Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T21:24:24.878Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion regulation across psychiatric disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 May 2024

Ibrahim H. Aslan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
Lucy Dorey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Jon E. Grant*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Samuel R. Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
*
Corresponding author: Jon E. Grant; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Difficulties with emotion regulation have been associated with multiple psychiatric conditions. In this study, we aimed to investigate emotional regulation difficulties in young adults who gamble at least occasionally (ie, an enriched sample), and diagnosed with a range of psychiatric disorders using the validated Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS).

Methods

A total of 543 non-treatment-seeking individuals who had engaged in gambling activities on at least 5 occasions within the previous year, aged 18–29 were recruited from general community settings. Diagnostic assessments included the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory, Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder World Health Organization Screening Tool Part A, and the Structured Clinical Interview for Gambling Disorder. Emotional dysregulation was evaluated using DERS. The profile of emotional dysregulation across disorders was characterized using Z-scores (those with the index disorder vs. those without the index disorder).

Results

Individuals with probable ADHD displayed the highest level of difficulties in emotional regulation, followed by intermittent explosive disorder, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder. In contrast, participants diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive disorder showed relatively lower levels of difficulties with emotional regulation.

Conclusions

This study highlights the importance of recognizing emotional dysregulation as a trans-diagnostic phenomenon across psychiatric disorders. The results also reveal differing levels of emotional dysregulation across diagnoses, with potential implications for tailored treatment approaches. Despite limitations such as small sample sizes for certain disorders and limited age range, this study contributes to a broader understanding of emotional regulation’s role in psychiatric conditions.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Emotional dysregulation, characterized by the inability to effectively manage and modulate emotions, is a prominent feature in various psychiatric disorders.Reference Gross and Thompson 1 -Reference Gross and Muñoz 3 This phenomenon encompasses a range of difficulties, including difficulties in regulating emotional responses, heightened emotional reactivity, and impaired emotional expression.Reference Gross and Thompson 1 , Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer 2 While emotional dysregulation has been extensively studied within individual psychiatric disorders, a comprehensive exploration of its manifestations across different diagnostic categories has been relatively limited. This study aims to address this gap by examining difficulties with emotional regulation across psychiatric disorders in one large community-recruited sample, specifically focusing on individuals who gamble at least occasionally, shedding light on its trans-diagnostic nature and potential implications for treatment strategies. By focusing on people who gamble at least occasionally, this yields an enriched sample, that is, a sample of people more likely to have various mental disorders than the general population (since gambling itself is linked to many mental disorders).

Previous studies showed that maladaptive emotion regulation strategies such as avoidance, rumination, and self-blame are frequently involved in the development and maintenance of various psychiatric disorders, including but not limited to moodReference Aslan and Baldwin 4 and anxiety disorders,Reference Cisler and Olatunji 5 obsessive-compulsive disorder,Reference Yap, Mogan and Moriarty 6 alcohol and substance use disorders,Reference Fox, Axelrod, Paliwal, Sleeper and Sinha 7 , Reference Berking, Margraf, Ebert, Wuppermann, Hofmann and Junghanns 8 gambling disorder,Reference Neophytou, Theodorou, Artemi, Theodorou and Panayiotou 9 , Reference Velotti, Rogier, Zobel and Billieux 10 eating disorders,Reference Vintró-Alcaraz, Munguía and Granero 11 and body-focused repetitive disordersReference Ricketts, Peris and Grant 12 (trichotillomania and skin-picking disorder); as well as in other clinically relevant syndrome such as in problematic exercise,Reference Chamberlain and Grant 13 problematic smartphone useReference Arrivillaga, Hallauer, Montag and Elhai 14 and compulsive sexual behavior.Reference Lew-Starowicz, Lewczuk, Nowakowska, Kraus and Gola 15 Individuals with mental health disorders often struggle to effectively recognize, regulate, and respond to emotions in a manner that aligns with the demands of their social and environmental contexts.Reference Gross and Thompson 1 , Reference Gross and Muñoz 3 Furthermore, models of gambling disorder,Reference Neophytou, Theodorou, Artemi, Theodorou and Panayiotou 9 , Reference Velotti, Rogier, Zobel and Billieux 10 eating disorders,Reference Fairburn, Norman, Welch, O’Connor, Doll and Peveler 16 , Reference McCarthy 17 and alcohol use disorderReference Sher and Grekin 18 , Reference Tice, Bratslavsky and Baumeister 19 suggest that individuals who cannot effectively regulate their emotions might frequently turn to gambling, food, or alcohol as emotional escape leading to negative reinforcement of unhelpful behavioral patterns.

