Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T21:04:32.545Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Australian public worries more about losing species than the costs of keeping them

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 March 2023

Kerstin K Zander*
Affiliation:
Northern Institute, Charles Darwin University, Darwin, Australia
Michael Burton
Affiliation:
School of Agriculture and Environment, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Ram Pandit
Affiliation:
School of Agriculture and Environment, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Stephen T Garnett
Affiliation:
Research Institute for the Environment and Livelihoods, Charles Darwin University, Darwin, Australia
*
Author for correspondence: Professor Kerstin K Zander, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Government conservation measures will always depend on public support. While more has been learnt about which species the public values and which conservation measures are socially acceptable, less is known about the criteria that the public thinks government should consider when making conservation investment decisions. This study uses a stated preference best–worst scaling method to gauge the views of a sample of the Australian public on what they think government should consider when allocating funding to threatened species conservation. We found that the three most important factors were the risk that a species might become extinct, the likelihood that a conservation intervention might be effective and the risk of unintended consequences for other species that could potentially arise if the measure was implemented. Costs of conservation measures and the degree to which the society accepts these costs were considered much less important. The latter aspect was consistent with the high level of trust that respondents placed in the judgement of experts and scientists concerning threatened species conservation. We conclude that the Australian Government has a societal mandate to spend more money on threatened species conservation, provided that there is little risk and that it is backed up by science.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Foundation for Environmental Conservation

Introduction

The loss of biodiversity is a major challenge facing humanity, despite global conservation efforts (Otto-Pörtner et al. Reference Otto-Pörtner, Scholes, Agard, Archer, Arneth and Bai2021). Reversing the accelerating rate of extinction, with 500 species likely to go extinct in the next two decades (Ceballos et al. Reference Ceballos, Ehrlich and Dirzo2017), requires translation of ambitious conservation goals into real-world action (Butchart et al. Reference Butchart, Di Marco and Watson2016); ‘urgent decisions are needed about where, when and how to allocate scarce conservation resources to mitigate threats and recover populations’ (Tulloch et al. Reference Tulloch, Hagger and Greenville2020).

Not all conservation measures that can be applied are necessarily acceptable to the broader public which, either through taxes or donations, pays for them. Scientists have debated, for example, the ethics of managing feral animals harmful to threatened species by either killing them (Wallach et al. Reference Wallach, Bekoff, Batavia, Nelson and Ramp2018, Hayward et al. Reference Hayward, Callen, Allen, Ballard, Broekhuis and Bugir2019) or containing the threatened species within protective fencing (Mallon & Price Reference Mallon and Price2013, Child et al. Reference Child, Selier, Radloff, Taylor, Hoffmann and Nel2019). Other ethical debates have been around taking threatened species into captivity, such as wildlife parks or zoos (Keulartz Reference Keulartz2015), and relocating them somewhere safer (referred to as assisted migration; Albrecht et al. Reference Albrecht, Brooke, Bennett and Garnett2013, Ahteensuu & Lehvävirta Reference Ahteensuu and Lehvävirta2014). One of the most controversial ethical issues is genetic management of threatened species, such as interbreeding previously separated populations (Frankham et al. Reference Frankham, Ballou, Eldridge, Lacy, Ralls, Dudash and Fenster2011), deliberate hybridization (Todesco et al. Reference Todesco, Pascual, Owens, Ostevik, Moyers and Hübner2016, Quilodrán et al. Reference Quilodrán, Montoya-Burgos and Currat2020) and applying genetic engineering and gene drives (Kirk et al. Reference Kirk, Kannemeyer, Greenaway, MacDonald and Stronge2020, Sandler Reference Sandler2020). As a consequence of some debates, society has determined that there should be legal constraints on action or at least stronger actions (e.g., Braverman Reference Braverman2017). To date, debates on the ethics of conservation have mainly been confined to conservation experts, who advise on conservation investment based on biological and physical factors (Manfredo et al. Reference Manfredo, Berl, Teel and Bruskotter2021), feasibility, effectiveness and costs (Joseph et al. Reference Joseph, Maloney and Possingham2009, Hagerman & Satterfield Reference Hagerman and Satterfield2013, Moore et al. Reference Moore, Camaclang, Moore, Hauser, Runge, Picheny and Rumpff2021), as well as the risk of doing harm (Hagerman & Satterfield Reference Hagerman and Satterfield2013, Meek et al. Reference Meek, Wells, Tomalty, Ashander, Cole and Gille2015, Robinson et al. Reference Robinson, Rhoades, Pierson, Lindenmayer and Banks2021). However, understanding the values of the broader society can help ensure conservation decisions are socially acceptable (Manfredo et al. Reference Manfredo, Berl, Teel and Bruskotter2021). Governments are more comfortable spending taxpayers’ money and licensing actions if they know that the investment and permission reflect societal preferences and values and the public’s priorities for what should be conserved and how (Kirk et al. Reference Kirk, Kannemeyer, Greenaway, MacDonald and Stronge2020, Zander et al. Reference Zander, St-Laurent, Hogg, Sunnucks, Woinarski and Legge2021).

Recent studies have investigated which conservation measures society finds socially acceptable and likes to see supported by their government (see St-Laurent et al. Reference St-Laurent, Hagerman, Findlater and Kozak2019, Pelai et al. Reference Pelai, Hagerman and Kozak2021, Zander et al. Reference Zander, St-Laurent, Hogg, Sunnucks, Woinarski and Legge2021, Eyster et al. Reference Eyster, Olmsted, Naidoo and Chan2022). Even more is known about the preferred characteristics of the species that the public likes to see prioritized for conservation, such as the taxa (Troudet et al. Reference Troudet, Grandcolas, Blin, Vignes-Lebbe and Legendre2017), the threat status (Ridley et al. Reference Ridley, McGowan and Mair2020), the appearance and charisma (de Pinho et al. Reference de Pinho, Grilo, Boone, Galvin and Snodgrass2014, Colléony et al. Reference Colléony, Clayton, Couvet, Saint Jalme and Prévot2017, Garnett et al. Reference Garnett, Ainsworth and Zander2018a) and intangible aspects such as cultural importance (Garibaldi & Turner Reference Garibaldi and Turner2004) and familiarity (Danley et al. Reference Danley, Sandorf and Campbell2021). However, what government policymakers need to know is the extent to which extinction risk should be traded off against costs, the acceptability of different conservation measures, their feasibility and likelihood of success and whether the measure is acceptable to the public. Previous studies have focused on these aspects separately, preventing comparisons of different approaches and the public acceptability of trade-offs.

