Recently, Culbert et al Reference Culbert, Breedlove, Burt and Klump1 suggested that prenatal exposure to gonadal hormones may contribute to the substantial gender difference in the prevalence of eating disorders. The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR) included 582 twins (113 opposite-gender twins) among whom levels of disordered eating in same- and opposite-gender twin pairs were compared to examine for a ‘free martin effect’ (i.e. in utero exposure to testosterone masculinises behaviour in females). Reference Lillie2
They assessed the free martin effect indirectly by examining opposite-gender twin pairs where the female twin shares a prenatal environment with her male co-twin and, therefore, should be exposed to testosterone in utero. Consistent with this effect, Culbert et al found that levels of disordered eating have a significant linear trend, with same-gender female twins exhibiting the highest levels followed by females from opposite-gender pairs, males from opposite-gender pairs, and finally same-gender male twin pairs exhibiting the lowest levels. In this report we attempt to replicate and extend these findings using the same statistical method as Culbert et al.
Method
The present sample, the Swedish Twin study of CHild and Adolescent Development (TCHAD), began with all twin pairs born in Sweden between May 1985 and December 1986. Reference Lichtenstein, Tuvblad, Larsson and Carlstrom3 Twins were recruited through the medical birth registry and identified twins/parents of twins were posted study questionnaires. Reference Lichtenstein and Svartengren4 Eighty-two per cent of participants responded to the questionnaires when disordered eating was examined. Reference Lichtenstein, Tuvblad, Larsson and Carlstrom3
The sample includes 461 and 439 monozygotic, 344 and 312 dizygotic individual females and males respectively from same-gender pairs, and 361 males and 371 females from opposite-gender pairs who are 15–17 years old. Zygosity was determined based on computer algorithms of questionnaire responses. Reference Lichtenstein, Tuvblad, Larsson and Carlstrom3 Questions were validated by a discriminant analysis of 106 same-gender pairs where zygosity had been determined by typing 16 polymorphic DNA markers. Reference Lichtenstein, Tuvblad, Larsson and Carlstrom3
The majority of the participants' parents were born in Sweden (85.8%). The highest level of education obtained by either parent was a university degree (47%). Overall, 15% of parents reported employment as ‘unskilled labourer’, 27.6% as ‘skilled labourer’, 28.6% reported a medium-level white-collar career, and 29% reported a professional/high-level white-collar career. Rates for professional careers are similar to those reported by Culbert et al (33%). Reference Culbert, Breedlove, Burt and Klump1 However, our sample consists of fewer medium-level careers (28% v. 43%) and more unskilled labourers (15% v. 3%).
Disordered eating was examined using three subscales from the Eating Disorder Inventory–2 Reference Garner5 (EDI–2): drive for thinness (i.e. excessive concern with dieting, weight preoccupation and extreme pursuit of thinness); bulimia (i.e. tendency towards episodes of binge eating followed with an impulse to purge); and body dissatisfaction (i.e. belief that specific body parts are too large). A total score was created using all available questions. Questionnaires were approved by the ethics committee of the Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.
Hierarchical linear models were used to examine mean differences in disordered eating across same- and opposite-gender twin pairs; such models can account for the non-independence of twin data. Two predictors were used: twin's gender (male v. female) and co-twin's gender (male v. female). These two variables were utilised and coded following Culbert et al: males=1 and females=–1. P<0.05 was considered significant.
If more than 10% of the questions for any specific subscale were missing, the score was coded as missing. Subscales were log transformed prior to analyses to correct for a positive skew.
