Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T07:27:48.201Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using Mendelian randomization analysis to better understand the relationship between mental health and substance use: a systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2021

Jorien L. Treur*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT) Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Marcus R. Munafò
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, the University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Emma Logtenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Reinout W. Wiers
Affiliation:
Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT) Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands Center for Urban Mental Health, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Karin J. H. Verweij
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: Jorien L. Treur, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Poor mental health has consistently been associated with substance use (smoking, alcohol drinking, cannabis use, and consumption of caffeinated drinks). To properly inform public health policy it is crucial to understand the mechanisms underlying these associations, and most importantly, whether or not they are causal.

Methods

In this pre-registered systematic review, we assessed the evidence for causal relationships between mental health and substance use from Mendelian randomization (MR) studies, following PRISMA. We rated the quality of included studies using a scoring system that incorporates important indices of quality, such as the quality of phenotype measurement, instrument strength, and use of sensitivity methods.

Results

Sixty-three studies were included for qualitative synthesis. The final quality rating was ‘−’ for 16 studies, ‘– +’ for 37 studies, and ‘+’for 10 studies. There was robust evidence that higher educational attainment decreases smoking and that there is a bi-directional, increasing relationship between smoking and (symptoms of) mental disorders. Another robust finding was that higher educational attainment increases alcohol use frequency, but decreases binge-drinking and alcohol use problems, and that mental disorders causally lead to more alcohol drinking without evidence for the reverse.

Conclusions

The current MR literature increases our understanding of the relationship between mental health and substance use. Bi-directional causal relationships are indicated, especially for smoking, providing further incentive to strengthen public health efforts to decrease substance use. Future MR studies should make use of large(r) samples in combination with detailed phenotypes, a wide range of sensitivity methods, and triangulate with other research methods.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Mental disorders have consistently been associated with substance use – in particular cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, cannabis use, and consumption of caffeinated drinks. Compared to the general population, individuals diagnosed with a mental disorder – or subclinical symptoms – are more likely to smoke (Garey et al., Reference Garey, Olofsson, Garza, Shepherd, Smit and Zvolensky2020), drink alcohol excessively (Stephen Rich & Martin, Reference Stephen Rich, Martin, Sullivan and Pfefferbaum2014), and use cannabis (Satre, Bahorik, Zaman, & Ramo, Reference Satre, Bahorik, Zaman and Ramo2018). For caffeine, there are conflicting findings with high(er) consumption being associated with a lower odds of depression (Grosso, Micek, Castellano, Pajak, & Galvano, Reference Grosso, Micek, Castellano, Pajak and Galvano2016) but a higher odds of schizophrenia (Williams & Gandhi, Reference Williams and Gandhi2008). A key factor in mental disorders is cognitive functioning, the majority of patients suffering from deficits in attention, learning and/or memory (Nieman et al., Reference Nieman, Chavez-Baldini, Vulink, Smit, Van Wingen, De Koning and Denys2020). In non-clinical populations, poor cognitive functioning has been associated with increased smoking (Campos, Serebrisky, & Castaldelli-Maia, Reference Campos, Serebrisky and Castaldelli-Maia2016), alcohol drinking (Topiwala & Ebmeier, Reference Topiwala and Ebmeier2018), and cannabis use (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Freeman, Mokrysz, Lewis, Morgan and Parsons2016), although for impaired response inhibition specifically there are contradicting findings (Liu et al., Reference Liu, van den Wildenberg, de Graaf, Ames, Baldacchino, Bø and Wiers2019b). Although caffeine is often thought to have acute beneficial effects on cognition (Irwin, Khalesi, Desbrow, & McCartney, Reference Irwin, Khalesi, Desbrow and McCartney2020), there is evidence that contests this (Galindo, Navarro, & Cavas, Reference Galindo, Navarro and Cavas2020; Weibel et al., Reference Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, Kolodyazhniy, Kistler and Reichert2020) and its long(er) term effects remain unclear (Cornelis, Weintraub, & Morris, Reference Cornelis, Weintraub and Morris2020; Panza et al., Reference Panza, Solfrizzi, Barulli, Bonfiglio, Guerra, Osella and Logroscino2015).

To properly inform public health policy it is crucial to understand the mechanisms underlying associations between poor mental health and substance use. Typically, a distinction is made between three, not mutually exclusive, mechanisms: (1) shared risk factors, (2) causal effects where poor mental health increases substance use, and (3) causal effects where substance use negatively affects mental health. As for mechanism 1, important non-genetic shared risk factors are the death of a loved one (Keyes et al., Reference Keyes, Pratt, Galea, McLaughlin, Koenen and Shear2014) or (other) childhood trauma (Setién-Suero et al., Reference Setién-Suero, Suárez-Pinilla, Ferro, Tabarés-Seisdedos, Crespo-Facorro and Ayesa-Arriola2020). Although note that these seemingly environmental factors might have a heritable component (Sallis et al., Reference Sallis, Croft, Havdahl, Jones, Dunn, Davey Smith and Munafò2020). Poor mental health and substance use are substantially heritable and there is evidence for considerable genetic correlation (Abdellaoui, Smit, van den Brink, Denys, & Verweij, Reference Abdellaoui, Smit, van den Brink, Denys and Verweij2020; Vink & Schellekens, Reference Vink and Schellekens2018). However, genetic correlations can also reflect causal relationships. If trait 1 causally affects trait 2, then genetic variants predictive of trait 1 will, indirectly, also predict trait 2 (Kraft, Chen, & Lindström, Reference Kraft, Chen and Lindström2020).

We review evidence from studies that applied ‘Mendelian randomization’ (MR) (Davies, Holmes, & Davey Smith, Reference Davies, Holmes and Davey Smith2018b; Lawlor, Harbord, Sterne, Timpson, & Davey Smith, Reference Lawlor, Harbord, Sterne, Timpson and Davey Smith2008) to assess causal effects between poor mental health and substance use. When we talk about a true causal effect (e.g. A is causal for B), we imply that if A were to be altered this would lead B to change accordingly. To some extent, MR is analogous to a randomized controlled trial (RCT). Instead of participants being assigned to experimental conditions, MR compares subgroups in the population which are at differing levels of genetic risk for a proposed risk factor. We include MR studies that look at cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, cannabis use, and/or caffeine consumption in relation to (symptoms of) a mental health disorder or cognitive functioning. Below, we briefly discuss epidemiological and (human) experimental evidence on these relationships and then introduce MR.

Epidemiological evidence

Causal inference can be attempted by looking at the temporal nature of relationships. For smoking, there is extensive longitudinal evidence that depression (Audrain-McGovern, Leventhal, & Strong, Reference Audrain-McGovern, Leventhal and Strong2015; Mathew, Hogarth, Leventhal, Cook, & Hitsman, Reference Mathew, Hogarth, Leventhal, Cook and Hitsman2017) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (van Amsterdam, van der Velde, Schulte, & van den Brink, Reference van Amsterdam, van der Velde, Schulte and van den Brink2018) are associated with increased odds of smoking initiation and persistence. In the other direction – from smoking to mental health – a systematic review study including 26 studies with a follow-up of between seven weeks and nine years concluded that smoking cessation is followed by reduced depression, anxiety, and stress (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, McNeill, Girling, Farley, Lindson-Hawley and Aveyard2014b). Smoking has also been associated with poorer cognitive performance, which improved after cessation (Vermeulen et al., Reference Vermeulen, Schirmbeck, Blankers, Van Tricht, Bruggeman, Van Den Brink and Van Winkel2018).

For alcohol, a review of 37 longitudinal studies found that (symptoms of) mental disorders in childhood predict an increased odds of alcohol dependence later on in life (Groenman, Janssen, & Oosterlaan, Reference Groenman, Janssen and Oosterlaan2017). In the other direction, alcohol dependence and heavy drinking predicted subsequent increases in depressive symptoms, but for heavy drinking, this association did not persist after adjustment for confounders (Li et al., Reference Li, Wang, Li, Shen, Li, Zhang and Peng2020). A systematic review of alcohol interventions reported that alcohol reduction led to a lower prevalence of psychiatric episodes, and improvement of anxiety and depressive symptoms, self-confidence, and mental quality of life (Charlet & Heinz, Reference Charlet and Heinz2017).

For cannabis use, the few available studies are smaller and the evidence is mixed. A 10-year prospective cohort study in 1395 adolescents found that symptoms of mental disorders (depression, bipolar, and anxiety disorder) increase the odds of cannabis initiation and cannabis use disorder (Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Fröhlich, Behrendt, Günther, Rehm, Zimmermann and Perkonigg2007). There was no indication that cannabis causes elevated anxiety symptoms (Twomey, Reference Twomey2017), but substantial evidence to support that it increases the risk of manic symptoms (Gibbs et al., Reference Gibbs, Winsper, Marwaha, Gilbert, Broome and Singh2015) and psychosis (Gage, Hickman, & Zammit, Reference Gage, Hickman and Zammit2016a). Another study found evidence that cannabis can be beneficial for post-traumatic stress disorder but is associated with short-term cognitive deficits (Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Gonzalez, Crosby, S. Thiessen, Carroll and Bonn-Miller2017).

For caffeine, research has focussed predominately on cognitive functioning or sleep. The largest available systematic review, including 28 studies, concluded that there is some evidence that caffeine is protective against cognitive decline (Panza et al., Reference Panza, Solfrizzi, Barulli, Bonfiglio, Guerra, Osella and Logroscino2015). Despite the fact that caffeine has stimulating properties which are thought to interfere with sleep acutely, a cohort study in 26 305 adolescents with a follow-up of 4 years found no association between average daily caffeine consumption and sleep duration (Patte, Qian, & Leatherdale, Reference Patte, Qian and Leatherdale2018).

Combined, the current epidemiological literature points topotential bi-directional effects between mental health and substance use. However, there are important methodological limitations to consider. First, there may be bias from confounders that were not included in the analysis or measured with considerable error (Gage, Munafò, & Davey Smith, Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith2016b). Second, reverse causality, where the outcome variable or a precursor of the outcome variable has affected the exposure, can induce spurious associations (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith2016).

Family-based studies are better suited for causal inference. Most notable are twin methods. Because monozygotic and dizygotic twins share 100% of their family environment and 100% or 50% of their genetic make-up, respectively, causality can be inferred by looking at within-twin pair differences. For instance, differences in ADHD symptoms were associated with differential progression to daily smoking, cigarettes per day and nicotine dependence in female monozygotic twin pairs, indicating that ADHD causally impacts smoking (Elkins et al., Reference Elkins, Saunders, Malone, Keyes, Samek, McGue and Iacono2018). A study that identified monozygotic twin pairs who were discordant for smoking (one smoked, the other did not), found evidence suggesting that smoking can also causally increase ADHD symptoms (Treur et al., Reference Treur, Willemsen, Bartels, Geels, van Beek, Huppertz and Vink2015). However, twin methods also have important limitations – there may be bias from confounders that led twins to differ on the exposure as well as on the outcome of interest, and reverse causation cannot be ruled out (McGue, Osler, & Christensen, Reference McGue, Osler and Christensen2010).

Experimental evidence from human studies

Experimentally induced stress increased the perceived value of cigarettes in smokers with depressive symptoms (Dahne, Murphy, & MacPherson, Reference Dahne, Murphy and MacPherson2017). Similarly, when tested after overnight sleep deprivation smokers were more inclined to pick cigarettes over money than when they were tested after a normal night's sleep (Hamidovic & de Wit, Reference Hamidovic and de Wit2009). In the other direction, a meta-analysis of 35 clinical trials concluded that participants who were randomly assigned to use nicotine patches to quit smoking experienced more sleep problems than participants assigned not to use them (Greenland, Satterfield, & Lanes, Reference Greenland, Satterfield and Lanes1998). After randomly assigning 31 smokers to continue smoking and 33 smokers to quit, anxiety and depressive symptoms decreased (more) in the latter group during 3 months follow-up (Dawkins, Powell, Pickering, Powell, & West, Reference Dawkins, Powell, Pickering, Powell and West2009).