Emotional dysregulation can lead to a range of negative outcomes, including impaired interpersonal relationships, hindered daily functioning, and increased susceptibility to comorbid disorders.Reference Salsman and Linehan 20 Furthermore, given the relationship between emotional dysregulation and impulsivity, it becomes evident that factoring in emotion regulation is essential when evaluating individuals at a heightened risk of developing an addiction.Reference Schreiber, Grant and Odlaug 21 Additionally, it is crucial to acknowledge the broader context of emotional dysregulation, especially its linkage to hypersexuality. Lew-Starowicz et al. explored the complex connection between compulsive sexual behavior (CSB) and emotion dysregulation, shedding light on how emotional dysregulation serves as a core element in individuals suffering from CSB. The study investigated patterns of emotion dysregulation as a common clinical feature, underlying mechanisms, and target for psychological and pharmacological interventions in individuals with CSB. The findings suggest that emotional dysregulation plays a pivotal role in the onset and severity of CSB, emphasizing its relevance in both understanding and treating compulsive sexual behavior.Reference Lew-Starowicz, Lewczuk, Nowakowska, Kraus and Gola 15

Recent advances in neuroimaging, neurobiology, and psychological research have provided a foundation for exploring emotional dysregulation as a cross-cutting construct. This approach allows researchers to investigate whether certain neural circuits, genetic factors, or psychological mechanisms are consistently implicated in emotional dysregulation across diverse psychiatric disorders.Reference van den Heuvel, van Wingen and Soriano-Mas 22 -Reference McTeague, Rosenberg and Lopez 24 Moreover, understanding the shared mechanisms underlying emotional dysregulation could pave the way for the development of interventions that target these mechanisms across multiple disorders, potentially offering more effective and personalized treatment approaches.

Despite the centrality of emotional (dys)regulation in various psychiatric conditions, research has primarily focused on understanding this phenomenon within isolated disorder-specific contexts. This compartmentalized approach may overlook the commonalities that exist across disorders, hindering our understanding of the broader patterns and mechanisms that contribute to emotional (dys)regulation. By adopting a trans-diagnostic perspective using the same recruitment techniques and methodology, we can unravel the shared underpinnings of emotional dysregulation across psychiatric disorders, potentially revealing novel insights into its etiology and treatment strategies; this can also help to identify the relative degree of emotional dysregulation across different disorders. Therefore, this study aimed to examine difficulties in emotion regulation across a range of psychiatric disorders using the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) in a diverse, non-treatment-seeking sample of young adults, in a single study setting.

Methods

Participants

A total of 543 participants (aged 18–29 years) were enrolled from general community settings. In this study, the criteria for participant inclusion were as follows: individuals who had engaged in gambling activities on at least 5 occasions within the previous year. This criterion was established as part of a larger longitudinal study that specifically focused on impulsivity in young adults. Additionally, participants needed to be available for in-person interviews. To minimize the potential influence of age-related factors on emotional regulation difficulties, a specific and limited age range was selected. The exclusion criteria for the study comprised individuals with hearing or vision impairments, as well as those lacking the capacity to comprehend the study’s objectives and provide informed consent. Participants were recruited through media advertisements in the Minneapolis and Chicago metropolitan regions and were compensated with a $50 gift card. The study’s design and consent statement were approved by the University of Chicago’s Institutional Review Board (IRB). Prior to enrolment, potential participants were fully informed about the study’s details, offering them the chance to seek clarification and address queries. Written informed consent was obtained from participants, ensuring their understanding and voluntary participation. All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Assessments

