Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T12:14:25.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Directed forgetting in depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2008

O. COTTENCIN*
Affiliation:
Department of Consultation Liaison Psychiatry, University of Medicine Paris 5, AP-HP, European Georges Pompidou Hospital, France Laboratoire Neurosciences Fonctionnelles & Pathologies, CNRS UMR 8160 Lille, France
G. GRUAT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University Lille 2, University Hospital of Lille, France.
P. THOMAS
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University Lille 2, University Hospital of Lille, France.
P. DEVOS
Affiliation:
Department of Statistics, University of Lille 2, University Hospital of Lille, France
M. GOUDEMAND
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University Lille 2, University Hospital of Lille, France.
S.M CONSOLI
Affiliation:
Department of Consultation Liaison Psychiatry, University of Medicine Paris 5, AP-HP, European Georges Pompidou Hospital, France
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Olivier Cottencin, University of Medicine, Paris 5, AP-HP, European Georges Pompidou Hospital, Department of Consultation-Liaison Psychiatry, 20 rue Leblanc, 75908 Paris Cedex 15, France. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Subjects with depression exhibit deficits in prefrontal function. We posited that as a result, in a supraspan memory test, they would be impaired in their ability to inhibit recall of irrelevant words, and because of consequent overload of working and episodic memory capacity, would be impaired in their ability to recall relevant words. We tested this hypothesis in 30 inpatients and outpatients with a diagnosis of major depressive disorder and 30 controls subjects using a form of the Directed Forgetting Paradigm using exclusively neutral words. The depressed subjects did exhibit deficits in prefrontal function. All subjects were given four lists of 24 items each, in which half the words were followed by the instruction “remember” and half by the instruction “forget.” Our hypothesis found support in a significant group by item type interaction effect exhibited when subjects were instructed to recall only those items followed by the “remember” instruction: depressed subjects recalled relatively more words to be forgotten and relatively fewer words to be remembered. A control experiment suggested that these results could not be accounted for by a differential effect of depression on memory encoding. (JINS, 2008, 14, 895–899.)

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

Depression is associated with impairment on tests of memory encoding (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Zembar and Niederehe1995), and impairment in autobiographical memory (Lemogne et al., Reference Lemogne, Piolino, Friszer, Claret, Girault, Jouvent, Allilaire and Fossati2006a). Autobiographical memory includes autobiographical events (episodic memory) and autobiographical facts (semantic memory) (Lemogne et al., Reference Lemogne, Piolino, Jouvent, Allilaire and Fossati2006b). Depressed patients often exhibit overgenerality of recall: an excessive tendency to recall repeated events or classes of events over single, specific events (Lemogne et al., Reference Lemogne, Piolino, Friszer, Claret, Girault, Jouvent, Allilaire and Fossati2006a; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Barnhofer, Crane, Herman, Raes, Watkins and Dalgleish2007). Recalled memories are less specific, irrespective of the emotional valence of the memory. This suggests a fundamental cognitive disorder rather than impairment linked specifically to the mood disturbance (Van Vreeswijk & De Wilde, Reference Van Vreeswijk and De Wilde2004).

Memory retrieval may occur automatically or intentionally. Executive processes are hypothesized to play an important role in intentional retrieval of specific autobiographical memories: to remember new information, it is necessary to distinguish it from older information, but also to stop old information from interfering with new information (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, Reference Conway and Pleydell-Pearce2000). Executive processes are also necessary to maintain a final search result in working memory (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Barnhofer, Crane, Herman, Raes, Watkins and Dalgleish2007). In a meta-analysis, Miyake et al. (Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000) identified three core executive processes relevant to intentional retrieval: shifting between mental sets, updating and monitoring of working memory representations, and inhibition of dominant, automatic, or prepotent responses.