Among 540 participants randomly assigned to receive different types of treatment for depression there were significant treatment effects on depressive symptoms, but no changes in alcohol consumption (Strid, Hallgren, Forsell, Kraepelien, & Öjehagen, Reference Strid, Hallgren, Forsell, Kraepelien and Öjehagen2019). A considerable amount of work has focussed on cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) to reduce alcohol consumption. A systematic review including eight RCTs concluded that CBT reduced alcohol use and depressive and/or anxiety symptoms, even when CBT targeted alcohol only (Baker, Thornton, Hiles, Hides, & Lubman, Reference Baker, Thornton, Hiles, Hides and Lubman2012). This could mean that decreases in alcohol use led to improvements in mental health, or that, though not targeted to it specifically, CBT affected depressive/anxiety symptoms.

As reflected in the work described here, only a limited number of causal questions can be answered with experimental designs. Moreover, these questions mostly relate to (relatively) short-term effects. Longer-term effects – for instance, potential effects of prolonged smoking on being diagnosed with a mental disorder, or the impact of lifetime alcohol use on the cognitive decline – cannot be investigated. There are also obvious ethical restrictions; it would not be acceptable to randomize people to initiate or increase their use of an addictive substance.

Mendelian randomization

MR has the potential to overcome (some of) the limitations of traditional epidemiological and experimental methods. We will explain MR's rationale by using one specific research question: does smoking (the ‘exposure’ of interest) causally impact depressive symptoms (the ‘outcome’ of interest)? As is the case for practically all human traits (Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Benyamin, De Leeuw, Sullivan, Van Bochoven, Visscher and Posthuma2015), individual differences in smoking can partly be explained by genetic differences (Vink, Willemsen, & Boomsma, Reference Vink, Willemsen and Boomsma2005). Genetic variants robustly associated with smoking have been identified through genome-wide association studies (GWAS) – the most notable variants in nicotinic receptor genes (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Jiang, Wedow, Li, Brazel, Chen and Vrieze2019a). Because the transmission of genetic variants from parents to offspring occurs randomly (Mendel's second law – ‘The law of independent assortment’), there should be minimal bias from confounders and subgroups of differing genetic risk can be thought of as RCT treatment groups. To determine whether smoking causally affects depression, we take genetic variants robustly associated with smoking and test if these also predict higher levels of depressive symptoms. The genetic variants act as proxies for measured smoking behavior, or instrumental variables (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Holmes and Davey Smith2018b). The most commonly used genetic variants are Single Nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). MR provides unbiased results if three assumptions are met: (1) the SNPs used as instrumental variables – together referred to as the ‘genetic instrument’ – are robustly associated with the exposure, (2) the genetic instrument is not directly associated with confounders and (3) the genetic instrument is not directly associated with the outcome, apart from any causal effect running through the exposure variable (Fig. 1a).

Fig. 1. The main principles of Mendelian randomization: (a) the conceptual model indicating the three core assumptions, (b) an illustration of vertical pleiotropy, that which causal inference is based on in a Mendelian randomization analysis, versus horizontal pleiotropy, which biases a Mendelian randomization analysis, and (c) an illustration of the framework and methods of Mendelian randomization using individual-level data versus summary-level data.

Since the second and third assumptions cannot be known or (exhaustively) tested, sensitivity analyses that assess the robustness of a causal finding are crucial. An important source of bias is pleiotropy, where a genetic variant affects multiple traits. Vertical pleiotropy (sometimes called mediated pleiotropy) occurs when a genetic variant affects the exposure and because of that indirectly also affects the outcome. This is not problematic and in fact is what an MR analysis aims to detect. Horizontal pleiotropy (sometimes referred to as biological pleiotropy) occurs when a genetic variant affects the outcome independently, not mediated through its effect on the exposure (Fig. 1b). This is problematic and could lead to bias.

There are two MR approaches: using individual-level data and using summary-level data from GWAS. Although MR using individual-level data requires a single data set of individuals with genotype data and information on both the exposure and outcome, MR using summary-level data takes summary estimates (i.e. the mean effect size for the genetic variants of interest) from separate GWAS for the exposure and the outcome. The two approaches use different methodology to estimate the causal effect (Burgess, Scott, Timpson, Davey Smith, & Thompson, Reference Burgess, Scott, Timpson, Davey Smith and Thompson2015; Burgess, Small, & Thompson, Reference Burgess, Small and Thompson2017) (Fig. 1c). MR using summary-level data has been the predominant method in recent years and currently has the most (powerful) sensitivity methods.

Methods

This study was pre-registered at PROSPERO (CRD42019133182; https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=133182). We performed a literature search of Medline, EMBASE, PsycINFO, and Web of Science for published, peer-reviewed papers describing MR of one or more type(s) of substance use in combination with mental health (including diagnoses, subclinical symptoms, and cognitive functioning). We also performed a search of pre-print servers (bioRxiv, medRxiv, and arXiv). We restricted our search to English-language publications (search terms provided in Supplementary Methods). Similar to a recent, high-impact review (Firth et al., Reference Firth, Solmi, Wootton, Vancampfort, Schuch, Hoare and Stubbs2020), we designed our search to pick up studies that performed analyses referred to as ‘Mendelian randomization’ (or (very) closely related methods such as ‘genetic instrumental variable regression’ (DiPrete, Burik, & Koellinger, Reference DiPrete, Burik and Koellinger2018)). The final search was performed on 27 February 2020, and a final update of all papers (to incorporate transitions from pre-print to a newer pre-print version or published paper) on 12 April 2021.

We followed PRISMA guidelines in extracting and selecting the data and used a flowchart to document the stages of screening. After a deduplication step, two of the co-authors independently selected potentially eligible studies based on title and abstract, and if necessary in the following step, based on the full text. In case of disagreement between the two main reviewers, this was resolved through discussion with a third co-author.

Qualitative synthesis

The studies included in this review use a wide range of genetic instruments, phenotypes, and methods. This precluded us from formally combining effect estimates through meta-analysis. Instead, we extracted the most important information from each study, judged the quality based on an extensive set of predetermined criteria, and summarized our findings stratified on the addictive substance.

We developed a scoring system incorporating the factors most important to the validity of an MR study (Supplementary Table S1), based on our collective knowledge of MR and cross-checked with the most recent (still evolving) MR guidelines (Davey Smith et al., Reference Davey Smith, Davies, Dimou, Egger, Gallo, Golub and Yarmolinsky2019). Important indices of quality are phenotype measurement (sample size and quality of the exposure and outcome measurements) and instrument strength (p value threshold used to select genetic variants, number of genetic variants included, biological knowledge, F-statistic for instrument strength, % variance that the instrument explains). Taking all quality indices into consideration, each study was given a total score of ‘’, ‘– +’or‘+’. We considered the total score based on a few key indicators that needed to be satisfied in order for the study to be considered sufficient (– + ), most notably: sufficient sample size and sufficient main analytical methods. When, on top of that, a study had used particularly extensive (sensitivity) methods, a total score of (+) was given. Two co-authors scored all studies independently and blind from each other, after which they compared their scores. In case of disagreement, a third co-author was consulted and together, all agreed on the final score.

Results

We identified 1464 potentially relevant records, of which 831 unique (Fig. 2). Of the final 63 studies included in qualitative synthesis, 40 investigated smoking, 24 investigated alcohol, 8 investigated cannabis, and 6 investigated caffeine (some investigated multiple substances; Table 1). The final quality rating was for 16 studies, – + for 37 studies, and + for 10 studies (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3 for MR using individual-level and summary-level data, respectively). Note that some summary-level studies obtained genetic estimates from partly/largely the same data sets, either for the exposure alone or for both exposure and outcome. This is inherent to MR, as it requires robust, replicated estimates from the largest available GWAS. However, this means that the causal findings presented should not be regarded as (completely) independent. The importance of a particular study and its findings is determined not only on the basis of the data used, but also the quality of the analysis and, importantly, sensitivity methods. If two studies use (almost) exactly the same data sets for exposure and outcome, this is indicated in the text. For a more detailed comparison of data sets see Table 2.

Fig. 2. PRISMA flow chart demonstrating the selection of articles to be included for qualitative synthesis.

Table 1. All Mendelian randomization (MR) studies included for qualitative synthesis, with their identifying information, description of the exposure and outcome variable(s), whether the study used individual-level and/or summary-level data, the total quality rating, and a brief summary of their findings

a This score pertains to the relationship that is of interest to the current systematic review, and not necessarily the whole study. For instance, it may be that in the study as a whole (more) extensive MR sensitivity methods were performed but for the causal estimate of interest no sensitivity methods were applied (e.g. when smoking is merely used as a mediator in a multivariable MR study).

b Pre-print publication (not peer-reviewed) obtained from bioRxiv.org, medRxiv.org or arXiv.org.

Note that the quality rating is based on a number of key indices, the most important being: phenotype measurement (sample size, quality of the exposure measurement, quality of the outcome measurement), instrument strength (p value threshold used to select genetic variants, number of genetic variants included, biological knowledge, F statistic for instrument strength, % variance that the instrument explains), and analytical factors (type of main analysis, whether or not basic sensitivity analyses were applied, whether or not additional sensitivity analyses were applied). Combined, these indices were weighted to come to a complete quality score (see Supplementary Table S1). A few important notes regarding this weighting of the evidence: (1) where absolute thresholds were used to judge the quality of a particular aspect of the study (e.g. sample size), it should be noted that these are somewhat arbitrary and were merely used to provide an indication of quality. (2) With regard to ‘phenotype measurement,’ a very well measured phenotype in a moderate sample size may be just as powerful as a more superficially measured phenotype in a very large sample. However, in case of very small sample sizes (e.g. n = 180 such as in the study by Irons et al., Reference Irons, McGue, Iacono and Oetting2007) even an extremely thoroughly measured phenotype will not lead to a high total score. (3) With regard to ‘instrument strength,’ when a study uses a single genetic variant that explains a relatively large amount of the variance and for which there is good biological knowledge, the fact that only one SNP was used is not necessarily problematic. For example, this is the case for SNP rs1051730 in the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor CHRNA5/A3/B4 gene cluster – each additional risk allele increases smoking heaviness with one additional cigarette smoked per day (Katikireddi, Green, Taylor, Davey Smith, and Munafò, Reference Katikireddi, Green, Taylor, Davey Smith and Munafò2018).

Table 2. All Mendelian randomization (MR) studies included for qualitative synthesis, with their identifying information, description of the data samples used for exposure and outcome variable(s), ancestry of those samples, the independence of the include SNPs, whether or not proxies were used, and whether or not a correction for multiple testing was applied

a Pre-print publication (not peer-reviewed) obtained from bioRxiv.org, medRxiv.org or arXiv.org.

Note that the complete references to the samples listed under ‘GWAS sample exposure variable(s)’ and ‘GWAS sample outcome variable(s)’ can be found in the original publications (1–63).

Cigarette smoking

Cognitive traits

There was consistent evidence that higher educational attainment decreases the odds of initiating smoking (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Gill, Davies, Taylor, Tillmann, Vaucher and Dehghan2019; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Dickson, Davey Smith, Windmeijer and van den Berg2018a; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hill, Anderson, Sanderson, Deary and Smith2019; Ding, Barban, & Mills, Reference Ding, Barban and Mills2019; Gage, Bowden, Davey Smith, & Munafo, Reference Gage, Bowden, Davey Smith and Munafo2018; Sanderson, Davey Smith, Bowden, & Munafò, Reference Sanderson, Davey Smith, Bowden and Munafò2019; Tillmann et al., Reference Tillmann, Vaucher, Okbay, Pikhart, Peasey, Kubinova and Holmes2017; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Ntalla, Kessler, Kastrati, Erdmann, Danesh and Schunkert2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a), increases the age at smoking initiation (Yuan, Xiong, Michaëlsson, Michaëlsson, & Larsson, Reference Yuan, Xiong, Michaëlsson, Michaëlsson and Larsson2020a; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a), increases smoking heaviness, and decreases the odds of quitting (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Bowden, Davey Smith and Munafo2018; Sanderson et al., Reference Sanderson, Davey Smith, Bowden and Munafò2019; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Ntalla, Kessler, Kastrati, Erdmann, Danesh and Schunkert2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a). One study triangulated self-report measures with cotinine (a metabolite of nicotine) in blood samples and found weak evidence that higher educational attainment causes lower cotinine levels (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Bowden, Davey Smith and Munafo2018). There was considerable overlap among the data sets used (Table 2). Two studies based their education-to-smoking estimate on the same data sets, one testing whether smoking mediated the effects of education on coronary heart disease, and the other whether smoking mediated the effects of education on lung cancer (Tillmann et al., Reference Tillmann, Vaucher, Okbay, Pikhart, Peasey, Kubinova and Holmes2017; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a). There was strong evidence that higher general cognitive ability decreases lifetime smoking (Adams, Reference Adams2019), but no clear evidence for effects on smoking initiation or cessation (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hill, Anderson, Sanderson, Deary and Smith2019). Two multivariable MR studies found that causal effects of education on smoking were not mediated by cognitive ability (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hill, Anderson, Sanderson, Deary and Smith2019; Sanderson et al., Reference Sanderson, Davey Smith, Bowden and Munafò2019).

