Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T03:15:58.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impaired cognitive plasticity and goal-directed control in adolescent obsessive–compulsive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2018

Julia Gottwald*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Sanne de Wit
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
Annemieke M. Apergis-Schoute
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Sharon Morein-Zamir
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, UK
Muzaffer Kaser
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Francesca Cormack
Affiliation:
Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK
Akeem Sule
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK
Winifred Limmer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK
Anna Conway Morris
Affiliation:
Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Trevor W. Robbins
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Barbara J. Sahakian
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Julia Gottwald, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Youths with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) experience severe distress and impaired functioning at school and at home. Critical cognitive domains for daily functioning and academic success are learning, memory, cognitive flexibility and goal-directed behavioural control. Performance in these important domains among teenagers with OCD was therefore investigated in this study.

Methods

A total of 36 youths with OCD and 36 healthy comparison subjects completed two memory tasks: Pattern Recognition Memory (PRM) and Paired Associates Learning (PAL); as well as the Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) task to quantitatively gauge learning as well as cognitive flexibility. A subset of 30 participants of each group also completed a Differential-Outcome Effect (DOE) task followed by a Slips-of-Action Task, designed to assess the balance of goal-directed and habitual behavioural control.

Results

Adolescent OCD patients showed a significant learning and memory impairment. Compared with healthy comparison subjects, they made more errors on PRM and PAL and in the first stages of IED involving discrimination and reversal learning. Patients were also slower to learn about contingencies in the DOE task and were less sensitive to outcome devaluation, suggesting an impairment in goal-directed control.

Conclusions

This study advances the characterization of juvenile OCD. Patients demonstrated impairments in all learning and memory tasks. We also provide the first experimental evidence of impaired goal-directed control and lack of cognitive plasticity early in the development of OCD. The extent to which the impairments in these cognitive domains impact academic performance and symptom development warrants further investigation.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Introduction

Obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) in children and adolescents is a distressing condition, which is often chronic and persists into adulthood (Micali et al. Reference Micali, Heyman, Perez, Hilton, Nakatani and Turner2010). Almost 90% of these young patients have problems at school, home or socially; with difficulties doing homework and concentrating at school being the two most common problems (Piacentini et al. Reference Piacentini, Bergman, Keller and McCracken2003). It is key that we identify and address underlying issues early to aid treatment and help patients to reach their full potential in life. Several cognitive domains are important for daily functioning and educational success. School performance has been shown to relate to memory capacity (Gathercole & Pickering, Reference Gathercole and Pickering2000; Gathercole et al. Reference Gathercole, Pickering, Knight and Stegmann2004), independent of socio-economic background (Engel de Abreu et al. Reference Engel de Abreu, Abreu, Nikaedo, Puglisi, Tourinho and Miranda2014). In addition, cognitive flexibility has been shown to be important for academic success, especially for math and reading skills (Yeniad et al. Reference Yeniad, Malda, Mesman, van IJzendoorn and Pieper2013). Finally, an intact balance between goal-directed and habitual control is crucial for daily functioning. Stimulus–response (S-R) habits are thought to be mediated by direct S-R associations, which renders them efficient but also inflexible. Goal-directed action control, on the other hand, allows us to base our actions on the value of the anticipated outcome and to flexibly adjust our actions when the outcome is no longer desirable (de Wit & Dickinson, Reference de Wit and Dickinson2009). Impairments in these domains could have serious widespread consequences, especially in young, still developing individuals.

Deficits in memory, cognitive flexibility and goal-directed control have been identified in adult OCD (Purcell et al. Reference Purcell, Maruff, Kyrios and Pantelis1998; Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg and Robbins2011; Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz and Mittelman2013). In particular, impaired goal-directed control is one of the hallmarks of adult OCD (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg and Robbins2011). The inability to stop a compulsion (such as excessive hand washing) despite negative consequences (like the development of painful skin condition or inability to work/study due to the time spent washing) can be interpreted as impaired goal-directed action control. Indeed, it has been proposed that a shift in the balance between flexible, goal-directed control and S-R habitual control plays an important role in the development of compulsions (Gillan & Robbins, Reference Gillan and Robbins2014).

However, these impairments remain poorly investigated in adolescent OCD. Findings from adult patients do not necessarily apply to younger ones, given that there are important differences between adult and adolescent OCD, such as gender distribution (Geller et al. Reference Geller, Biederman, Faraone, Agranat, Cradock and Hagermoser2001), degree of insight (Storch et al. Reference Storch, Milsom, Merlo, Larson, Geffken and Jacob2008) and genetic influence (van Grootheest et al. Reference van Grootheest, Cath, Beekman and Boomsma2005).

The few neuropsychological studies in youths with OCD have yielded inconsistent results (Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz, Mittelman, Stark, Ramsey and Geller2015). Visuospatial memory has been found to be impaired among juvenile patients in some (Andrés et al. Reference Andrés, Boget, Lázaro, Penadés, Morer and Salamero2007; Chang et al. Reference Chang, McCracken and Piacentini2007; Lewin et al. Reference Lewin, Larson, Park, McGuire, Murphy and Storch2014) but not in other studies (Shin et al. Reference Shin, Choi, Kim, Hwang, Kim and Cho2008; Zandt et al. Reference Zandt, Prior and Kyrios2009; Ornstein et al. Reference Ornstein, Arnold, Manassis, Mendlowitz and Schachar2010; Geller et al. Reference Geller, Abramovitch, Mittelman, Stark, Ramsey and Cooperman2017; Hybel et al. Reference Hybel, Mortensen, Lambek, Thastum and Thomsen2017). For cognitive flexibility and set shifting, some studies have shown impairments in youths with OCD (Andrés et al. Reference Andrés, Boget, Lázaro, Penadés, Morer and Salamero2007; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Choi, Kim, Hwang, Kim and Cho2008; Isik Taner et al. Reference Isik Taner, Erdogan Bakar and Oner2011; Gruner et al. Reference Gruner, Vo, Ikuta, Mahon, Peters and Malhotra2012), while others found no deficits (Beers et al. Reference Beers, Rosenberg, Dick, Williams, O'Hearn and Birmaher1999; Ornstein et al. Reference Ornstein, Arnold, Manassis, Mendlowitz and Schachar2010; Kodaira et al. Reference Kodaira, Iwadare, Ushijima, Oiji, Kato and Sugiyama2012; Hybel et al. Reference Hybel, Mortensen, Lambek, Thastum and Thomsen2017). To our knowledge, no study has yet investigated the relative balance of goal-directed and habitual control in youths with OCD.