The aim of this study was to understand which aspects of threatened species conservation that the Australian public think should be afforded the highest priority. This we did by means of a best–worst scaling (BWS) experiment with a panel of survey respondents that revealed what they thought the government should trade off when allocating conservation funds. Australia has a history of species extinction (Woinarski et al. Reference Woinarski, Burbidge and Harrison2015). Most threatened Australian species are endemic (Chapman Reference Chapman2009), so their persistence largely depends on the conservation measures licensed and funded within the country (Wintle et al. Reference Wintle, Cadenhead, Morgain, Legge, Bekessy and Cantele2019). The research also has relevance to Australia’s national action plan for threatened species (DCCEEW 2022), particularly the targets for species conservation and community engagement. The results have relevance to discussions in government about what voters consider most important when conservation budgets are being determined. Our paper further contributes to the methodological advancement of the use of BWS experiments by using a split sampling design addressing the effect of whether respondents were asked to choose the best or worst item first, an important consideration for BWS designs.

Material and methods

Data collection and sampling

An online survey was conducted in October 2021. For this, we paid a research company (Dynata), which maintains a panel of people living in Australia recruited through different online and offline approaches and representing a snapshot of Australian society. The panel sample is representative of people in Australia with access to the Internet, which is c. 92% of the Australian population. The panel includes c. 400 000 people (c. 2.0% of the Australian adult population) from which the company selected 30 000 potential respondents. These 30 000 people were selected randomly while fulfilling certain criteria to make the sample representative of the Australian public in terms of gender, age and location (states and territories and urban/regional) because the research company assumes a 10% response rate, and we aimed for 3000 responses. Participants were remunerated for their time according to the company’s policies.

The questionnaire was tested with 20 people face to face, and then a pilot study with 200 respondents was carried out. The final questionnaire consisted of four parts. The first part included an introductory text about the aim of this survey, the ethical considerations and explanations about respondents’ rights. After this, we presented the BWS tasks, followed by questions on environmental and conservation attitudes and finally on the respondent’s demographic background. To gauge respondents’ connection to nature, we assessed their level of agreement with two statements (‘I always think about how my actions affect the environment’ and ‘I take notice of wildlife wherever I am’) that have been found to be helpful for characterizing a respondent’s attitude towards the environment. The questions were adopted from the ‘Nature Relatedness Scale’ (Nisbet & Zelenski Reference Nisbet and Zelenski2013). Respondents were asked about their degree of agreement with potential responses on a four-point scale (‘Strongly agree’, ‘Agree’, ‘Disagree’, ‘Strongly disagree’). Two more statements, assessed on the same scale, were included to assess respondents’ attitudes towards the economy (‘The best measure of progress is economic growth’) and technological solutions for resource shortages (‘Future resource shortages will be solved by technology’). These two questions are meant to assess respondents’ beliefs about economic growth and technology advancements and were adopted from the Dominant Social Paradigm scale (Kilbourne et al. Reference Kilbourne, Beckmann, Lewis and van Dam2001) and other studies which used them to explore risk perceptions and attitudes (e.g., Fletcher et al. Reference Fletcher, Higham and Longnecker2021, Zander et al. Reference Zander, St-Laurent, Hogg, Sunnucks, Woinarski and Legge2021).

To gauge conservation attitude, we included responses to four statements (‘Species extinction should be prevented regardless of the costs’, ‘It is more important to spend taxpayers’ money on things such as education and healthcare than on saving threatened species’, ‘It is the government’s responsibility to save our threatened species’ and ‘Decisions about which threatened species to save should be made by experts’). The questionnaire was developed in Qualtrics and tested online and offline with 20 people before the main survey began. Ethical approval for the survey was obtained from the Charles Darwin University ethics committee (H19047).

BWS design

In the BWS case 1 design (Louviere et al. Reference Louviere, Flynn and Marley2015), as used here, respondents were asked to state which of a set of items they consider as Best/Most and Worst/Least (the exact framing depending on context). The question associated with the BWS tasks was: ‘When the Australian Government considers how to invest in threatened species conservation, what do you think should have least and most influence on their decision?’.

Our BWS experiment had seven items from which to choose based on a literature review and our aims (terms in brackets signify terms used for each item throughout this paper):

  • Risk of measure (to threatened species, to other species and to humans) [Consequences]

  • Cultural importance of species [Cultural importance]

  • Acceptability of the measure [Acceptance]

  • Difference of the species from others [Distinctiveness]

  • Proximity to extinction [Extinction risk]

  • Cost of measure [Costs]

  • Likelihood of success in preventing extinction [Feasibility]

As recommended by Louviere et al. (Reference Louviere, Flynn and Marley2015), we applied a Balanced Incomplete Block Design (BIBD), which stipulates that every respondent see each item the same number of times and that each item co-occurred with another item the same number of times. Using the R package ‘crossdes’ (Sailer Reference Sailer2015), we created a design in which the seven items were always grouped into four. In each of the seven different tasks we created, we included four of the items in combinations that ensured each item co-occurred with each of the other items twice. Respondents were presented with all seven of the BWS tasks (see Fig. 1 for an example).

Fig. 1. Example of one of the seven best–worst scaling tasks. We applied a split design and half of the respondents saw one of the two versions each, and we also randomized the order in which the four items appeared in each task.

