Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T12:23:52.720Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Communicative remembering: Revisiting a basic mnemonic concept

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 January 2024

Christian Pentzold*
Affiliation:
Department for Communication and Media Studies, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
Christine Lohmeier
Affiliation:
Department for Communication, Paris Lodron University Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
Thomas Birkner
Affiliation:
Department for Communication, Paris Lodron University Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
*
Corresponding author: Christian Pentzold; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

The article attempts to clarify what today constitutes communicative remembering. To revisit this basic mnemonic concept, our theoretical contribution starts from available approaches in social memory studies that assume a binary distinction between cultural and communicative modes of memory making. In contrast, we use concepts that treat them not as structural, historically and culturally distinct registers but as a repertoire of retrospection that hinges on the evoked temporal horizon and media usage. To further interrogate this practical articulation of memories, we direct our attention to the habitual, communicatively realised engagement with the past. We finally turn to the ways communicative remembering is done in digitally networked environments, which provide us with a pertinent mnemonic arena where rigid dichotomies of communicative memory versus cultural memory are eroded.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Collective memory comes alive in communication. It happens as an exchange about what is to be remembered and, in a more reflexive twist, about the ways in which it is to be remembered, for example, in the circle of a family, between generations, or in engagement with media messages. Indeed, if collective memory is understood ‘not as a collection of individual memories or some magically constructed reservoir of ideas, but rather as a socially articulated and socially maintained “reality of the past,” then it also makes sense to look at the most basic and accessible means for memory articulation and maintenance – talk’, as Irwin-Zarecka (Reference Irwin-Zarecka1994, 54–55) puts it. We start from this elementary idea. Or, to follow Huyssen (Reference Huyssen1995, 3): ‘The past is not simply there in memory, but it must be articulated to become memory’.

Although talk is a prime mode of collective memory making, recollections do not necessarily take shape in face-to-face conversation. In fact, communicative remembering, which is first and foremost a practice and not a state of communicative memory, encompasses various ways of conveying the past. It involves all sorts of expressive gestures, semiotic resources, and forms of cultural representation (Erll Reference Erll2011, 113). In this kaleidoscope of articulation, the available past is reactivated in light of current societal conditions, cultural relations, and systems of knowledge and is therefore constituted as meaningful and significant in a specific present (Bal et al. Reference Bal, Crewe and Spitzer1999; Bietti Reference Bietti2014; Lowenthal Reference Lowenthal2015). What is more, acknowledging that collective memory is an inherently communicative affair implies that it is processual in nature. A principal tenet of social memory studies is that remembering is a process through which temporally anchored and spatially bounded versions of the past are reconstructed. This processuality has been stressed by Olick (Reference Olick, Erll and Nünning2010, 159), who notes that ‘[c]ollective memory is something – or rather many things – we do, not something – or many things – we have’.

Surprisingly, while communication processes are fundamentally important for collective remembering, theories of and empirical research on social memory studies have focused elsewhere, namely on media formats, public institutions, and technologies that influence the propagation and canonisation of references to the past. In these studies, cultural history and the genealogy of memory practices intertwine with media archaeology (Erll and Rigney Reference Erll and Rigney2009; Garde-Hansen Reference Garde-Hansen2011). In other studies, communicative memory is often contrasted with cultural memory and located within a time period of three to four human generations, during which the exchange of immediate, lived experience gives way to transmission through hearsay and material records (Assmann Reference Assmann2011; Vansina Reference Vansina1985). In yet another set of studies, periods in history, during which particular media are believed to have moulded the cultural dynamics and practices, are consecutively placed one after another (LeGoff Reference LeGoff1992; Leroi-Gourhan Reference Leroi-Gourhan1993). In such longue durée approaches, communicative remembering stands at the dawn of civilisation as a trait of oral cultures that did not have access to writing, recording, and storage media, meaning that memory had to depend on rhetorical devices such as formulaic chants and metrical poems (Havelock Reference Havelock1988; Lord Reference Lord1991; Parry Reference Parry1987).

However, communicative remembering cannot be reduced to a practice of bygone eras or to an ever-shifting phenomenon of intergenerational communion, since the techniques and cultural forms that carry it are always accomplished according to the affordances of expression, mediation, and preservation available at a given time. As such, they criss-cross established categorisations that hope to distinguish moments of crystallised cultural memorialisation from more fluid kinds of reminiscence. In this respect, Welzer (Reference Welzer, Erll and Nünning2010, 285) writes that ‘[c]ultural’ and ‘communicative memory’ can only be strictly separated in a theoretical context; in the actual memory practice of individuals and social groups, their forms and methods are linked together’. Rather than using the notion to distinguish periods in time or to mark out historical transitions, we face the task of clarifying what constitutes communicative remembering as a perennial, though shifting, set of mnemonic practices.

Taking the practical side of communicative remembering seriously, our conceptual contribution revisits this basic mnemonic concept by looking at the routine articulation and propagation of memories in their corporeal and mediated accomplishment. To this end, we discuss the term communicative remembering, bring together existing approaches that aim to capture the habitual, communicative engagement with the past and address the ways communicative remembering is done in digitally networked environments, where rigid distinctions of communicative memory versus cultural memory fall apart.

Drawing this eclectic collection of perspectives together, we argue that communicative remembering is not the counterpart to cultural memory. In fact, the dichotomy may serve analytical purposes but it does not help us to appreciate the forms of expressive and interactional memory making. Rather than establishing a catalogue of criteria that define and distinguish communicative memory, we suggest interrogating the inconstant practice of communicative remembering, which changes with shifting social constellations and media environments. By focusing on communicative remembering, we seek to avoid hierarchising the relationship between cultural memory and communicative memory.

Communicative memory/cultural memory

Memories are commonly invoked as ‘the present past’, Terdiman (Reference Terdiman1993, 8) states. They are not portrayals of past events or experiences but present-day recollections of the past. In Halbwachs's (1992 [Reference Halbwachs1925], 1980 [Reference Halbwachs1950]) classical reflections on socially shaped individual memory, the collectively shared frames of reference (cadres sociaux) – and thus the horizons of meaning and interpretation in which and through which remembering becomes meaningfully possible – are conveyed primarily through communicative exchanges. In social groups, above all the family, religious communities, and social classes, collective memory is constituted in interaction and communication, and members of these constellations locate their personal reminiscences within these mnemonic frames, through colloquial talk and narration or via the production and sharing of images or written records. ‘All individual remembering, that is, takes place with social materials, within social contexts, and in response to social cues’, as Olick et al. (Reference Olick, Vinitzky-Seroussi, Levy, Olick, Vinitzky-Seroussi and Levy2011, 19) have summarised.