Demographic variables (age, biological sex at birth, gender, and highest level of education) were recorded. Several previously psychometrically validated instruments were used. Participants were evaluated for psychiatric disorders using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory (MINI)Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier and Sheehan 25 (The MINI covers a range of psychiatric disorders, including but not limited to major depressive disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, social anxiety disorder, OCD, PTSD, alcohol dependence, alcohol abuse, substance dependence, substance abuse, bulimia, generalized anxiety disorder [GAD], antisocial personality disorder, gambling disorder), the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview (MIDI) (which screens for compulsive buying, kleptomania, trichotillomania, skin-picking disorder, pyromania, intermittent explosive disorder, compulsive sexual behavior, and binge-eating disorder),Reference Chamberlain and Grant 26 , Reference Grant 27 the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) World Health Organization Screening Tool Part A (ASRS v1.1),Reference Kessler, Adler and Ames 28 and the Structured Clinical Interview for Gambling Disorder (SCI-GD).Reference Grant, Steinberg, Kim, Rounsaville and Potenza 29 Those meeting WHO recommended scoring criteria for probable ADHD were referred to as “probable ADHD” to recognize that this is not a formal diagnostic instrument (unlike the other instruments).

In addition to diagnostic measures, emotion (dys)regulation was assessed with the well-established DERS.Reference Gratz and Roemer 30 This instrument has demonstrated high internal consistency and test–retest reliability. The DERS measures all main features of emotion regulation, encompassing aspects such as accurately identifying emotional states, responding to negative emotions with secondary negative emotions, maintaining goal-directed behavior in the presence of negative emotions, refraining from impulsive actions during negative emotional states, attending to emotional experiences, believing in limited efficacy for emotion regulation. Responses to the 36 items of the DERS are recorded on a scale ranging from 0 to 5, corresponding to “almost never” to “almost always.” The measure of interest in this study was the total DERS score, with a higher total score indicating greater difficulties regulating emotions.

Data analysis

For analysis, only psychiatric diseases that were identified in at least 1% of the participants (ie, at least 5 individuals per diagnosis) were considered. By calculating Z-scores in relation to normative data from study participants who did not have the reference mental disorder (hereafter referred to as controls), the profile of difficulties in emotion regulation for disorders was quantified. In this context, Z-scores are equivalent to Cohen’s d and represent effect sizes. By convention, effect sizes of 0.3, 0.5, and 0.8 are considered small, medium, and large, respectively. Each analysis compared all individuals with the given mental disorder of interest (irrespective of whether they had other disorders or not) to controls (people without the given mental disorder of interest, whether or not they had other morbidities). This allowed for a thorough examination of the emotional regulation difficulties associated with individual psychiatric disorders, even in the presence of comorbidity, in a naturalistic fashion.

Results

A total of 543 young adults [mean (standard deviation) age = 22.3 (3.6) years; n = 192 female (35.4%)] were enrolled. Of the 543 participants, 482 (88.8%) had completed some college education or higher. The numbers [%] of participants with each disorder of interest were as follows (Table 1): major depressive disorder 12 [2.2%], panic disorder 7 [1.3%], agoraphobia 24 [4.4%], social anxiety disorder 23 [4.2%], OCD 12 [2.2%], PTSD 6 [1.1%], alcohol dependence 76 [14.0%], alcohol abuse 70 [13.0%], substance dependence 45 [8.3%], substance abuse 41 [7.6%], bulimia 10 [1.8%], generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) 25 [4.6%], antisocial personality disorder 29 [5.3%], probable ADHD 30 [7.0%], intermittent explosive disorder 10 [2.1%], gambling disorder 94 [19.3%], compulsive sexual disorder 14 [2.9%], compulsive buying disorder 23 [4.7%], and binge-eating disorder 7 [1.4%]. Note that percentages do not always reflect a denominator of 543 cases as this refers to percentage of those who completed DERS (ie, for whom data were available).