Respondents may process the information provided by the presented BWS tasks differently, and the positioning of the items within a task can affect how people make their choices. Campbell and Erdem (Reference Campbell and Erdem2015), for example, found that respondents were more likely to choose the item at the top of a list. To overcome this problem, we randomized the positioning of the four items within each BWS task. Another important aspect of the design is the order in which the response for the best and for the worst item occurs (either best–worst from left to right or worst–best). Hawkins et al. (Reference Hawkins, Marley, Heathcote, Flynn, Louviere and Brown2014) found that this order affects decision-making, in terms of decision-making time, and recommended varying the response order, which we did in our design. Using a split sample approach, half of the respondents saw all seven BWS tasks in which the response for the best item had to be made in the left-hand column, the other half saw seven BWS tasks where the best item had to be selected in the right-hand column (see Fig. 1 for the two designs and the associated questions). Each respondent was randomly assigned to one of the versions.

Data analysis

Data from a BWS experiment can be analysed using two broad approaches: a count approach and a statistical model approach. For designs that are BIBD, Marley and Louviere (Reference Marley and Louviere2005) showed that results from the counting approach are almost identical to the results from the statistical model approach. First, we applied the counting approach to show the relative importance of the items. For this, we tallied how often each item was chosen as best and worst and then subtracted the total number of ‘WORST’ (least important) choices for each item across all respondents (i.e., on an aggregated level) from the total number of ‘BEST’ (most important) choices. This resulted in a best–worst (BW) score for each item. A positive BW score indicated that the item was chosen more often as ‘most preferred’ than as ‘least preferred’. Each respondent saw each item four times in different combinations, which meant an item could get a maximal BW score of 4 if always chosen as most important and a minimal score of –4 if always chosen as least important (see Louviere et al. Reference Louviere, Flynn and Marley2015). The standardized BW scores for each item were calculated by dividing these BW scores by the number of times each item was seen by a respondent (here 4). We used the software R for all data analyses and the package ‘support.BWS’ (Aizaki Reference Aizaki2021) to organize and analyse the BWS data.

We then applied non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis H tests to assess the effect of independent variables (gender, age, education, income and attitudes, as well as the order in which the response for the best and for the worst item occurred) on the individual BW scores for each item.

Finally, we clustered the individual BW scores for each item using a polytomous variable latent class analysis (LCA). For this, we used the ‘poLCA’ R package (Linzer & Lewis Reference Linzer and Lewis2011). The LCA clusters respondents with similar scores for the seven BWS items and thereby identifies latent patterns of respondents’ values regarding what to save. This model is often described as a special case of cluster analysis – a probabilistic extension of the k-means method (McLachlan & Basford Reference McLachlan and Basford1988) that is particularly applicable when the variables in question are not continuous, such as the scores used here (Linzer & Lewis Reference Linzer and Lewis2011). The application of LCA also permits the inclusion of covariates to predict latent class membership; here, we included the same demographic and attitudinal parameters as tested before for the BW scores. While the non-parametric tests only tested whether these parameters had a statistically significant impact on the item selection, the LCA can cluster those respondents who score the items in a similar way and test whether respondents belonging to one class can be characterized by the same demographic background and attitudes.

Before the LCA, we carried out a correlation analysis using the R package ‘corrplot’, which showed four pairs of strongly correlated variables: ‘education’ and ‘income’; ‘prevent extinction at all costs’ and ‘government responsibility’; ‘economic growth’ and ‘technology’; and ‘wildlife’ and ‘think about actions’ (Fig. S1). We subsequently dropped ‘education’, ‘government responsibility’, ‘technology’ and ‘think about actions’ from the final model.

Results

Sample description

Of the 3000 completed surveys, 513 could not be used for analysis; 392 surveys were incomplete and 121 were submitted in an unacceptably short time (<4 min; mean completion time was 14 min). The final dataset contained 2487 valid and complete responses. The sample contained as many responses from male as from female respondents (Table S1). On average, respondents were 48 years old (SD: 16), with a median of 48 years, and thus were older than the national median (38 years; ABS 2022). Most respondents (71%) had an annual personal gross income of less than AU$80 000 and 39% had a university degree. This means that our sample had a similar income distribution to people across the whole of Australia (Table S1). People in our sample were on average slightly better educated than the average for people in Australia, a common feature of online surveys (e.g., Fenner et al. Reference Fenner, Garland, Moore, Jayasinghe, Fletcher and Tabrizi2012, Zander et al. Reference Zander, Simpson, Mathew, Nepal and Garnett2019, Hemsworth et al. Reference Hemsworth, Rice, Hemsworth and Coleman2021). One reason for our sample being older and better educated than the average is that we only included adults, whereas the data from the national census contain all age groups and therefore many young people still being educated. Most respondents lived in the densely populated south-eastern parts of Australia (Victoria, New South Wales and Queensland; Table S1), as do most Australians, although there was a very slight over-representation of respondents from South Australia and Tasmania. Seventeen percent of respondents participated in voluntary conservation activities or worked in the conservation sector.

More than 80% of respondents thought that it was the government’s responsibility to save Australia’s threatened species, that conservation decisions should be made by experts and that species extinction should be prevented regardless of the costs (Fig. S2). Approximately half of the respondents thought that it was more important to spend taxpayers’ money on things such as education and healthcare than on saving threatened species, while the other half did not think so.

Importance of aspects in conservation decision-making

The aggregate counting approach showed that three aspects were considered more often as ‘most important’ than ‘least important’ (i.e., they had a positive standardized BW score; Fig. 2; see Fig. S3 for histograms of the BW scores). The highest importance was assigned to the extinction risk of the threatened species to be conserved, followed by the likelihood of success of the conservation measure (its feasibility) and the potential consequences. The costs were regarded as least important (see Table S2 for the detailed BW scores). The standardized scores were very similar across the two versions of the BWS design (i.e., there was no order effect; Fig. 2 & Table 1).

Fig. 2. Standardised best–worst scores – by design version and overall (n = 2487).

Table 1. Summary of bivariate analyses of the effect of independent variables on the best–worst scores for each of the seven items.

**p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01, ns = not significant.

Prevent extinction: agreed that ‘Species extinction should be prevented regardless of the costs.’