Results
Hierarchical linear model results indicate a significant main effect for twin gender on the three subscales, with females reporting significantly higher levels for drive of thinness (t(2050)=–14.25, P<0.001), body dissatisfaction (t(2185)=<16.37, P<0.001) and total score (t(1994)=–15.80, P<0.001). However, a main effect was not found for bulimia (t(2059)=–1.00, P=0.43). This indicates no gender difference for aspects related to the bulimia subscale in our sample. Contrary to Culbert et al's findings, no significant main effects were found for co-twin gender with any subscale. Results remained consistent utilising only dizygotic twin pairs. Because no difference was exhibited between opposite- and same-gender twins, this suggests that intrauterine testosterone exposure does not have an impact on the risk for disordered eating. Mean scores for EDI–2 subscales are shown in Fig. 1 as a function of gender and zygosity.
Discussion
The aim of this report was to replicate the findings of Culbert et al. Reference Culbert, Breedlove, Burt and Klump1 Using a different self-report measure and similar analytical methods, our results did not corroborate. There are four possible reasons for this. First, the current study could have been under-powered. However, a power analysis conducted with same-gender females and opposite-gender females as comparison groups, with an effect size provided by Culbert (K. Culbert, personal communication, 1 May 2008), revealed a power of 0.99. Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner6 Thus, our negative results could not plausibly arise from low power.
Second, the two samples differed. Our participants were adolescent Swedish twins while Culbert et al's was an ethnically diverse sample of young adults (mean 20 years) living in the midwestern USA. Our sample may also be more representative of its respective general population. The TCHAD sample was obtained by contacting twins through a medical birth registry, whereas most MSUTR twins are recruited through advertisement and live within a 2 h radius of MSUTR headquarters. Reference Culbert, Breedlove, Burt and Klump1 However, both samples are volunteer-based. The two populations could also have a differential prevalence of eating disorders. However, studies indicate that the prevalence of eating disorders in Sweden and other Scandinavian countries is similar, if slightly less prevalent than in the USA. Reference Ghaderi and Scott7,Reference Rastam, Gillberg and Garton8 Similarly, prenatal hormone exposure is a biological effect that occurs in utero and one might expect its effects to remain constant across age levels and populations.
Third, different measures of disordered eating were used. Culbert et al used a total score derived from the Minnesota Eating Behaviors Survey (MEBS). Reference von Ranson, Klump, Iacono and McGue9 There is one main difference between this survey and our EDI–2 subscales. The MEBS divides binge eating and compensatory behaviours into two separate subscales allowing for more information to be obtained about each variable, whereas the EDI–2 combines these into the bulimia subscale. For example, the MEBS includes questions about several different types of purging behaviours and the EDI–2 only enquires about self-induced vomiting.
Finally, the EDI–2 may not be an adequate measure of disordered eating for males Reference Anderson and Bulik10 or for a Swedish population. For example, the bulimia subscale may represent more normative aspects of behaviours in males. Reference Eiben11 Many of the questions on this subscale deal with binge-eating behaviours and 15- to 17-year-old boys may commonly consume large amounts of food. Reference Eiben11 Sources of a drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction are also likely to vary between genders and the EDI–2 focuses on core areas of the female body with which women are more typically dissatisfied (e.g. stomach and thighs). The EDI–2 was also normalised and created with a clinical sample of females with eating disorders from the USA, so its constructs may not extrapolate to a Swedish population. However, studies indicate that the EDI–2 may be an acceptable measure of disordered eating in both a male Reference Spillane, Clinton and Norring12,Reference Nevonen, Clintion and Norring13 and a Swedish population. Reference Spillane, Clinton and Norring12,14 For example, in a study utilising the identical adolescent sample used in our study, Cronbach's alpha coefficients were estimated at 0.81, 0.70 and 0.88 for the drive for thinness, bulimia and body dissatisfaction subscales respectively for males, indicating high internal reliability. Reference Eiben11
Taken together, the results of our study are inconsistent with previous research. Because of the similarities between our report and the previous report, Reference Culbert, Breedlove, Burt and Klump1 the evidence for the hypothesis that prenatal hormone exposure has an impact on the development of eating disorders and disordered eating is lacking.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Charles O. Gardner for helpful references and suggestions on hierarchical linear modelling methodology.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.