Previous studies often involved small sample sizes. For example, Chang et al. (Reference Chang, McCracken and Piacentini2007) tested 16 OCD patients and compared them with 15 healthy controls and 15 patients with Tourette's syndrome. Rubia et al. included only male subjects: 10 with OCD, 18 with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and 20 healthy boys (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Cubillo, Woolley, Brammer and Smith2011). Shin et al. (Reference Shin, Choi, Kim, Hwang, Kim and Cho2008) compared 17 OCD patients to 18 healthy volunteers, while Britton et al. (Reference Britton, Rauch, Rosso, Killgore, Price and Ragan2010) administered tasks to 15 OCD patients and 20 healthy controls. Another limitation of previous studies is the inclusion of patients with comorbidities. While it has been reported that around half of youth with OCD fulfil criteria for anxiety or depressive disorder, with another 16% suffering from externalizing disorders (Peris et al. Reference Peris, Rozenman, Bergman, Chang, O'Neill and Piacentini2017), previous studies have tested patients with a wide range of comorbidities, including depression, generalized anxiety disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, specific or social phobia, separation anxiety disorders, ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder and Tourette's syndrome (Chang et al. Reference Chang, McCracken and Piacentini2007; Britton et al. Reference Britton, Rauch, Rosso, Killgore, Price and Ragan2010; Huyser et al. Reference Huyser, Veltman, Wolters, De Haan and Boer2010; Ornstein et al. Reference Ornstein, Arnold, Manassis, Mendlowitz and Schachar2010; Gruner et al. Reference Gruner, Vo, Ikuta, Mahon, Peters and Malhotra2012; Kodaira et al. Reference Kodaira, Iwadare, Ushijima, Oiji, Kato and Sugiyama2012; Lewin et al. Reference Lewin, Larson, Park, McGuire, Murphy and Storch2014; Geller et al. Reference Geller, Abramovitch, Mittelman, Stark, Ramsey and Cooperman2017). Though some of these disorders might share overlapping impairments, the different profiles of comorbidities add to the heterogeneity of the samples. Finally, many studies used non-computerized tasks, which heavily rely on the interaction of participants and experimenters. These characteristics add unnecessary variability to the data set, which might partially explain why the findings thus far have been so inconsistent. We therefore aimed to assess learning, memory and cognitive flexibility rigorously with extensively validated, computerized tests from the CANTAB (Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery) in a larger sample free from comorbid disorders. In addition, the balance between goal-directed and habitual control was assessed with a newly developed instrumental discrimination learning task. The ability to learn about the consequences of actions was studied by comparing acquisition of (‘differential outcomes’, DOs) discriminations that could be supported by (stimulus–outcome–response, S-O-R) learning about unique outcomes v. (‘common outcomes’, COs) discriminations that could only be learned by relying on direct S-R associations. Subsequently, we assessed participants’ ability to flexibly adjust their performance to changes in outcome value in a Slips-of-Action Test (SOAT) (de Wit et al. Reference de Wit, Watson, Harsay, Cohen, van de Vijver and Ridderinkhof2012).

Methods and materials

Participants

Thirty-six participants diagnosed with OCD (69% female) were recruited from Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services, through independent charities, and adverts on social media. Patients were screened by an experienced psychiatrist in an extended clinical interview supplemented by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI for participants over 18, MINI-KID for participants under 18; Sheehan et al. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998, Reference Sheehan, Sheehan, Shytle, Janavs, Bannon, Rogers, Milo, Stock and Wilkinson2010) to ensure that they met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) criteria for OCD and were free of co-morbid Axis-I disorders. A total of 67 patients was screened, of which 31 had to be excluded due to comorbidities. Of the final 36 subjects, 23 patients were prescribed selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: one took citalopram (20 mg), eight fluoxetine (range 7.5–60 mg), 14 sertraline (50–200 mg); two patients were also on circadin (4 and 6 mg) and one was also on pregabalin (150 mg).

Control subjects (n = 36, 69% female) were recruited through local adverts and screened for psychiatric disorders using the age-appropriate version of the MINI (see above) by the same clinician as the patients. All participants were between 12 and 19 years old, fluent in English, and reported no history of psychiatric or neurological disorder or brain injury. The study was approved by the East of England – Essex Research Ethics Committee (REC 10/H0301/49). It was also adopted by the NIHR Clinical Research Network (CRN) Portfolio (9532) and the study information was publicly available on the UK CRN Portfolio website. All volunteers gave written informed consent before beginning the testing and received monetary compensation for their participation. For participants under 16 years, parental written consent was also obtained.