Communicative remembering thus prefigures individual perception and memory (Fentress and Wickham Reference Fentress and Wickham1992; Middleton and Edwards Reference Middleton, Edwards, Middleton and Edwards1990; Welzer Reference Welzer, Erll and Nünning2010). It provides fertile ground for interdisciplinary inquiry, including in cognitive science and (neuro-)psychology. One notable area of interest pertains to collaborative remembering, that is, studies of the interplay of individual and group processes. Collaborative remembering, meaning ‘remembering with an intended future audience, remembering in the presence of others, remembering in direct collaboration with others, and remembering in larger social and cultural contexts’ (Meade et al. Reference Meade, Harris, Van Bergen, Sutton, Barnier, Meade, Harris, Van Bergen, Sutton and Barnier2018), is predicated on conversation. Research has, for instance, shown how individuals tune their stories with respect to audience reactions, to serve an intended audience and in line with cultural expectations. Further, work by Maswood et al. (Reference Maswood, Rasmussen and Rajaram2019) indicates that collaborative remembering of a shared event leads to down-regulated negative emotions and increases the recall of details from the event and its environment.

This does not mean that remembering must always be consensual or generate uniform binding memories. Rather, the unifying element lies in the continuously performed acts of shared, intergenerational and intragenerational remembering in which pasts are retold and passed on (Bellah et al. 2007 [Reference Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler and Tipton1985]; Bietti Reference Bietti2014; Markowitsch and Welzer Reference Markowitsch and Welzer2009). Social affiliations and the formation of identities, both individual and collective, therefore rely on the communal evocation of the past.

Uses of the past: communicative memory versus cultural memory

Arguably, the most influential treatise on communicative memory comes from Jan Assmann (Reference Assmann2011) and Aleida Assmann (Reference Assmann, Goodin and Tilly2006, Reference Assmann2012). In their writings, they mainly conceptualise it as a foil for the more prominent notion of cultural memory, which also features in the title of Jan Assmann's (Reference Assmann2011) seminal study on memorialisation in Ancient Egypt and among the Israelites. Communicative memory and cultural memory are understood as two distinct registers of collective memory; the former is based on everyday fleeting conversations, whereas the latter is tied to permanent objectifications that are passed on over time.

Cultural memory, then, is defined as ‘a collective concept for all knowledge that directs behaviour and experience in the interactive framework of a society and one that obtains through generations in repeated societal practice and initiation’ (Assmann Reference Assmann1995, 126). As such, it relies on material storage and preservation. Its fixed documents and canonised interpretations are recalled and reconfirmed over long periods of time in recurrent, ritualised form and through fixed, ceremonialised procedures. Often, this repetitive commemorative re-enactment is reserved for members of the cultural elite such as priests, shamans, or seers.

In contrast, communicative memory ‘includes those varieties of collective memory that are based exclusively on everyday communications’, Jan Assmann (Reference Assmann1995, 126) writes. It is bound to its bearers and moves along with the succession of generations and their horizons of experience and reminiscence. It thus includes, he (Reference Assmann2011, 36) further notes, ‘memories related to the recent past. These are what the individual shares with his [sic] contemporaries’. The process in which communicatively mediated memories are lost or reshaped as part of cultural memory is what Vansina (Reference Vansina1985, 23) calls the ‘floating gap’ between two memory frames that replace each other (Barnes 1990 [Reference Barnes and Barnes1947]). Because communicative memory is deemed to hinge on the presence of generational contemporaries, the transition from communicative to cultural memory seems unidirectional – once the carriers have disappeared, there is only a chance for cultural memorialisation that may perpetuate some but not all lived memories.

In Jan Assmann's conception, the ‘two uses of the past’ (Reference Assmann2011, 37) of communicative memory and cultural memory are distinguished from each other by five polarities (Table 1). At the level of time structure, they differ in scope and thus either encompass three to four generations in a timeframe of 80–100 years or extend to an imaginary prehistory. The second level, the level of forms, locates communicative memory in everyday life and thus in informal routines, while cultural memory is marked as being extraordinary; it has its place in festive events and days of significance within liturgical, regimented, and repeatedly performed rituals. Hence, a distinction is made between the profane and the sacred. This results in a contrast at the level of content, between memory as biographical experiences in the recent past versus the memory of an individually inaccessible, mythical time in a distant past. In this way, a polarised structure of participation is created, with diffuse involvement in communicative remembering, in which the carriers of memory share various and differently elaborated accounts and experts take no priority on one side, and cultural memory, where participation is exclusive and limited to specialists like scholars, scribes and clergy members on the other. Finally, the two uses of the past are separated by their media. Here, orally transmitted experiences and hearsay are juxtaposed with symbolically coded and fixed objectifications as well as ritual celebrations.

Table 1. Communicative memory and cultural memory

Source: Assmann (Reference Assmann2011, 41).

The two registers and their differences are metaphorically captured by the terms ‘fluid’ and ‘fixed’ memory (Assmann Reference Assmann2011, 43). In the same vein, Aleida Assmann (Reference Assmann2012) has gone on to speak of an ‘inhabited’ functional memory and an ‘uninhabited’ storage memory (Erll Reference Erll2011, 36). In their relationship, dynamics of actualisation and forgetting become salient, with functional memory constituting the selective, present-day commemoration and storage memory providing a bank of potentially rememberable content that is curated and manifested in art, literature, media, museums, or science. Selectively, not in total, can they pass into functional memory, where they become reappropriated and reconfigured.