Other priorities: agreed that ‘It is more important to spend taxpayers’ money on things such as education and healthcare than on saving threatened species.’

Economic growth: agreed that ‘The best measure of progress is economic growth.’

Belief in experts: agreed that ‘Decisions about which threatened species to save should be made by experts.’

Wildlife: agreed that ‘I take notice of wildlife wherever I am.’

Order effect: order in which the response for the best and for the worst item occurred in the best–worst tasks.

Determinants of importance

Female respondents had significantly higher BW scores for the cultural importance of species (H = 2.77, df = 1, p = 0.0959) than males (Table 1). Older respondents placed higher importance on ‘Consequences’ (H = 14.89, df = 8, p = 0.0613) and lower importance on ‘Cultural importance’ (H = 19.14, df = 8, p = 0.0142) than younger respondents. High income was associated with higher BW scores for ‘Costs’ (H = 16.95, df = 8, p = 0.0397) and lower scores for ‘Feasibility’ (H = 18.09, df = 8, p = 0.0205).

Those who believed that economic growth is essential placed greater importance on ‘Consequences’ (H = 4.45, df = 1, p = 0.0350) and lower importance on ‘Costs’ (H = 2.99, df = 1, p = 0.0834) than those not believing economic growth to be essential. Those who believed in expert decisions placed less importance on ‘Feasibility’ (H = 4.25, df = 1, p = 0.0382). Respondents strongly connected to wildlife had lower values for ‘Distinctiveness’ (H = 3.23, df = 1, p = 0.0724) than those without this connection. The order in which the responses for the best and for the worst item occurred in the BWS tasks had no significant impact on any of the seven items.

Clusters of respondents

Using the individual specific results from the counting approach, we tested latent class models with two, three, four, five and six classes and found that, based on the Bayesian information criterion, a model with four classes performed best (Table S3), with a distribution of c. 26%, 22%, 21% and 31% of respondents within classes 1–4, respectively. The LCA statistically confirmed latent classes present in the sample which were influenced by gender, not by age or income, and particularly by environmental and conservation attitudes (Table S4).

Respondents belonging to class 1 (26%) had a strong preference for ‘Extinction risk’, with all respondents within this class choosing this item as most important three or four times. ‘Feasibility’ was also important for respondents belonging to this class, while ‘Cultural importance’ had a negative BW score for 65% of respondents. Women were more likely to belong to this class. Those who strongly believed in economic growth were less likely to belong to this class. We labelled this group ‘Save everything’.

Respondents in class 2 (22%) considered ‘Costs’ unimportant in decision-making, with 95% of respondents having chosen this item as least important every time (BW score = –4). Respondents within this class were likely to think that species extinction should be prevented at all costs, that taxpayers’ money should be used for threatened species conservation and that they were connected to nature (‘wildlife’). We labelled this group ‘Costs irrelevant’.

Respondents belonging to class 3 (21%) had a strong preference for ‘Feasibility’, with half of the respondents having chosen this item as most important three or four times (Fig. 3): they had a BW score of 3 or 4 (see Table S4). Respondents in this class also favoured ‘Extinction risk’ and ‘Consequences’, albeit to a lower extent. We labelled this group ‘Save if possible’.

Fig. 3. Summary results of a four-class latent class model with covariates, showing the distribution of bestworst scores of each item across the classes.

Respondents in class 4 (31%) did not show strong preferences for any of the items; they placed importance on ‘Feasibility’ and ‘Consequences’, not on ‘Extinction risk’. Of all respondents, respondents in this class placed the highest importance on ‘Acceptance’, ‘Cultural importance’ and ‘Costs’. Respondents within this class were characterized by low connection to nature, low level of belief in conservation experts, high belief in the benefits of economic growth and a belief that taxpayers’ money should be spent on causes other than preventing threatened species extinction. We labelled this group ‘Save if convenient’.

Discussion

Important versus not-so-important aspects

The main message from this survey is that the Australian public thinks that governments should spend what is needed to prevent extinction provided the actions have a reasonable chance of success or cause no unintended harm. This reflects the three considerations considered most important by our sample of the Australian public when allocating resources to threatened species: how close a species is to extinction, the feasibility that the proposed conservation measure will prevent extinction and the risk that the conservation measure will have unintended consequences for humans and/or other species. Of these, proximity to extinction received the greatest support; this corroborates an aversion to extinction among the Australian public regardless of cost, with a willingness to pay substantial amounts towards preventing the extinction of almost any entity (a high existence value for species), not just pretty parrots and plants but also rats and snails (Gunawardena et al. Reference Gunawardena, Burton, Pandit, Garnett, Zander and Pannell2021).

Of least importance to the public were the costs of conservation. When asked directly to trade off spending on health and education against conservation of threatened species, half considered the latter more important. However, that means that half expressed a willingness to support persistence of another species over their own self-interest. The results are broadly similar to those of a survey in the USA where 74% of people supported the implementation of biodiversity conservation and 53% did so when people were asked to trade conservation off against economic development (Wang Reference Wang2022). Support for conservation is also reflected in the ongoing popularity of the Endangered Species Act in the USA (Tulchin et al. Reference Tulchin, Krompak and Brunner2015), which demands the protection of endangered species regardless of the costs.

Also unimportant was the cultural significance of a taxon, its distinctiveness from other species or the acceptability of the methods used to conserve it. Such results are consistent with the great faith placed in conservation scientists and managers (91% trusted scientists in their decision-making in conservation and their ability to make the right decisions when it comes to threatened species conservation and 86% agreed that decisions about which threatened species to save should be made by experts). Trust in Australian conservation scientists, which has emerged in other studies (Garnett et al. Reference Garnett, Zander, Hagerman, Satterfield and Meyerhoff2018b), is implied for all stages in the process of threatened species conservation: taxonomists are trusted to identify which forms of life are different from others; conservation biologists are trusted to identify which of these life forms are threatened with extinction; and conservation managers are trusted to do what is needed and to do whatever is necessary to save those species.