Substantially fewer studies looked at causal effects ofsmoking on cognitive functioning. There was consistent evidence that smoking initiation and lifetime smoking decrease educational attainment (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Sallis, Lassi, Wootton, Mokrysz, Davey Smith and Munafò2020; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Davies, Dickson, Tyrrell, Green, Katikireddi and Howe2020b), and weaker evidence that they decrease cognitive ability (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Sallis, Lassi, Wootton, Mokrysz, Davey Smith and Munafò2020). Two other studies found no clear evidence for causal effects of smoking initiation on cognitive functioning (Adams, Reference Adams2019; North et al., Reference North, Palmer, Lewis, Cooper, Power, Pattie and Day2015), but note that the analysis by Gage et al. (Reference Gage, Sallis, Lassi, Wootton, Mokrysz, Davey Smith and Munafò2020) was superior (+ v. – +). There was also no clear evidence that smoking affects working memory, response inhibition, or emotion recognition, but these analyses were likely underpowered (Mahedy et al., Reference Mahedy, Wootton, Suddell, Caroline, Heron, Hickman and Munafò2021). A single analysis (rated –) reported that current smoking increases the odds of cognitive impairment (Fu, Faul, Jin, Ware, & Bakulski, Reference Fu, Faul, Jin, Ware and Bakulski2019). Two studies found weak evidence that smoking decreases the odds of Alzheimer's disease (Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Traylor, Malik, Dichgans, Burgess and Markus2017; Østergaard et al., Reference Østergaard, Mukherjee, Sharp, Proitsi, Lotta, Day and Wareham2015), but a more recent analysis, rated as superior (+ v. – +), found no effects of smoking on Alzheimer's disease (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Fulton-Howard, O'Reilly, Marcora, Goate, Farrer and Wang2021). The seemingly protective effect of smoking is likely survival bias – smokers who do not die from smoking-related diseases are less prone to diseases making them less likely to develop Alzheimer's disease. Of note, smoking initiation is not an ideal measure – there is accumulating evidence that genetic variants associated with this phenotype are horizontally pleiotropic (Khouja, Wootton, Taylor, Smith, & Munafò, Reference Khouja, Wootton, Taylor, Smith and Munafò2020). A conclusion on the causal effects of smoking can more reliably be made by testing the effects of smoking heaviness.

Sleep problems

There was weak evidence that insomnia increases smoking heaviness and decreases cessation from two studies (Gibson, Munafò, Taylor, & Treur, Reference Gibson, Munafò, Taylor and Treur2019; Jansen et al., Reference Jansen, Watanabe, Stringer, Skene, Bryois, Hammerschlag and Posthuma2019), but not from a third (Lane et al., Reference Lane, Jones, Dashti, Wood, Aragam, van Hees and Saxena2019). In contrast, there was no clear evidence that sleep duration impacts smoking (Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Munafò, Taylor and Treur2019). There was particularly strong evidence that smoking heaviness impacts chronotype, decreasing the odds of being a morning person (Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Munafò, Taylor and Treur2019; Millard, Munafò, Tilling, Wootton, & Davey Smith, Reference Millard, Munafò, Tilling, Wootton and Davey Smith2019), but no clear evidence that smoking influences insomnia risk or sleep duration (Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Munafò, Taylor and Treur2019; Jansen et al., Reference Jansen, Watanabe, Stringer, Skene, Bryois, Hammerschlag and Posthuma2019).

Internalizing/mood disorders

There was some evidence for causal, increasing effects of depression (Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Richmond, Stuijfzand, Lawn, Sallis, Taylor and Munafò2019), feelings of loneliness (Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Greenstone, Abdellaoui, Denys, Verweij, Munafò and Treur2020), and neuroticism (Sallis, Smith, & Munafo, Reference Sallis, Smith and Munafo2019) on smoking behavior. Adams (Reference Adams2019), using a larger data set for the outcome than Sallis et al. (Reference Sallis, Smith and Munafo2019), did not find clear evidence that neuroticism affects smoking. There was no clear evidence for causal effects of depression or bipolar disorder on smoking (Barkhuizen, Dudbridge, & Ronald, Reference Barkhuizen, Dudbridge and Ronald2020; Vermeulen et al., Reference Vermeulen, Wootton, Treur, Sallis, Jones, Zammit and Munafò2019). Most studies also tested effects in the other direction. Earlier studies showed no clear evidence for causal effects (Bjorngaard et al., Reference Bjorngaard, Gunnell, Elvestad, Smith, Skorpen, Krokan and Romundstad2013; Skov-Ettrup, Nordestgaard, Petersen, & Tolstrup, Reference Skov-Ettrup, Nordestgaard, Petersen and Tolstrup2017; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Fluharty, Bjørngaard, Gabrielsen, Skorpen, Marioni and Munafò2014a; Wium-Andersen, Orsted, & Nordestgaard, Reference Wium-Andersen, Orsted and Nordestgaard2015a), with one exception: a small study (n = 6294) of low-rated quality reporting that smoking decreased depression during pregnancy (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Araya, Smith, Freathy, Gunnell, Palmer and Munafo2011). More recent studies, employing much larger samples, found strong evidence for causal, increasing effects of smoking initiation and lifetime smoking on depression and bipolar disorder risk (Barkhuizen et al., Reference Barkhuizen, Dudbridge and Ronald2020; Vermeulen et al., Reference Vermeulen, Wootton, Treur, Sallis, Jones, Zammit and Munafò2019; Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Richmond, Stuijfzand, Lawn, Sallis, Taylor and Munafò2019). There was weak evidence that smoking initiation increases feelings of loneliness (a phenotype closely related to depression) from one study (Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Greenstone, Abdellaoui, Denys, Verweij, Munafò and Treur2020), but no such evidence from another (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Davies, Dickson, Tyrrell, Green, Katikireddi and Howe2020b). One study reported weak evidence that smoking initiation decreases neuroticism (Sallis et al., Reference Sallis, Smith and Munafo2019), whereas another, better-powered study found that lifetime smoking increases neuroticism (Adams, Reference Adams2019). Finally, there was no clear evidence that smoking causally impacts suicidal ideation (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Munafò, Davey Smith and Wootton2020a).

Externalizing disorders

There was strong evidence that ADHD liability increases smoking initiation, smoking heaviness and lifetime smoking, and decreases cessation (Fluharty, Sallis, & Munafò, Reference Fluharty, Sallis and Munafò2018; Leppert et al., Reference Leppert, Riglin, Dardani, Thapar, Staley, Tilling and Stergiakouli2019; Sallis et al., Reference Sallis, Smith and Munafo2019; Treur et al., Reference Treur, Demontis, Sallis, Richardson, Wiers, Borglum and Munafo2019). There was no clear evidence that aggression causally affects smoking, but this analysis was likely underpowered (Fluharty et al., Reference Fluharty, Sallis and Munafò2018). One study also tested reverse effects, reporting weak evidence that smoking initiation increases ADHD risk, but with important cautionary notes about the pleiotropic nature of the initiation measure (Treur et al., Reference Treur, Demontis, Sallis, Richardson, Wiers, Borglum and Munafo2019).

Psychotic disorders

Multiple studies reported evidence (ranging from weak to strong) that smoking causally increases schizophrenia risk (Barkhuizen et al., Reference Barkhuizen, Dudbridge and Ronald2020; Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Ferreira, Xue, Lindström, Jiang, Yang and Chenevix-Trench2019; Gage et al., Reference Gage, Jones, Taylor, Burgess, Zammit and Munafò2017b; Wium-Andersen et al., Reference Wium-Andersen, Orsted and Nordestgaard2015a, ; Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Richmond, Stuijfzand, Lawn, Sallis, Taylor and Munafò2019). In the other direction there was no clear evidence for causal effects of liability to schizophrenia on smoking from one study (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Jones, Burgess, Bowden, Davey Smith, Zammit and Munafò2017a, Reference Gage, Jones, Taylor, Burgess, Zammit and Munafò2017b), and some evidence for such effects from two recent, better-powered studies with largely overlapping samples (Barkhuizen et al., Reference Barkhuizen, Dudbridge and Ronald2020; Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Richmond, Stuijfzand, Lawn, Sallis, Taylor and Munafò2019).

Alcohol use

Cognitive traits

There was strong evidence that higher educational attainment increases alcohol use frequency (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Dickson, Davey Smith, Windmeijer and van den Berg2018a; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hill, Anderson, Sanderson, Deary and Smith2019; Rosoff et al., Reference Rosoff, Clarke, Adams, McIntosh, Davey Smith, Jung and Lohoff2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a, Reference Zhou, Sun, Li, Ma, Heianza and Qi2019b) and wine intake (Rosoff et al., Reference Rosoff, Clarke, Adams, McIntosh, Davey Smith, Jung and Lohoff2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a, Reference Zhou, Sun, Li, Ma, Heianza and Qi2019b), whereas it decreases beer/cider intake (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Yang, Fang, Hong and Zhang2019a, Reference Zhou, Sun, Li, Ma, Heianza and Qi2019b), and the risk of binge-drinking and alcohol use disorder (Rosoff et al., Reference Rosoff, Clarke, Adams, McIntosh, Davey Smith, Jung and Lohoff2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020). Ding et al. (Reference Ding, Barban and Mills2019) did not find clear evidence for causality from education to alcohol use, but this analysis was likely underpowered. There was also evidence that general cognitive ability increases alcohol use frequency (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hill, Anderson, Sanderson, Deary and Smith2019) and decreases the risk of alcohol use disorder (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020). In the other direction, Rosoff et al. (Reference Rosoff, Clarke, Adams, McIntosh, Davey Smith, Jung and Lohoff2019) found weak evidence that higher alcohol use decreases educational attainment, whereas another study did not (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Davies, Dickson, Tyrrell, Green, Katikireddi and Howe2020b). A third, high-quality rated study found that liability to alcohol use disorder negatively impacts educational attainment (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020). Note that GWAS of current alcohol use have largely been performed in adults, reflecting alcohol use after maximum educational attainment occurred for most. Although the genetic instrument may also reflect alcohol use at younger ages, this needs to be taken into account. There was no clear evidence that drinking more alcohol impacts cognition, but this was based on (very) small, low-quality rated studies (Almeida, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge, & Flicker, Reference Almeida, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge and Flicker2014a, Reference Almeida, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge and Flicker2014b; Au Yeung et al., Reference Au Yeung, Jiang, Cheng, Liu, Zhang, Lam and Schooling2012; Kumari et al., Reference Kumari, Holmes, Dale, Hubacek, Palmer, Pikhart and Bobak2014; Mahedy et al., Reference Mahedy, Suddell, Skirrow, Fernandes, Field, Heron and Munafò2020; Ritchie et al., Reference Ritchie, Bates, Corley, McNeill, Davies, Liewald and Deary2014). There were contradicting findings for Alzheimer's disease, with one study finding no causal effects of alcohol (Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Traylor, Malik, Dichgans, Burgess and Markus2017) and another finding that while a higher number of drinks caused an earlier onset of Alzheimer's disease, alcohol use disorder caused a later onset (Andrews, Goate, & Anstey, Reference Andrews, Goate and Anstey2020). The latter likely reflects survival bias.