With Jan Assmann (Reference Assmann2011, 41) stressing that communicative memory and cultural memory are seldom sharply separated realms but reside on a ‘sliding scale’, the relationship between the two registers is far from straightforward. Even though the two registers are treated as discrete mnemonic modes separated by time structures, forms, contents, carriers, and media, it is difficult to establish and uphold such a partition in practice. Neither the binary of ‘fluid’ and ‘fixed’ nor that of ‘functional’ or ‘storage’ memory neatly map onto the distinction of communicative versus cultural memory. As a case in point, when Jan Assmann (Reference Assmann1995, 130) claims that memory might appear ‘first in the mode of potentiality of the archive whose accumulated texts, images, and rules of conduct act as a total horizon, and second in the mode of actuality, whereby each contemporary context puts the objectivised meaning into its own perspective, giving it its own relevance’, he is referring to cultural memory, not communicative memory. Indeed, when conceived as such, communicative memory becomes confined to recollection tied to the embodied experiences of a finite group of human carriers and their mundane communications. This, however, does not include all types of communicative actualisation that may happen in the exclusive rituals of cultural memory.

Nevertheless, for all the inconsistencies that emerge when seeking to uphold a meaningful distinction, the catchy juxtaposition of two forms of collective memory surfaces in various concepts, such as vernacular and official memory (Bodnar Reference Bodnar1992), lived and distant memory (Hirst and Manier Reference Hirst, Manier, Echterhoff and Saar2002), or fluid and crystallised memory (Pentzold Reference Pentzold2009). All of these try to capture the conditionality of memory as well as the different practices, media, and social contexts of remembering. They are all rendered problematic when the dichotomies are not primarily taken as analytical categories but treated as if they are empirical characteristics.

Modi memorandi: communicative memory and cultural memory

Acknowledging the somewhat artificial nature of a clear-cut division into communicative versus cultural memory, Jan Assmann (Reference Assmann2011, 37) asserts that there are ‘two modes of remembering – two uses of the past – that need to be carefully distinguished even though they are largely connected in real historical culture’. Regarding the different kinds of media associated with communicative memory and cultural memory, he (Reference Assmann2011, 43) further stresses that this does not necessarily coincide with the distinction between cultures based on orality and those based on writing.

Indeed, communicative memory or, to put it more succinctly, remembering, is not diachronically replaced by cultural memory once communities are able to chronicle their memories. Instead, both registers coexist synchronously, though writing cultures usually possess a more elaborate apparatus of media-based mnemotechniques and means of expression. Yet, in terms of engagement with a past, communicative memory may not be deficient per se as it, too, rests on a versatile repertoire of practices. In turn, the formation and instantiation of cultural memory are grounded on conversations and personal storytelling, whereas cultures without scripture rely on oral traditions that afford both a communicative and a cultural framework of memory making.

Ultimately, the question then is if these suggestive dichotomies should be treated as conceptual or as historical categories. In these terms, Erll (Reference Erll2011, 31) explains that the pairs of terms may serve as designations for two modi memorandi: they are distinct from one another when theoretically regarded as different ways of remembering and different modes of mnemonic referentiality. But the attempt to locate the abstract categories empirically is prone to fail, she argues, when the modes are meant to be bound to concrete historical moments and cultural circumstances. Consequently, whether a past is remembered in everyday communication or through ritual depends on the chosen register and not vice versa. Thus, communicative memory and cultural memory present themselves as a repertoire of retrospection that can be employed to articulate memories within the finite timescale of a given situation or by invoking the temporally distant horizon of bygone times.

In other words, recalling and mobilising a past might be done in the communicative mode within a narrow scope of the everyday, based on biographical experiences and in relation to social reference groups. Or it can be done in the cultural mode that reaches more distant temporal horizons, pertains to events that assume collective significance and mobilises far-ranging systems of meaning. ‘This means’, Erll (Reference Erll2011, 31; emphases in original) concludes, ‘that in a given historical context, the same event can become simultaneously an object of the Cultural Memory and of the communicative memory’. It is not the temporal structure of an experienced and non-experienced past or the linear progression of time that is crucial, but a temporal consciousness that renders it possible to modulate and adjust the mnemonic proximity or distance of events (Wodianka Reference Wodianka, Erll and Nünning2005). ‘The central criterion to differentiate the “Cultural” from the “communicative” mode of remembering is therefore, it seems, not the measurable time . . . It is rather the way of remembering chosen by a community, the collective idea of the meaning of past events’, Erll (Reference Erll2011, 32) adds. One example mentioned by Erll is the Christian practice of biblical reading, which can take various forms, from creating distance through liturgy and dogma, making it the property of the few, to creating biographical closeness by personal practices of piety. Another one could be the classical ideals of friendship in antiquity, which can either be an object of learned study or a guide for practical friendship. Hence, the intertwining of cultural and communicative memory predates digital media.

Media, as communication technologies and institutional systems, exert an important influence on the mode in which a past is or ought to be remembered. This mnemonic potential of news media and documentary formats, but also of fictional reappraisals of historical moments, has become particularly evident with events such as 9/11 (Grusin Reference Grusin2010; Simpson Reference Simpson2006), the fall of the Berlin Wall (Sonnevend Reference Sonnevend2016), the assassination of John F. Kennedy (Zelizer Reference Zelizer1992), the death of Lady Diana (Seidler Reference Seidler2013), the release of Nelson Mandela from Victor Verster prison (Wasserman Reference Wasserman2018), or the Bhopal gas disaster (Bisht Reference Bisht2013). As media events, according to Dayan and Katz (Reference Dayan and Katz1992), they interrupted everyday life; as fixed points in a culture of memory, they monumentalise public remembering and shape personal reminiscence. From the second half of the 20th century until recently, television not only created the possibility of worldwide attention and mediated eye-witnessing, it also opened up the framework in which an event could be remembered at all. ‘Media events endow collective memory not only with a substance but with a frame: they are mnemonics for organising personal and historical time. To members of the same generation, media events provide shared reference points, the sense of a common past, as well as bridges between personal and collective history’, as Dayan and Katz (Reference Dayan and Katz1992, 212) have put it.