Table 1. Numbers of Participants

Figure 1 illustrates the extent of difficulties in emotion regulation across different psychiatric disorders, as indicated by the Z-scores. Participants meeting criteria for probable ADHD (Z-score = 0.966) exhibited the highest Z-score, indicating greater difficulties in emotional regulation in this group. This was followed by intermittent explosive disorder (Z-score: 0.809), social phobia (Z-score: 0.668), depression (Z-score: 0.662), generalized anxiety disorder (Z-score: 0.656), antisocial personality disorder (Z-score: 0.603), agoraphobia (Z-score: 0.592), bulimia nervosa (Z-score: 0.583), PTSD (Z-score: 0.571), panic disorder (Z-score: 0.559), binge eating disorder (Z-score: 0.514), compulsive sexual behavior disorder (Z-score: 0.470), any MINI (Z-score: 0.387), substance abuse (Z-score: 0.349), gambling disorder (0.343), alcohol dependence (Z-score: 0.281), compulsive buying disorder (Z-score: 0.266) and alcohol abuse (Z-score: 0.209) reflecting notable levels of emotional dysregulation in these conditions. On the other hand, participants diagnosed with OCD (Z-score = −0.177) displayed a lower Z-score than their respective control group, suggesting comparatively lower levels of difficulties in emotional regulation in this group. Disorders such as depression, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and PTSD showed moderate Z-scores, indicating a moderate degree of difficulties in emotional regulation.

Figure 1. Profile of difficulties in emotion regulation across the range of mental health conditions. The panel shows Z-scores for DERS total score in patient groups versus controls. The dotted lines indicate the threshold for at least a small effect size deficit (Z-score ≥ 0.3), and the dashed lines show the threshold for at least a medium effect size deficit (Z-score ≥ 0.5), versus controls. The bar “Any MINI” shows the Z-Score for participants who had ANY mental disorder(s) as compared to those who had none, as a visual reference point.

Discussion

This study introduces a comprehensive evaluation of difficulties in emotional regulation across a diverse range of psychiatric disorders using the DERS. The findings reveal differential patterns of difficulties in emotional regulation, with some disorders displaying more pronounced difficulties in emotion regulation than others. Notably, ADHD emerges as a disorder characterized by substantial difficulties in emotional regulation, as evidenced by the highest Z-score observed of all the psychiatric conditions that were examined. This observation aligns with previous literature indicating a link between ADHD and emotional dysregulation.Reference Soler-Gutiérrez, Pérez-González and Mayas 31 The heightened emotional dysregulation in anxiety-related disorders aligns with the well-established connection between anxiety and emotional reactivity.Reference Mennin, Heimberg, Turk and Fresco 32 , Reference Amstadter 33

Similarly, individuals with compulsive sexual behavior disorder demonstrate distinctive patterns of emotional dysregulation. The literature on hypersexuality suggests a crucial interplay between emotion dysregulation and compulsive sexual behavior.Reference Lew-Starowicz, Lewczuk, Nowakowska, Kraus and Gola 15 , Reference Ryan, Rahm-Knigge and Mark 34 Those affected by compulsive sexual behavior often struggle with managing emotional states, and sexual arousal and engagement may serve as maladaptive coping mechanisms to regulate negative mood states. Our findings further support and extend this literature, revealing heightened emotional dysregulation within individuals with compulsive sexual behavior disorder. This emphasizes the intricate relationship between emotional dysregulation and compulsive sexual behavior, contributing to our understanding of this disorder within the broader spectrum of emotional regulation difficulties.

Personality disorders, too, have links with emotional dysregulation.Reference Salsman and Linehan 20 , Reference Carpenter and Trull 35 Previous research has illuminated the association between antisocial personality traits and challenges in effectively modulating and responding to emotions. Individuals with antisocial personality disorder may exhibit impulsivity, a characteristic linked to emotional dysregulation, further emphasizing the relevance of considering emotion regulation difficulties within this diagnostic category.Reference Garofalo, Neumann, Kosson and Velotti 36 , Reference Carlo Garofalo and Mark 37 Our study aligns with existing literature, indicating that individuals with antisocial personality disorder indeed manifest heightened emotional dysregulation. This supports the notion that exploring emotion regulation difficulties within this diagnostic category is crucial for a comprehensive understanding of the disorder.

In contrast to the above conditions, participants diagnosed with OCD showed relatively lower levels of difficulties in emotional regulation, in fact relatively less than controls with a small effect size. Earlier studies highlighted the association between OCD severity and challenges in accepting and tolerating negative emotions, aligning with the cognitive model of OCD by Salkovskis (1985).Reference Bardeen and Fergus 38 -Reference Robinson and Freeston 40 Similarly, studies on obsessive-compulsive personality disorder reported opposite directions in the link between the DERS and obsessive-compulsive symptoms.Reference Garofalo, Velotti, Callea, Popolo, Salvatore, Cavallo and Dimaggio 41 , Reference Dimaggio, Popolo and Montano 42 This discrepancy raises intriguing questions about the nuanced relationship between emotional dysregulation and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Such discrepancy may reflect the unique profile associated with OCD, which is linked to a variety of high-order executive dysfunctions whereas perhaps emotional processing is relatively spared. It is also possible that specific OCD subtypes may exhibit different levels of difficulties in emotion regulation. These results highlight the relevance of assessing emotional dysregulation across various psychiatric diagnoses and provide insights into potential areas of focus for therapeutic interventions.

While this study is one of the first to present the profile of emotional regulation across a range of mental health disorders within a single study setting, there are several important limitations that warrant consideration. Firstly, the participant sample consisted of individuals who had engaged in gambling activities on at least 5 occasions within the previous year and were not actively seeking treatment, potentially limiting the applicability of the findings to clinical populations or alternative settings. At the same time, it is worth noting of course that gambling is commonplace in most countries across the globe (eg, in the UK, around half of the adult population gambles to some degree) (source: Gambling Commission, UK). Secondly, the study’s design did not incorporate controls for confounding variables that could be differentially associated with various disorders (eg, rates of depressive symptoms) and had relatively small sample sizes for some of the disorders of interest. Relatedly, we did not control for comorbidities since the sample sizes would have been too small to facilitate this (given that comorbidity is common per disorder). It is arguably more important to consider whether affected individuals experience relative difficulties in emotional regulation in vivo, as opposed to “once comorbidities are controlled for” since the latter then would potentially underrepresent the actual problems experienced by those individuals. Similarly, we could not examine the potential role of other variables such as age, gender distribution, duration of illness, and treatments received, due to the relatively small cell sizes. For the same reason, we did not describe each disorder’s characteristics in more detail. Though the current study involved a non-treatment-seeking sample, some individuals would have been receiving previously established treatments and this information (including the duration of any such treatments) was not available nor could it have been analyzed due to the sample sizes. Additionally, although we kept the age range narrow to reduce age effects on DERS scores, the age range of participants in this study may influence the generalizability of findings. Furthermore, these results emerged from a sample of young adults with a relatively short duration of illness for most of these disorders, and therefore these findings may differ in adults with a long duration of illness (and/or duration of untreated illness). Ideally, future work would use a similar “single study” approach to look at different aspects of DERS across different disorders in a much larger sample, with a wider age range, to validate these findings and ensure they are reproducible. Lastly, we used clinical instruments that were previously validated, but did not conduct further psychometric validation (since this was outside the scope of this study).

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study sheds light on the extent of emotional dysregulation within a diverse range of psychiatric disorders. The results reveal differing patterns of emotional dysregulation across diagnoses, with probable ADHD demonstrating the highest levels of emotional dysregulation and OCD showing comparatively low levels. These findings emphasize the importance of recognizing emotional dysregulation as a trans-diagnostic feature that requires tailored treatment approaches. Future research endeavors should employ longitudinal designs and explore underlying mechanisms to enhance our understanding of emotional regulation’s role in psychiatric disorders.