Clinical assessment

Participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory for Youth and Beck Anxiety Inventory for Youth from the Beck Youth Inventories Second Edition (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Beck, Jolly and Steer2005). Self-reported obsessive–compulsive symptomatology was assessed with the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory Revised (Foa et al. Reference Foa, Huppert, Leiberg, Langner, Kichic and Hajcak2002) and in patients additionally with the Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Scahill et al. Reference Scahill, Riddle, McSwiggin-Hardin, Ort, King and Goodman1997).

Neuropsychological measures

Participants completed two subtests (vocabulary and matrix reasoning) of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) and the following three CANTAB tasks: Pattern Recognition Memory (PRM) assessing visual pattern recognition (Sahakian et al. Reference Sahakian, Morris, Evenden, Heald, Levy and Philpot1988), Paired Associates Learning (PAL) in high functioning mode (up to 12 patterns) testing visuospatial learning (assessed by total errors made) and memory (measured as first trial memory score; Sahakian et al. Reference Sahakian, Morris, Evenden, Heald, Levy and Philpot1988), and Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) measuring discrimination learning and set formation [stages before the extradimensional shift (EDS)] as well as cognitive flexibility, measured at the extradimensional shift stage (Downes et al. Reference Downes, Roberts, Sahakian, Evenden, Morris and Robbins1989). Subjects also completed a newly developed task described below to assess instrumental learning and goal-directed v. habitual control.

Differential-Outcome Effect task followed by SOAT (for a detailed task description, see online Supplemental Material). At the start of this four-stage task, participants were informed that they were to play a game in which they could earn vouchers for six different rewards by defeating eight different monsters (pictures on the screen that served as the discriminative stimuli). They were told that the person who would collect the most vouchers would be given £15 at the end of the study. Participants chose their preferred six rewards from 13 different rewards, each worth about £15 (cinema voucher, sports equipment, accessory gift card, McDonald's gift card, portable phone charger, hair straightener, magazine subscription, cosmetics, headphones, Starbucks gift card, clothes shop gift card, phone case and HMV gift card). The six chosen rewards were subsequently incorporated into the game.

Stage 1: Pavlovian training

In order to establish strong stimulus–outcome (S-O) associations between the discriminative stimuli and instrumental outcomes, participants first received Pavlovian training. They passively observed the relationships between the eight (discriminative) stimuli and six rewards. Four monsters were each paired with a unique outcome (DO stimuli: S1-O1; S2-O2; S3-O3; S4-O4), while the remaining four stimuli formed two pairs where each was associated with a CO (CO stimuli: e.g. S5-O5; S6-O5; S7-O6; S8-O6).

Stage 2: Instrumental discrimination training

At the start of training, participants were instructed that they could defeat the different monsters by pressing either the right or the left arrow key on the keyboard. The four CO and four DO monsters functioned as discriminative stimuli, with half signalling that the left key was correct and the other half the right key. Participants could learn by trial and error to press the correct key. The stimuli were still associated with the same outcomes as during the Pavlovian training (S1:right → DO1; S2:left → DO2; S3:right → DO3; S4:left → DO4; S5:right → CO5; S6:left → CO5; S7:right → CO6; S8:left → CO6). Performance was separately assessed on the DO and the CO trials, to allow us to compare learning rates. Previous studies have shown that animals and humans acquire the DO discriminations faster than the CO discriminations (Trapold, Reference Trapold1970; Estevez et al. Reference Estevez, Fuentes, Mari-Beffa, Gonzalez and Alvarez2001). This ‘DO effect’ (DOE) is generally believed to reflect the usage of the differential properties of outcomes to aid learning of the DO discriminations (Trapold, Reference Trapold1970). In contrast, in the CO discrimination, each outcome should become associated with both the left and right response, and it should therefore rely on relatively slow S-R habit learning. Consequently, a general reliance on habitual control – at the expense of goal-directed control – should be reflected in a reduced DOE. The associative structures are illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. The difference between differential and common outcomes in training and associative structure. Differential outcomes: In the training phase, participants learn to associate stimuli with correct responses and differential outcomes. The ideal strategy to learn about these contingencies is to apply a goal-directed strategy and form stimulus–outcome–response (S-O-R) associations. However, participants can also apply a more habitual strategy to form stimulus–response (S-R) associations. Common outcomes: Here, two common-outcomes stimuli are associated with different correct responses but the same outcome. Therefore, these discriminations should be hard, if not impossible, to learn with S-O-R associations, because one outcome would be associated with two different responses. The favourable strategy is the habitual S-R association to prevent this conflict.

Stage 3: SOAT and Baseline test

The speeded SOAT and Baseline test were conducted in counterbalanced order in each group. During the SOAT, each block started with 10-s ‘instructed devaluation’ screen showing all six outcomes, but with a cross superimposed on either two DOs (one for a right response and one for a left) or on one CO to indicate that these outcomes were devalued, and that participants should therefore refrain from responding for these. Participants were instructed that they would be shown the eight monster stimuli in quick succession, and that they should withhold their learnt responses for the devalued outcomes, while pressing for the still-valuable outcomes. Thus, participants were required to withhold their responses to two discriminative monster stimuli, while continuing to press for the other six stimuli.

The baseline test was identical except that the monster stimuli were initially shown to be devalued (‘disarmed’) rather than the outcomes. During each block, two stimuli were devalued: either two CO or two DO stimuli, but always where one had signalled a right and one a left response. This test should impose similar working memory and response inhibition demands, but does not require evaluation of an anticipated outcome.

Stage 4: Choice test of R-O knowledge

During this self-paced instructed outcome-devaluation test, each trial showed a pair of DOs: one previously earned by a right response and one by a left response. One of the two outcomes was devalued as signalled by a superimposed red cross. Participants were instructed to press the key that previously earned the still-valuable outcome. This test assessed participants’ ability to base choices between the outcomes on their memory of the R-O contingencies

Data analysis

Data were analysed in SPSS version 21 and R version 3.3.3. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was carried out using multiple linear regression to test for between-group differences, with age and gender as covariates. In addition, a between-subject factor was used when appropriate to assess the effect of task difficulty or level. When the homogeneity of variances assumption was not met, a Welch correction was used. When the assumption of sphericity was violated for analyses involving repeated measures, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used. To assess the memory demand of the DOE task, a priori Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated for difference scores (per cent responses to still-valuable stimuli minus per cent responses to devalued stimuli), PAL and PRM performance. Data were analysed for 36 OCD patients and 36 healthy control participants for PRM immediate recall, PAL and IED. 30 participants in each group completed the PRM with a 20 min delayed recall and the DOE task, as these tasks were added at a later point in the study. One subject did not complete the choice test of R-O knowledge of the DOE task.

Results

Clinical and psychological scales

The demographic and clinical assessment data are shown in Table 1. OCD patients and healthy comparison subjects were well matched for age, gender and IQ. The mean Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale was 25.1 (s.d. = 5.0, range 15–34). Patients had worse symptoms of OCD, depression and anxiety, though none of the participants fulfilled clinical criteria for depression or anxiety disorder.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics and cognitive performance measures

Standard deviations are in parentheses. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001; OCD, obsessive–compulsive disorder; R–O, response-outcome.

CANTAB measures

Table 1 summarizes performance on the CANTAB tasks and results of the ANCOVA. The main results are shown in Fig. 2. In summary, on the PRM task, patients identified significantly fewer patterns correctly in both immediate and delayed recall (both p < 0.001) even accounting for age and gender, while they were also slower to respond in immediate (p = 0.003), but not delayed recall (p = 0.374).

Fig. 2. Impaired learning and memory in adolescent obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Error bars denote s.e.m. (a) Pattern Recognition Memory task. Patients identified significantly fewer patterns correctly both in immediate and 20-min delayed recall. (b) Paired Associates Learning task. Youths with OCD made significantly more errors. (c) Intra-Extra Dimensional Shift Task. Groups did not differ in their errors at the extradimensional shift, but patients made more errors in the stages before the EDS.

Young OCD patients also showed impairments on the PAL task: they had a significantly lower first trial memory score and made more errors in total, even when age and gender were controlled for. Planned post hoc comparisons, again adjusted for age and gender, showed a significant main effect of difficulty (F 4,254 =3.567, p = 0.0075) and a significant interaction between group and task difficulty (F 4,254 = 59.081, p < 0.001). Specifically, OCD patients made significantly more errors than controls at the more difficult 6, 8 and 12 pattern stages (F 1,50.4 = 12.947, p < 0.001; F 1,57.8 = 5.351, p = 0.024 and F 1,55.3 = 9.376, p = 0.003, respectively).

The analysis of learning curves at the eight- and 12-shape stage of the task revealed a significant interaction between trial and group (F 1,2.0 = 4.754, p = 0.011 and F 1,2.2 = 4.269, p = 0.013, respectively; for description of learning curves see Sahakian et al. Reference Sahakian, Morris, Evenden, Heald, Levy and Philpot1988). While all participants eventually identified all patterns correctly, the OCD patients took more trials to do so.

On the IED task, patients made significantly more errors in the stages before the extradimensional shift (p < 0.05). In contrast, patients and controls did not differ from each other at the extradimensional shift stage.

DOE and SOAT

The main results are summarized in Table 1 and Fig. 3. Data were analysed using a repeated measures approach, where the effect of discrimination was treated as a within-subject factor, and group as a between-subjects factor. During the first half of instrumental training, there was a significant interaction of discrimination (DO, CO) and group (OCD, controls; F 1,58 = 5.236, p = 0.026). Post hoc analysis revealed that controls showed a DOE and learned better about DOs than COs (F 1,29 = 12.115, p = 0.002), while OCD patients did not (F 1,29 = 0.002, p = 0.961). OCD patients had lower average response accuracies for DOs (F 1,58 = 5.659, p = 0.021), but not COs compared with controls (F 1,58 = 1.141, p = 0.290). Follow-up analyses showed that this effect was driven by the first stages of the training: response accuracies for DOs were significantly different between groups in the first half of the training (F 1,58 = 5.856, p = 0.019), but not during the second half (F 1,42.2 = 3.858, p = 0.056). The groups had similar scores on the questionnaires testing explicit knowledge of S-R and S-O contingencies (p = 0.276 and p = 1, respectively) and similar response accuracies on the choice test of R-O knowledge (p = 0.272).

Fig. 3. Impaired training accuracy and poorer adjustment to stimulus/outcome devaluation in youths with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Error bars denote s.e.m. (a) Instrumental learning stage. Participants learned to associate stimuli (monsters) with correct responses (left or right button press) and outcomes (rewards). In the first half of the training, patients performed less well for differential outcomes but not common outcomes compared to control participants, but accuracy did not differ between the groups by the end of the training. (b) Stimulus/outcome devaluation. During the Baseline and Slips-of-Action tests, some monsters or rewards were devalued, respectively. In the Baseline test, participants were instructed to withhold a response for the devalued stimuli. In the Slips-of-Action test, they had to stop responding for stimuli that were associated with now devalued outcomes (the explicit indication of ‘GO’ and ‘NO-GO’ stimuli was added here for demonstration purposes, but was not shown in the task). There was no main effect of task. The combined analysis of Baseline and Slips-of-Action tasks revealed a significantly lower difference score (% responses towards valuable minus % responses towards devalued stimuli) in the patient group, suggesting an impaired ability to adjust learnt responses to instructed changes in stimulus/outcome value among youths with OCD. (c) Response–outcome knowledge test. Participants were shown two differential outcomes simultaneously on the screen, one of which was devalued. They were instructed to make a response towards the still valuable outcome, by pressing the key they would have to press to defeat the enemy associated with this reward. There were no group differences in accuracy, suggesting that patients and controls had learned equally well about response–outcome contingencies for differential outcomes.