So, for instance, the memory of 9/11 was not only borne by the communicative exchange of experiences of a worldwide life audience. Quickly thereafter, it became the focus of official commemoration ventures meant to anchor and direct biographical historical experiences (Hess and Herbig Reference Hess and Herbig2013). This entanglement of cultural memorialisation and communicative memory work has, it can be assumed, gained traction in the age of reflexive modernisation, when ways of life and experiences are short-circuited with mediated stores of meaning (Giddens Reference Giddens, Beck, Giddens and Lash1994; Levy Reference Levy, Gudehus, Eichenberg and Welzer2010; Pentzold et al. Reference Pentzold, Lohmeier, Hajek, Hajek, Lohmeier and Pentzold2016). In contrast with these types of immediate ceremonialisation, musealisation, and monumentalisation, Erll (Reference Erll2011, 32–33) points out that events that happened far in the past may again become part of communicative remembering, for example, when the study of religious texts is not first and foremost perceived as a tradition with a complex legacy of translation and redaction but as a living source of experience that is intimately connected to personal life.

Furthermore, Levy (Reference Levy, Gudehus, Eichenberg and Welzer2010) has underscored that the modes of cultural and communicative remembering are not congruent and that a publicly established cultural memory does not need to determine more personally shared communicative mnemonic practices. The mode of communicative memory is not inescapably subject to dominant institutionalised cultural memories, and communication research on media appropriation has not ceased to emphasise the resistance and self-determination of audiences and the polysemy of media offerings (Fiske Reference Fiske1989; Hall Reference Hall and Hall1997). Neither are mnemonic media formats read and understood in an unambiguous way nor is there a direct transfer into the beliefs and knowledge of a public. It is precisely in the relation between familial and personal recollections that possibilities of creative and resistant readings of the available mnemonic narratives open up, and it is questionable how much attention and allegiance is to be demanded from purportedly authoritative views. Thus, Kansteiner (Reference Kansteiner2002, 193) suggests that ‘the more “collective” the medium (the larger its potential or actual audience), the less likely it is that its representation will reflect the collective memory of that audience.’ The patterns of interpretation and symbolic cues taken for granted by social elites, the media, educators, and cultural institutions may not, as a matter of course, also be carried by the people or even be acknowledged at all.

Still, with media, and mass media in particular, there seems to be a collectivisation of communicative memory, as it informs media events and broadcasts. This, however, does not challenge the relationship between the two modes, nor does it call into question the significance of communicative memory vis-à-vis cultural memory. It instead makes us aware of yet another false distinction that underpins this dichotomy. Communicative memory is not per se bound to personal communication, and cultural memory does not hinge on collective arenas. In contrast, the two modes scale from individual memory work to extensive societal endeavours (Keightley et al. Reference Keightley, Pickering, Bisht, Park and Maurantonio2019).

Communicative remembering as practice

Having discarded the strict dichotomy between cultural and communicative memory in favour of a more flexible notion of convertible modi memorandi, we now turn our attention to the practical articulation of this repertoire of retrospection. Instead of being a structural condition, it becomes a matter of practical choices and media usages that evoke the registers of communicative remembering or cultural memory. This consequently requires us to explain the kinds of practices through which they come into being, are formed, and are collectively shared in patterns of action. To grasp communicative remembering, we need to inquire into the practical accomplishment of mnemonic practices, which include, according to Olick (Reference Olick, Erll and Nünning2010, 158), communicative activities such as ‘reminiscence, recall, representation, commemoration, celebration, regret, renunciation, disavowal, denial, rationalisation, excuse, acknowledgment, and many others’.

Basically speaking, communicative remembering is done through practices that evoke a past. More precisely, Connerton (Reference Connerton1989) distinguishes between inscribing practices and incorporating practices. Inscribing practices are used to produce representations; incorporating practices refer to the corporeal rehearsal of knowledge and the learning of skills that are directed towards evoking a past. Thus, the activities aimed at materialising memory artefacts, media content, and memorabilia together with bodily anchored and performed doings and sayings are catalysts for and components of collective memory. Of course, inscribing practices in the arts and media, in journalism, and in culture have received most of the attention, whereas incorporating practices have still attracted comparatively little critical reflection. Likewise, early contributions, such as Bergson's (1990 [Reference Bergson1896]) notion of ‘habit memory’, which is thought to consist of the habitual dispositions gained through mimicry and repeated acts of memorialisation, have not yet been analysed in terms of communication. More than creating all kinds of material mnemonic texts and artefacts, the practices themselves are memory devices too that carry and perpetuate habitual ways of enacting retrospective references.

Engaging more seriously with remembering as a practice, Schatzki (Reference Schatzki2010, 219–220) employs the term ‘practice memory’, that is, ‘the continuing presence of an ability acquired in the past, not knowledge, belief, or thought about the past’. The practical side of communicative remembering is, however, not only an individual matter. Mnemonic routines are socially shared, and it is through the collective enactment, propagation, and regulation of the corporeal conduct of recollection and commemoration that its carriers come to form part of mnemonic communities of practice (Wagner-Pacifici Reference Wagner-Pacifici1996). Hence, communicative remembering is structured by narrative templates, dramaturgical schemes, and conventional combinations of textual, visual, audio, and audiovisual elements. They are furthermore accompanied by expectations regarding communication roles and by the relational ties established with them. In essence, such patterns can be understood, drawing on Bergmann and Luckmann (Reference Bergmann, Luckmann and Quasthoff1995, 289), as communicative genres. They are ‘solutions to specifically communicative problems’.

Genres solidify structures of communication and constellations of participation that guide the realisation of a particular communicative act in a concrete situation. In terms of memory, Knoblauch and Günthner (Reference Knoblauch and Günthner1995) use the concept of reconstructive communicative genres to stress that communicative remembering is based on the institutionalised forms through which mnemonic references are established and memory practices become socially intelligible. This constitutive relationship between communicative routines and sociality has, for example, been at the heart of the formation and transformation of ars memoriae since antiquity (Carruthers Reference Carruthers1990; Yates 2011 [Reference Yates1966]). They afford the ways by which remembering happens in particular constellations of memory agents, and they continue to shape orders of knowledge and recurring tropes (Blair Reference Blair, Shepherd, St. John and Striphas2006; Hutton Reference Hutton1993).