Because executive resources seem to play an important role in generative retrieval of specific autobiographical memories, and subjects with depression exhibit deficits in executive function (Uekermann et al., Reference Uekermann, Channon, Lehmkamper, Abdel-Hamid, Vollmoeller and Daum2008), we hypothesized that the difficulties in retrieving specific memories exhibited by subjects with depression (the overgenerality phenomenon) may be related to a deficit in executive inhibitory mechanisms. Bjork (Reference Bjork, Tulving, Roediger and Craiks1989) studied these mechanisms in normal subjects using the Directed Forgetting Paradigm. In this test, a series of stimuli is presented, some of which are “to be remembered,” and others “to be forgotten.” Subjects are then asked to recall as many of the items “to be remembered” as possible, then to remember all the items, both those “to be remembered” and those “to be forgotten.” To the degree that one is successful at this task, there are few intrusions of to be forgotten items when participants are asked to report only to be remembered items and performance on to be forgotten items is relatively poor when, on a later retention test, participants are asked to report both to be remembered and to be forgotten items (Zacks et al., Reference Zacks, Radvansky and Hasher1996). In this study, we used the Directed Forgetting Paradigm to test our hypothesis regarding the mechanism underlying the overgenerality of memory phenomenon in depression. Our hypothesis was that the difficulty in retrieval of specific episodic memories exhibited by subjects with major depressive disorder may be caused by difficulty in inhibiting irrelevant information, thereby overloading working memory. We posited that depressed subjects should recall more words to be forgotten than normal controls, because of defective executive inhibitory mechanisms, and depressed subjects should recall less words to be remembered because of the resultant overloading of working and episodic memory capacity. We also used a delayed test of recall of all items to rule out the possibility that the results of our experiment might reflect differential effects of depression on memory encoding.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The study was performed on consecutive inpatients and outpatients examined at the Psychiatric department of the University Hospital of LILLE (France). It was approved by the Ethics Committee. All subjects gave their informed consent to participate in the study. To be included, patients had to have a diagnosis of major depressive episode, according to the DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), that had been present for at least 2 weeks. They were screened for the study using a semi-structured interview, MINI DSM IV; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998). We excluded patients with a score lower than 25 on the Montgomery and Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS, Montgomery & Asberg, Reference Montgomery and Asberg1979). We also excluded patients with cerebral lesions, those with known memory disorders before the current episode of depression, those with other psychiatric or addictive disorders (assessed with MINI DSM IV; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998), and those with paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal, antisocial, or borderline personality disorders (clinical assessment according to the DSM-IV Axis II criteria; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The patients were allowed to continue the psychotropic treatment they were receiving at the beginning of the study but no psychotropic drug treatment changes were allowed during the study. We included 30 patients and compared them with 30 healthy controls.

Neuropsychological evaluation included the Verbal Fluency Test, the Stroop Task (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935), an abridged version of Wechsler's Revised Intelligence Scale for Adults (Britton & Savage, Reference Britton and Savage1966; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1989), including four subtests (Crawford et al., Reference Crawford, Allan and Jack1992), and the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1991).

We designed a Directed Forgetting Task that was an adaptation of the Directed Forgetting Paradigm of Zacks et al. (Reference Zacks, Radvansky and Hasher1996). Four lists of 24 words each were successively shown to the subjects. The 24 words in each list belonged to 6 different semantic categories. Each word was shown on a card for 5 seconds. On the next card, the instruction: “REMEMBER” or “FORGET,” was shown for 2 seconds. Each list included 12 words to be remembered and 12 words to be forgotten. The instruction was given after the word had been shown so that the subjects did not know in advance which words were to be remembered and which were to be forgotten. The instruction “REMEMBER” was randomly assigned to two of the four words in each semantic category. The words in each list were chosen from commonly-known French literature, controlling for frequency (between 100 and 10,000/million) using the BRULEX database (Content et al., Reference Content, Rousty and Radeau1990). Different semantic categories included words of comparable frequency. Only emotionally neutral categories and words were used. Homophonic words were excluded. There was no significant difference in the frequency of the words in the four lists (p = .95).