However, trust in science to do what is right does not extend to scientists imposing their own values on conservation. As has been found in other studies, scientists may consider distinctive forms more important than those with many close relatives (e.g., Garnett et al. Reference Garnett, Zander, Hagerman, Satterfield and Meyerhoff2018b), but not so the public. While the public may be willing to pay more for the conservation of some species than they are for others (Pandit et al. Reference Pandit, Burton, Gunawardena, Garnett, Zander and Pannell2022), they do not want any form actually to go extinct (Zander et al. Reference Zander, St-Laurent, Hogg, Sunnucks, Woinarski and Legge2021). Similarly, while scientists and some advocacy groups may be concerned about the ethics or efficacy of particular approaches (Subroy et al. Reference Subroy, Rogers and Kragt2018), the public is not troubled by such matters provided they are effective, whether it be killing feral animals (Zander et al. Reference Zander, St-Laurent, Hogg, Sunnucks, Woinarski and Legge2021) or intensive genetic management (Zander et al. Reference Zander, Burton, Pandit, Gunawardena, Pannell and Garnett2022).

Similarly, cultural values include not just culture or spirituality, but also aesthetic, place and educational aspects (Belaire et al. Reference Belaire, Lynne, Westphal, Whelan and Minor2015), and the public and local people place value on cultural ecosystem services (Milcu et al. Reference Milcu, Hanspach, Abson and Fischer2013) and culturally important species (Garibaldi & Turner Reference Garibaldi and Turner2004). However, such values were given low priority here compared to extinction risk. This may reflect the make-up of the panel. In Australia, the cultural value of a species is often linked to species that have connection to First Nations Peoples. However, as in the Australian population more broadly, only a small proportion (5%) of our sample identified as Indigenous. Had this share been larger, cultural aspects might have been judged as having a higher priority.

Sample heterogeneity

Within the broad trends in priorities, there was substantial variation in the priority placed on some of the variables, though not all, with the population falling into four classes. People in the ‘Save if convenient’ group were characterized by their lack of strong opinion about any of the factors affecting fund allocation, with only a mild preference for reducing extinction risk, feasibility and consequences. Based on the pattern of funding made available for threatened species to date, this group of 31% of respondents could be said to represent the status quo in terms of policy, given that people in this group have a low connection to nature, a low level of belief in conservation experts and strong beliefs in the benefits of economic growth and that taxpayers’ money should be spent on causes other than preventing threatened species extinction. This leaves nearly 70% of the respondents not wanting extinction, but with three classes of people, each of similar size (between 21% and 26%). Those in the largest of the groups, the ‘Save everything’ group, were adamant that nothing actually goes extinct, but they thought that factors such as feasibility and unintended consequences should be considered. Members of this group acknowledged the likelihood that cost may be limiting, suggesting trade-offs may be needed, but they suggested that the resources needed to prevent extinction itself should be quarantined because extinction is unacceptable, an approach embodied in McDonald et al. (Reference McDonald, Carwardine, Joseph, Klein, Rout and Watson2015). People in the ‘Costs irrelevant’ group were emphatic that cost should not be a consideration in any prioritization, and they had the strongest affinity with ‘Nature’. Although the beliefs of this group were not challenged by real dollar figures, which can sometimes be very high (Wintle et al. Reference Wintle, Cadenhead, Morgain, Legge, Bekessy and Cantele2019), these group members would be the most likely to reject arguments that imply resources are both fixed and constrained, rejecting concepts of ‘triage’ that might allow some species to go extinct because recovery is too expensive (Vucetich et al. Reference Vucetich, Nelson and Bruskotter2017). However, this group was willing to consider issues such as feasibility and unintended consequences in addition to extinction risk when deciding how to allocate resources. The third group was much more cautious, with extinction risk considered less important than feasibility and the risk of unintended consequences. However, in other ways this group was more closely aligned with the two other groups, favouring extinction prevention than the ‘Save if convenient’ group.

Also noteworthy were the factors of low importance. The distinctiveness, cultural importance and the acceptability of the management approaches were largely unimportant to those in the ‘Save everything’ and the ‘Save if possible’ groups, and even the ‘Costs irrelevant’ group was less likely to consider these factors than the ‘Save if convenient’ group. Together these results suggest that over two-thirds of the respondents were more concerned about extinctions than many of the drivers that have tended to characterize government policy on the environment.

Policy implications

The strong support for the prevention of species extinction found here and in other studies (e.g., Rogers Reference Rogers2013, Subroy et al. Reference Subroy, Rogers and Kragt2018, Zander et al. Reference Zander, Burton, Pandit, Gunawardena, Pannell and Garnett2022) has not been reflected historically in legislation or budgetary support. For example, unlike the widely supported Endangered Species Act in the USA, the Australian Endangered Species and Biodiversity Conservation Act 1999 (EPBC Act 1999) does not require that species extinction be prevented. Similarly, annual spending on targeted threatened species recovery in Australia has been far less than that spent by the USA on endangered species recovery and a fraction of what is thought to be needed to avoid extinctions and recover threatened species (Wintle et al. Reference Wintle, Cadenhead, Morgain, Legge, Bekessy and Cantele2019). Policy, however, has been more responsive. Australia’s first Threatened Species Strategy (Department of the Environment 2015) aimed to halt the decline of Australia’s threatened plants and animals and support their recovery by addressing the threats and by acting to support recovery, while at the same time ensuring that the development that underpins the country’s economic and social wellbeing is sustainable. Six years later, the aspiration to prevent extinction is explicit in ‘Objective 3: New extinctions of plants and animals are prevented’ (Australian Government 2022). Our results suggest that the Australian Government could support the strategy with substantial resources, especially as the species in greatest peril are known (Garnett et al. Reference Garnett, Hayward-Brown, Kopf, Woinarski and Cameron2022).

