2000; Meister & Zahodne, Reference Meister and Zahodne2022; Zahodne, Ajrouch, Sharifian, & Antonucci, Reference Zahodne, Ajrouch, Sharifian and Antonucci2019). Other studies also found associations between contact with family and/or friends and cognitive decline and dementia (Meister & Zahodne, Reference Meister and Zahodne2022; Sharifian, Kraal, Zaheed, Sol, & Zahodne, Reference Sharifian, Kraal, Zaheed, Sol and Zahodne2020; Sommerlad, Sabia, Singh-Manoux, Lewis, & Livingston, Reference Sommerlad, Sabia, Singh-Manoux, Lewis and Livingston2019; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Ajrouch, Sharifian and Antonucci2019). While directionally similar, these associations were not significant in the current study. In general, a disaggregation of social relationship types, as suggested by our factor analysis and in line with the social convoy model (Antonucci, Ajrouch, & Birditt, Reference Antonucci, Ajrouch and Birditt2014), may be useful in light of the different functions and resources offered by different people in someone's social network (Antonucci et al., Reference Antonucci, Ajrouch and Birditt2014; Gurung, Taylor, & Seeman, Reference Gurung, Taylor and Seeman2003; Meister & Zahodne, Reference Meister and Zahodne2022; Sharifian et al., Reference Sharifian, Manly, Zahodne, Morris, Schupf, Mayeux and Brickman2021).
The finding that more social leisure activity engagement was associated with lower dementia risk aligns with a growing body of literature suggesting its potential role in curbing age-related cognitive decline and lowering dementia risk (Duffner et al., Reference Duffner, Deckers, Cadar, Steptoe, de Vugt and Köhler2022; Su et al., Reference Su, Shi, Zheng, Sun, Huang, Zhang and Lu2022). Likewise, the finding that lower levels of negative experiences of social support and loneliness were associated with lower dementia risk, is in line with some studies (Khondoker et al., Reference Khondoker, Rafnsson, Morris, Orrell and Steptoe2017; Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Zuidersma, Oude Voshaar, Zuidema, van den Heuvel, Stolk and Smidt2015). However, evidence for the role of loneliness as a risk factor for dementia is generally more mixed. This may be due to heterogeneity in study populations and the operationalization of loneliness (Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Zuidersma, Oude Voshaar, Zuidema, van den Heuvel, Stolk and Smidt2015; Penninkilampi, Casey, Singh, & Brodaty, Reference Penninkilampi, Casey, Singh and Brodaty2018; Qiao et al., Reference Qiao, Wang, Tang, Zhou, Min, Yin and Li2022; Samtani et al., Reference Samtani, Mahalingam, Lam, Lipnicki, Lima-Costa, Blay and Brodaty2022).
Notably, we found little evidence for reverse causation. A large prospective study with modestly longer follow-up times of up to 20 years suggested that reverse causation might explain the association between social leisure activity engagement and dementia (Floud et al., Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Reus, Hofman and Beral2021). In that study, authors found weaker associations with longer follow-up duration, which was not the case in ELSA. It was, however, conducted in a women-only cohort, which may hamper comparability with the current study. Likewise, while Floud et al. (Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Reus, Hofman and Beral2021) did include a range of activities with a social component (e.g., attending groups for art, craft, or music), the only overlap with activities included in the current study was voluntary work.
Another study assessed reverse causation in the association between trajectories of loneliness and cognition using cross-legged panel models. While cognition predicted changes in loneliness, there was no evidence for the pathway being the other way around (Okely, Deary, & Gutchess, Reference Okely, Deary and Gutchess2019). However, in the current study, adding baseline cognition as a covariate or excluding incident dementia cases of the first 24 and 48 months did not weaken associations between the social relationship factors and incident dementia. Also splitting follow-up time, while lowering the number of incident cases per stratum and thus reducing statistical power, was not suggestive of reverse causation. Yet, in light of the observational nature of the current study, the possibility of reverse causation can never be entirely disregarded. It must also be noted that explanations are not mutually exclusive but that a bi-directional relationship might exist, in which poor social relationships increase dementia risk while impending dementia lowers social participation and contact frequency (Xiang et al., Reference Xiang, Lai, Bao, Sun, Chen, Dunkle and Maust2021).
Potential socioemotional and neurobiological pathways
The significant associations between the social relationship factors and dementia risk, as suggested by survival analyses, may align with the concept of cognitive reserve. This heuristic concept postulates that life experiences may lead to more flexibility in cognitive processes, which may help maintain mental functioning in light of neuropathology (Stern, Reference Stern2002; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat and Vuoksimaa2018). In addition to a potential effect on cognition via such functional mechanisms, different aspects of social relationships may also be directly related to brain structure. More specifically, larger social network size, more social support, lower levels of loneliness, and more social activity engagement have been positively associated with brain volumes and microstructural integrity, also in brain areas implicated in cognitive aging (Duffner et al., Reference Duffner, DeJong, Jansen, Backes, de Vugt and Köhler2023).