The order in which the lists were shown, the categories within the lists, and the words within categories were randomized. This order was used with all participants. After being shown each of the four lists, the subjects had to remember as many as possible of the words to be remembered, in a maximum time of 2 minutes (the immediate conditional recall phase). Over the next 10 minutes, the subjects were asked for biographical information [last name, first name, date of birth, address, phone number, medical situation, family situation, job situation, years of education, and treatment (psychotherapy, psychotropic drugs, and others)]. They were then asked to remember all of the words from the 4 lists, both those to be remembered and those to be forgotten, in a maximum time of 5 minutes (the final unconditional recall phase). Subject scores for each phase consisted of number of words to be remembered that were recalled, number of word to be forgotten that were recalled, and the number of repetitions and intrusions (words not in the lists, but recalled by the participants).

Neuropsychological testing was performed by the same two trained psychologists (G.deF., J.S.). All other tests were performed by the same two trained psychiatrists (O.C., G.G.).

Statistical Analysis

Analysis was performed using SAS (V8.0, SAS Institute) software. Comparison of the mean values for the Directed Forgetting Task was made using a two-way analysis of covariance controlling for age and verbal IQ. Comparison of neuropsychological test results (verbal fluency, Stroop task, WAIS-R, and WMS) for the depressed subjects and the controls was performed using t tests.

RESULTS

We examined 95 patients presenting with depression; 53 were not eligible for inclusion because of a history of alcohol abuse, cannabis abuse, personality disorder, bipolar disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, or MADRS < 25. Of the 42 who were eligible for inclusion, 30 were included in the study, and 12 refused to participate. Demographic data are presented in Table 1. There was no difference between the depressed and control groups in gender distribution or education. The depressed subjects were significantly older (t = 2.063; p = .044).

Table 1. Description of the population and results of the complementary tests

Results of the Directed Forgetting Task

Immediate conditional recall (Figure 1)

Analysis of covariance controlling for the effects of age and verbal IQ revealed a group effect (p = .0255), meaning that the depressed subjects recalled significantly fewer words overall than the controls [controls = 9.44 (SD 1.67); depressed subjects = 8.46 (SD 1.84)]; a directed forgetting effect (p = .0001), meaning that individually, the two groups recalled significantly more words “to be remembered” than words “to be forgotten”; and an interaction effect (p = .0024). Figure 1 reveals that this interaction reflects the combined effect of worse recall by depressed subjects of to be remembered items and greater intrusion in depressed subjects of words to be forgotten.

Fig. 1. Items recalled by depressed subjects and controls during the immediate conditional recall phase.

Final unconditional recall (Figure 2)

Analysis of covariance controlling for the effects of age and verbal IQ revealed a group effect (p = .0290), meaning that the depressed subjects recalled significantly fewer words overall than the controls [controls = 42.00 (SD 11.22); depression = 31.13 (SD 10.50)]; a directed forgetting effect (p < .0001), meaning that individually, the two groups recalled significantly more words “to be remembered,” than words “to be forgotten”; but no interaction effect (p = .1455). Figure 2 reveals that this absence of a significant interaction reflects the finding that the likelihood of intrusion of words to be forgotten was not less for control subjects than for depressed subjects.

Fig. 2. Items recalled by depressed subjects and controls during the final unconditional recall phase.

We did not find any list or verbal IQ effect, any significant effect from intrusions or word repetition, or any significant difference between patients taking or not taking benzodiazepines. However, we did find that age had an influence on immediate (p = .01) and delayed recall (p = .002) for both items to be remembered and items to be forgotten in depressed subjects.

Results of the complementary tests (Table 1)

The intelligence quotients of depressed subjects did not differ significantly from those of controls (Table 1). However, the depressed group did score significantly lower on the Wechsler Memory Scale, the Verbal Fluency Test, and the Stroop. These results are consistent with published data.