Sleep problems

There was some evidence that drinking more alcohol per week increases sleep duration, but this was based on only one, low-quality rated study (Nishiyama et al., Reference Nishiyama, Nakatochi, Goto, Iwasaki, Hachiya, Sutoh and Suzuki2019).

Internalizing/mood disorders

A recent, particularly large study reported strong evidence that major depressive disorder (MDD) liability increases alcohol use disorder risk (Polimanti et al., Reference Polimanti, Peterson, Ong, MacGregor, Edwards, Clarke and Derks2019). Similarly, there was evidence that worrying and neuroticism increase alcohol use disorder risk (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020). There was no clear evidence that feelings of loneliness affect alcohol use (disorder) (Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Greenstone, Abdellaoui, Denys, Verweij, Munafò and Treur2020). In the other direction, there was no clear evidence that alcohol use (disorder) causally impacts internalizing symptoms (Almeida et al., Reference Almeida, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge and Flicker2014a, Reference Almeida, Hankey, Yeap, Golledge and Flicker2014b; Chao, Li, & McGue, Reference Chao, Li and McGue2017; Lim et al., Reference Lim, Rijsdijk, Hagenaars, Socrates, Choi, Coleman and Pingault2020; Polimanti et al., Reference Polimanti, Peterson, Ong, MacGregor, Edwards, Clarke and Derks2019; Wium-Andersen, Orsted, Tolstrup, & Nordestgaard, Reference Wium-Andersen, Orsted, Tolstrup and Nordestgaard2015b; Wootton et al., Reference Wootton, Greenstone, Abdellaoui, Denys, Verweij, Munafò and Treur2020; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020).

Externalizing disorders

There was weak evidence that ADHD liability increases alcohol use disorder risk (Treur et al., Reference Treur, Demontis, Sallis, Richardson, Wiers, Borglum and Munafo2019). In the other direction, there was some evidence that higher alcohol use frequency increases aggression and attention problems from one, small (n = 1608) low-rated analysis (Chao et al., Reference Chao, Li and McGue2017), and no evidence for causal effects on antisocial behavior from another very small (n = 180) low-rated analysis (Irons, McGue, Iacono, & Oetting, Reference Irons, McGue, Iacono and Oetting2007).

Psychotic disorders

There was no clear evidence for causal effects, in either direction, between alcohol use disorder and schizophrenia risk (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020).

Cannabis use

Cognitive traits

There was no evidence for causal effects from cannabis initiation to cognitive functioning (Mahedy et al., Reference Mahedy, Wootton, Suddell, Caroline, Heron, Hickman and Munafò2021).

Internalizing disorders

There was neither clear evidence for causal effects in either direction between cannabis initiation and MDD (Hodgson et al., Reference Hodgson, Coleman, Hagenaars, Purves, Glanville, Choi and Lewis2020), nor was there evidence for causal effects from cannabis initiation to self-harm (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Rijsdijk, Hagenaars, Socrates, Choi, Coleman and Pingault2020).

Externalizing disorders

There was evidence that ADHD liability increases cannabis initiation without clear evidence for the reverse (Soler Artigas et al., Reference Soler Artigas, Sánchez-Mora, Rovira, Richarte, Garcia-Martínez, Pagerols and Ribasés2019; Treur et al., Reference Treur, Demontis, Sallis, Richardson, Wiers, Borglum and Munafo2019).

Psychotic disorders

Out of eight studies that included cannabis, three looked at schizophrenia. One tested causality from cannabis initiation to schizophrenia risk only, finding evidence for an increasing effect (Vaucher et al., Reference Vaucher, Keating, Lasserre, Gan, Lyall, Ward and Holmes2018). Two other studies tested causal effects in both directions and found weak evidence that cannabis initiation increases schizophrenia risk and strong evidence that schizophrenia liability increases the odds of cannabis initiation (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Jones, Burgess, Bowden, Davey Smith, Zammit and Munafò2017a, Reference Gage, Jones, Taylor, Burgess, Zammit and Munafò2017b; Pasman et al., Reference Pasman, Verweij, Gerring, Stringer, Sanchez-Roige, Treur and Vink2018).

Caffeine consumption

Cognitive traits

There was weak evidence that higher coffee consumption increases Alzheimer's risk from one study (Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Traylor, Malik, Dichgans, Burgess and Markus2017), but no clear evidence from another (Kwok, Leung, & Schooling, Reference Kwok, Leung and Schooling2016). There was also no clear evidence for causal effects of coffee on general cognitive functioning (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Taylor, Karhunen, Zhan, Rovio, Lahti and Hypponen2018).

Sleep problems

There was weak evidence that higher plasma caffeine levels decrease the odds of being a morning person, but no clear evidence for causal effects between self-reported caffeine consumption and sleep duration, insomnia, or chronotype (Treur et al., Reference Treur, Gibson, Taylor, Rogers, Munafo and Munafò2018).

Internalizing disorders

There was no clear evidence for causal effects between caffeine consumption and ADHD, in either direction (Treur et al., Reference Treur, Demontis, Sallis, Richardson, Wiers, Borglum and Munafo2019),

Externalizing disorders

There was no evidence for causal effects of caffeine consumption on depression (Kwok et al., Reference Kwok, Leung and Schooling2016).

Discussion

We conducted the first systematic review of MR studies investigating causal relationships between mental health and substance use. From a total of 63 studies, we can draw important conclusions regarding if and how mental health and substance use are causally related.

Smoking was the most investigated, resulting in particularly strong evidence that higher educational attainment causally decreases smoking (lower risk of initiating, smoking fewer cigarettes, and more likely to quit). Although smoking prevalence has rapidly decreased in the past two decades, this decline has been most prominent among those with high educational attainment, leading to an increasing (health) gap (Agaku, Odani, Okuyemi, & Armour, Reference Agaku, Odani, Okuyemi and Armour2020). The causal role of education we report is important for policy-makers going forward. Interestingly, causal effects from education are neither mediated by cognitive ability (Sanderson et al., Reference Sanderson, Davey Smith, Bowden and Munafò2019) nor were there clear evidence that cognitive ability by itself affects smoking (Adams, Reference Adams2019; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hill, Anderson, Sanderson, Deary and Smith2019). The studies included in this review cannot determine exactly why educational attainment affects smoking. Smoking initiation usually occurs during adolescence, at which time the home environment and peer influences are important. Adolescents in lower educational groups tend to experience lower levels of parental involvement, parental monitoring, and self-perceived social competence, factors associated with a higher odds of initiating smoking (Mahabee-Gittens, Xiao, Gordon, & Khoury, Reference Mahabee-Gittens, Xiao, Gordon and Khoury2013; Simons-Morton, Reference Simons-Morton2002). As for smoking heaviness and difficulty quitting, causal mechanisms may involve job opportunities that depend on educational attainment. A lower education often leads to jobs characterized by low skill discretion, high psychological demands and high physical exertion, potentially leading to stress and smoking to cope (Dobson, Gilbert-Ouimet, Mustard, & Smith, Reference Dobson, Gilbert-Ouimet, Mustard and Smith2018a).

Another striking pattern was that of bi-directional, increasing effects between smoking and mental disorders. There was more robust evidence that smoking causally increases the odds of mental disorders than vice versa – most notably for depression, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia. This concurs with accumulating evidence from longitudinal cohort studies (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, McNeill, Girling, Farley, Lindson-Hawley and Aveyard2014b) and animal research (Jobson et al., Reference Jobson, Renard, Szkudlarek, Rosen, Pereira, Wright and Laviolette2019) indicating neuropsychiatric effects of smoking. A causal mechanism may be that nicotine binds to nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the brain, given that these are involved in regulating central nervous system pathways relevant to mental disorders (Berk et al., Reference Berk, Kapczinski, Andreazza, Dean, Giorlando, Maes and Malhi2011). There is some evidence that repeated nicotine exposure can lead to desensitization of these receptors (Mineur & Picciotto, Reference Mineur and Picciotto2009). Inflammation and oxidative stress induced by toxic compounds from inhaled cigarette smoke is another potential mechanism (Berk et al., Reference Berk, Kapczinski, Andreazza, Dean, Giorlando, Maes and Malhi2011). Our conclusion that smoking is detrimental to the brain warrants increased efforts to prevent (heavy) substance use. For individuals with a mental disorder, it implies that smoking cessation may be beneficial to alleviate symptoms. This is an important message given that smokers in this population are not always encouraged to quit (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Baker, Fox, Kessler, Aveyard and Munafò2020a). Although not an easy task, it should be communicated to health professionals that there are effective ways to help smokers with a co-morbid mental disorder quit.

A higher education increased alcohol use frequency but decreased the risk of problematic use. Those with higher education tend to drink alcohol more often but spread across multiple drinking occasions, and without developing a dependency. Those with lower education, on the other hand, are at increased odds of developing a problematic relationship with alcohol. This pattern of opposite effects was recently also highlighted in a study that computed genetic correlations and reported high alcohol use frequency to be genetically correlated with higher socio-economic status and lower risk of psychiatric disorders, whereas high alcohol consumption quantity was genetically correlated with lower socio-economic status and higher psychiatric disorder risk (Marees et al., Reference Marees, Smit, Ong, MacGregor, An, Denys and van den Brink2019). Similar to smoking, it could be that excessive alcohol use is a way to cope with job stress (Dobson, Ibrahim, Gilbert-Ouimet, Mustard, & Smith, Reference Dobson, Ibrahim, Gilbert-Ouimet, Mustard and Smith2018b). There was also consistent evidence that mental disorders increase (problematic) alcohol use, without strong effects in the other direction. The latter implies that observational findings indicating that alcohol use increases mental disorders were due to confounding and/or reverse causality. Indeed, associations between heavy drinking and subsequent increases in depressive symptoms disappeared after adjustment for confounders (Li et al., Reference Li, Wang, Li, Shen, Li, Zhang and Peng2020). It should be noted that this is in contrast to clinical observations where in the short-term, treating alcohol use disorder makes pre-existing depression symptoms disappear (Charlet & Heinz, Reference Charlet and Heinz2017). This discrepancy may be because MR assesses ‘lifetime’ (longer-term) effects of alcohol on mental health (Labrecque & Swanson, Reference Labrecque and Swanson2019), and the fact that only a small proportion of those with an alcohol use disorder will receive treatment [<9% (Mark, Kassed, Vandivort-Warren, Levit, & Kranzler, Reference Mark, Kassed, Vandivort-Warren, Levit and Kranzler2009)]. In sum, the current MR literature suggests that co-morbidity between poor mental health and alcohol use is primarily the result of alcohol being used as a type of ‘self-medication.’

There was stronger evidence that liability to schizophrenia increases the odds to initiate cannabis, than that cannabis initiation increases schizophrenia risk, as also indicated recently by others (Gillespie & Kendler, Reference Gillespie and Kendler2021). However, these results should be regarded as tentative, given that the genetic instrument for schizophrenia was more powerful than that for cannabis use, and more insightful analyses, with measures of cannabis use frequency, have not yet been performed. This is an important direction for future MR studies, now that such large-scale cannabis studies are becoming available (Hines, Treur, Jones, Sallis, & Munafò, Reference Hines, Treur, Jones, Sallis and Munafò2020).

For caffeine, the predominantly studied relationships were with cognitive functioning and sleep. Overall, there was no clear evidence that a high average intake of caffeine (negatively or positively) affects cognitive measures or sleep. This is consistent with recent evidence that average (high) caffeine intake does not necessarily result in changes in alertness or sleep patterns, due to the fact that adaptation occurs after repeated intake (Weibel et al., Reference Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, Kolodyazhniy, Kistler and Reichert2020).