For the SOAT and Baseline test, we first performed an ANOVA with test type (SOAT, Baseline), devaluation (valuable, devalued) and discrimination (DO, CO) as within-subject factors and group as between-subjects factor. There was a significant interaction of devaluation and group (F 1,58 = 6.419, p = 0.014), but no interaction of devaluation, group and test type (F 1,58 = 0.370, p = 0.546). Therefore, we calculated the overall difference scores (per cent responses to still-valuable stimuli minus per cent responses to devalued stimuli) across the two tests and found them to be significantly lower in OCD patients compared with controls, even when controlling for age and gender (F 3,56 = 2.869, p < 0.05), as shown in Fig. 3. Pearson's correlations revealed that the difference scores correlated with accuracy on PRM and PAL total errors (r = 0.329, p = 0.010 and r = 0.446, p < 0.001, respectively). There was also a significant interaction of test type, devaluation and discrimination (F 1,58 = 8.350, p = 0.005), which did not differ between the groups. Both OCD patients and controls made more errors for COs than DOs in the SOAT (F 1,58 = 18.066, p < 0.001), but – as expected – not in the Baseline task, which does not rely on retrieval of outcome knowledge.

Discussion

Using standardized CANTAB tasks and a newly developed DOE and SOAT, we show that teenagers with OCD have significant learning and memory impairments compared with healthy control subjects. Patients identified fewer patterns correctly and were slower on the PRM task. They also made more errors on the PAL task, similar to adult OCD patients (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Craig, Ersche, Abbott, Muller and Fineberg2010). Performance at the discrimination and reversal learning stages of the IED task was also impaired, but not at the extradimensional set shift stage. The observed difficulties at the learning stages probably impaired formation of a stable attentional set. More errors on the extradimensional shift stage, as seen in adult OCD, are usually interpreted as impaired cognitive flexibility (Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Fineberg, Menzies, Blackwell, Bullmore and Robbins2007), but the present pattern of results precludes firm conclusions about the existence of a similar deficit in young OCD patients.

In this study, we provide the first experimental evidence of impaired goal-directed control in youths with OCD. First of all, the DOE task revealed a specific learning impairment in patients. They were slower to learn instrumental discriminations with DOs (but not CO) than controls, which may be indicative of an impairment in goal-directed learning. However, patients did learn about these contingencies later in the training, as shown by their explicit knowledge of contingencies and good performance on the choice test of R-O knowledge.

During the subsequent SOAT and Baseline test, patients were generally less able to selectively respond on valuable trials, while withholding their response when either the antecedent stimulus or the consequent outcome had been devalued. Previously, it has been argued that the SOAT provides a better measure of relative goal-directed/habitual control than the Baseline test because it relies on accurate prediction of the outcomes of one's actions, while the Baseline test merely requires one to remember to which stimuli one should no longer respond (Worbe et al. Reference Worbe, Savulich, de Wit, Fernandez-Egea and Robbins2015). According to that logic, the present finding of an impairment across the two tests provides support for a more general impairment in response inhibition or working memory in youths with OCD. However, the results could equally well be explained by a greater reliance on learnt S-R habits, as this should interfere with suppression of learnt responses in both tests. We argue that such a habitual style of responding could also be interpreted as an impairment in goal-directed control in a broader sense. If patients formed rigid S-R associations (as indicated by the lack of DOE in the training phase among patients, suggesting that they employed a S-R strategy to learn about both DOs and COs), discriminative stimuli should continue to strongly trigger associated responses – regardless of whether the stimulus or the outcome is devalued. In both cases, rigid S-R associations would over-ride the devaluation. Therefore, we argue that our results are in line with an imbalance between goal-directed and habitual control towards inflexible habits.

Our analyses suggest that the ability to differentiate between valuable and devalued stimuli/outcomes relies on general memory capacity: difference scores was moderately correlated with PRM accuracy and PAL total errors. It has been shown that an increased memory load can impair goal-directed behavioural control and promote habitual actions (Otto et al. Reference Otto, Gershman, Markman and Daw2013a). The memory impairment observed in patients could contribute to reliance on S-R learning, which is thought to be less cognitively demanding than goal-directed learning. At first glance, the results from the present study may seem to partially contradict those of a previous study in adult OCD that provided direct evidence for an impairment in goal-directed control (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg and Robbins2011). However, there are notable differences between the studies. The previous study did not include Pavlovian training and fast responses were encouraged in the instrumental discrimination training (participants were instructed to respond quickly as fast responses were rewarded with more points than slow responses). In contrast, the paradigm described here facilitated outcome-learning by prior Pavlovian (S-O) training and by imposing an S-R delay in the instrumental discrimination training, which allowed participants more time to retrieve the appropriate outcome before responding. This difference might explain why adult OCD patients in the previous study were impaired on explicit knowledge of S-O-R contingencies and a choice test of R-O knowledge (Gillan et al. Reference Gillan, Papmeyer, Morein-Zamir, Sahakian, Fineberg and Robbins2011), while adolescent OCD patients in the present study were not.

