Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T17:26:53.890Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Beijing Twin Study (BeTwiSt): A Longitudinal Study of Child and Adolescent Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2012

Jie Chen
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Xinying Li*
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Jianxin Zhang
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Misaki N. Natsuaki
Affiliation:
University of California, CA, USA
Leslie D. Leve
Affiliation:
Oregon Social Learning Center, Eugene, OR, USA
Gordon T. Harold
Affiliation:
University of Leicester, Leicester, United Kingdom
Zhiyan Chen
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Xiaodong Yang
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Fei Guo
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Jie Zhang
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, China
Xiaojia Ge
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota, MN, USA
*
address for correspondence: Xinying Li, 4A Datun Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing 100101, China. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Rates of emotional and behavioral problems among children and adolescents in China are increasing and represent a major public health concern. To investigate the etiology of such problems, including the effects and interplay of genes and environment, the Beijing Twin Study (BeTwiSt) was established. A representative sample of adolescent twins in Beijing (N = 1,387 pairs of adolescent twins, mostly between the ages of 10 and 18 years) was recruited and assessed longitudinally. Data collection included the following: emotional and behavioral problems (e.g., depressive symptoms, anxiety, delinquency, drinking, and smoking); family, peer, and school environments; stress; social and academic competence; cognitive traits (e.g., emotion suppression, rumination, and effortful control); and saliva samples for DNA genotyping and sequencing. The combination of quantitative and molecular genetic approaches and the timeliness of the project, with the sample residing in a region with a rapidly changing economic and cultural climate, are particular strengths of this study. Findings from this study are expected to help understanding of the etiological mechanisms underlying child and adolescent normal and abnormal development in regions undergoing substantial social, cultural, and economic changes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Child and adolescent psychopathology in China has received increasing attention from academic, social, clinical, and policy communities. The prevalence data suggest that more than 30 million Chinese children and adolescents under age 17 are affected by emotional and behavioral problems (Fourth International Conference on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 2009), which is about three times greater than the same population in the United States (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui, Benjet, Georgiades and Swendsen2010). Child and adolescent psychopathology has both short- and long-term deleterious impacts (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, Nakamura and Kessler2009), and youth mental health has become a topic of major public health concern in China. An improved understanding of the etiological mechanisms underlying child and adolescent psychopathology — especially how these mechanisms function in the context of China's rapid economic growth and social changes — may contribute to the development of empirically based prevention and intervention programs in China. Given that psychopathology is likely to be a developmental product of the complex interplay of genes and environment, a longitudinal study enriched by behavioral, genetic, and environmental measurement can help further the understanding of the etiology of child and adolescent mental health problems.

As an ‘experiment of nature’, twin studies have been a valuable tool for investigating the genetic and environmental origins of human behavior (Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, Busjahn and Peltonen2002; Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Simmonff, Silberg, Bouchard and Propping1993). Over the past few decades, a number of well-designed twin studies have been conducted on child and adolescent psychopathology (see Table 1), mainly in the United States and Europe (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, Beijsterveldt, Derks, Stroet, Polderman, Hudziak and Boomsma2007; Eaves et al., Reference Eaves, Silberg, Meyer, Maes, Simonoff, Pickles, Rutter, Neale, Reynolds and Erikson1997; Hewitt et al., Reference Hewitt, Silberg, Rutter, Simonoff, Meyer, Maes, Pickles, Neal, Loeber and Erickson1997; Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, McGue and Krueger2006; Kaprio, Reference Kaprio2006; Lemery-Chalfant et al., Reference Lemery-Chalfant, Goldsmith, Schmidt, Arneson and Van Hulle2006; Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Tuvblad, Larsson and Carlstr2007; Neiderhiser et al., Reference Neiderhiser, Reiss and Hetherington2007; Trouton et al., Reference Trouton, Spinath and Plomin2002; van den Bree et al., Reference van den Bree, Rice, Fowler, Shelton, Lifford, Scourfield, Harold and Thapar2007), with a few studies in Asia and Australia (Ando et al., Reference Ando, Nonaka, Ozaki, Sato, Fujisawa, Suzuki, Yamagata, Takahashi, Nakajima and Kato2006; Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Hay, Piek, Pearsall-Jones, Levy and Martin2006; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Collier and Li2009; Hur et al., Reference Hur, Shin, Jeong and Han2006; Kuo et al., Reference Kuo, Pi-Hua, Chuang and Chen2006). These studies have contributed significantly to the field by disentangling the intertwined roles of genetic and environmental factors in complex human traits, as well as by providing new information about the complex interplay between genes and the environment.