From the perspective of the sociology of knowledge, the practical linkage between the two registers of communicative remembering and cultural memory can be understood as resulting in steps of externalisation, objectivation, and sedimentation – of both mnemonic content and mnemonic forms, the references to pasts and the communicative practices with which they become articulated. For this purpose, Berek (Reference Berek, Sebald and Wagle2016) uses the idea of an inventory of knowledge, which, according to Berger and Luckmann (Reference Berger and Luckmann1967) and Schütz (Reference Schütz1967), represents the reservoir of personal and communal objectivations. Collective memory then refers to a subset of the more general inventory of available practical and propositional knowledge by its retrospective orientation. Experiences, opinions, and beliefs take shape in objectivations, whereby semiotic systems, first and foremost language, ensure that these can be intersubjectively shared. ‘As far as social relations are concerned, language “makes present” for me not only fellowmen [sic!] who are physically absent at the moment, but fellowmen in the remembered or reconstructed past’, as Berger and Luckmann (Reference Berger and Luckmann1967, 54) have declared. Objectivations render individual memories explicit and thus transferable to other temporal or spatial contexts, that is, they form sediments upon which ongoing memory work rests. Put differently, individual impressions and experiences must be publicly expressed in order to feed into the collective stores of knowledge that make up social reality. Being more than the sum of personal ideas and statements, they are organised along shared epistemic classifications and cultural orders of value and worth (Dimbath and Heinlein Reference Dimbath and Heinlein2022).

Communicative remembering in digitally networked media

The distinction between cultural and communicative memory is not only conceptually problematic; the boundaries between the two mnemonic modes have also been blurred by the shift towards digital, networked communication services, and platforms. Indeed, communicative remembering is done in digitally networked environments. These digital environments constitute a pertinent mnemonic arena where rigid dichotomies of communicative memory versus cultural memory practically fall apart.

The constitutive entanglement of communicative remembering and cultural memory has become particularly evident with today's comprehensive mediatisation: while, in writing cultures, little communicative remembering has typically occurred in pure face-to-face exchange without any kind of technological mediation, the volume and systemic impact of media in all spheres of life and society, propelled by digitisation and networking, has fundamentally reshuffled the two registers of memory making beyond written language (Birkner and Donk Reference Birkner and Donk2020). The increasing penetration of all spheres of life and work contexts in and through media – the ‘mediation of everything’, as Livingstone (Reference Livingstone2009, 1) has dubbed it – has deprived existing mnemonic distinctions of their plausibility. In effect, as van Dijck (Reference van Dijck2007, 15) claims, they are ‘fallacious binary oppositions’ with regard to the importance of media for all kinds of memory making that are reconfigured in the course of an ongoing mediatisation and cannot be clearly separated into cultural or communicative modes. Instead, she (Reference van Dijck2007, 21) proposes to speak of ‘mediated memories’ as ‘activities and objects we produce and appropriate by means of media technologies, for creating and re-creating a sense of past, present, and future of ourselves and in relation to others’. A case in point concerns the difficulties in defining much of what is written online in terms of existing text genres. Given its stylistic orientation towards spoken discourse and the constant innovation of paratextual elements such as emoticons and emojis, it might be more helpful to think of the pragmatics of online communication on a continuum between orality and literality (Herring et al. Reference Herring, Stein and Virtanen2013).

Moreover, Wertsch (Reference Wertsch2002, 6) underscores the essential mediation of collective memory that is always dependent on being expressed in some kind of semiotic vessel. Consequently, there is no socially relevant remembering without the multi-layered media-based articulation, transmission, and propagation of things past. ‘Memory – both individual and collective – is viewed as “distributed” between agents and texts, and the task becomes one of listening for the texts and the voices behind them as well as the voices of the particular individuals using these texts in particular settings’ he asserts. Mediated remembering, in which media technologies and communicative formats are fundamental conditions and not mere facilities, makes it necessary to redefine the relationships between communicatively and culturally realised memory references, between past-oriented self-reassurance and communalisation, and between differently public forms of retrospective communication (Pentzold et al. Reference Pentzold, Lohmeier, Hajek, Hajek, Lohmeier and Pentzold2016).

At the same time, the obsolete distinction between communicative and cultural memory demonstrates how closely the categoric pairs were tied to given media (Neiger et al. Reference Neiger, Zandberg and Meyers2011). This holds true for other similar dichotomies, such as the ones between internalised corporeal memory and externalised media memory or private and public memories. Yet whereas mnemonic concepts from the beginning of the 20th century were closely linked to the establishment of mass media, such as the press, cinema, radio, and television, the rise of the internet and services emanating from it, as well as the ubiquitous use of mobile media, has required a reorientation. In that sense, Hoskins (Reference Hoskins and Hoskins2018, 85–86) explains: ‘Collective memory . . . needs upgrading in light of the digital's ushering in of much more complex dialogic modes of communication undermining previous configurations of individual-group-societal relations, and the forging of new flexible community types’.

Whereas memory collectives used to be formed along the lines of a common orientation towards media events, which were transported and produced by mass media, these synchronous collective experiences are vanishing. In media terms, they are being complemented by automated, algorithmically controlled mechanisms for recording, evaluating, curating, moderating, and personalising information and messages in ‘multimodal memory practices’ (Burkey Reference Burkey2020, 185). For the retrospective repertoire, this change means that the roles of producers and recipients of mnemonic communication are being modified. Their experiences with and participation in collective memory are no longer organised through the active production of content and passively imagined spectatorship but along unequal possibilities of participation and diverse memory practices in the networked evocation of the past (Garde-Hansen et al. Reference Garde-Hansen, Hoskins and Reading2009; Lohmeier and Pentzold Reference Lohmeier and Pentzold2014; Neiger Reference Neiger2020; Prey and Smit Reference Prey, Smit and Papacharissi2018).

To make this shift explicit, Hoskins (Reference Hoskins and Hoskins2018, 86) has spoken of the ‘memory of the multitude’, which is characterised by technological and social connectivity but not by traditional forms of collectivity. Elsewhere, he (Reference Assmann2011, 272) explains what this means: ‘Memory is not in this way a product of individual or collective remembrances but is instead generated through the flux of contacts between people and digital technologies and media’. This reconfiguration of communicative and thus also of mnemonic references and types of participation is not to be equated with the dissolution of collectively connecting and binding memories. Rather, the traditional institutions and actors that have controlled a collective's coming to terms with the past have been supplemented and reshaped by new ones, prominently in the guise of platforms as well as transnationally emerging, only loosely organised ad-hoc movements (Kaun Reference Kaun2016; Merrill et al. Reference Merrill, Keightley and Daphi2020; Smit et al. Reference Smit, Heinrich and Broersma2018). The services and applications they employ also aggregate and channel representations and reports; however, they follow different criteria oriented towards measurable attention and quantifiable engagement and do not obey the agendas of mass media and legacy institutions as the traditional arbiters of collective memory.