Acknowledgments

The authors are entirely responsible for the scientific content of the paper.

Author contribution

Conceptualization: I.H.A., L.D., S.R.C., J.E.G.; Formal analysis: I.H.A., S.R.C.; Writing – original draft: I.H.A., L.D.; Writing – review & editing: I.H.A., L.D., S.R.C., J.E.G.; Supervision: S.R.C., J.E.G.; Data curation: J.E.G.; Funding acquisition: J.E.G.; Investigation: J.E.G.; Methodology: J.E.G.; Project administration: J.E.G.; Resources: J.E.G.

Financial support

This research was funded by the National Center for Responsible Gaming (NCRG) Center of Excellence grant to J.E.G.

Disclosure

J.E.G. receives yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Norton Press, and McGraw Hill. J.E.G. has received research grants from Janssen and Biohaven Pharmaceuticals. S.R.C. receives a stipend from Elsevier for editorial work. I.H.A. and L.D. report no disclosures.

References

Gross, JJ, Thompson, RA. Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. In: Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: The Guilford Press; 2007:324.Google Scholar
Aldao, A, Nolen-Hoeksema, S, Schweizer, S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):217237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, JJ, Muñoz, RF. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 1995;2(2):151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aslan, IH, Baldwin, DS. Ruminations and their correlates in depressive episodes: between-group comparison in patients with unipolar or bipolar depression and healthy controls. J Affect Disord. 2021;280(Pt A):16. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisler, JM, Olatunji, BO. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2012;14:182187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, K, Mogan, C, Moriarty, A, et al. Emotion regulation difficulties in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2018;74(4):695709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, H, Axelrod, S, Paliwal, P, Sleeper, J, Sinha, R. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;89(2–3):298301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berking, M, Margraf, M, Ebert, D, Wuppermann, P, Hofmann, S, Junghanns, K. Emotion regulation skills as a predictor of relapse during and after treatment of alcohol dependence. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011;79(3):307318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neophytou, K, Theodorou, M, Artemi, T-F, Theodorou, C, Panayiotou, G. Gambling to escape: a systematic review of the relationship between avoidant emotion regulation/coping strategies and gambling severity. J Context Behav Sci. 2023;27:126142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velotti, P, Rogier, G, Zobel, SB, Billieux, J. Association between gambling disorder and emotion (dys) regulation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021;87:102037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vintró-Alcaraz, C, Munguía, L, Granero, R, et al. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in eating disorders and gambling disorder: treatment outcome implications. J Behav Addict. 2022;11(1):140146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ricketts, EJ, Peris, TS, Grant, JE, et al. Clinical characteristics of youth with trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder) and excoriation (skin-picking) disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2022. doi:10.1007/s10578-022-01458-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Grant, JE. Is problematic exercise really problematic? A dimensional approach. CNS Spectr. 2020;25(1):6470. doi:10.1017/S1092852919000762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arrivillaga, C, Hallauer, CJ, Montag, C, Elhai, JD. Emotion dysregulation factors associated with problematic smartphone use severity: the mediating role of fear of missing out. Addict Behav. 2023;143:107708. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2023.107708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Starowicz, M, Lewczuk, K, Nowakowska, I, Kraus, S, Gola, M. Compulsive sexual behavior and dysregulation of emotion. Sex Med Rev. 2020;8(2):191205. doi:10.1016/j.sxmr.2019.10.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairburn, CG, Norman, PA, Welch, SL, O’Connor, ME, Doll, HA, Peveler, RC. A prospective study of outcome in bulimia nervosa and the long-term effects of three psychological treatments. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(4):304312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, M. The thin ideal, depression and eating disorders in women. Behav Res Ther. 1990;28(3):205214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sher, KJ, Grekin, ER. Alcohol and affect regulation; 2007.Google Scholar