Widespread effects of learning and memory problems

The most consistent finding across the tasks of this study is a significant learning and memory impairment in youths with OCD, as it is evident in all four tasks. We postulate that the deficits demonstrated in this study and others could have important consequences for juvenile OCD patients. Experiencing learning and memory difficulties in childhood could lead to lower confidence in memory. Elevated doubt regarding memory and compulsive behaviours is often reported in OCD patients (Tolin et al. Reference Tolin, Abramowitz, Brigidi, Amir, Street and Foa2001; Zitterl et al. Reference Zitterl, Urban, Linzmayer, Aigner, Demal and Semler2001), as well as increased intolerance of doubt and uncertainty (Rasmussen & Eisen, Reference Rasmussen and Eisen1989). These characteristics could be vulnerability factors for OCD. Compulsive checking can be used as an example: after checking once, patients who are aware of their memory problems and therefore doubt their memories from the last checking, could be encouraged to check again and again. It has been shown that reduced confidence in memory abilities is associated with OCD symptoms, and especially checking in healthy participants (Nedeljkovic & Kyrios, Reference Nedeljkovic and Kyrios2007).

These learning and memory problems in youths could be amplified by stress. It has been shown that children show significant increases in stress hormone levels when they enrol in school (Russ et al. Reference Russ, Herbert, Cooper, Gunnar, Goodyer and Croudace2012). Stress, in turn, is known to impair memory, especially declarative and episodic memory of non-emotional material (Wolf, Reference Wolf2009). It is conceivable that entering school with pre-existing memory impairments and experiencing difficulties at school would increase this stress response even more, starting a circle of negative influences. Furthermore, both stress and memory deficits are known to tip the balance between goal-directed and habitual behavioural control towards inflexible habits (Schwabe & Wolf, Reference Schwabe and Wolf2011; Otto et al. Reference Otto, Raio, Chiang, Phelps and Daw2013b). These two factors – stress and memory impairments – could work in concert to make children and adolescents more reliant on S-R habits and less able to flexibly adjust their actions, and may predispose them to develop OCD.

Once OCD has developed, memory impairments could also weaken the effect of therapy. One of the most effective therapies for paediatric OCD is cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT), which requires the patient to understand and remember what is being discussed and practiced in therapy. There is evidence to suggest that performance on a memory task can predict treatment response to CBT among children and adolescents with OCD, such that non-responders showed significantly lower memory performance than responders (Flessner et al. Reference Flessner, Allgair, Garcia, Freeman, Sapyta and Franklin2010). Importantly, it has also been shown that successful treatment is associated with an improvement in non-verbal memory, such that juvenile OCD patients were impaired before, but not after treatment compared with healthy controls (Andrés et al. Reference Andrés, Lázaro, Salamero, Boget, Penadés and Castro-Fornieles2008).

Future studies will be necessary to show if these learning and memory impairments are clinically significant in adolescent OCD. It is conceivable that we will need to address memory impairments in these children early on to break the circle of negative influences and aid recovery.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003464

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by a Wellcome Trust Grant (grant number 089589/Z/09/Z) awarded to T.W. Robbins, B.J. Everitt, A.C. Roberts, J.W. Dalley and B.J. Sahakian, and a Wellcome Trust Senior Investigator Award (104631/Z/14/Z) to T.W. Robbins. The research was conducted in the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, which is supported by a joint award from the Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust (G00001354). J Gottwald was supported by a BCNI MRC PhD studentship and St John's College, Cambridge.