Methodological implications

Making choices is a complex psychological concept, and it is known that the design of tasks created by a researcher can affect how people process the information presented and how they make their choices (Hensher Reference Hensher2006, Greiner et al. Reference Greiner, Bliemer and Ballweg2014). Understanding how stated preference questionnaire design affects responses can prevent bias in future designs. While a large body of literature exists on the design and context effects of choice experiments (e.g., Greene & Hensher Reference Greene and Hensher2010, Leong & Hensher Reference Leong and Hensher2012), there has been less research on the impacts of survey design on BWS. One study (Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Marley, Heathcote, Flynn, Louviere and Brown2014) postulated that the way people process items in a BWS task might depend on whether respondents encounter the ‘Best’ or the ‘Worst’ items on the left-hand side. Both versions of the design are used in the literature (‘Worst’ on the left: Louviere et al. Reference Louviere, Flynn and Marley2015, White Reference White2021, Zander et al. Reference Zander, St-Laurent, Hogg, Sunnucks, Woinarski and Legge2021; ‘Worst’ on the right: Campbell & Erdem Reference Campbell and Erdem2015, Tyner & Boyer Reference Tyner and Boyer2020, Bhatta et al. Reference Bhatta, Zander and Garnett2022). Both designs have psychological and philosophical merits. Cultures in which people read from the left to the right might expect the ‘Best’ choice to be made first, but scales often start low and with better or higher scores on the right (e.g., other ranking or Likert-scale questions; e.g., Kazandjian & Chokron Reference Kazandjian and Chokron2008). We know of no publications in which the orders Best–Worst and Worst–Best in BWS have been compared. We found that the order was unimportant for all seven items that we presented. We therefore do not support recommendations that the order in such experiments be alternated (Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Marley, Heathcote, Flynn, Louviere and Brown2014).

Conclusions

Our results from an Australia-wide online survey using a BWS experiment – a stated preference method – suggest that the trend in Australian government policy towards a commitment to prevent future extinctions has strong support from the Australian people, particularly if the proposed actions have a reasonable chance of success and do not place other species at risk. This commitment held true even when traded off against healthcare and education. The strong support for preventing extinction regardless of the costs suggests that there would be public support for embedding the sentiment within threatened species legislation just as it was nearly 50 years ago with the US Endangered Species Act. The results also imply that rhetoric should be supported by adequate resourcing to make up for what appears to be a substantial deficit in threatened species funding compared to most other countries (Wintle et al. Reference Wintle, Cadenhead, Morgain, Legge, Bekessy and Cantele2019). However, our research establishing that people support greater investment in principle could usefully be extended to explore some of the limits of acceptability around cost and feasibility using quantitative case studies.

A second major finding was that the public retains a strong belief that conservation managers will make sound decisions on how to prevent extinction. This also reflects sentiment in the USA (Tulchin et al. Reference Tulchin, Krompak and Brunner2015) and places a heavy responsibility on conservation professionals to meet public expectations not only to prevent extinctions but to do so efficiently with minimum risk of unintended consequences.

Third, this study makes a significant contribution to the application of BWS experiments. We split the sample to test whether or not the order in which the response for the best and for the worst item occurs (the column for selecting the best item being either on the left or on the right) significantly affected respondents’ choices. The standardized scores were not statistically different across the two versions of the BWS design, and we conclude that the positioning of the column for selecting the best and the worst items (right-hand or left-hand) does not influence respondents’ decision-making.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0376892923000073.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial support

The research was funded by the Australian Government’s National Environmental Science Program (NESP) Threatened Species Recovery Hub.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Ethical standards

None.