DISCUSSION

We posited that subjects with depression have deficits in prefrontal function and consequently, in the Directed Forgetting Paradigm, may not be able to inhibit words “to be forgotten,” may recall proportionally more words “to be forgotten” (the overgenerality phenomenon), and because of the resultant burden placed on working and episodic memory, may recall less words to be remembered. We found, as have others, that our depressed subjects did exhibit deficits in prefrontal function, and our results for the immediate conditional recall phase of our Directed Forgetting Test support our hypothesis that depressed subjects would recall proportionately less words to be remembered and more words to be forgotten (as reflected in the significant group by item type interaction effect). One could argue that the deficits exhibited by our depressed subjects reflected a problem with encoding. However, the equal effect on items to be remembered and items to be forgotten in depressed and control populations that was evident during the final unconditional recall phase suggests that the two classes of items were encoded similarly in the two groups. Therefore, it seems likely that the results during the immediate conditional recall phase did indeed reflect what happens at time of retrieval.

It is possible that the depressed subjects, rather than exhibiting impaired ability to inhibit recall of to be forgotten items, were unduly susceptible at time of retrieval to the salience in a memory task of the somewhat peculiar instruction to “FORGET” following certain words. The relative impact of deficits in inhibiting recall of irrelevant information/excessive susceptibility to salience of certain stimuli on early episodic memory and memory in various degrees of consolidation (e.g., after a night's sleep) remains to be determined.

Although the depressed subjects were older than the controls, and age did negatively impact recall, our findings on the Directed Forgetting Test cannot be accounted for by age effects because they were significant when age was controlled.

The results from published studies of the Directed Forgetting Task (DFT) in depressed subjects have until now been contradictory. Dumont (Reference Dumont2000) found a similar directed forgetting effect in depressed patients and in healthy controls, but Power et al. found that patients with more severe depression recalled more negative words “to be forgotten” (Power et al., Reference Power, Dalgleish, Claudio, Tata and Kentish2000). Thus, the results of Power et al. were similar to ours, but the effect they observed could have been due to the emotional valence of the words. We have replicated their results using neutral words.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank G. de Falco and J. Soller for their help in testing the patients. Authors declare no sources of financial support for this research.

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-IV (4thed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptative mechanism in human memory. In Tulving, E., Roediger, H.L., & Craiks, F.I.M. (Eds.), Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 309330). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Britton, P.G. & Savage, R.D. (1966). A short form of the WAIS for use with the aged. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 417418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, D.B., Zembar, M.J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Content, A., Rousty, P., & Radeau, M. (1990). Brulex : Une base de données lexicales informatisée pour le français écrit et parlé. L'année Psychologique, 90, 551566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M.A. & Pleydell-Pearce, C.W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J.R., Allan, K.M., & Jack, A.M. (1992). Short-forms of the UK WAIS-R: Regression equations and their predictive validity in a general population sample. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31(Pt 2), 191202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumont, M. (2000). Directed forgetting and memory bias for emotion-congruent information in clinical depression. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 19, 171188.Google Scholar
Lemogne, C., Piolino, P., Friszer, S., Claret, A., Girault, N., Jouvent, R., Allilaire, J.F., & Fossati, P. (2006a). Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: Specificity, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 258268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemogne, C., Piolino, P., Jouvent, R., Allilaire, J.F., & Fossati, P. (2006b). Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: A review. Encephale, 32(Pt 1), 781788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, S.A. & Asberg, M. (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 382389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, M.J., Dalgleish, T., Claudio, V., Tata, P., & Kentish, J. (2000). The directed forgetting task: Application to emotionally valent material. Journal of Affective Disorders, 57, 147157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G.C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(20yes), 2233; quiz 34–57.Google ScholarPubMed
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interferences in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uekermann, J., Channon, S., Lehmkamper, C., Abdel-Hamid, M., Vollmoeller, W., & Daum, I. (2008). Executive function, mentalizing and humor in major depression. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 5562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Vreeswijk, M.F. & De Wilde, E.J. (2004). Autobiographical memory specificity, psychopathology, depressed mood and the use of the Autobiographical Memory Test: A meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 731743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. Paris: Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Paris: Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.Google Scholar
Williams, J.M., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Herman, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 122148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zacks, R.T., Radvansky, G., & Hasher, L. (1996). Studies of directed forgetting in older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 143156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Description of the population and results of the complementary tests

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Items recalled by depressed subjects and controls during the immediate conditional recall phase.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Items recalled by depressed subjects and controls during the final unconditional recall phase.