Tice, DM, Bratslavsky, E, Baumeister, RF. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! In: Self-Regulation and Self-Control. London: Routledge; 2018:267298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salsman, NL, Linehan, MM. An investigation of the relationships among negative affect, difficulties in emotion regulation, and features of borderline personality disorder. J Psychopathol Behavior Assess. 2012;34:260267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreiber, LR, Grant, JE, Odlaug, BL. Emotion regulation and impulsivity in young adults. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(5):651658. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.02.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, OA, van Wingen, G, Soriano-Mas, C, et al. Brain circuitry of compulsivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26(5):810827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, JL, Drevets, WC. Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(1):6171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McTeague, LM, Rosenberg, BM, Lopez, JW, et al. Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in emotional processing across psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177(5):411421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(20):2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Grant, JE. Minnesota Impulse Disorders Interview (MIDI): validation of a structured diagnostic clinical interview for impulse control disorders in an enriched community sample. Psychiatry Res. 2018;265:279283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, JE. Impulse Control Disorders: A Clinician’s Guide to Understanding and Treating Behavioral Addictions. New York: WW Norton and Company; 2008.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Adler, L, Ames, M, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):245256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, JE, Steinberg, MA, Kim, SW, Rounsaville, BJ, Potenza, MN. Preliminary validity and reliability testing of a structured clinical interview for pathological gambling. Psychiatry Res. 2004;128(1):7988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratz, KL, Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26:4154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soler-Gutiérrez, A-M, Pérez-González, J-C, Mayas, J. Evidence of emotion dysregulation as a core symptom of adult ADHD: a systematic review. PloS One. 2023;18(1):e0280131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mennin, DS, Heimberg, RG, Turk, CL, Fresco, DM. Preliminary evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43(10):12811310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amstadter, A. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2008;22(2):211221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, L, Rahm-Knigge, NG, Mark, K. Eli Coleman identifying relationships between difficulties with emotion regulation and compulsive sexual behavior. Arch Sexual Behav. 2023;52:34433455. doi:10.1007/s10508-023-02690-8.Google Scholar
Carpenter, RW, Trull, TJ. Components of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15(1):335. doi:10.1007/s11920-012-0335-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garofalo, C, Neumann, CS, Kosson, DS, Velotti, P. Psychopathy and emotion dysregulation: more than meets the eye. Psychiatry Res. 2020;290:113160. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlo Garofalo, CSN, Mark, D. Associations between psychopathy and the trait meta-mood scale in incarcerated males: a combined latent variable- and person-centered approach. Inter Assoc Correct Forensic Psychol. 2019;47(3):331351. doi:10.1177/0093854819891460.Google Scholar
Bardeen, JR, Fergus, TA. An examination of the incremental contribution of emotion regulation difficulties to health anxiety beyond specific emotion regulation strategies. J Anxiety Disorder. 2014;28(4):394401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, PM. Obsessional-compulsive problems: a cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behav Research Ther. 1985;23(5):571583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, LJ, Freeston, MH. Emotion and internal experience in obsessive compulsive disorder: reviewing the role of alexithymia, anxiety sensitivity and distress tolerance. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(3):256271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garofalo, C, Velotti, P, Callea, A, Popolo, R, Salvatore, G, Cavallo, F, Dimaggio, G. Emotion dysregulation, impulsivity and personality disorder traits: a community sample study. Psychiatry Res. 2018;266:186192. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.05.067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimaggio, G, Popolo, R, Montano, A, et al. Emotion dysregulation, symptoms, and interpersonal problems as independent predictors of a broad range of personality disorders in an outpatient sample. Psychol Psychother. 2017;90(4):586599. doi:10.1111/papt.12126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Numbers of Participants

Figure 1

Figure 1. Profile of difficulties in emotion regulation across the range of mental health conditions. The panel shows Z-scores for DERS total score in patient groups versus controls. The dotted lines indicate the threshold for at least a small effect size deficit (Z-score ≥ 0.3), and the dashed lines show the threshold for at least a medium effect size deficit (Z-score ≥ 0.5), versus controls. The bar “Any MINI” shows the Z-Score for participants who had ANY mental disorder(s) as compared to those who had none, as a visual reference point.