Limitations

Although our scoring system was carefully designed [using the collective experience of the authors and the tentative, developing STROBE-MR ("Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology using Mendelian Randomization") guidelines (Davey Smith et al., Reference Davey Smith, Davies, Dimou, Egger, Gallo, Golub and Yarmolinsky2019)] it should be noted that it was not previously validated. As for the included MR studies, while the more recent were sufficiently powered and some included thorough sensitivity methods and triangulation, many earlier studies were low-quality. In the coming years, it will be important to extend and strengthen the current evidence through MR studies that combine better-powered data sets, preferably with more fine-grained phenotypes and extensive sensitivity methods. An important focal point for smoking and cannabis use as exposure variables is to not only investigate initiation, but also the heaviness of use. There is ample evidence that measurements of initiation can introduce bias due to horizontal pleiotropy and reverse causality (Khouja et al., Reference Khouja, Wootton, Taylor, Smith and Munafò2020; Li et al., Reference Li, Wang, Li, Shen, Li, Zhang and Peng2020; Treur et al., Reference Treur, Demontis, Sallis, Richardson, Wiers, Borglum and Munafo2019; Yuan, Yao, & Larsson, Reference Yuan, Yao and Larsson2020b). Another important addition to future work is multivariable MR, which allows the inclusion of multiple exposures to further decrease the risk of horizontal pleiotropy and provide more extensive testing of causal mechanisms. In addition, triangulating with high-quality observational analyses, or as was done by Davies et al. (Reference Davies, Dickson, Davey Smith, Windmeijer and van den Berg2018a) with results from policy reform, would be ideal. There are three important sources of potential bias that are not (sufficiently) accounted for in current MR studies; genetic nurturing (genetic variants that are not transmitted from parents to offspring still affecting offspring phenotype), assortative mating (spouses genetically resembling each other more than by chance because they selected each other based on a genetically influenced trait), and geographic genetic clustering (Brumpton et al., Reference Brumpton, Sanderson, Hartwig, Harrison, Vie, Cho and Bielak2020). These phenomena may re-introduce bias from potential confounders, shifting the MR estimate towards the observational association. This can be prevented by performing MR with genetic estimates from within-family GWAS, as these will be corrected for all factors shared within families. Finding large enough family samples will be an important challenge in coming years [the first of such efforts recently became available (Howe et al., Reference Howe, Nivard, Morris, Hansen, Rasheed, Cho and Davies2021). Finally, it is important to acknowledge that almost all MR studies were based on cohorts including participants of European descent. Because of the lack of diversity in the field of genetic research, genetic instruments needed to perform MR for other ethnic groups are rarely available. Increasing diversity in genetic research will be pivotal if we want to reach a comprehensive understanding of the genetic etiology of mental health and substance use, as well as the causal nature of their relationship (Abdellaoui & Verweij, Reference Abdellaoui and Verweij2021).

Conclusion

In this systematic review of MR studies, we found strong evidence that higher educational attainment decreases smoking and that there is a bi-directional, increasing relationship between smoking and (symptoms of) mental disorders (depression, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia). Another robust finding was that higher educational attainment increases alcohol use frequency, whereas it decreases the risk of binge-drinking and alcohol use problems, and that (symptoms of) mental disorders causally lead to more alcohol drinking without evidence for the reverse. Future work should attempt to tackle important limitations that were highlighted in this review. An approach that is particularly noteworthy, and should be used more routinely, is multivariable MR. The etiology of mental health traits is complex and we have only a limited understanding of the biological pathways from SNP to phenotype. It is, therefore, important to test whether key variables act as confounders (inducing a false-positive causal finding) or mediate the causal relationship (i.e. are part of the causal chain from exposure to the outcome). This is especially relevant for MR studies investigating educational attainment as an exposure (McMartin & Conley, Reference McMartin and Conley2020). Multivariable MR allows the modeling of complex networks of genetic effects linking different mental health traits. Finally, triangulation of MR results with other research methods is crucial. This includes comparison to other genetically informative methods such as twin studies, latent causal variable analysis (O'Connor & Price, Reference O'Connor and Price2018), or genomic structural equation modeling (Grotzinger et al., Reference Grotzinger, Rhemtulla, de Vlaming, Ritchie, Mallard, Hill and Tucker-Drob2019), carefully conducted longitudinal analyses of cohort data, and/or instrumental variable methods that use environmental factors (e.g. policy changes) instead of genes as an instrument.

Taken together, the current body of MR studies is a valuable addition to the literature on mental health and substance use. It has provided more robust evidence that substance use (most notably smoking) can cause mental health problems, thereby (further) strengthening the incentive to decrease substance use, particularly among populations with poor mental health.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172100180X.

Acknowledgements

MRM is a member of the MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol (MC_UU_00011/7), and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at the University Hospitals Bristol National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust, and the University of Bristol.

Financial support

JLT was supported by a Veni grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO; grant number 016.Veni.195.016). KJHV and JLT were supported by the Foundation Volksbond Rotterdam.

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have any conflicts of interest to declare, financial or otherwise.