The basis for this transformation is the archival function of digital networked communication media. Today's Big Five (Alphabet-Google, Meta-Facebook, Apple, Amazon, and Microsoft) and their Chinese counterparts Tencent, Alibaba, Baidu, and JD.com provide a comprehensive infrastructure through which many communications and transactions take place. Yet their mnemonic potential lies in the permanent recording, accumulation, and perpetual evaluation and reorganisation of data (Annabell Reference Annabell2022; Corry Reference Corry2023; Kang et al. Reference Kang, Kim, Baek and Park2023; Kidd and McAvoy Reference Kidd and McAvoy2023). Invoking a distinction made by Garde-Hansen (Reference Garde-Hansen2011, 72), we can say that digital media first create archives of past activities. As archival instruments and vast commercial repositories, they occupy a powerful position in processing and mediating socially relevant memory. Second, digital media are self-archiving. As a ‘system that is permanently archiving presence’ (Ernst Reference Ernst and Hoskins2018, 144), they are not merely conductors, since all the data they capture are also stored, recombined, and transmitted in dynamically adapted arrangements. Finally, digital media are creative archives, or ‘rogue archives’, as De Kosnick (Reference De Kosnick2016) calls them; they allow users to engage with the media evidence of their past or materials of recorded events made available to them, and this affirmative or critical engagement can range from reproductive repetition to creative redesign. That includes, for example, Facebook Memories features such as ‘On this day’ or the Timehop app, which retrospectively pulls entries and material from other platforms and services on a given calendar day and invites users to revisit these memories and again make them part of their communication (Hoskins Reference Hoskins, Hajek, Lohmeier and Pentzold2016).

The permanent availability and reactivity of data traces increase their mnemonic potential. Sure, not all postings and comments, images, or videos are stored and shared with explicit mnemonic intent. However, they are constantly available for memory work and are also placed in retrospective contexts by platform features that relate personal and social remembering to each other (Holloway and Green Reference Holloway and Green2017). ‘Products of memory . . . are rarely the result of a simple desire to produce a mnemonic aid or capture a moment for future recall’, van Dijck (Reference van Dijck2007, 7) explains; she continues: ‘Instead, we may discern different intentions in the creation of memory products: we can take a picture just for the sake of photographing or to later share the photographed moment with friends’. On a similar note, Burkey (Reference Burkey2020) highlights the mnemonic potential of shared content, which might prompt, accidentally or not, recollections and further memory work.

In this context, the boundaries between functional and storage memory ultimately become blurred, because reciprocal processes of archiving and updating take place dynamically and have become an almost inevitable aspect of communication. Modes of communicative remembering and cultural memory deemed separated from one another by temporal, material, and sociostructural conditions have been superseded by datafied memory practices. Databases are not archives in which information is immediately visible or available. Their relevance, presence, and presentation instead hinge on the interlocking selection, filtering, and evaluation by automated operations and user choice (Jacobsen and Beer Reference Jacobsen and Beer2021; Makhortykh Reference Makhortykh2021). Because references to things past are not only intentional and planned but also arise randomly and in unforeseen ways, Schwarz (Reference Schwarz2014, 7) speaks of ‘neighbourly relations’ of sharing potentially memorable content, by means of which users will encounter the past based on data that feed into communication. Similarly, Hoskins (Reference Hoskins and Hoskins2018, 92) has pointed to the ‘accumulative digital potential to return (and transform) past personal, semi-public and public relations through the unforeseeable re-activation of latent and semi-latent connections of shadow archives’. This does not necessarily just include reactions to happy memories – algorithmically determined relevance can also confront users with painful recollections (Humphreys Reference Humphreys2018).

Conclusion

In this article, we have revisited the positioning of communicative remembering in conceptualisations dealing with the articulation of memory. Scrutinising theoretical approaches in social memory studies, we have questioned the binary distinction between cultural and communicative modes of memory making. Especially when we include recent technological innovation and everyday mnemonic practices, we find that the intuitive distinction between communicative memory and cultural memory as originally outlined by Jan and Aleida Assmann no longer holds. Indeed, the mnemonic dichotomy between communicative memory and cultural memory that undergirds other binary distinctions, like vernacular and official memory (Bodnar Reference Bodnar1992), lived and distant memory (Hirst and Manier Reference Hirst, Manier, Echterhoff and Saar2002), or fluid and crystallised memory (Pentzold Reference Pentzold2009), is conceptually problematic and empirically hard to pin down. Rather than denoting states of what memory is, they at best serve as analytical sensibilities that direct our view towards the varying ways, media and social constellations people employ to engage in memory making.

Instead, building on Erll's understanding of modi memorandi, we proposed to conceptualise memories as part of a mnemonic repertoire for evoking the past in two different registers, one referring to a distant past of cultural commemoration and canonisation and one to the recent past of social and biographical sensemaking. The articulation of specific memorial incidences is interconnected to different temporal horizons and the type of media available at a given moment. Digitally networked environments bring their inherent logic, including algorithmic sorting, to the practical articulation of memory. Although questions of the power and agency needed to actively shape mnemonic articulations are certainly relevant, it is the inner workings of the current media that configure the practical possibilities of expressive memory work. This means that it becomes difficult to determine the characteristics of communicative remembering once and for all, as it is an ever-changing socio-material practice that evolves with the available media.

Now, the ongoing formation of expressive memory work is going in different directions. One is the more-than-human dimension of today's communicative remembering. In the past, the classic binary construction emphasised the material side of cultural memory. This is predicated on the durability and recognition of media carriers, which afford certain kinds of records and acts of recollection, be they collective or individual. In contrast, communicative memory was believed to not require any material objects except its human carriers, who are in conversation. We found that these strict separations were no longer plausible when considering the multifarious semiotic ways of expression available in computer-mediated communication. This has made us aware of the contribution of non-human agents to memory work.