References

ABS (2022) Snapshot of Australia – Age and sex. Australian Bureau of Statistics [www document]. URL https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/people/people-and-communities/snapshot-australia/latest-release (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Ahteensuu, M, Lehvävirta, S (2014) Assisted migration, risks and scientific uncertainty, and ethics: a comment on Albrecht et al.’s review paper. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 27: 471477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aizaki, H (2021) support.BWS: Tools for Case 1 Best-Worst Scaling. Version 0.4-4 [www document]. URL https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/support.BWS/index.html (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Albrecht, GA, Brooke, C, Bennett, DH, Garnett, ST (2013) The ethics of assisted colonization in the age of anthropogenic climate change. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 26: 827845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Australian Government (2022) 2022–2032 Threatened Species Action Plan: towards zero extinctions [www document]. URL https://www.dcceew.gov.au/sites/default/files/documents/threatened-species-action-plan-2022-2032.pdf (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Belaire, JA, Lynne, M, Westphal, C, Whelan, J, Minor, ES (2015) Urban residents’ perceptions of birds in the neighborhood: biodiversity, cultural ecosystem services, and disservices. The Condor 117: 192202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhatta, M, Zander, KK, Garnett, ST (2022) Governance of forest resource use in western Nepal: current state and community preferences. Ambio 51: 17111725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braverman, I (ed.) (2017) Gene Editing, Law, and the Environment: Life Beyond the Human. London, UK: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butchart, SHM, Di Marco, M, Watson, JEM (2016) Formulating smart commitments on biodiversity: lessons from the Aichi Targets. Conservation Letters 9: 457468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D, Erdem, S (2015) Position bias in best–worst scaling surveys: a case study on trust in institutions. American Journal of Agricultural Economics 97: 526545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceballos, G, Ehrlich, PR, Dirzo, P (2017) Biological annihilation via the ongoing sixth mass extinction signaled by vertebrate population losses and declines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114: E6089E6096.Google ScholarPubMed
Chapman, A (2009) Numbers of Living Species in Australia and the World. Canberra, Australia: Department of the Environment, Water, Heritage and the Arts.Google Scholar
Child, MF, Selier, SAJ, Radloff, FGT, Taylor, WA, Hoffmann, M, Nel, L et al. (2019) A framework to measure the wildness of managed large vertebrate populations. Conservation Biology 33: 11061119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colléony, A, Clayton, S, Couvet, D, Saint Jalme, M, Prévot, A-C (2017) Human preferences for species conservation: animal charisma trumps endangered status. Biological Conservation 206: 263269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danley, B, Sandorf, E, Campbell, D (2021) Putting your best fish forward: investigating distance decay and relative preferences for fish conservation. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 108: 102475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DCCEEW (2022) Threatened Species Strategy Action Plan 2022–2032. Canberra, Australia: Department of Climate Change, Energy, the Environment and Water.Google Scholar
de Pinho, JR, Grilo, C, Boone, RB, Galvin, KA, Snodgrass, JG (2014) Influence of aesthetic appreciation of wildlife species on attitudes towards their conservation in Kenyan agropastoralist communities. PLoS ONE 9: e88842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of the Environment (2015) Threatened Species Strategy. Canberra, Australia: Australian Government.Google Scholar
EPBC Act (1999) Environment Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act 1999. Canberra, Australia: Australian Government, Federal Register of Legislation [www document]. URL https://www.legislation.gov.au/Series/C2004A00485 (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Eyster, HN, Olmsted, P, Naidoo, R, Chan, KMA (2022) Motivating conservation even for widespread species using genetic uniqueness and relational values. Biological Conservation 266: 109438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenner, Y, Garland, SM, Moore, EE, Jayasinghe, Y, Fletcher, A, Tabrizi, SN et al. (2012) Web-based recruiting for health research using a social networking site: an exploratory study. Journal of Medical Internet Research 14: e20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, J, Higham, J, Longnecker, N (2021) Climate change risk perception in the USA and alignment with sustainable travel behaviours. PLoS ONE 16: e024454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankham, R, Ballou, JD, Eldridge, MDB, Lacy, RC, Ralls, K, Dudash, MR, Fenster, CB (2011) Predicting the probability of outbreeding depression. Conservation Biology 25: 465475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garibaldi, A, Turner, N (2004) Cultural keystone species: implications for ecological conservation and restoration. Ecology and Society 9: 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnett, ST, Ainsworth, GB, Zander, KK (2018a) Are we choosing the right flagships? The bird species and traits Australians find most attractive. PLoS ONE 13: e0199253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnett, ST, Hayward-Brown, BK, Kopf, RK, Woinarski, JC, Cameron, KA et al. (2022) Australia’s most imperilled vertebrates. Biological Conservation 270: 109561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnett, ST, Zander, KK, Hagerman, S, Satterfield, T, Meyerhoff, J (2018b) Social preferences for adaptation measures to conserve Australian birds threatened by climate change. Oryx 52: 325335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, WH, Hensher, DA (2010) Ordered choice, heterogeneity, and attribute processing. Journal of Transport Economics and Policy 44: 331–264.Google Scholar
Greiner, R, Bliemer, M, Ballweg, J (2014) Design considerations of a choice experiment to estimate likely participation by north Australian pastoralists in contractual biodiversity conservation. Journal of Choice Modelling 10: 3445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunawardena, A, Burton, M, Pandit, R, Garnett, ST, Zander, KK, Pannell, D (2021) Valuing multiple threatened species and ecological communities in Australia. Brisbane, Australia: National Environment Science Program, Department of Agriculture, Water and the Environment [www document]. URL https://www.nespthreatenedspecies.edu.au/media/ldyhvt3b/6-1-valuing-multiple-threatened-species-and-ecological-communities-in-australia-report_v6.pdf (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Hagerman, SM, Satterfield, T (2013) Entangled judgments: expert preferences for adapting biodiversity conservation to climate change. Journal of Environmental Management 129: 555563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, GE, Marley, AAJ, Heathcote, A, Flynn, TN, Louviere, JJ, Brown, SD (2014) Integrating cognitive process and descriptive models of attitudes and preferences. Cognitive Science 38: 701735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayward, MW, Callen, A, Allen, BL, Ballard, G, Broekhuis, F, Bugir, C et al. (2019) Deconstructing compassionate conservation. Conservation Biology 33: 760768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemsworth, LM, Rice, M, Hemsworth, PH, Coleman, GJ (2021) Telephone survey versus panel survey samples assessing knowledge, attitudes and behavior regarding animal welfare in the red meat industry in Australia. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 581928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hensher, DA (2006) How do respondents process stated choice experiments? Attribute consideration under varying information load. Journal of Applied Econometrics 21: 861878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, L, Maloney, R, Possingham, H (2009) Optimal allocation of resources among threatened species: a project prioritization protocol. Conservation Biology 23: 328338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazandjian, S, Chokron, S (2008) Paying attention to reading direction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keulartz, J (2015) Captivity for conservation? Zoos at a crossroads. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 28: 335351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kilbourne, WE, Beckmann, SC, Lewis, A, van Dam, Y (2001) A multinational examination of the role of the dominant social paradigm in environmental attitudes of university students. Environment and Behavior 33: 209228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, N, Kannemeyer, R, Greenaway, A, MacDonald, E, Stronge, D (2020) Understanding attitudes on new technologies to manage invasive species. Pacific Conservation Biology 26: 3544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leong, W, Hensher, DA (2012) Embedding multiple heuristics into choice models: an exploratory analysis. Journal of Choice Modelling 5: 131144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linzer, DA, Lewis, JB (2011) poLCA: an R package for polytomous variable latent class analysis. Journal of Statistical Software 42: 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Louviere, JL, Flynn, TN, Marley, AAJ (2015) Best–Worst Scaling: Theory, Methods and Application. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mallon, DP, Price, MRS (2013) The fall of the wild. Oryx 47: 467468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manfredo, MJ, Berl, REW, Teel, TL, Bruskotter, JT (2021) Bringing social values to wildlife conservation decisions. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 19: 355362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marley, AA, Louviere, JJ (2005) Some probabilistic models of best, worst, and best–worst choices. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 49: 464480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, JA, Carwardine, J, Joseph, LN, Klein, CJ, Rout, TM, Watson, JEM et al. (2015) Improving policy efficiency and effectiveness to save more species: a case study of the megadiverse country Australia. Biological Conservation 182: 102108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLachlan, GJ, Basford, KE (1988) Mixture Models: Inference and Application to Clustering. New York, NY, USA: Marcel Dekker.Google Scholar
Meek, MH, Wells, C, Tomalty, KM, Ashander, J, Cole, EM, Gille, DA et al. (2015) Fear of failure in conservation: the problem and potential solutions to aid conservation of extremely small populations. Biological Conservation 184: 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milcu, AI, Hanspach, J, Abson, D, Fischer, J (2013) Cultural ecosystem services: a literature review and prospects for future research. Ecology and Society 18: 44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, JL, Camaclang, AE, Moore, AL, Hauser, CE, Runge, MC, Picheny, V, Rumpff, L (2021) A framework for allocating conservation resources among multiple threats and actions. Conservation Biology 35: 16391649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nisbet, E, Zelenski, J (2013) The NR-6: a new brief measure of nature relatedness. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otto-Pörtner, H, Scholes, B, Agard, J, Archer, E, Arneth, A, Bai, X et al. (2021) Scientific Outcome of the IPBES–IPCC Co-sponsored Workshop on Biodiversity and Climate Change. Bonn, Germany: IPBES Secretariat.Google Scholar
Pandit, R, Burton, M, Gunawardena, A, Garnett, ST, Zander, KK, Pannell, D (2022) The scope effect in multiple species valuation. Manuscript under review [www document]. URL https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=4084339 (last accessed February 2023).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelai, R, Hagerman, S, Kozak, R (2021) Whose expertise counts? Assisted migration and the politics of knowledge in British Columbia’s public forests. Land Use Policy 103: 105296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quilodrán, CS, Montoya-Burgos, JI, Currat, M (2020) Harmonizing hybridization dissonance in conservation. Communications Biology 3: 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridley, FA, McGowan, PJ, Mair, L (2020) The scope and extent of literature that maps threats to species: a systematic map protocol. Environmental Evidence 9: 23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, NM, Rhoades, C, Pierson, J, Lindenmayer, DB, Banks, SC (2021) Prioritising source populations for supplementing genetic diversity of reintroduced southern brown bandicoots Isoodon obesulus obesulus . Conservation Genetics 22: 341353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, AA (2013) Social welfare and marine reserves: is willingness to pay for conservation dependent on management process? a discrete choice experiment of the Ningaloo Marine Park in Australia. Canadian Journal of Agricultural Economics 61: 217238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sailer, O (2015) Crossdes: construction of crossover designs. Version 1.1-1 [www document]. URL https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/crossdes/index.html (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Sandler, R (2020) The ethics of genetic engineering and gene drives in conservation. Conservation Biology 34: 378385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St-Laurent, GP, Hagerman, S, Findlater, KM, Kozak, R (2019) Public trust and knowledge in the context of emerging climate-adaptive forestry policies. Journal of Environmental Management 242: 474486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subroy, V, Rogers, AA, Kragt, ME (2018) To bait or not to bait: a discrete choice experiment on public preferences for native wildlife and conservation management in Western Australia. Ecological Economics 147: 114122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todesco, M, Pascual, MA, Owens, GL, Ostevik, KL, Moyers, BT, Hübner, S et al. (2016) Hybridization and extinction. Evolutionary Applications 9: 892908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troudet, J, Grandcolas, P, Blin, A, Vignes-Lebbe, R, Legendre, F (2017) Taxonomic bias in biodiversity data and societal preferences. Scientific Reports 7: 9132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulchin, B, Krompak, B, Brunner, K (2015) Poll finds overwhelming, broad-based support for the Endangered Species Act among voters nationwide. Tulchin Research, San Francisco [www document]. URL https://www.biologicaldiversity.org/campaigns/esa/pdfs/2015_Poll_on_Endangered_Species_Act.pdf (last accessed February 2023).Google Scholar
Tulloch, AIT, Hagger, V, Greenville, AC (2020) Ecological forecasts to inform near-term management of threats to biodiversity. Global Change Biology 26: 58165828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyner, EH, Boyer, TA (2020) Applying best–worst scaling to rank ecosystem and economic benefits of restoration and conservation in the Great Lakes. Journal of Environmental Management 255: 109888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vucetich, JA, Nelson, MP, Bruskotter, JT (2017) Conservation triage falls short because conservation is not like emergency medicine. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 5: 45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, AD, Bekoff, M, Batavia, C, Nelson, MP, Ramp, D (2018) Summoning compassion to address the challenges of conservation. Conservation Biology 32: 12551265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, M (2022) Do people really support conservation? Evidence from China and the United States. Chinese Journal of Population, Resources and Environment 20: 1218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, MH (2021) bwsTools: an R package for case 1 best–worst scaling. Journal of Choice Modelling 39: 100289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wintle, BA, Cadenhead, NCR, Morgain, RA, Legge, SM, Bekessy, SA, Cantele, M et al. (2019) Spending to save: what will it cost to halt Australia’s extinction crisis? Conservation Letters 12: e12682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woinarski, JC, Burbidge, AA, Harrison, PL (2015) Ongoing unraveling of a continental fauna: decline and extinction of Australian mammals since European settlement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112: 45314540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zander, KK, Burton, M, Pandit, R, Gunawardena, A, Pannell, D, Garnett, ST (2022) How public values for threatened species are affected by conservation management. Journal of Environmental Management 319: 115659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zander, KK, Simpson, G, Mathew, S, Nepal, R, Garnett, ST (2019) Preferences for and potential impacts of financial incentives to install residential rooftop solar systems in Australia. Journal of Cleaner Production 230: 328338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zander, KK, St-Laurent, GP, Hogg, CJ, Sunnucks, P, Woinarski, J, Legge, S et al. (2021) Measuring social preferences for conservation management in Australia. Biological Conservation 262: 109323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Example of one of the seven best–worst scaling tasks. We applied a split design and half of the respondents saw one of the two versions each, and we also randomized the order in which the four items appeared in each task.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Standardised best–worst scores – by design version and overall (n = 2487).

Figure 2

Table 1. Summary of bivariate analyses of the effect of independent variables on the best–worst scores for each of the seven items.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Summary results of a four-class latent class model with covariates, showing the distribution of bestworst scores of each item across the classes.

Supplementary material: PDF

Zander et al. supplementary material

Zander et al. supplementary material

Download Zander et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 484.1 KB