TABLE 1 Twin Studies on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology Across the World

The twin studies are listed alphabetically.

In contrast to the proliferation of twin studies in Western countries, the twin study approach to understanding the etiology of psychopathology has only recently been adopted in China. Findings from Western twin studies may not be readily translated to the Chinese context for several reasons. Most importantly, the context surrounding twins and their families in China is culturally unique. As a result of China's strict one-child policy, the majority of Chinese families (especially those in urban areas) have only one child, with the exception of families with twin children (Falbo & Poston, Reference Falbo and Poston1993). Therefore, knowledge of family environments, particularly sibling interactions, cannot be readily introduced from Western countries to China; the knowledge must originate directly from a Chinese context. Second, the prevalence of certain alleles has been shown to differ between China and Western countries. For example, population frequency data has shown that the ALDH2*487Lys allele, which is related to protection against alcohol dependence, is extremely rare in Europeans but is widely prevalent among East Asians (Goedde et al., Reference Goedde, Agarwal, Fritze, Meier-Tackmann, Singh, Beckmann, Bhatia, Chen, Fang and Lisker1992). The BDNF Met66 allele, which is related to depression, has also shown higher prevalence in East Asians than in Europeans (Petryshen et al., Reference Petryshen, Sabeti, Aldinger, Fry, Fan, Schaffner, Waggoner, Tahl and Sklar2009). Finally, and perhaps most obviously, developmental contexts, such as cultural norms and values, differ between China and Western countries (Chen & French, Reference Chen and French2008), which may have differing effects on the development of psychopathology. Hence, studies based on a genetically informative design, such as twin studies, are needed to clarify how genes, environment, and their interplay influence the development of psychopathology in children and youth in Chinese population.

Despite their importance, twin studies on psychopathology and psychological development of Chinese youth, especially studies with a longitudinal component that incorporate a molecular genetic approach, are lacking. To fill this gap, a longitudinal twin project, the Beijing Twin Study (BeTwiSt), was launched in 2005. The overarching goal of the BeTwiSt is to prospectively investigate genetic and environmental influences on developmental psychopathology among Chinese youth between the ages of 10 and 18 years old. To our knowledge, the Twin Registry in Southwestern China (TRiSC; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Collier and Li2009), which mainly focuses on child cognitive ability and behavioral development, and the BeTwiSt are the only projects that investigate child and adolescent psychopathology in Mainland China. Each study focuses on different aspects of development, with the BeTwiSt specializing in emotional and behavioral problems and their correlates.

The conceptual model for the BeTwiSt is based on the following hypotheses: (a) most types of psychopathology are influenced by genes (G), environment (E), and their interplay (G × E interaction and GE correlation); (b) change and continuity in psychopathology are influenced by both genes and environment; and (c) regulatory processes (e.g., emotional suppression and effortful control) may mediate or moderate the influence of genes, environment, and their interplay on psychopathology. We focus on two major developmental pathways: internalizing and externalizing problems. Other related problems, such as substance use, Internet use, and sleeping disorders, are also of interest.

Research Aims

The BeTwiSt is guided by three primary aims. The first aim is to understand how genes, environment, and their interplay affect adolescent psychopathology (Aim 1). There is a great consensus that most psychopathologies result from genetic and environmental influences and their interplay. However, the specific genetic and environmental factors that play these key roles in the development of psychopathology and how they do so continue to be poorly understood. The BeTwiSt examines the effect of specific genetic and environmental factors. Moreover, this study examines the genetic homogeneity between different types of psychopathology to suggest possible avenues for molecular genetic research, given the evidence of multi-finality (i.e., different outcomes from a single etiology) in the development of psychopathology.

The second aim is to investigate the effects of genes, environment, and their interplay, as identified in Aim 1, in a longitudinal framework (Aim 2). A growing body of evidence suggests that the heritability of psychopathology, including anxiety, depression, and externalizing behaviors, increases during the transitional period from adolescence to adulthood (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, Beijsterveldt, Derks, Stroet, Polderman, Hudziak and Boomsma2007; Rice et al., Reference Rice, Harold and Thapar2003; Van Beijsterveldt et al., Reference Van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Hudziak and Boomsma2003). Furthermore, the development of psychopathology during the transition from childhood to adolescence seems to be affected by both age-stable and age-specific genetic effects (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner and Lichtenstein2008; Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Tuvblad, Larsson and Carlstr2007). However, the mechanisms that are responsible for this changing heritability during developmental transitions remain unclear. The longitudinal design of the study will permit an examination of developmental changes and stability of genetic and environmental influences on psychopathology from adolescence to adulthood in China.