For one, there are platforms and apps that prefigure people's ability to recollect things past. They reposit digital keepsakes, be them images, music, or texts, and they provide forums for reminiscence and commemoration. What is more, next to human users, other communicative agents like virtual personas, ‘thanabots’, and voice assistants are taking on an increasingly important role in shaping communicative remembering (Henrickson Reference Henrickson2023; Kansteiner Reference Kansteiner2022; Sisto and Mclellan-Broussard Reference Sisto and Mclellan-Broussard2020). Their power is grounded on the exploitation of large troves of data, which in itself renders them inherently past bound. Together with other AI-driven creative software, such as image generators, they are likely to alter our understanding and practice of collective remembering once again.

Data availability statement

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Funding statement

Funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – Project-ID 389196641 – AOBJ (641868)

References

Annabell, T (2022) Sharing ‘Memories’ on Instagram. Narrative Inquiry 33(2), 317341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assmann, A (2006) Memory, individual and collective. In Goodin, R and Tilly, C (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Contextual Political Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 210222.Google Scholar
Assmann, A (2012) Cultural Memory and Western Civilization: Functions, Media, Archives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Assmann, J (1995) Collective memory and cultural identity. New German Critique 65, 125133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assmann, J (2011) Cultural Memory and Early Civilization: Writing, Remembrance, and Political Imagination. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bal, M, Crewe, J and Spitzer, L (eds.) (1999) Acts of Memory: Cultural Recall in the Present. Hanover: University Press of New England.Google Scholar
Barnes, JA (1990 [1947]) Structural amnesia. In Barnes, JA (ed.), Models and Interpretation. Selected Essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 227229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellah, R, Madsen, R, Sullivan, WM, Swidler, A and Tipton, SM (2007 [1985]) Habits of the Heart. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Berek, M (2016) The construction of coherence. Reconsidering the political functions of memory. In Sebald, G and Wagle, J (eds.), Theorizing Social Memories. Concepts and Contexts. London/New York: Routledge, 122137.Google Scholar
Berger, PL and Luckmann, T (1967) The Social Construction of Reality. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bergmann, PL and Luckmann, T (1995) Reconstructive genres of everyday communication. In Quasthoff, U (ed.), Aspects of Oral Communication. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter, 289304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergson, H (1990 [1896]) Matter and Memory. New York: Zone Books.Google Scholar
Bietti, LM (2014) Discursive Remembering. Berlin: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkner, T and Donk, A (2020) Collective memory and social media. Memory Studies 13(4), 367383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisht, P (2013) The politics of cosmopolitan memory. Media, Culture & Society 35(1), 1320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C (2006) Communication as collective memory. In Shepherd, GJ, St. John, J and Striphas, T (eds.), Communication as … Perspectives on Theory. London: Sage, 5159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodnar, J (1992) Remaking America: Public Memory, Commemoration, and Patriotism in the Twentieth Century. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Burkey, B (2020) Repertoires of remembering: A conceptual approach for studying memory practices in the digital ecosystem. Journal of Communication Inquiry 44(2), 178197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, M (1990) The Book of Memory. A Study of Memory in Medieval Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Connerton, P (1989) How Societies Remember. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corry, F (2023) Never forget? Memory maintenance on an aging platform. Convergence 29(3), 602619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dayan, D and Katz, E (1992) Media Events: The Live Broadcasting of History. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Kosnick, A (2016) Rogue Archives: Digital Cultural Memory and Media Fandom. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimbath, O and Heinlein, M (2022) Social Memory. Paderborn: Brill Fink.10.30965/9783846767399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erll, A (2011) Memory in Culture. Basingstoke: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erll, A and Rigney, A (eds.) (2009) Mediation, Remediation, and the Dynamics of Cultural Memory. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110217384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ernst, W (2018) Tempor(e)alities and archive-textures of media-connected memory. In Hoskins, A (ed.), Digital Memory Studies. London: Routledge, 143155.Google Scholar
Fentress, JJ and Wickham, C (1992) Social Memory. London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fiske, J (1989) Understanding Popular Culture. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Garde-Hansen, J (2011) Media and Memory. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Garde-Hansen, J, Hoskins, A and Reading, A (eds.) (2009) Save as … Digital Memories. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giddens, A (1994) Living in a post-traditional society. In Beck, U, Giddens, A and Lash, S (eds.), Reflexive Modernization. Cambridge: Polity Press, 56109.Google Scholar
Grusin, R (2010) Premediation: Affect and Mediality After 9/11. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halbwachs, M (1980 [1950]) The Collective Memory. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Halbwachs, M (1992 [1925]) On Collective Memory. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, S (1997) The work of representation. In Hall, S (ed.), Cultural Representation and Signifying Practices. London: Routledge, 1374.Google Scholar
Havelock, EA (1988) The Muse Learns to Write. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Henrickson, L (2023) Chatting with the dead. The hermeneutics of Thanabots. Media, Culture & Society 45(5), 949966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herring, S, Stein, D and Virtanen, T (eds.) (2013) Pragmatics of Computer-Mediated Communication. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, A and Herbig, A (2013) Recalling the ghosts of 9/11: Convergent memorialization at the opening of the national 9/11 memorial. International Journal of Communication 7, 22072230.Google Scholar
Hirst, W and Manier, D (2002) The diverse forms of collective memory. In Echterhoff, G and Saar, M (eds.), Contexts and Cultures of Memory. Konstanz: UVK, 75102.Google Scholar
Holloway, D and Green, L (2017) Mediated memory making: The virtual family photograph album. Communications 42(3), 351368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoskins, A (2016) Archive me! Memory, media, uncertainty. In Hajek, A, Lohmeier, C and Pentzold, C (eds.), Memory in a Mediated World. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, 1335.Google Scholar
Hoskins, A (ed.) (2018) Memory of the multitude. In Hoskins, A (ed.), Digital Memory Studies. London: Routledge, 85109.Google Scholar
Humphreys, L (2018) The Qualified Self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutton, PH (1993) History as an Art of Memory. Hanover: University of New England Press.Google Scholar