References

Abdellaoui, A., Sanchez-Roige, S., Sealock, J., Treur, J. L., Dennis, J., Fontanillas, P., … Boomsma, D. I.. (2019). Phenome-wide investigation of health outcomes associated with genetic predisposition to loneliness. Human Molecular Genetics, 28(22), 38533865. https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddz219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abdellaoui, A., Smit, D., van den Brink, W., Denys, D., & Verweij, K. (2020). Genomic relationships across psychiatric disorders including substance use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 220, 108535. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.08.20125732.Google Scholar
Abdellaoui, A., & Verweij, K. J. H. (2021). Dissecting polygenic signals from genome-wide association studies on human behaviour. Nature Human Behaviour. Online ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01110-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, C. (2019). Mendelian randomization analysis of smoking behavior and cognitive ability on the Big five. MedRxiv, 2019.12.11.19014530. https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.11.19014530.Google Scholar
Agaku, I. T., Odani, S., Okuyemi, K. S., & Armour, B. (2020). Disparities in current cigarette smoking among US adults, 2002–2016. Tobacco Control, 29, 269276. https://doi.org/10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2019-054948.Google Scholar
Almeida, O. P., Hankey, G. J., Yeap, B. B., Golledge, J., & Flicker, L. (2014a). The triangular association of ADH1B genetic polymorphism, alcohol consumption and the risk of depression in older men. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(9), 9951000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almeida, O. P., Hankey, G. J., Yeap, B. B., Golledge, J., & Flicker, L. (2014b). Alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment in older men: A mendelian randomization study. Neurology, 82, 10381044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S. J., Fulton-Howard, B., O'Reilly, P., Marcora, E., Goate, A. M., Farrer, L. A., … Wang, L. S. (2021). Causal associations between modifiable risk factors and the Alzheimer's phenome. Annals of Neurology, 89(1), 5465. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.25918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, S. J., Goate, A., & Anstey, K. J. (2020). Association between alcohol consumption and Alzheimer's disease: A Mendelian randomization study. Alzheimer's and Dementia, 16(2), 345353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2019.09.086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audrain-McGovern, J., Leventhal, A. M., & Strong, D. R.. (2015). The role of depression in the uptake and maintenance of cigarette smoking. International Review of Neurobiology, 124, 209243. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.irn.2015.07.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au Yeung, S. L., Jiang, C. Q., Cheng, K. K., Liu, B., Zhang, W. S., Lam, T. H., … Schooling, C. M. (2012). Evaluation of moderate alcohol use and cognitive function among men using a mendelian randomization design in the Guangzhou biobank cohort study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 175(10), 10211028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, A. L., Thornton, L. K., Hiles, S., Hides, L., & Lubman, D. I. (2012). Psychological interventions for alcohol misuse among people with co-occurring depression or anxiety disorders: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 139, 217229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.08.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkhuizen, W., Dudbridge, F., & Ronald, A. (2020). Genetic overlap and causal associations between smoking behaviours and psychiatric traits and disorders in adolescents and adults. MedRxiv, 2020.02.07.20021089. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.07.20021089.Google Scholar
Berk, M., Kapczinski, F., Andreazza, A. C., Dean, O. M., Giorlando, F., Maes, M., … Malhi, G. S. (2011). Pathways underlying neuroprogression in bipolar disorder: Focus on inflammation, oxidative stress and neurotrophic factors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 804817. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorngaard, J. H., Gunnell, D., Elvestad, M. B., Smith, G. D., Skorpen, F., Krokan, H., … Romundstad, P. (2013). The causal role of smoking in anxiety and depression: A Mendelian randomization analysis of the HUNT study. Psychological Medicine, 43(4), 711719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brumpton, B., Sanderson, E., Hartwig, F. P., Harrison, S., Vie, G. Å., Cho, Y., … Bielak, L. (2020). Within-family studies for Mendelian randomization: Avoiding dynastic, assortative mating, and population stratification biases. Nature Communications, 11, 3519. https://doi.org/10.1101/602516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, S., Scott, R. A., Timpson, N. J., Davey Smith, G., & Thompson, S. G., & EPIC- InterAct Consortium. (2015). Using published data in Mendelian randomization: A blueprint for efficient identification of causal risk factors. European Journal of Epidemiology, 30(7), 543552. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-015-0011-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, S., Small, D. S., & Thompson, S. G. (2017). A review of instrumental variable estimators for Mendelian randomization. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(5), 23332355. https://doi.org/10.1177/0962280215597579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, E. M., Ferreira, M. A. R., Xue, A., Lindström, S., Jiang, X., Yang, J., … Chenevix-Trench, G. (2019). Is schizophrenia a risk factor for breast cancer?-evidence from genetic data. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(6), 12511256. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campos, M. W., Serebrisky, D., & Castaldelli-Maia, J. M. (2016). Smoking and cognition. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 9(2), 7679. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874473709666160803101633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, A. R., Gill, D., Davies, N. M., Taylor, A. E., Tillmann, T., Vaucher, J., … Dehghan, A. (2019). Understanding the consequences of education inequality on cardiovascular disease: Mendelian randomisation study. The BMJ, 365, l1855. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l1855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chao, M., Li, X., & McGue, M. (2017). The causal role of alcohol use in adolescent externalizing and internalizing problems: A Mendelian randomization study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 41(11), 19531960. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlet, K., & Heinz, A. (2017). Harm reduction—a systematic review on effects of alcohol reduction on physical and mental symptoms. Addiction Biology, 22(5), 11191159. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelis, M. C., Byrne, E. M., Esko, T., Nalls, M. A., Ganna, A., Paynter, N., … Chasman, D. I. (2015). Genome-wide meta-analysis identifies six novel loci associated with habitual coffee consumption. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(5), 647656. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelis, M., Kacprowski, T., Menni, C., Gustafsson, S., Pivin, E., Adamski, J., … Ingelsson, E. (2016). Genome-wide association study of caffeine metabolites provides new insights to caffeine metabolism and dietary caffeine-consumption behavior. Human Molecular Genetics, 25(24), ddw334. https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddw334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelis, M. C., Weintraub, S., & Morris, M. C. (2020). Caffeinated coffee and tea consumption, genetic variation and cognitive function in the UK biobank. Journal of Nutrition, 150(8), 21642174. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/nxaa147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curran, H. V., Freeman, T. P., Mokrysz, C., Lewis, D. A., Morgan, C. J. A., & Parsons, L. H. (2016). Keep off the grass? Cannabis, cognition and addiction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, 293306. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahne, J., Murphy, J. G., & MacPherson, L. (2017). Depressive symptoms and cigarette demand as a function of induced stress. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 19(1), 4958. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntw145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davey Smith, G., Davies, N. M., Dimou, N., Egger, M., Gallo, V., Golub, R., … Yarmolinsky, J. (2019). STROBE-MR: Guidelines for strengthening the reporting of Mendelian randomization studies. https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.27857v1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, N., Dickson, M., Davey Smith, G., Windmeijer, F., & van den Berg, G. (2018a). The effect of education on adult mortality, health, and income: Triangulating across genetic and policy reforms. BioRxiv, 250068. https://doi.org/10.1101/250068.Google Scholar
Davies, NM, Hill, W. D., Anderson, E. L., Sanderson, E., Deary, I. J., & Smith, G. D. (2019). Multivariable two-sample mendelian randomization estimates of the effects of intelligence and education on health. ELife, 8, e43990. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.43990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, NM, Holmes, M. V, & Davey Smith, G. (2018b). Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: A guide, glossary, and checklist for clinicians. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 362, k601. https://doi.org/10.1136/BMJ.K601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, G., Marioni, R. E., Liewald, D. C., Hill, W. D., Hagenaars, S. P., Harris, S. E., … Deary, I. J. (2016). Genome-wide association study of cognitive functions and educational attainment in UK biobank (N = 112 151). Molecular Psychiatry, 21(6), 758767. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, L., Powell, J. H., Pickering, A., Powell, J., & West, R. (2009). Patterns of change in withdrawal symptoms, desire to smoke, reward motivation and response inhibition across 3 months of smoking abstinence. Addiction, 104(5), 850858. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02522.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., … Neale, .B M. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 6375. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ding, X., Barban, N., & Mills, M. C. (2019). Educational attainment and allostatic load in later life: Evidence using genetic markers. Preventive Medicine, 129, 105866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.105866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiPrete, T. A., Burik, C. A. P., & Koellinger, P. D. (2018). Genetic instrumental variable regression: Explaining socioeconomic and health outcomes in nonexperimental data. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(22), E4970E4979. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1707388115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobson, K. G., Gilbert-Ouimet, M., Mustard, C. A., & Smith, P. M. (2018a). Association between dimensions of the psychosocial and physical work environment and latent smoking trajectories: A 16-year cohort study of the Canadian workforce. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 75(11), 814821. https://doi.org/10.1136/oemed-2018-105138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobson, K. G., Ibrahim, S., Gilbert-Ouimet, M., Mustard, C. A., & Smith, P. M. (2018b). Association between psychosocial work conditions and latent alcohol consumption trajectories among men and women over a 16-year period in a national Canadian sample. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 72(2), 113120. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2017-209691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elkins, I. J., Saunders, G. R. B., Malone, S. M., Keyes, M. A., Samek, D. R., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2018). Increased risk of smoking in female adolescents who had childhood ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(1), 6370. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsworth, B., Mitchell, R., Raistrick, C., Paternoster, L., Hemani, G., & Gaunt, T. (2017). MRC IEU UK Biobank GWAS pipeline version 1. Bristol, UK: University of Bristol.Google Scholar
Firth, J., Solmi, M., Wootton, R. E., Vancampfort, D., Schuch, F. B., Hoare, E., … Stubbs, B. (2020). A meta-review of “lifestyle psychiatry”: The role of exercise, smoking, diet and sleep in the prevention and treatment of mental disorders. World Psychiatry, 19(3), 360380. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fluharty, M. E., Sallis, H., & Munafò, M. R. (2018). Investigating possible causal effects of externalizing behaviors on tobacco initiation: A Mendelian randomization analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 191, 338342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.07.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, M., Faul, J., Jin, Y., Ware, E., & Bakulski, K. (2019). Mendelian randomization of smoking behavior on cognitive status among older Americans. MedRxiv, 2019.12.11.19014522. https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.11.19014522.Google Scholar
Gage, S. H., Bowden, J., Davey Smith, G., & Munafo, M. R. (2018). Investigating causality in associations between education and smoking: A two-sample Mendelian randomization study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 47(4), 11311140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, S. H., Hickman, M., & Zammit, S. (2016a). Association between cannabis and psychosis: Epidemiologic evidence. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 549556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gage, S. H., Jones, H. J., Burgess, S., Bowden, J., Davey Smith, G., Zammit, S., & Munafò, M. R. (2017a). Assessing causality in associations between cannabis use and schizophrenia risk: A two-sample Mendelian randomization study. Psychological Medicine, 47(5), 971980. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gage, S. H., Jones, H. J., Taylor, A. E., Burgess, S., Zammit, S., & Munafò, M. R. (2017b). Investigating causality in associations between smoking initiation and schizophrenia using Mendelian randomization. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep40653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gage, S. H., Munafò, M. R., & Davey Smith, G. (2016b). Causal inference in Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) research. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 567585. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gage, S. H., Sallis, H. M., Lassi, G., Wootton, R. E., Mokrysz, C., Davey Smith, G., … Munafò, M. R. (2020). Does smoking cause lower educational attainment and general cognitive ability? Triangulation of causal evidence using multiple study designs. Psychological Medicine. Online ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720003402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galindo, M. N., Navarro, J. F., & Cavas, M. (2020). The influence of placebo effect on craving and cognitive performance in alcohol, caffeine, or nicotine consumers: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 849. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garey, L., Olofsson, H., Garza, T., Shepherd, J. M., Smit, T., & Zvolensky, M. J. (2020). The role of anxiety in smoking onset, severity, and cessation-related outcomes: A review of recent literature. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22(8), 38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-01160-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, M., Winsper, C., Marwaha, S., Gilbert, E., Broome, M., & Singh, S. P. (2015). Cannabis use and mania symptoms: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 171, 3947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, M., Munafò, M. R., Taylor, A. E., & Treur, J. L. (2019). Evidence for genetic correlations and bidirectional, causal effects between smoking and sleep behaviors. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 21(6), 731738. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/nty230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillespie, N. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2021). Use of genetically informed methods to clarify the nature of the association between Cannabis Use and risk for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(5), 467468. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenland, S., Satterfield, M. H., & Lanes, S. F. (1998). A meta-analysis to assess the incidence of adverse effects associated with the transdermal nicotine patch. Drug Safety, 18(4), 297308. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9565740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groenman, A. P., Janssen, T. W. P., & Oosterlaan, J. (2017). Childhood psychiatric disorders as risk factor for subsequent substance abuse: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56, 556569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosso, G., Micek, A., Castellano, S., Pajak, A., & Galvano, F. (2016). Coffee, tea, caffeine and risk of depression: A systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of observational studies. Molecular Nutrition and Food Research, 60(1), 223234. https://doi.org/10.1002/mnfr.201500620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grotzinger, A. D., Rhemtulla, M., de Vlaming, R., Ritchie, S. J., Mallard, T. T., Hill, W. D., … Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2019). Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 513525. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0566-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamidovic, A., & de Wit, H. (2009). Sleep deprivation increases cigarette smoking. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93(3), 263269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2008.12.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammerschlag, A. R., Stringer, S., De Leeuw, C. A., Sniekers, S., Taskesen, E., Watanabe, K., … Posthuma, D. (2017). Genome-wide association analysis of insomnia complaints identifies risk genes and genetic overlap with psychiatric and metabolic traits. Nat Genet, 49, 15841592. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, S., Davies, A. R., Dickson, M., Tyrrell, J., Green, M. J., Katikireddi, S. V., … Howe, L. D. (2020b). The causal effects of health conditions and risk factors on social and socioeconomic outcomes: Mendelian randomization in UK Biobank. International Journal of Epidemiology, 49(5), 16611681. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyaa114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, R., Munafò, M. R., Davey Smith, G., & Wootton, R. E. (2020a). Examining the effect of smoking on suicidal ideation and attempts: Triangulation of epidemiological approaches. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 217(6), 701707. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, W. D., Marioni, R. E., Maghzian, O., Ritchie, S. J., Hagenaars, S. P., Mcintosh, A. M., … Deary, I. J. (2019). A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(2), 169181. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hines, L. A., Treur, J. L., Jones, H. J., Sallis, H. M., & Munafò, M. R. (2020). Using genetic information to inform policy on cannabis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(12), P1002-1003. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30377-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodgson, K., Coleman, J. R. I., Hagenaars, S. P., Purves, K. L., Glanville, K., Choi, S. W., … Lewis, C. M. (2020). Cannabis use, depression and self-harm: Phenotypic and genetic relationships. Addiction, 115(3), 482492. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. M., Adams, M. J., Clarke, T. K., Hafferty, J. D., Gibson, J., Shirali, M., … Mcintosh, A. M. (2019). Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 22(3), 343352. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0326-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howe, L. J., Nivard, M. G., Morris, T. T., Hansen, A. F., Rasheed, H., Cho, Y., … Davies, N. M. (2021). Within-sibship GWAS improve estimates of direct genetic effects. BioRxiv, 2021.03.05.433935. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.03.05.433935.Google Scholar