The third aim is to examine the role of regulatory processes in mediating the effects of genes, environment, and their interplay on psychopathology (Aim 3). Before any palpable symptoms are manifested, compromised development of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral regulatory processes may occur, presaging the subsequent psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009). In fact, empirical studies have shown that several maladaptive processes, such as emotional suppression, rumination, dysfunctional attitude, and low effortful control, are associated with adolescent internalizing and externalizing psychopathologies (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Valiente, Spinrad, Cumberland, Liew, Reiser, Zhou and Losoya2009; Hilt et al., Reference Hilt, McLaughlin and Nolen-Hoeksema2010). Examining the genetic and environmental origins of these hypothesized regulatory processes will advance our understanding of the developmental pathways of psychopathology and may contribute to the development of prevention and intervention strategies to reduce rates of psychopathology in China.

Design and Methods of the BeTwiSt

The BeTwiSt is founded on four methodologically unique features. First, this study is based on a large, representative twin sample from Beijing, China. To ensure representativeness, over 600 public schools were randomly selected from all 18 districts and counties in Beijing municipality. All twins aged 10–18 years old in these schools were invited to participate in the study. The representativeness of the sample was ensured via the comparison of this sample to a population-based school student sample in Beijing (see below). Second, the BeTwiSt utilizes a longitudinal design. Currently, the twins have been assessed twice (the T0 [pilot] and T1 assessments) within a 1.5-year interval. We are in the process of collecting the T2 data that focuses on young adulthood, which will allow for an investigation of developmental stability and change in genetic and environmental effects on emotion and behaviors across adolescence and adulthood. Third, the BeTwiSt includes both quantitative and molecular genetic approaches. Quantitative genetic analyses are useful in defining heritable phenotypes and selecting appropriate sub-samples for molecular genetic studies (Wood & Neale, Reference Wood and Neale2010). Molecular genetic analyses allow for an investigation of interactions between specific genes and measured environment (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2006; Dodge, Reference Dodge2009; Moffitt, Reference Moffitt2005). The combination of the two approaches will provide more information than the use of a single approach. Fourth, multiple informants, including parents, twins, and teachers, participated in the study. The use of multiple reporters partially resolves rater bias in assessment and captures a more comprehensive view of children's emotion and behaviors in different contexts (e.g., home, school, and society; Achenbach, Reference Achenbach2006; De Los Reyes, Reference De Los Reyes2011).

Recruitment

A pilot survey (the T0 assessment) was conducted during the 2006–2007 school year, followed by a comprehensive survey (the T1 assessment) during the 2008–2009 school year. The purpose of the T0 assessment was to establish a feasible and efficient recruitment procedure and to obtain basic demographic information about adolescent twins in Beijing. In the T0 data collection, 620 elementary and secondary schools were randomly selected from all 18 districts and counties in Beijing municipality, and 1,675 pairs of twins enrolled in these schools were identified and recruited as the T0 sample. All 1,675 pairs of twins at T0 completed a brief self-report questionnaire.

In the T1 assessment, we contacted the families from the T0 sample by mail or telephone and invited them to participate in the longitudinal part of the project. Of the 1,675 pairs from the T0 sample, 806 pairs and their parents agreed to participate, representing a 48.1% participation rate. The remaining 869 pairs in the T0 sample refused to participate in the study, either because they were reluctant to provide saliva samples or they decided not to commit to a longitudinal study. To supplement the attrition rate, 581 additional pairs of twins were recruited through a procedure similar to the T0 recruitment methods. Therefore, the full T1 sample (the core BeTwiSt sample) consists of 1,387 families, each of which has one participating twin pair. Both twins and at least one parent participated in the T1 assessment. The BeTwiSt is now in the process of following the core sample in a new wave of assessment (T2) during the transition to young adulthood.