This study also explored a potential indirect socioemotional pathway by which social relationship factors may affect dementia risk, in addition to the postulated direct effects on brain structure and function. Specifically, mediation analyses showed that a significant proportion of the association between identified social relationship factors and dementia risk was explained by differences in depressive symptoms, ranging from 41% for contact with children to 79% for loneliness. While the role of social relationships in mood regulation, including the prevention and treatment of depression, has been recognized by the scientific community, this study is the first to structurally assess this in relation to long-term dementia risk (Campos-Paíno et al., Reference Campos-Paíno, Moreno-Peral, Gómez-Gómez, Conejo-Cerón, Galán, Reyes-Martín and Bellón2023; Lee, Lee, & Yu, Reference Lee, Lee and Yu2022). Social relationships serve as a source of instrumental, informational, and emotional support and may thus play a role in actively coping with psychological distress and depressive symptoms. Some studies proposed a potential ‘social buffering’ of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis, which is suggested to be especially responsive to psychological stressors (Hennessy, Kaiser, & Sachser, Reference Hennessy, Kaiser and Sachser2009). Long-term exposure to such stressors, in turn, has been associated with Alzheimer's disease pathology (Bisht, Sharma, & Tremblay, Reference Bisht, Sharma and Tremblay2018). However, different types of chronic stressors (e.g., poverty) have also been identified as risk factors for depression itself (Chung et al., Reference Chung, Chung, Gordon, Wong, Chan, Lau and Wong2018; McDonald, Thompson, Perzow, Joos, & Wadsworth, Reference McDonald, Thompson, Perzow, Joos and Wadsworth2020). In addition, depressed individuals also show a heightened vulnerability to psychological stressors in general, which may create a vicious cycle (Leonard, Reference Leonard2010). While some studies have suggested a potential mediating role of social relationship factors in the association between broader life stressors and depression, their potential role in breaking such a vicious cycle, also in light of dementia risk, should be further examined in future studies (McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Thompson, Perzow, Joos and Wadsworth2020).
Implications and further directions
This study adds to the theoretical foundation concerning the interconnectedness of social relationship constructs and their potential pathways to dementia. In addition to this, it may also have practical implications. Our findings support the idea that dementia risk reduction programs might benefit from addressing broader socioemotional factors, such as feelings of loneliness as well as social activity engagement and intergenerational contact, in addition to a brain-healthy lifestyle in general. The stimulation of social interaction has also become a central element in ongoing multi-component lifestyle intervention trials, such as worldwide equivalents of the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (WW-FINGER; Kivipelto et al., Reference Kivipelto, Solomon, Ahtiluoto, Ngandu, Lehtisalo, Antikainen and Soininen2013, Reference Kivipelto, Mangialasche, Snyder, Allegri, Andrieu, Arai and Carrillo2020). The specific contribution of social relationship factors in such trials, however, should be subject to further research, as should be the identification of subpopulations that could benefit most from a more socially active lifestyle. Furthermore, social relationships are likely subject to change over time. To better capture such changes, studies with multiple measurement points over an extended period of time are necessary. Taking such a life-course perspective of social relationships may help identify possible ‘windows of opportunity’ for prevention.
Strengths and limitations
This study has several strengths, including a large cohort that is representative of the general English population and a long follow-up. Social relationship factors were derived from comprehensive factor analysis rather than face validity or single items, and putative mediation was tested in structural equation modeling. Yet, it also has some limitations. Firstly, most data used in the current study is based on self-report, which can be subject to social desirability and other sources of information bias (Althubaiti, Reference Althubaiti2016). This may also concern information about dementia status and, even though in the current study we resorted to a combination of self- and informant-report, the number of people with dementia within ELSA may still be underestimated.
Furthermore, due to missing values on some covariates, the sample in the Cox analyses was restricted to those with complete information on included variables. However, comparisons between those with and without missing information did not suggest the selection of a generally healthier sample. Notably, missing information on the main predictors could be restricted to those with missing values on all of the social relationship items included in the factor analysis (n = 58), as otherwise missing factor scores were imputed in factor analysis by means of maximum likelihood.
Next, model selection following EFA and CFA was, in part, based on known fit indices. The appropriateness of such scaled indices in models using WLSMV estimators has been questioned. More specifically, it has been proposed that they may be too liberal and likely to not identify model-data misfit, especially when data is of a categorical nature (Xia & Yang, Reference Xia and Yang2019). Given the absence of viable alternatives, a factor analytical approach to studying social relationship constructs may still be superior to merely relying on face validity.
Lastly, the associations between factor scores and incident dementia might have been driven by variables not considered in the current analyses, such as mobility factors, occupational complexity, or parental socioeconomic status.
Conclusion
In the current study, the frequency and quality of social relationships were associated with lower dementia risk and may be suitable targets for dementia risk reduction initiatives in addition to social activity engagement. Mediation analyses suggested that these factors are partly associated with dementia risk via depressive symptoms. Additional analyses provided little evidence for reverse causation.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001272.
Data availability statement
ELSA data is freely accessible via the UK data service (https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/series/series?id=200011)
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the ELSA study participants.
Author contributions
Lukas A. Duffner: conceptualization and study design, data analysis, drafting the manuscript. Kay Deckers: conceptualization and study design, data analysis, critical revision of the manuscript. Dorina Cadar: critical revision of the manuscript. Marjolein de Vugt: conceptualization, critical revision of the manuscript. Sebastian Köhler: conceptualization and study design, data analysis, critical revision of the manuscript.
Funding statement
Lukas Duffner was supported by the Stichting Adriana van Rinsum-Ponsen and the Health Foundation Limburg.
Competing interests
The authors declare none.