Declaration of Interest

S. de Wit is supported by a Vidi grant (452-13-006) of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NOW). M. Kaser is supported by a Cambridge-IDB International Scholarship. T.W. Robbins declares consultancy and royalties for CANTAB, Cambridge Cognition. He consults for Lundbeck, Mundipharma and Unilever and has received research grants from Lundbeck and Shionogi. He also receives editorial honoraria from Springer Verlag and Elsevier. B.J. Sahakian consults for Cambridge Cognition, Peak, Otsuka, Servier, Mundipharma and Lundbeck. In March 2016, she completed a study which was funded by a grant from Janssen/J&J. A. Apergis-Schoute, S. Morein-Zamir, A. Sule, W. Limmer and A. Conway Morris report no competing interests.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Abramovitch, A, Abramowitz, JS and Mittelman, A (2013) The neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 11631171.Google Scholar
Abramovitch, A, Abramowitz, JS, Mittelman, A, Stark, A, Ramsey, K and Geller, DA (2015) Research review: neuropsychological test performance in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder – a meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 56, 837847.Google Scholar
Andrés, S, Boget, T, Lázaro, L, Penadés, R, Morer, A, Salamero, M et al. (2007). Neuropsychological performance in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder and influence of clinical variables. Biological Psychiatry 61, 946951.Google Scholar
Andrés, S, Lázaro, L, Salamero, M, Boget, T, Penadés, R and Castro-Fornieles, J (2008) Changes in cognitive dysfunction in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder after treatment. Journal of Psychiatric Research 42, 507514.Google Scholar
Beck, JS, Beck, AT, Jolly, JB and Steer, RA (2005) Beck Youth Inventories – Second Edition for Children and Adolescents. NCS Pearson, Inc.: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Beers, SR, Rosenberg, DR, Dick, EL, Williams, T, O'Hearn, KM, Birmaher, B et al. (1999) Neuropsychological study of frontal lobe function in psychotropic-naive children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 777779.Google Scholar
Britton, JC, Rauch, SL, Rosso, IM, Killgore, WDS, Price, LM, Ragan, J et al. (2010) Cognitive inflexibility and frontal-cortical activation in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 944953.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Fineberg, NA, Menzies, LA, Blackwell, AD, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW et al. (2007) Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 335338.Google Scholar
Chang, SW, McCracken, JT and Piacentini, JC (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of child obsessive compulsive disorder and Tourette syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 29, 724733.Google Scholar
de Wit, S and Dickinson, A (2009) Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: a case for animal-human translational models. Psychological Research 73, 463476.Google Scholar
de Wit, S, Watson, P, Harsay, HA, Cohen, MX, van de Vijver, I and Ridderinkhof, KR (2012) Corticostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in the balance between habitual and goal-directed action control. The Journal of Neuroscience 32, 1206612075.Google Scholar
Downes, JJ, Roberts, AC, Sahakian, BJ, Evenden, JL, Morris, RG and Robbins, TW (1989) Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction. Neuropsychologia 27, 13291343.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, PMJ, Abreu, N, Nikaedo, CC, Puglisi, ML, Tourinho, CJ, Miranda, MC et al. (2014) Executive functioning and reading achievement in school: a study of Brazilian children assessed by their teachers as ‘poor readers’. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 550.Google Scholar
Estevez, AF, Fuentes, LJ, Mari-Beffa, P, Gonzalez, C and Alvarez, D (2001) The differential outcome effect as a useful tool to improve conditional discrimination learning in children. Learning and Motivation 32, 4864.Google Scholar
Flessner, CA, Allgair, A, Garcia, A, Freeman, J, Sapyta, J, Franklin, ME et al. (2010) The impact of neuropsychological functioning on treatment outcome in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety 27, 365371.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Huppert, JD, Leiberg, S, Langner, R, Kichic, R, Hajcak, G et al. (2002) The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment 14, 485496.Google Scholar
Gathercole, SE and Pickering, SJ (2000) Working memory deficits in children with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. The British Journal of Educational Psychology 70, 177194.Google Scholar
Gathercole, SE, Pickering, SJ, Knight, C and Stegmann, Z (2004) Working memory skills and educational attainment: evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18, 116.Google Scholar
Geller, DA, Abramovitch, A, Mittelman, A, Stark, A, Ramsey, K, Cooperman, A et al. (2017) Neurocognitive function in paediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 110. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28090807.Google Scholar
Geller, DA, Biederman, J, Faraone, S, Agranat, A, Cradock, K, Hagermoser, L et al. (2001) Developmental aspects of obsessive compulsive disorder: findings in children, adolescents, and adults. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 189, 471477.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Papmeyer, M, Morein-Zamir, S, Sahakian, BJ, Fineberg, NA, Robbins, TW et al. (2011) Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 718726.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM and Robbins, TW (2014) Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369, 2013047520130475.Google Scholar
Gruner, P, Vo, A, Ikuta, T, Mahon, K, Peters, BD, Malhotra, AK et al. (2012) White matter abnormalities in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 27302739.Google Scholar
Huyser, C, Veltman, DJ, Wolters, LH, De Haan, E and Boer, F (2010) Functional magnetic resonance imaging during planning before and after cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 12381248.e5.Google Scholar
Hybel, KA, Mortensen, EL, Lambek, R, Thastum, M and Thomsen, PH (2017) Cool and hot aspects of executive function in childhood obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 45, 11951205.Google Scholar
Isik Taner, Y, Erdogan Bakar, E and Oner, O (2011) Impaired executive functions in paediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 23, 272281.Google Scholar
Kodaira, M, Iwadare, Y, Ushijima, H, Oiji, A, Kato, M, Sugiyama, N et al. (2012) Poor performance on the Iowa gambling task in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Annals of General Psychiatry 11, 25.Google Scholar
Lewin, AB, Larson, MJ, Park, JM, McGuire, JF, Murphy, TK and Storch, EA (2014) Neuropsychological functioning in youth with obsessive compulsive disorder: an examination of executive function and memory impairment. Psychiatry Research 216, 108115.Google Scholar
Micali, N, Heyman, I, Perez, M, Hilton, K, Nakatani, E, Turner, C et al. (2010) Long-term outcomes of obsessive-compulsive disorder: follow-up of 142 children and adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 128134.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Craig, KJ, Ersche, KD, Abbott, S, Muller, U, Fineberg, NA et al. (2010) Impaired visuospatial associative memory and attention in obsessive compulsive disorder but no evidence for differential dopaminergic modulation. Psychopharmacology 212, 357367.Google Scholar
Nedeljkovic, M and Kyrios, M (2007) Confidence in memory and other cognitive processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 28992914.Google Scholar
Ornstein, TJ, Arnold, P, Manassis, K, Mendlowitz, S and Schachar, R (2010) Neuropsychological performance in childhood OCD: a preliminary study. Depression and Anxiety 27, 372380.Google Scholar
Otto, AR, Gershman, SJ, Markman, AB and Daw, ND (2013 a). The curse of planning: dissecting multiple reinforcement-learning systems by taxing the central executive. Psychological Science 24, 751761.Google Scholar
Otto, AR, Raio, CM, Chiang, A, Phelps, EA and Daw, ND (2013 b). Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 2094120946.Google Scholar
Peris, TS, Rozenman, M, Bergman, RL, Chang, S, O'Neill, J and Piacentini, J (2017) Developmental and clinical predictors of comorbidity for youth with obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 93, 7278.Google Scholar
Piacentini, J, Bergman, RL, Keller, M and McCracken, J (2003) Functional impairment in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 13, 6169.10.1089/104454603322126359Google Scholar
Purcell, R, Maruff, P, Kyrios, M and Pantelis, C (1998) Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 55, 415423.Google Scholar
Rasmussen, SA and Eisen, JL (1989) Clinical features and phenomenology of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Annals 19, 6773.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Cubillo, A, Woolley, J, Brammer, MJ and Smith, A (2011) Disorder-specific dysfunctions in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder compared to patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder during interference inhibition and attention allocation. Human Brain Mapping 32, 601611.Google Scholar
Russ, SJ, Herbert, J, Cooper, P, Gunnar, MR, Goodyer, I, Croudace, T et al. (2012) Cortisol levels in response to starting school in children at increased risk for social phobia. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 462474.Google Scholar
Sahakian, BJ, Morris, RG, Evenden, JL, Heald, A, Levy, R, Philpot, M et al. (1988) A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain 111, 695718.Google Scholar
Scahill, L, Riddle, MA, McSwiggin-Hardin, M, Ort, SI, King, RA, Goodman, WK et al. (1997) Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 844852.Google Scholar
Schwabe, L and Wolf, OT (2011) Stress-induced modulation of instrumental behavior: from goal-directed to habitual control of action. Behavioural Brain Research 219, 321328.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R and Dunbar, GC (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59, 2233.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Sheehan, KH, Shytle, RD, Janavs, J, Bannon, Y, Rogers, JE, Milo, KM, Stock, SL and Wilkinson, B (2010) Reliability and validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID). The Journal of clinical psychiatry 71, 313326.10.4088/JCP.09m05305whiGoogle Scholar
Shin, M-S, Choi, H, Kim, H, Hwang, J-W, Kim, B-N and Cho, S-C (2008) A study of neuropsychological deficit in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. European Psychiatry 23, 512520.Google Scholar
Storch, EA, Milsom, VA, Merlo, LJ, Larson, M, Geffken, GR, Jacob, ML et al. (2008) Insight in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder: associations with clinical presentation. Psychiatry Research 160, 212220.Google Scholar
Tolin, DF, Abramowitz, JS, Brigidi, BD, Amir, N, Street, GP and Foa, EB (2001) Memory and memory confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 39, 913927.Google Scholar
Trapold, MA (1970) Are expectancies based upon different positive reinforcing events discriminably different? Learning and Motivation 1, 129140.Google Scholar
van Grootheest, DS, Cath, DC, Beekman, AT and Boomsma, DI (2005) Twin studies on obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review. Twin Research and Human Genetics 8, 450458.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999) Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: TP Corporation.Google Scholar
Wolf, OT (2009) Stress and memory in humans: twelve years of progress? Brain Research 1293, 142154.Google Scholar
Worbe, Y, Savulich, G, de Wit, S, Fernandez-Egea, E and Robbins, TW (2015) Tryptophan depletion promotes habitual over goal-directed control of appetitive responding in humans. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 18, pyv013.Google Scholar
Yeniad, N, Malda, M, Mesman, J, van IJzendoorn, MH and Pieper, S (2013) Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: a meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences 23, 19.Google Scholar
Zandt, F, Prior, M and Kyrios, M (2009) Similarities and differences between children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and those with obsessive compulsive disorder. Autism 13, 4357.Google Scholar
Zitterl, W, Urban, C, Linzmayer, L, Aigner, M, Demal, U, Semler, B et al. (2001) Memory deficits in patients with DSM-IV obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychopathology 34, 113117.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. The difference between differential and common outcomes in training and associative structure. Differential outcomes: In the training phase, participants learn to associate stimuli with correct responses and differential outcomes. The ideal strategy to learn about these contingencies is to apply a goal-directed strategy and form stimulus–outcome–response (S-O-R) associations. However, participants can also apply a more habitual strategy to form stimulus–response (S-R) associations. Common outcomes: Here, two common-outcomes stimuli are associated with different correct responses but the same outcome. Therefore, these discriminations should be hard, if not impossible, to learn with S-O-R associations, because one outcome would be associated with two different responses. The favourable strategy is the habitual S-R association to prevent this conflict.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics and cognitive performance measures