Huang, K. L., Marcora, E., Pimenova, A. A., Di Narzo, A. F., Kapoor, M., Jin, S. C., ... Goate, A. M.. (2017). A common haplotype lowers PU.1 expression in myeloid cells and delays onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature Neuroscience, 20(8), 10521061. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irons, D. E., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Oetting, W. S. (2007). Mendelian randomization: A novel test of the gateway hypothesis and models of gene-environment interplay. Special Issue: Gene-Environment Interaction, 19(4), 11811195.Google ScholarPubMed
Irwin, C., Khalesi, S., Desbrow, B., & McCartney, D. (2020). Effects of acute caffeine consumption following sleep loss on cognitive, physical, occupational and driving performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 877888. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, P. R., Watanabe, K., Stringer, S., Skene, N., Bryois, J., Hammerschlag, A. R., … Posthuma, D. (2019). Genome-wide analysis of insomnia in 1 331 010 individuals identifies new risk loci and functional pathways. Nature Genetics, 51(3), 394403. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0333-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jobson, C. L. M., Renard, J., Szkudlarek, H., Rosen, L. G., Pereira, B., Wright, D. J., … Laviolette, S. R. (2019). Adolescent nicotine exposure induces dysregulation of mesocorticolimbic activity states and depressive and anxiety-like prefrontal cortical molecular phenotypes persisting into adulthood. Cerebral Cortex, 29(7), 31403153. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. E., Tyrrell, J., Wood, A. R., Beaumont, R. N., Ruth, K. S., Tuke, M. A., … Weedon, M. N. (2016). Genome-wide association analyses in 128,266 individuals identifies new morningness and sleep duration loci. PLoS Genet, 12(8), e1006125. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1006125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorgenson, E., Thai, K. K., Hoffmann, T. J., Sakoda, L. C., Kvale, M. N., Banda, Y., … Choquet, H. (2017). Genetic contributors to variation in alcohol consumption vary by race/ethnicity in a large multi-ethnic genome-wide association study. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(9), 13591367. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson Linnér, R., Biroli, P., Kong, E., Meddens, S. F. W., Wedow, R., Fontana, M. A., … Wagner, G. G. (2019). Genome-wide association analyses of risk tolerance and risky behaviors in over 1 million individuals identify hundreds of loci and shared genetic influences. Nature Genetics, 51(2), 245257. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0309-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katikireddi, S. V., Green, M. J., Taylor, A. E., Davey Smith, G., & Munafò, M. R. (2018). Assessing causal relationships using genetic proxies for exposures: An introduction to Mendelian randomization. Addiction, 113(4), 764774. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, K. M., Pratt, C., Galea, S., McLaughlin, K. A., Koenen, K. C., & Shear, M. K. (2014). The burden of loss: Unexpected death of a loved one and psychiatric disorders across the life course in a national study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(8), 864871. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13081132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khouja, J. N., Wootton, R. E., Taylor, A. E., Smith, G. D., & Munafò, M. R. (2020). Association of genetic liability to smoking initiation with e-cigarette use in young adults. MedRxiv, 2020.06.10.20127464. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.10.20127464.Google Scholar
Kraft, P., Chen, H., & Lindström, S. (2020). The use of genetic correlation and Mendelian randomization studies to increase our understanding of relationships between Complex traits. Current Epidemiology Reports, 7(2), 104112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40471-020-00233-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumari, M., Holmes, M. V, Dale, C. E., Hubacek, J. A., Palmer, T. M., Pikhart, H., … Bobak, M. (2014). Alcohol consumption and cognitive performance: A Mendelian randomization study. Addiction, 109(9), 14621471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kunkle, B. W., Grenier-Boley, B., Sims, R., Bis, J. C., Damotte, V., Naj, A. C., … Pericak-Vance, M. A. (2019). Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity and lipid processing. Nature Genetics, 51(3), 414430. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0358-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwok, M. K., Leung, G. M., & Schooling, C. M. (2016). Habitual coffee consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes, ischemic heart disease, depression and Alzheimer's disease: A Mendelian randomization study. Scientific Reports, 6(101563288), 36500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labrecque, J. A., & Swanson, S. A. (2019). Interpretation and potential biases of Mendelian randomization estimates with time-varying exposures. American Journal of Epidemiology, 188(1), 231238. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwy204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, J. C., Ibrahim-Verbaas, C. A., Harold, D., Naj, A. C., Sims, R., Bellenguez, C., … Seshadri, S. (2013). Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer’s disease. Nature Genetics, 45(12), 14521458. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.2802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, J. M., Jones, S. E., Dashti, H. S., Wood, A. R., Aragam, K. G., van Hees, V. T., … Saxena, R. (2019). Biological and clinical insights from genetics of insomnia symptoms. Nature Genetics, 51, 387393. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0361-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, S. C., Traylor, M., Malik, R., Dichgans, M., Burgess, S., & Markus, H. S. (2017). Modifiable pathways in Alzheimer's disease: Mendelian randomisation analysis. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 359, j5375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawlor, D. A., Harbord, R. M., Sterne, J. A. C., Timpson, N., & Davey Smith, G. (2008). Mendelian randomization: Using genes as instruments for making causal inferences in epidemiology. Statistics in Medicine, 27(8), 11331163. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.3034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J. J., Wedow, R., Okbay, A., Kong, E., Maghzian, O., Zacher, M., … Turley, P. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50(8), 11121121. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppert, B., Riglin, L., Dardani, C., Thapar, A., Staley, J., Tilling, K., … Stergiakouli, E. (2019). ADHD Genetic liability and physical health outcomes – A two-sample Mendelian randomization study. BioRxiv, 630467. https://doi.org/10.1101/630467.Google Scholar
Lewis, S. J., Araya, R., Smith, G. D., Freathy, R., Gunnell, D., Palmer, T., … Munafo, M. (2011). Smoking is associated with, but does not cause, depressed mood in pregnancy-A Mendelian randomization study. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e21689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, J., Wang, H., Li, M., Shen, Q., Li, X., Zhang, Y., … Peng, Y. (2020). Effect of alcohol use disorders and alcohol intake on the risk of subsequent depressive symptoms: A systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Addiction, 115, 12241243. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, K. X., Rijsdijk, F., Hagenaars, S. P., Socrates, A., Choi, S. W., Coleman, J. R. I., … Pingault, J. B. (2020). Studying individual risk factors for self-harm in the UK Biobank: A polygenic scoring and Mendelian randomisation study. PLoS Medicine, 17(6), e1003137. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, M., Jiang, Y., Wedow, R., Li, Y., Brazel, D. M., Chen, F., … Vrieze, S. (2019a). Association studies of up to 1.2 million individuals yield new insights into the genetic etiology of tobacco and alcohol use. Nature Genetics, 51, 237244. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0307-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., de Graaf, Y., Ames, S. L., Baldacchino, A., , R., … Wiers, R. W. (2019b). Is (poly-) substance use associated with impaired inhibitory control? A mega-analysis controlling for confounders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 105, 288304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lo, M. T., Hinds, D. A., Tung, J. Y., Franz, C., Fan, C. C., Wang, Y., … Chen, C. H. (2017). Genome-wide analyses for personality traits identify six genomic loci and show correlations with psychiatric disorders. Nature Genetics, 49(1), 152156. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahabee-Gittens, E. M., Xiao, Y., Gordon, J. S., & Khoury, J. C. (2013). The dynamic role of parental influences in preventing adolescent smoking initiation. Addictive Behaviors, 38(4), 19051911. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.01.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahedy, L., Suddell, S., Skirrow, C., Fernandes, G. S., Field, M., Heron, J., … Munafò, M. R. (2020). Alcohol use and cognitive functioning in young adults: Improving causal inference. Addiction, 116(2), 292302. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahedy, L., Wootton, R., Suddell, S., Caroline, S., Heron, J., Hickman, M., … Munafò, M. R. (2021). Testing the association between tobacco and cannabis use and cognitive functioning: Findings from an observational and Mendelian randomization study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 221, 108591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.108591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marees, A. T., Smit, D. J. A., Ong, J.-S., MacGregor, S., An, J., Denys, D., … van den Brink, W. D. E. (2019). Potential influence of socio-economic status on genetic correlations between alcohol consumption measures and mental health. Psychological Medicine, 15, 115.Google Scholar
Mark, T. L., Kassed, C. A., Vandivort-Warren, R., Levit, K. R., & Kranzler, H. R. (2009). Alcohol and opioid dependence medications: Prescription trends, overall and by physician specialty. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 99(1–3), 345349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.07.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathew, A. R., Hogarth, L., Leventhal, A. M., Cook, J. W., & Hitsman, B. (2017). Cigarette smoking and depression comorbidity: Systematic review and proposed theoretical model. Addiction, 112, 401412. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., Keyes, M., Sharma, A., Elkins, I, Legrand, L., Johnson, W., … Iacono, W. G.. (2007). The environments of adopted and non-adopted youth: Evidence on range restriction from the Sibling Interaction and Behavior Study (SIBS). Behavior Genetics, 37(3), 449462. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-007-9142-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGue, M., Osler, M., & Christensen, K. (2010). Causal inference and observational research: The utility of twins. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 5(5), 546556. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610383511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMartin, A., & Conley, D. (2020). Commentary: Mendelian randomization and education–challenges remain. International Journal of Epidemiology, 49(4), 11931206. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyaa160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millard, L. A. C., Munafò, M. R., Tilling, K., Wootton, R. E., & Davey Smith, G. (2019). MR-pheWAS with stratification and interaction: Searching for the causal effects of smoking heaviness identified an effect on facial aging. PLoS Genetics, 15(10), e1008353. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mineur, Y. S., & Picciotto, M. R. (2009). Biological basis for the co-morbidity between smoking and mood disorders. Journal of Dual Diagnosis, 5, 122130. https://doi.org/10.1080/15504260902869964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagel, M., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., De Leeuw, C. A., Bryois, J., … Posthuma, D. (2018). Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism in 449,484 individuals identifies novel genetic loci and pathways. Nature Genetics, 50(7), 920927. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0151-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieman, D. H., Chavez-Baldini, U., Vulink, N. C., Smit, D. J. A., Van Wingen, G., De Koning, P., … Denys, D. (2020). Protocol across study: Longitudinal transdiagnostic cognitive functioning, psychiatric symptoms, and biological parameters in patients with a psychiatric disorder. BMC Psychiatry, 20(1), 212. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02624-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishiyama, T., Nakatochi, M., Goto, A., Iwasaki, M., Hachiya, T., Sutoh, Y., … Suzuki, S. (2019). Genome-wide association meta-analysis and Mendelian randomization analysis confirm the influence of ALDH2 on sleep durationin the Japanese population. Sleep, 42(6), zsz046. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsz046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
North, T.-L., Palmer, T. M., Lewis, S. J., Cooper, R., Power, C., Pattie, A., … Day, I. N. M. (2015). Effect of smoking on physical and cognitive capability in later life: A multicohort study using observational and genetic approaches. BMJ Open, 5(12), e008393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, L. J., & Price, A. L. (2018). Distinguishing genetic correlation from causation across 52 diseases and complex traits. Nature Genetics, 50(12), 17281734. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0255-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okbay, A., Beauchamp, J. P., Fontana, M. A., Lee, J. J., Pers, T. H., Rietveld, C. A., … Benjamin, D. J. (2016). Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature, 533(7604), 539542. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature17671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortega-Alonso, A., Ekelund, J., Sarin, A. P., Miettunen, J., Veijola, J., Järvelin, M. R., … Hennah, W. (2017). Genome-Wide Association Study of Psychosis Proneness in the Finnish Population. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(6), 13041314. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Østergaard, S. D., Mukherjee, S., Sharp, S. J., Proitsi, P., Lotta, L. A., Day, F., … Wareham, N. J. (2015). Associations between potentially modifiable risk factors and Alzheimer disease: A Mendelian randomization study. PLoS Medicine, 12(6), e1001841. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pain, O., Dudbridge, F., Cardno, A. G., Freeman, D., Lu, Y., Lundstrom, S., … Ronald, A. (2018). Genome-wide analysis of adolescent psychotic-like experiences shows genetic overlap with psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 177(4), 416425. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panza, F., Solfrizzi, V., Barulli, M. R., Bonfiglio, C., Guerra, V., Osella, A., … Logroscino, G. (2015). Coffee, tea, and caffeine consumption and prevention of late-life cognitive decline and dementia: A systematic review. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 19(3), 313328. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-014-0563-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pappa, I., St Pourcain, B., Benke, K., Cavadino, A., Hakulinen, C., Nivard, M. G., … Tiemeier, H. (2016). A genome-wide approach to children’s aggressive behavio: The EAGLE consortium. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(5), 562572. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pasman, J. A., Verweij, K. J. H., Gerring, Z., Stringer, S., Sanchez-Roige, S., Treur, J. L., … Vink, J. M. (2018). GWAS Of lifetime cannabis use reveals new risk loci, genetic overlap with psychiatric traits, and a causal influence of schizophrenia. Nature Neuroscience, 21(9), 11611170. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0206-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patte, K. A., Qian, W., & Leatherdale, S. T. (2018). Modifiable predictors of insufficient sleep durations: A longitudinal analysis of youth in the COMPASS study. Preventive Medicine, 106, 164170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.10.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polderman, T. J. C., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702709. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polimanti, R., Peterson, R. E., Ong, J. S., MacGregor, S., Edwards, A. C., Clarke, T. K., … Derks, E. M. (2019). Evidence of causal effect of major depression on alcohol dependence: Findings from the psychiatric genomics consortium. Psychological Medicine, 49(7), 12181226. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C., Corley, J., McNeill, G., Davies, G., Liewald, D. C., … Deary, I. J. (2014). Alcohol consumption and lifetime change in cognitive ability: A gene × environment interaction study. Age, 36(3), 14931502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosoff, D. B., Clarke, T. K., Adams, M. J., McIntosh, A. M., Davey Smith, G., Jung, J., … Lohoff, F. W. (2019). Educational attainment impacts drinking behaviors and risk for alcohol dependence: Results from a two-sample Mendelian randomization study with ~780000 participants. Molecular Psychiatry, 26, 111911132. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0535-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sallis, H. M., Croft, J., Havdahl, A., Jones, H. J., Dunn, E. C., Davey Smith, G., … Munafò, M. R. (2020). Genetic liability to schizophrenia is associated with exposure to traumatic events in childhood. Psychological Medicine. Online ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sallis, H. M., Smith, G. D., & Munafo, M. R. (2019). Cigarette smoking and personality: Investigating causality using Mendelian randomization. Psychological Medicine, 25, 19. https://doi.org/10.1101/246181.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Roige, S., Palmer, A. A., Fontanillas, P., Elson, S. L., Adams, M. J., Howard, D. M., … Wilson, C. H.. (2019). Genome-wide association study meta-analysis of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT) in two population-based cohorts. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(2), 107118. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18040369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanderson, E., Davey Smith, G., Bowden, J., & Munafò, M. R. (2019). Mendelian randomisation analysis of the effect of educational attainment and cognitive ability on smoking behaviour. Nature Communications, 10(1), 2949. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-10679-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satre, D. D., Bahorik, A., Zaman, T., & Ramo, D. (2018). Psychiatric disorders and comorbid Cannabis Use. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 79(5), 18ac12267. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.18ac12267.Google ScholarPubMed