Assessment Procedures

For the T0 assessment, we obtained informed consents from the twins and the school administrators. Then twins completed a brief questionnaire after school. For the T1 assessment, after obtaining written informed consents from the twins and their parents, arrangements were made for the twins to stay in their classrooms after school. After describing the purpose and procedures of the study, trained research staff distributed the questionnaires to the twin participants and instructed them to complete the questionnaires independently. Research staff members were present to answer any questions that the students might have about the questionnaires. After the twins completed the questionnaires, they were asked to provide saliva samples using the Oragene DNA self-collection kit (DNA Genotek Inc., Ontario, Canada). Questionnaires for parents were taken home by the twins and were mailed back to our laboratory. The T2 assessment, which is currently ongoing, takes place in the twins’ homes. Two research staff members visited each family in their home to administer the assessment. The confidentiality and anonymity of the participants’ genetic information and their responses to the questionnaires are ensured. The Institutional Review Board has approved all research protocols for BeTwiSt.

Overview of Measures

Despite the ongoing debate over the dimensional or categorical view of psychopathology, scholars have accepted the notion that normal and abnormal trait variation can be treated within a single, unified structural framework and that abnormal traits can be modeled as extremes of normal trait variation (Hudziak et al., Reference Hudziak, Achenbach, Althoff and Pine2007; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Watson and Barlow2005). In fact, a growing body of evidence suggests continuous liability between normal and abnormal traits, which may be subject to the influence of similar genes and environmental factors (Rutter, Reference Rutter2011). Hence, the BeTwiSt adopts a dimensional view of psychopathology when assessing children's emotion and behaviors. Table 2 summarizes the variables of interest.

TABLE 2 The Variables of Interest in this Study

When applicable, multiple informants provided information. The twins, parents, and teachers rated the twins’ emotional and behavioral outcomes, which included both internalizing (depression and anxiety) and externalizing problems (aggression, delinquency, and attention problems). The participation rates of parents and teachers were 99% and 64%, respectively. Children also reported their smoking and drinking behaviors, sleep problems, and Internet use. Environmental factors, such as stressful life events, parenting practices, deviant peer affiliation, and sibling interactions, were reported by both children and parents. Children reported cognitive and emotional factors (e.g., emotional regulation strategies, rumination, dysfunctional attitudes, and effortful control) that putatively link genes and environment to psychopathology. Fathers and mothers also reported their own emotional and behavioral problems, such as depression, anxiety, hostility, smoking, and drinking behavior. Finally, information about the children's height, weight, and pubertal status were collected to reflect their physical development. Genomic DNA was extracted from the participants’ saliva samples, and genetic polymorphisms, located in 5-HTT, 5-HT1AR, BDNF, COMT, and MAOA genes, were assayed.

Zygosity Determination

Among 1,387 pairs of twins (T1 sample), 262 pairs are opposite-sex dizygotic (DZ) twins. The zygosity of the remaining 1,125 pairs of same-sex twins was determined by DNA analyses and the questionnaire method (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Li, Chen, Yang, Zhang, Duan and Ge2010). Among the 1,125 pairs of same-sex twins, 990 pairs were determined by DNA analyses, and 116 pairs, whose saliva samples yielded insufficient DNA, were determined by the questionnaire method. Zygosity of 19 pairs was unable to be identified because of missing responses to the zygosity questionnaire and insufficient DNA. For the DNA analyses, nine short tandem repeat loci, which are highly heterogeneous in the Chinese population, were used. Same-sex twins with at least one different genetic marker were classified as DZ twins; otherwise, the twins were classified as monozygotic (MZ) twins. The posterior probability of being MZ for same-sex twins with the same genotype in all nine loci was estimated to be 99.99%. The validity of the questionnaire method was examined through the comparison with the results of DNA analyses. The predictive accuracy of the questionnaire method used in this study reached 91% (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Li, Chen, Yang, Zhang, Duan and Ge2010).

Sample Description

Sample Size by Age, Gender, and Zygosity

Table 3 shows the sample size by age, gender, and zygosity group of the BeTwiSt core sample (N = 1,387 pairs of adolescent twins). For the 1,368 pairs of twins with known zygosity, 795 pairs were MZ twins (380 male pairs, 415 female pairs), 311 pairs were same-sex DZ twins (146 male pairs, 165 female pairs), and 262 pairs were opposite-sex twins. The MZ to DZ ratio in our sample was 1.39, consistent with that found in most Asian twin samples (Ando et al., Reference Ando, Nonaka, Ozaki, Sato, Fujisawa, Suzuki, Yamagata, Takahashi, Nakajima and Kato2006; Hur et al., Reference Hur, Shin, Jeong and Han2006). The mean age of the T1 sample at the time of the assessment was 13.52 (SD = 2.67) years.