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Impaired learning and memory in adolescent obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Error bars denote s.e.m. (a) Pattern Recognition Memory task. Patients identified significantly fewer patterns correctly both in immediate and 20-min delayed recall. (b) Paired Associates Learning task. Youths with OCD made significantly more errors. (c) Intra-Extra Dimensional Shift Task. Groups did not differ in their errors at the extradimensional shift, but patients made more errors in the stages before the EDS.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Impaired training accuracy and poorer adjustment to stimulus/outcome devaluation in youths with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Error bars denote s.e.m. (a) Instrumental learning stage. Participants learned to associate stimuli (monsters) with correct responses (left or right button press) and outcomes (rewards). In the first half of the training, patients performed less well for differential outcomes but not common outcomes compared to control participants, but accuracy did not differ between the groups by the end of the training. (b) Stimulus/outcome devaluation. During the Baseline and Slips-of-Action tests, some monsters or rewards were devalued, respectively. In the Baseline test, participants were instructed to withhold a response for the devalued stimuli. In the Slips-of-Action test, they had to stop responding for stimuli that were associated with now devalued outcomes (the explicit indication of ‘GO’ and ‘NO-GO’ stimuli was added here for demonstration purposes, but was not shown in the task). There was no main effect of task. The combined analysis of Baseline and Slips-of-Action tasks revealed a significantly lower difference score (% responses towards valuable minus % responses towards devalued stimuli) in the patient group, suggesting an impaired ability to adjust learnt responses to instructed changes in stimulus/outcome value among youths with OCD. (c) Response–outcome knowledge test. Participants were shown two differential outcomes simultaneously on the screen, one of which was devalued. They were instructed to make a response towards the still valuable outcome, by pressing the key they would have to press to defeat the enemy associated with this reward. There were no group differences in accuracy, suggesting that patients and controls had learned equally well about response–outcome contingencies for differential outcomes.

Supplementary material: File

Gottwald et al. supplementary material

Gottwald et al. supplementary material 1

Download Gottwald et al. supplementary material(File)
File 18.9 KB