Savage, J. E., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., Bryois, J., De Leeuw, C. A., … Posthuma, D. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics, 50(7), 912919. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Setién-Suero, E., Suárez-Pinilla, P., Ferro, A., Tabarés-Seisdedos, R., Crespo-Facorro, B., & Ayesa-Arriola, R. (2020). Childhood trauma and substance use underlying psychosis: A systematic review. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1748342. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2020.1748342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons-Morton, B. G. (2002). Prospective analysis of peer and parent influences on smoking initiation among early adolescents. Prevention Science, 3(4), 275283. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020876625045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skov-Ettrup, L. S., Nordestgaard, B. G., Petersen, C. B., & Tolstrup, J. S. (2017). Does high tobacco consumption cause psychological distress? A Mendelian randomization study. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 19(1), 3238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soler Artigas, M., Sánchez-Mora, C., Rovira, P., Richarte, V., Garcia-Martínez, I., Pagerols, M., … Ribasés, M. (2019). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and lifetime cannabis use: Genetic overlap and causality. Molecular Psychiatry, 25, 24932503. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0339-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahl, E. A., Breen, G., Forstner, A. J., Mcquillin, A., Ripke, S., Trubetskoy, V., … Sklar, P. (2019). Genome-wide association study identifies 30 loci associated with bipolar disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(5), 793803. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0397-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephen Rich, J., & Martin, P. R. (2014). Co-occurring psychiatric disorders and alcoholism. In Sullivan, E. V., & Pfefferbaum, A. (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 125, pp. 573588). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-62619-6.00033-1.Google Scholar
Strid, C., Hallgren, M., Forsell, Y., Kraepelien, M., & Öjehagen, A. (2019). Changes in alcohol consumption after treatment for depression: A secondary analysis of the Swedish randomised controlled study REGASSA. BMJ Open, 9(11), e028236. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-028236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stringer, S., Minică, C. C., Verweij, K. J. H., Mbarek, H., Bernard, M., Derringer, J., … Vink, J. M. (2016). Genome-wide association study of lifetime cannabis use based on a large meta-analytic sample of 32 330 subjects from the International Cannabis Consortium. Translational Psychiatry, 6(3), e769. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, G. M. J., Baker, A. L., Fox, N., Kessler, D. S., Aveyard, P., & Munafò, M. R. (2020a). Addressing concerns about smoking cessation and mental health: Theoretical review and practical guide for healthcare professionals. BJPsych Advances, 27, 8595. https://doi.org/10.1192/bja.2020.52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. E., Fluharty, M. E., Bjørngaard, J. H., Gabrielsen, M. E., Skorpen, F., Marioni, R. E., … Munafò, M. R. (2014a). Investigating the possible causal association of smoking with depression and anxiety using Mendelian randomisation meta-analysis: The CARTA consortium. BMJ Open, 4(10), e006141. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, G., McNeill, A., Girling, A., Farley, A., Lindson-Hawley, N., & Aveyard, P. (2014b). Change in mental health after smoking cessation: Systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ, 348, g1151. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.g1151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, G. M., Sawyer, K., Kessler, D., Munafo, M., Aveyard, P., & Shaw, A. (2020b). Views about integrating smoking cessation treatment within psychological services for patients with common mental illness: A multi-perspective qualitative study. MedRxiv, 2020.02.18.20024596. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.18.20024596.Google Scholar
Tillmann, T., Vaucher, J., Okbay, A., Pikhart, H., Peasey, A., Kubinova, R., … Holmes, M. V. (2017). Education and coronary heart disease: Mendelian randomisation study. BMJ (Online), 358, j3542. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j3542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tobacco and Genetics Consortium, . (2010). Genome-wide meta-analyses identify multiple loci associated with smoking behavior. Nature Genetics, 42(5), 441447. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topiwala, A., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2018). Effects of drinking on late-life brain and cognition. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 21(1), 1215. https://doi.org/10.1136/eb-2017-102820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treur, J. L., Demontis, D., Sallis, H., Richardson, T., Wiers, R., Borglum, A., … Munafo, M. (2019). Investigating causal pathways between liability to ADHD and substance use, and liability to substance use and ADHD risk, using Mendelian randomization. Addiction Biology, 26(1), e12849. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12849.Google ScholarPubMed
Treur, J. L., Gibson, M., Taylor, A. E., Rogers, P. J., Munafo, M. R., & Munafò, M. R. (2018). Investigating genetic correlations and causal effects between caffeine consumption and sleep behaviours. Journal of Sleep Research, 27(5), e12695. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treur, J. L., Willemsen, G., Bartels, M., Geels, L. M., van Beek, J. H. D. A., Huppertz, C., … Vink, J. (2015). Smoking during adolescence as a risk factor for attention problems. Biological Psychiatry, 78(9), 656663. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2014.06.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twomey, C. D. (2017). Association of cannabis use with the development of elevated anxiety symptoms in the general population: A meta-analysis. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 71, 811816. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2016-208145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Amsterdam, J., van der Velde, B., Schulte, M., & van den Brink, W. (2018). Causal factors of increased smoking in ADHD: A systematic review. Substance Use & Misuse, 53(3), 432445. https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2017.1334066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaucher, J., Keating, B. J., Lasserre, A. M., Gan, W., Lyall, D. M., Ward, J., … Holmes, M. V. (2018). Cannabis use and risk of schizophrenia: A Mendelian randomization study. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5), 12871292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeulen, J. M., Schirmbeck, F., Blankers, M., Van Tricht, M., Bruggeman, R., Van Den Brink, W., … Van Winkel, R. (2018). Association between smoking behavior and cognitive functioning in patients with psychosis, siblings, and healthy control subjects: Results from a prospective 6-year follow-up study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11211128. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18010069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeulen, J. M., Wootton, R. E., Treur, J. L., Sallis, H. M., Jones, H. J., Zammit, S., … Munafò, M. R. (2019). Smoking and the risk for bipolar disorder: Evidence from a bidirectional Mendelian randomisation study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 218(2), 8894. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vink, J., & Schellekens, A. (2018). Relating addiction and psychiatric disorders. Science (New York, N.Y.), 361, 13231324. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav3928.Google ScholarPubMed
Vink, J. M., Willemsen, G., & Boomsma, D. I. (2005). Heritability of smoking initiation and nicotine dependence. Behavior Genetics, 35(4), 397406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-004-1327-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakai, K, Hamajima, N, Okada, R, Naito, M, Morita, E, Hishida, A, … Tanaka, H. (2011). Profile of participants and genotype distributions of 108 polymorphisms in a cross-sectional study of associations of genotypes with lifestyle and clinical factors: A project in the Japan Multi-Institutional Collaborative Cohort (J-MICC) study. Journal of Epidemiology, 21(3), 223235. https://doi.org/10.2188/jea.JE20100139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, Z., Gonzalez, R., Crosby, K., S. Thiessen, M., Carroll, C., & Bonn-Miller, M. O. (2017). Medical cannabis and mental health: A guided systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 51, 1529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, R. K., Polimanti, R., Johnson, E. C., Mcclintick, J. N., Adams, M. J., Adkins, A. E., … Agrawal, A. (2018). Transancestral GWAS of alcohol dependence reveals common genetic underpinnings with psychiatric disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 21(12), 16561669. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0275-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, J. J., Chen, X., Vink, J., Loukola, A., Minica, C., Pool, R., … Munafò, M. R. (2016). Genome-wide meta-analysis of cotinine levels in cigarette smokers identifies locus at 4q13.2. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 20092. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weibel, J., Lin, Y. S., Landolt, H. P., Garbazza, C., Kolodyazhniy, V., Kistler, J., … Reichert, C. F. (2020). Caffeine-dependent changes of sleep-wake regulation: Evidence for adaptation after repeated intake. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 99, 109851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. M., & Gandhi, K. K. (2008). Use of caffeine and nicotine in people with schizophrenia. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 1, 155161. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874473710801020155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H. U., Fröhlich, C., Behrendt, S., Günther, A., Rehm, J., Zimmermann, P., … Perkonigg, A. (2007). Cannabis use and cannabis use disorders and their relationship to mental disorders: A 10-year prospective-longitudinal community study in adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88(Suppl 1), S60S70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wium-Andersen, M. K., Orsted, D. D., & Nordestgaard, B. G. (2015a). Tobacco smoking is causally associated with antipsychotic medication use and schizophrenia, but not with antidepressant medication use or depression. International Journal of Epidemiology, 44(2), 566577. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyv090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wium-Andersen, M. K., Orsted, D. D., Tolstrup, J. S., & Nordestgaard, B. G. (2015b). Increased alcohol consumption as a cause of alcoholism, without similar evidence for depression: A Mendelian randomization study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 44(2), 526539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wootton, R. E., Greenstone, H. S. R., Abdellaoui, A., Denys, D., Verweij, K. J. H., Munafò, M. R., … Treur, J. L. (2020). Bidirectional effects between loneliness, smoking and alcohol use: Evidence from a Mendelian randomization study. Addiction, 116(2), 400406. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wootton, R. E., Richmond, R. C., Stuijfzand, B. G., Lawn, R. B., Sallis, H. M., Taylor, G. M. J., … Munafò, M. R. (2019). Evidence for causal effects of lifetime smoking on risk for depression and schizophrenia: A Mendelian randomisation study. Psychological Medicine, 50(14), 24352443. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291719002678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Ripke, S., Mattheisen, M., Trzaskowski, M., Byrne, E. M., Abdellaoui, A., … Sullivan, P. F. (2018). Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nature Genetics, 50(5), 668681. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, X., Tian, F., Zhang, H., Zeng, J., Chen, T., Wang, S., … Gong, Q. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter shrinkage in alcohol-use disorders: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 66, 92103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, S., Xiong, Y., Michaëlsson, M., Michaëlsson, K., & Larsson, S. C. (2020a). Health-related effects of education level: A Mendelian randomization study. MedRxiv, 2020.02.01.20020008. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.01.20020008.Google Scholar
Yuan, S., Yao, H., & Larsson, S. C. (2020b). Associations of cigarette smoking with psychiatric disorders: Evidence from a two-sample Mendelian randomization study. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 13807. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70458-4.. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70458-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeng, L., Ntalla, I., Kessler, T., Kastrati, A., Erdmann, J., Danesh, J., … Schunkert, H. (2019). Genetically modulated educational attainment and coronary disease risk. European Heart Journal, 40(29), 24132420. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehz328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, H., Sealock, J. M., Sanchez-Roige, S., Clarke, T. K., Levey, D. F., Cheng, Z., … Gelernter, J. (2020). Genome-wide meta-analysis of problematic alcohol use in 435 563 individuals yields insights into biology and relationships with other traits. Nature Neuroscience, 23(7), 809818. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0643-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, T., Sun, D., Li, X., Ma, H., Heianza, Y., & Qi, L. (2019b). Educational attainment and drinking behaviors: Mendelian randomization study in UK biobank. Molecular Psychiatry. online ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0596-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, A., Taylor, A. E., Karhunen, V., Zhan, Y., Rovio, S. P., Lahti, J., … Hypponen, E. (2018). Habitual coffee consumption and cognitive function: A Mendelian randomization meta-analysis in up to 415 530 participants. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, H., Zhang, Y., Liu, J., Yang, Y., Fang, W., Hong, S., … Zhang, L. (2019a). Education and lung cancer: A Mendelian randomization study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 48(3), 743750. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyz121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. The main principles of Mendelian randomization: (a) the conceptual model indicating the three core assumptions, (b) an illustration of vertical pleiotropy, that which causal inference is based on in a Mendelian randomization analysis, versus horizontal pleiotropy, which biases a Mendelian randomization analysis, and (c) an illustration of the framework and methods of Mendelian randomization using individual-level data versus summary-level data.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. PRISMA flow chart demonstrating the selection of articles to be included for qualitative synthesis.

Figure 2

Table 1. All Mendelian randomization (MR) studies included for qualitative synthesis, with their identifying information, description of the exposure and outcome variable(s), whether the study used individual-level and/or summary-level data, the total quality rating, and a brief summary of their findings

Figure 3

Table 2. All Mendelian randomization (MR) studies included for qualitative synthesis, with their identifying information, description of the data samples used for exposure and outcome variable(s), ancestry of those samples, the independence of the include SNPs, whether or not proxies were used, and whether or not a correction for multiple testing was applied

Supplementary material: File

Treur et al. supplementary material

Treur et al. supplementary material 1

Download Treur et al. supplementary material(File)
File 33.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Treur et al. supplementary material

Treur et al. supplementary material 2

Download Treur et al. supplementary material(File)
File 33.3 KB