Huyssen, A (1995) Twilight Memories. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Irwin-Zarecka, I (1994) Frames of Remembrance. The Dynamics of Collective Memory. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jacobsen, B and Beer, D (2021) Social Media and the Automatic Production of Memory. Bristol: Bristol University Press.Google Scholar
Kang, H, Kim, HW, Baek, G and Park, D (January-March 2023) Recalling the past within a social network's collective memory work. Social Media + Society 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/20563051231156365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kansteiner, W (2002) Finding meaning in memory: A methodological critique of collective memory studies.. History and Theory 41(2), 179197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kansteiner, W (2022) Digital doping for historians? Can history, memory, and historical theory be rendered artificially intelligent? History and Theory 61(4), 119133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaun, A (2016) Crisis and Critique. London: Zed Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keightley, E, Pickering, M and Bisht, P (2019) Interscalarity and the memory spectrum. In Park, D and Maurantonio, N (eds.), Communicating Memory & History. New York: Peter Lang, 1738.Google Scholar
Kidd, J and McAvoy, EN (2023) Deep nostalgia: Remediated memory, algorithmic nostalgia and technological ambivalence. Convergence 29(3), 620640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblauch, H and Günthner, S (1995) Culturally patterned speaking practices – the analysis of communicative genres. Pragmatics 5(1), 132.Google Scholar
LeGoff, J (1992) History and Memory. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Leroi-Gourhan, A (1993) Gesture and Speech. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levy, D (2010) Das kulturelle Gedächtnis. In Gudehus, C, Eichenberg, A and Welzer, H (eds.), Gedächtnis und Erinnerung. Ein interdisziplinäres Handbuch. Stuttgart/Weimar: J.B. Metzler, 93101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingstone, S (2009) On the mediation of everything. Journal of Communication 59(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohmeier, C and Pentzold, C (2014) Making mediated memory work. Cuban-Americans, Miami media and the doings of diaspora memories. Media, Culture & Society 36(6), 776789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, A (1991) Epic Singers and Oral Traditions. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Lowenthal, D (2015) The Past is a Foreign Country. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139024884CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makhortykh, M (2021) Memoriae ex machina: How algorithms make us remember and forget. Georgetown Journal of International Affairs 22(2), 180185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markowitsch, HJ and Welzer, H (2009) The Development of Autobiographical Memory. London: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maswood, R, Rasmussen, A and Rajaram, S (2019) Collaborative remembering of emotional autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 148(1), 6579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meade, M, Harris, C, Van Bergen, P, Sutton, J and Barnier, A (2018) Collaborative remembering: Background and approaches. In Meade, M, Harris, C, Van Bergen, P, Sutton, J and Barnier, A (eds.), Collaborative Remembering. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 318.Google Scholar
Merrill, S, Keightley, E and Daphi, P (eds.) (2020) Social Movements, Cultural Memory and Digital Media. Mobilising Mediated Remembrance. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Middleton, D and Edwards, D (1990) Introduction. In Middleton, D and Edwards, D (eds.), Collective Remembering. London: Sage, 111.Google Scholar
Neiger, M (2020) Theorizing media memory: Six elements defining the role of the media in shaping collective memory in the digital age. Sociology Compass 14(5), e12782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neiger, M, Zandberg, E and Meyers, O (eds.) (2011) On Media Memory. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1057/9780230307070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olick, J (2010) From collective memory to the sociology of mnemonic practices and products. In Erll, A and Nünning, A (eds.), Cultural Memory Studies. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter, 151162.Google Scholar
Olick, J, Vinitzky-Seroussi, V and Levy, D (eds.) (2011) Introduction. In Olick, J, Vinitzky-Seroussi, V and Levy, D (eds.), The Collective Memory Reader. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 362.Google Scholar
Parry, M (1987) The Making of Homeric Verse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pentzold, C (2009) Fixing the floating gap. The online encyclopaedia Wikipedia as a global memory place. Memory Studies 2(2), 255272.10.1177/1750698008102055CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pentzold, C, Lohmeier, C and Hajek, A (2016) Introduction: Remembering and reviving in states of flux. In Hajek, L, Lohmeier, C and Pentzold, C (eds.), Memory in a Mediated World: Rememberance and Reconstruction. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, 112.Google Scholar
Prey, R and Smit, R (2018) From personal to personalized memory: Social media as mnemotechnology. In Papacharissi, Z (ed.), A Networked Self and Birth, Life, Death. New York: Routledge, 209223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatzki, T (2010) The Timespace of Human Activity. Lanham: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Schütz, A (1967) The Phenomenology of the Social World. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, O (2014) The past next door: Neighborly relations with digital memory artifacts. Memory Studies 7(1), 721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidler, VJ (2013) Remembering Diana. Cultural Memory and the Reinvention of Authority. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, D (2006) 9/11. The Culture of Commemoration. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sisto, D and Mclellan-Broussard, B (2020) Online Afterlives. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smit, PH, Heinrich, A and Broersma, M (2018) Activating the past in the Ferguson protests: Memory work, digital activism and the politics of platforms. New Media & Society 20(9), 31193139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonnevend, J (2016) Stories Without Borders. The Berlin Wall and the Making of a Global Iconic Event. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terdiman, R (1993) Present Past: Modernity and the Memory Crisis. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Dijck, J (2007) Mediated Memories in the Digital Age. Stanford: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vansina, J (1985) Oral Tradition as History. London: James Currey.Google Scholar
Wagner-Pacifici, R (1996) Memories in the making: The shapes of things that went. Qualitative Sociology 19, 301321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasserman, H (2018) Media, Geopolitics, and Power. Urbana: Illinois University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welzer, H (2010) Communicative memory. In Erll, A and Nünning, A (eds.), A Companion to Cultural Memory Studies. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter, 285298.Google Scholar
Wertsch, JV (2002) Voices of Collective Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wodianka, S (2005) Time, literature, and memory. In Erll, A and Nünning, A (eds.), Memory Concepts in Literary Studies. Theoretical Grounding and Perspectives on Application. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter, 179203.Google Scholar
Yates, F (2011 [1966]) The Art of Memory. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Zelizer, B (1992) Covering the Body. The Kennedy Assassination, the Media, and the Shaping of Collective Memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Communicative memory and cultural memory