TABLE 3 T1 Sample Composition by Sex, Age, and Zygosity

OST = opposite sex twins; MZ = monozygotic twins; DZ = dizygotic twins.

Sample Representativeness

To assess the representativeness of the core sample (i.e., the T1 sample), we compared the basic demographic characteristics of the T1 twin sample to a general youth population in Beijing. We randomly selected one twin from each pair between the ages of 12 and 18 (N = 973). Using a frequency matching method, an age- and gender-matched sub-sample (N = 969) was randomly selected from a large population-based representative youth sample in Beijing (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Guo, Yang, Li, Duan, Zhang and Ge2009). Comparisons were conducted between the two samples in terms of basic demographic information. The results are presented in Table 4. No significant differences were detected in terms of perceived family social economic status, fathers’ educational attainment, or parents’ marital status or marital quality, with one exception: mothers’ educational level was significantly higher in the twin sample than in the general youth sample, t(1,898) = –2.29, p < .05. Overall, these comparisons suggest that the representativeness of the BeTwiSt twin sample was acceptable.

TABLE 4 Comparison of Socio-Demographic Characteristics Between Twin Sample and General Youth Sample in Beijing, China

Future Plans

The BeTwiSt is a prospective longitudinal twin study that aims to unravel the intricate interplay between genes and environment in the development of psychopathology. After the completion of the T2 assessment, we will continue to conduct follow-up studies until the participating adolescent twins enter adulthood. A long-term longitudinal investigation, such as the current study, enables us to examine our research aims across different stages of the life span, and to advance our understanding of how adult psychopathology is linked to earlier experience during adolescence. Future work will be funded by National Natural Science Foundation of China.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Knowledge Innovation Program of the Chinese Academy of Sciences (KSCX2-EW-J-8), funds for young scholars of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Science (Y0CX351S01), and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31170993). The original principal investigator, Dr. Xiaojia Ge, passed away during the course of this study. The other authors dedicate this paper to the memory of Dr. Ge for his great contribution in founding the BeTwiSt. We are grateful to participating twin families and schools. Special thanks to our sample recruitment team, Ting Chen, Yanyan Chen, and Xue Wang; and to our post-graduate students, Lan Wang, Jinqin Hou, Junxian Zhang, Mengjiao Li, and Mianbo Wang.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (2006). As others see us: Clinical and research implications of cross-informant correlations for psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 9498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ando, J., Nonaka, K., Ozaki, K., Sato, N., Fujisawa, K. K., Suzuki, K., Yamagata, S., Takahashi, Y., Nakajima, R., & Kato, N. (2006). The Tokyo Twin Cohort Project: Overview and initial findings. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 817826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, M., Beijsterveldt, C. E. M. T., Derks, E. M., Stroet, T. M., Polderman, T. J. C., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A longitudinal multiple informant study of problem behavior. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, K. S., Hay, D. A., Piek, J., Pearsall-Jones, J., Levy, F., & Martin, N. (2006). The Australian Twin ADHD Project: Current status and future directions. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boomsma, D., Busjahn, A., & Peltonen, L. (2002). Classical twin studies and beyond. Nature Reviews Genetics, 3, 872882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Review Neuroscience, 7, 583590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., & French, D. C. (2008). Children's social competence in cultural context. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 591616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Z., Guo, F., Yang, X., Li, X., Duan, Q., Zhang, J., & Ge, X. (2009). Emotional and behavioral effects of romantic relationships in Chinese adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 12821293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Li, X., Chen, Z., Yang, X., Zhang, J., Duan, Q., & Ge, X. (2010). Optimization of zygosity determination by questionnaire and DNA genotyping in Chinese adolescent twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 13, 194200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Los Reyes, A. (2011). Introduction to the special section: More than measurement error: Discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessments of children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (2009). Mechanisms of gene-environment interaction effects in the development of conduct disorder. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 408414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eaves, L. J., Silberg, J. L., Meyer, J. M., Maes, H. H., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Rutter, M., Neale, M. C., Reynolds, C. A., & Erikson, M. T. (1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The main effects of genes and environment on behavioral problems in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 965980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., Reiser, M., Zhou, Q., & Losoya, S. H. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children's effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45, 9881008.10.1037/a0016213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falbo, T., & Poston, D. L. Jr. (1993). The academic, personality, and physical outcomes of only children in China. Child Development, 64, 1835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goedde, H., Agarwal, D., Fritze, G., Meier-Tackmann, D., Singh, S., Beckmann, G., Bhatia, K., Chen, L. Z., Fang, B., & Lisker, R. (1992). Distribution of ADH 2 and ALDH2 genotypes in different populations. Human Genetics, 88, 344346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewitt, J. K., Silberg, J. L., Rutter, M., Simonoff, E., Meyer, J. M., Maes, H., Pickles, A., Neal, M. C., Loeber, R., & Erickson, M. T. (1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 1. Phenotypic assessment in the Virginia twin study of adolescent behavioral development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 943964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilt, L. M., McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Examination of the response styles theory in a community sample of young adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 545556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y., Collier, D., & Li, T. (2009). A prospective twin registry in southwestern China (TRiSC): Exploring the effects of genetic and environmental factors on cognitive and behavioral development and mental health wellbeing in children and adolescents. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 312319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudziak, J. J., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., & Pine, D. S. (2007). A dimensional approach to developmental psychopathology. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 16, 1623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hur, Y. M., Shin, J. S., Jeong, H. U., & Han, J. Y. (2006). The South Korean Twin Registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 838843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacono, W. G., McGue, M., & Krueger, R. F. (2006). Minnesota Center for Twin and Family Research. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 978984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaprio, J. (2006). Twin studies in Finland 2006. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 772777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K., Gardner, C., & Lichtenstein, P. (2008). A developmental twin study of symptoms of anxiety and depression: Evidence for genetic innovation and attenuation. Psychological Medicine, 38, 15671575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., & Barlow, D. H. (2005). Introduction to the special section: Toward a dimensionally based taxonomy of psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 491493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, S. Y., Pi-Hua, P. H, Chuang, L. M., & Chen, W. J. (2006). The Taipei Adolescent Twin/Sibling Family Study II: Depression, insulin resistance, and hormonal factors. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 895898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemery-Chalfant, K., Goldsmith, H. H., Schmidt, N. L., Arneson, C. L., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Wisconsin Twin Panel: Current directions and findings. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 10301037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Tuvblad, C., Larsson, H., & Carlstr, M. E. (2007). The Swedish twin study of child and adolescent development: The TCHAD-study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 6773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., Benjet, C., Georgiades, K., & Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 980989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikangas, K. R., Nakamura, E. F., & Kessler, R. C. (2009). Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 11, 720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E. (2005). The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: Gene–environment interplay in antisocial behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 533554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neiderhiser, J. M., Reiss, D., & Hetherington, E. M. (2007). The Nonshared Environment in Adolescent Development (NEAD) project: A longitudinal family study of twins and siblings from adolescence to young adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 7483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petryshen, T., Sabeti, P., Aldinger, K., Fry, B., Fan, J., Schaffner, S., Waggoner, S. G., Tahl, A. R, & Sklar, P. (2009). Population genetic study of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 810815.10.1038/mp.2009.24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, F., Harold, G. T, & Thapar, A. (2003). Negative life events as an account of age-related differences in the genetic aetiology of depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 977987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2011). Child psychiatric diagnosis and classification: Concepts, findings, challenges and potential. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 647660.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02367.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Simmonff, E., & Silberg, J. (1993). How informative are twin studies of child psychopathology? Twins as a tool of behavioral genetics. In Bouchard, T. J. & Propping, P. (Eds.), Life sciences research report (pp. 179194). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Trouton, A., Spinath, F., & Plomin, R. (2002). Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5, 444448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Beijsterveldt, C., Bartels, M., Hudziak, J., & Boomsma, D. (2003). Causes of stability of aggression from early childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal genetic analysis in Dutch twins. Behavior Genetics, 33, 591605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Bree, M. B. M., Rice, F., Fowler, T. A., Shelton, K. H., Lifford, K., Scourfield, J., Harold, G. T, & Thapar, A. (2007). The Cardiff Study of all Wales and North West of England Twins (CaStANET): A longitudinal research program of child and adolescent development. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, A. C., & Neale, M. C. (2010). Twin studies and their implications for molecular genetic studies: Endophenotypes integrate quantitative and molecular genetics in ADHD research. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 874883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Twin Studies on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology Across the World

Figure 1

TABLE 2 The Variables of Interest in this Study

Figure 2

TABLE 3 T1 Sample Composition by Sex, Age, and Zygosity

Figure 3

TABLE 4 Comparison of Socio-Demographic Characteristics Between Twin Sample and General Youth Sample in Beijing, China