Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T05:22:19.049Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Glycaemic load and cognition: comments concerning Marchand et al.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2020

Hayley A. Young
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Swansea University, Wales, UK
David Benton*
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Swansea University, Wales, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Letter to the Editor
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

We read with interest the study by Marchand et al. (Reference Marchand, Kendall and Rapsey1) who compared high and low glycaemic load (GL) trifles and found no significant effects on cognitive performance in young adults. As they suggest, this is a topic where there are inconsistencies due to differences in methodology. We would like to suggest three factors that may have contributed to their negative findings.

The first is the GL of the two meals. Although total carbohydrate (50 g) is reported, it is unclear how much of this came from the experimental sugars and how much came from other ingredients in the trifle. An important observation is that a relatively small difference in GL occurred (44 v. 33). The pattern of changes in blood glucose was considerably less than in many studies(Reference Young and Benton2Reference Benton, Maconie and Williams7). Studies that have compared isomaltulose and glucose, rather than sucrose, produce a bigger difference in blood glucose(Reference Young and Benton2,Reference Young and Benton3) . An obvious suggestion is that the small difference in GL played a part in the present negative findings. Additionally, there is large interpersonal variability in postprandial glucose response to the same food(Reference Zeevi, Korem and Zmora8), although this was not considered by Marchand et al. (Reference Marchand, Kendall and Rapsey1). Studies that have considered individual glycaemic responses have often found this factor to be important(Reference Young and Benton3).

Although great trouble was taken to ensure that the two breakfast items were identical in all respects other than the type of sugar, this approach ignores the interaction with other nutrients. It is possible that the nature of the foods consumed played a role in the non-significant findings. A trifle, for those unfamiliar with this English dessert, contains fruit, cake, custard, cream and often alcohol. We can only guess as to the actual ingredients but given the overall fat content you can guess that it contained dairy products, possibly milk in custard and cream. In this area, using milk is a problem, given its insulinogenic properties and the likelihood that insulin, and the consequent fall in blood glucose, is a potential mechanism(Reference Salehi, Gunnerud and Muhammed9,Reference Gunnerud, Holst and Östman10) . The insulinotropic nature of dairy products may disguise any benefits associated with a lower rather than a higher GL. Although they report that their meals were similar in all respects, they also say that some were served vegan alternatives. This must have influenced the composition of the meal as dairy products would not be served.

It is particularly surprising that the study only monitored changes for 2 h, as a frequent finding has been that changes do not occur in the first 2 h but after at least 3 h and more commonly 4 h. One study in young adults found no influence on memory after 30 or 90 min although a low GL was associated with better memory after 150 and 210 min(Reference Benton, Ruffin and Lassel11). Similarly, in children a meal sweetened with isomaltulose resulted in better mood and memory after 3 but not 1 h(Reference Young and Benton2).

We agree with the authors’ assessment that methodological issues may explain the inconsistent findings regarding glycaemia and cognitive functioning. There remains a need for studies that establish those aspects of the diet that are critical. Although negative findings have been reported, there are sufficient positive findings in the existing literature to suggest that there is an underlying beneficial mechanism. There remains the need to go beyond studies that consider GL in isolation and place it in a wider context. We hope that the suggestions presented here may lead the way to profitable future research in this area.

The authors declare they have no conflicts of interest.

References

Marchand, OM, Kendall, FE, Rapsey, CM, et al. (2020) The effect of postprandial glycaemia on cognitive function: a randomised crossover trial. Br J Nutr 123, 13571364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, H & Benton, D (2015) The effect of using isomaltulose (Palatinose™) to modulate the glycaemic properties of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children. Eur J Nutr 54, 10131020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, H & Benton, D (2014) The glycemic load of meals, cognition and mood in middle and older aged adults with differences in glucose tolerance: a randomized trial. e-SPEN J 9, e147e154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, SB, Bandelow, S, Nute, ML, et al. (2012) Breakfast glycaemic index and cognitive function in adolescent school children. Br J Nutr 107, 18231832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brindal, E, Baird, D, Slater, A, et al. (2013) The effect of beverages varying in glycaemic load on postprandial glucose responses, appetite and cognition in 10–12-year-old school children. Br J Nutr 110, 529537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, RJ & Greenwood, CE (2002) Influence of dietary carbohydrates and glycaemic response on subjective appetite and food intake in healthy elderly persons. Int J Food Sci Nutr 53, 305316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D, Maconie, A & Williams, C (2007) The influence of the glycaemic load of breakfast on the behaviour of children in school. Physiol Behav 92, 717724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeevi, D, Korem, T, Zmora, N, et al. (2015) Personalized nutrition by prediction of glycemic responses. Cell 163, 10791094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salehi, A, Gunnerud, U, Muhammed, SJ, et al. (2012) The insulinogenic effect of whey protein is partially mediated by a direct effect of amino acids and GIP on β-cells. Nutr Metab 9, 48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnerud, U, Holst, JJ, Östman, E, et al. (2012) The glycemic, insulinemic and plasma amino acid responses to equi-carbohydrate milk meals, a pilot-study of bovine and human milk. Nutr J 11, 83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D, Ruffin, M-P, Lassel, T, et al. (2003) The delivery rate of dietary carbohydrates affects cognitive performance in both rats and humans. Psychopharmacology 166, 8690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed