Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T06:16:40.234Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Living the good life? A systematic review of behavioural signs of affective state in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) and factors relating to quality of life. Part 2: Horse-human interactions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2024

Carol Hall*
Affiliation:
School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Southwell, Nottinghamshire NG25 0QF, UK National Equine Welfare Council, Slad Lane, Princes Risborough, Bucks HP27 0PP, UK
Rachel Kay
Affiliation:
School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Southwell, Nottinghamshire NG25 0QF, UK National Equine Welfare Council, Slad Lane, Princes Risborough, Bucks HP27 0PP, UK
*
Corresponding author: Carol Hall; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Quality of life is dependent upon the extent to which behavioural needs are met, and the balance between pleasant and unpleasant lifetime experiences. In Part II of this systematic review, articles (n = 109) relating to horse-human interactions were reviewed to identify behavioural evidence of their positive or negative impact on the horse. The number of articles (n = 22) relating to the recognition of pain in horses, indicated the importance of identifying health issues, which are also likely to increase the aversiveness of interactions. These and articles relating to emotional reactivity testing in horses (n = 19), the behaviour of horses during handling and management procedures (n = 17), behaviour of the horse when ridden (n = 17), non-procedural horse-human interactions (n = 13), horse behaviour during transportation (n = 12) and behaviour during training other than when ridden (n = 9) were reviewed. During most interactions, horse behaviour is controlled and/or restricted by the human, masking negative or positive signs, and may be confounded by factors including fear and individual differences. In situations involving freedom of movement, positive experiences of horses with humans were associated with approach behaviour, negative ones with avoidance, but training could affect both. Undoubtedly, change is needed to reduce the extent to which interactions with humans are unpleasant for the horse. Only when the needs of the horse are fulfilled and interactions with humans are predominantly pleasurable will their quality of life improve.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

The findings presented in Part I of this review (a companion paper published simultaneously; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024) provided evidence of what horses want most (space, company, and forage) and how providing them with the opportunity to fulfil their species-specific needs is reflected in their behaviour. In agreement with Dawkins (Reference Dawkins2004), and the adaptation of the Five Domains model developed by Mellor et al. (Reference Mellor, Beausoleil, Littlewood, McLean, McGreevy, Jones and Wilkins2020) in the guiding principles for humane livestock farming in The Netherlands (Council on Animal Affairs 2021), it was concluded that the horse could only experience a good life if their natural behavioural needs were met. In addition, the many and varied roles of the domestic horse (Equus caballus) in relation to their human counterparts have resulted in multifaceted experiences, many of which could be deemed aversive to the horse. For example, transportation by road (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Kay and Green2020), the clipping of excessive hair (Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall and Billet2013), and the use of restrictive equipment during ridden activities (Condon et al. Reference Condon, McGreevy, McLean, Williams and Randle2022). Given that an animal’s quality of life (QOL) is determined by the balance between pleasant and unpleasant experiences over time (Yeates Reference Yeates2016), the cumulative effect of repeated negative experiences will undoubtedly have a detrimental effect on the horse.

The importance of considering the cumulative life experiences of an animal when assessing its QOL was incorporated in the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid (AWAG), developed initially to assess the welfare of experimental animals (Honess & Wolfensohn Reference Honess and Wolfensohn2010). As well as physical, behavioural/psychological, and environmental parameters, challenges associated with experimental and management procedures are included in this welfare assessment protocol (for an example of its application, see Ryan et al. Reference Ryan, Waters and Wolfensohn2021). Although the welfare parameters that form the basis of this system are not dissimilar to other approaches, the emphasis put on the impact of cumulative life experiences when assessing QOL is of relevance to all animals, including the domestic horse.

Interactions with humans occur in many different situations and could be experienced as positive or negative by the horse. However, their behavioural response may not convey this accurately, or may be misinterpreted by the human. For example, during a sham-clipping procedure, the behaviour of horses considered compliant differed from those considered non-compliant, but physiological measures (salivary cortisol) suggested that all horses found the procedure aversive (Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall and Billet2013). The interpretation of behavioural signs in the horse can be influenced by the experience and/or the role of the observer. It was found that when judging the behaviour of ridden horses for signs indicative of positive or negative subjective state, results varied between different equestrian professions (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Kay and Yarnell2014).

When assessing the impact of human interactions on the QOL of the horse, it is important to consider the first of the questions posed by Dawkins (Reference Dawkins2004): is the animal healthy? Although physical health can, to a certain extent, be measured, behavioural signs of underlying pain may be missed. To ensure that subsequent interactions are not associated with unpleasant painful experiences, resulting in them becoming aversive to the horse, further evidence to facilitate the recognition of behavioural signs of pain is required.

As reported in Part I (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024), a systematic review of scientific evidence was conducted to derive observable, evidence-based behavioural measures of positive or negative affective state and factors that relate to QOL in the domestic horse. In Part II, the retained articles relating to horse-human interactions will be reviewed to identify behavioural evidence of the positive or negative impact of these on the horse. As used in this review, the term horse-human interaction refers to the many and varied situations where the behaviour of the horse is determined by human action, and where accurate interpretation of horse behaviour should guide subsequent interactions. Features of these interactions, associated with whether they are experienced as positive or negative, will be identified to facilitate the development of less-aversive approaches. The aim of this review is to promote more positive horse-human interactions that will contribute to a better life for horses. In combination with the findings of Part I of this systematic review (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024), the changes in management and training that are required for horses to live a good life will be explored.

Materials and methods

During February 2023, a systematic literature search of five electronic databases (Science Direct, PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science and Pubpsych) was performed. Two separate search queries were specified to ensure the potential inclusion of behaviour indicative of both negative and positive affect:

Search 1: ‘(emotion* OR affect* OR cognit*) AND (behav* OR welfare) AND (horse OR pony OR equine)’

Search 2: ‘(stress AND behav*) AND (horse OR pony OR equine)’

Following a preliminary assessment of the relevance of each peer-reviewed article, those reporting primary research findings with a clear account of the methods used (experimental and/or observational), a subject number greater than four, and justification for any subsequent attribution of affective valence to aspects of recorded behaviour, were retained. No criteria relating to the date of publication were used in the searches. Only articles involving horses and/or ponies were retained.

The following information was extracted from the retained articles: the scenario involved, the behaviour recorded, the subjective experience attributed to specific behaviours together with the supporting evidence for this, and factors that affected the behaviour expressed. Features of the study design, including details of the horses/ponies involved in each study, behaviour recorded, and the nature of the supporting evidence are available in the Supplementary material.

See Part I (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024) of this systematic review for full details of the methods used to select the articles for retention, and the data extracted from these. Articles relating to the fulfilment of species-specific needs (n = 70) were included in Part I, and those relating to horse-human interactions (including all management and training procedures) (n = 109) are included here in Part II.

Results and Discussion

The search terms used in this systematic review identified articles relating to most aspects of the life of the domestic horse. The study scenarios involving horse-human interactions (as defined above) and the number of articles per scenario are shown in Table 1. The scenarios are listed in descending order according to the number of articles retained. The number of scenarios identified during this literature search exemplifies the multi-faceted life of the domestic horse and the demands associated with their role as a sporting, leisure, or companion animal. The many aspects of management and training included in the scenarios listed in Table 1 reflect both the multiple ways in which domestic horse welfare may be compromised, but also the potential for reviewing all aspects of horse husbandry to promote a better quality of life. The behaviour attributed to affective state and the factors affecting this behaviour are reported for each separate scenario. Further details of the experimental design used in the articles retained under each scenario are available in the Supplementary material (Tables S2.1S2.7). General behavioural signs of affective state during interactions with humans and the subsequent welfare implications are discussed below.

Table 1. Study scenarios relating to horse behaviour during horse-human interactions identified in the literature search, the related table in the Supplementary material and the number of articles retained per scenario. The scenarios are listed in descending order according to the number of articles retained

Behavioural signs of pain in the horse

The recognition of behavioural signs of pain in the horse is key to answering the first of the questions posed by Dawkins (Reference Dawkins2004): is the animal healthy? The importance of identifying signs that the horse is experiencing or anticipating pain was reflected in the number of articles on this topic that resulted from the search terms. A total of twenty-two articles relating to behavioural signs of pain associated with either specific health issues or in general were retained. Pain was induced in five of the studies, with the aim of identifying behavioural signs to facilitate the recognition of pain for future application. Behaviour recorded in four of these studies (Bussières et al. Reference Bussières, Jacques, Lainay, Beauchamp, Leblond, Cadoré, Desmaizières, Cuvelliez and Troncy2008; Grint et al. Reference Grint, Beths, Yvorchuk-St Jean, Whay and Murrell2017; Reid et al. Reference Reid, Rogers, Gronqvist, Gee and Bolwell2017; Egan et al. Reference Egan, Kearney, Brama, Parnell and McGrath2021) is shown in Table 2. In the study by Carvalho et al. (Reference Carvalho, Trindade, Conde, Antonioli, Funnicelli, Dias, Canola, Chinelatto and Ferraz2022) induced, low-grade, inflammatory pain was not detectable via analysis of facial expression (and consequently is not included in Table 2). Low-level pain was not found to affect typical feeding behaviour, but behavioural variability increased as horses recovered from induced bilateral lameness resulting from mild inflammation in the joints, although this varied for individual horses (Egan et al. Reference Egan, Kearney, Brama, Parnell and McGrath2021). There was also variation in individual behavioural responses to the induced pain and recovery, with no generic ‘normal’ behaviour identified (Egan et al. Reference Egan, Kearney, Brama, Parnell and McGrath2021).

Table 2. Behavioural signs of pain in the horse (in relation to specific health issues and in general)

The differentiation between behaviour resulting from anxiety or pain was explored by Reid et al. (Reference Reid, Rogers, Gronqvist, Gee and Bolwell2017), by testing responses to anxiety-provoking situations (social isolation) and/or a pain-eliciting stimulus (neck pinch). Pain alone resulted in reduced locomotion and reduced contact-seeking behaviour, whereas anxiety resulted in increased locomotion, increased vocalisation, restlessness, and contact-seeking behaviour. Anxiety and pain together caused restlessness. The authors concluded that social isolation (and potentially other sources of anxiety) may affect behaviour associated with pain responses, or potentially be the cause of behaviour typically cited as being indicative of pain (Reid et al. Reference Reid, Rogers, Gronqvist, Gee and Bolwell2017).

Behavioural signs of pain were also found to be confounded by associated stressors by Erber et al. (Reference Erber, Wulf, Becker-Birck, Kaps, Aurich, Möstl and Aurich2012). A comparison between the effect of hot branding and microchipping (as a means of identification) on the behavioural and physiological responses of foals concluded that both processes were stressful and that the associated restraint was a major stressor. No specific behavioural details were provided but the authors noted that painful procedures should always be avoided, particularly in young animals where early experiences may have an impact both on future pain perception, and on responses to handling and other management procedures (Erber et al. Reference Erber, Wulf, Becker-Birck, Kaps, Aurich, Möstl and Aurich2012). Dalla Costa et al. (Reference Dalla Costa, Pascuzzo, Leach, Dai, Lebelt, Vantini and Minero2018) found that certain facial expressions included in the Horse Grimace Scale (developed as a means of identifying pain in horses) were also associated with fear-eliciting situations (stiffly backward ears and prominent strained chewing muscles). Given that facial characteristics vary according to breed and type of horse (Hintze et al. Reference Hintze, Smith, Patt, Bachmann and Würbel2016) and Carvalho et al. (Reference Carvalho, Trindade, Conde, Antonioli, Funnicelli, Dias, Canola, Chinelatto and Ferraz2022) found that low-grade pain was not identifiable from facial expression, interpretation of these subtle behavioural signs should be treated with caution.

Evidence suggests that anticipation of pain and pain in general results in immobility and the avoidance of human contact (Pritchett et al. Reference Pritchett, Ulibarri, Roberts, Schneider and Sellon2003; Burn et al. Reference Burn, Dennison and Whay2010; Fureix et al. Reference Fureix, Menguy and Hausberger2010; Reid et al. Reference Reid, Rogers, Gronqvist, Gee and Bolwell2017). Severe and/or acute pain was recognised by the horses’ behavioural attempts at avoiding contact with humans (Burn et al. Reference Burn, Dennison and Whay2010; Fureix et al. Reference Fureix, Menguy and Hausberger2010), lack of any movement likely to worsen the pain (Pritchett et al. Reference Pritchett, Ulibarri, Roberts, Schneider and Sellon2003; Reid et al. Reference Reid, Rogers, Gronqvist, Gee and Bolwell2017) and decreased time spent eating (Trindade et al. Reference Trindade, Taffarel and Luna2021). Monitoring any changes in these features of behaviour can provide important insights into the ongoing health status of the horse.

Certain behaviours have been shown to be associated with specific health issues (as shown in Table 2), but individual variation occurs. For example, although Oliveira et al. (Reference Oliveira, Santos, Silva, Trindade, Yamada, Jaramillo, Silva and Baccarin2022) found that the severity of osteoarthritis affected the time horses spent in recumbency during hospitalisation (see Table 2), Kelemen et al. (Reference Kelemen, Grimm, Long, Auer and Jenner2021) noted that neither age nor lameness due to chronic orthopaedic disease had an impact on the time horses spent recumbent.

Behavioural signs of pain in the horse (in relation to specific health issues and in general) are shown in Table 2. See Table S2.1 in the Supplementary material for details of each study.

In summary

The recognition of behavioural signs of pain in horses remains one of the most important areas of research in terms of improving their quality of life. Distinguishing between behaviour indicative of pain and that associated with confounding factors including fear and anxiety, isolation, anticipation of pain and other treatment-related stressors, is further complicated by individual variation. Familiarity with an individual horse may be valuable in determining whether changes in behaviour signify underlying pain. In addition, where specific behavioural issues occur, for example, avoidance behaviour during preparation for ridden work, these may relate to anticipation of pain or discomfort and should not be ignored. See Behaviour of the horse when ridden.

Tests of emotional reactivity in the horse

Challenges to human safety during interactions with horses, both during handling and ridden work, have been associated with sudden/unexpected, often intense, behavioural responses of the horse (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jastrzębska, Gajewska, Muszyńska and Pieniąże2015; Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Bonneau, Parias and Biau2019). Tests of emotional reactivity to various acute stressors (for example, social separation/isolation, startling/sudden occurrences, and novelty) have been developed to explore factors affecting such behavioural responses, with the aim of enabling better assessment and selection of horses and reducing human risk. Nineteen articles reporting the results of experimental tests of emotional reactivity, including novel object, novel environment, startle, and isolation tests, were retained. Factors found to affect reactivity were age (Baragli et al. Reference Baragli, Banti, Vitale and Sighieri2014; Bulens et al. Reference Bulens, Sterken, Van Beirendonck, Van Thielen and Driessen2015; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Kim, Lee and Kim2021), breed/type (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Kim, Lee and Kim2021), type of work/role (Hausberger et al. Reference Hausberger, Muller and Lunel2011; Mendonça et al. Reference Mendonça, Bienboire-Frosini, Kowalczyk, Leclercq, Arroub and Pageat2019), the effect of company (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Neveux and Levy2012; Rørvang & Christensen Reference Rørvang and Christensen2018; Ricci-Bonot et al. Reference Ricci-Bonot, Romero, Nicol and Mills2021), training (Squibb et al. Reference Squibb, Griffin, Favier and Ijichi2018) and laterality (Larose et al. Reference Larose, Richard-Yris, Hausberger and Rogers2006; De Boyer Des Roches et al. Reference De Boyer Des Roches, Richard-Yris, Henry, Ezzaouïa and Hausberger2008; Baragli et al. Reference Baragli, Scopa, Felici and Reddon2021). As above, individual differences in the behavioural reactions elicited by these tests were reported by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Bouissou and Erhard2008), Pérez Manrique et al. (Reference Pérez Manrique, Hudson, Bánszegi and Szenczi2019) (see also Behaviour associated with weaning in Part 1; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024), Safryghin et al. (Reference Safryghin, Hebesberger and Wascher2019), and Manrique et al. (Reference Manrique, Bánszegi, Hudson and Szenczi2021).

Fear and frustration

The effect of experimentally induced negative or positive situations on learning, using startle tests and food, respectively, was reported by Fortin et al. (Reference Fortin, Valenchon, Lévy, Calandreau, Arnould and Lansade2018) and Olczak et al. (Reference Olczak, Klocek and Christensen2021). Improved cognitive flexibility in an instrumental learning task was associated with an environment where positive events had occurred (Fortin et al. Reference Fortin, Valenchon, Lévy, Calandreau, Arnould and Lansade2018). Fear generated by waving a plastic bag was associated with an increase in the number of trials required to reach criterion in a clicker-training (positively reinforced) task (Olczak et al. Reference Olczak, Klocek and Christensen2021). The inability to succeed at a task, regardless of training, has been shown to produce behavioural signs of frustration and an increase in HR (Rørvang et al. Reference Rørvang, Ničová, Sassner and Nawroth2021). These findings have important implications for horse training in general, where potential sources of fear should be avoided, and the tasks involved must be within the capabilities of the horse.

Age, training, and role of horse

Age-related differences in both behavioural and physiological responses in a visual startle test were found by Baragli et al. (Reference Baragli, Banti, Vitale and Sighieri2014). Young horses displayed a greater behavioural response (avoidance) and more exploratory behaviour than older horses, but older horses had a more pronounced physiological response (decrease in HRV). Similarly, during novel object tests, younger horses showed a more pronounced behavioural reaction than older horses (Bulens et al. Reference Bulens, Sterken, Van Beirendonck, Van Thielen and Driessen2015), whereas older horses showed more physiological signs of anxiety (decrease in HRV; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Kim, Lee and Kim2021).

Inconsistency between physiological (HR, HRV, rectal temperature, eye temperature) and behavioural signs of anxiety in adult horses during novel handling procedures (walking over a tarpaulin and through plastic streamers) was attributed to the overriding effect of training on the behavioural response by Squibb et al. (Reference Squibb, Griffin, Favier and Ijichi2018), which may at least in part explain the age-related differences. These findings suggest that because adult horses do not always behave in a way that reflects their emotional response, the potentially negative impact of events may be underestimated. In part, this reduced behavioural response could be explained by differing levels of training, as suggested by Squibb et al. (Reference Squibb, Griffin, Favier and Ijichi2018). Early restrictive training of neonates was indeed shown to subsequently reduce active locomotor responses to social separation and novelty (Durier et al. Reference Durier, Henry, Sankey, Sizun and Hausberger2012).

The extent to which training is responsible for individual differences in behavioural responses is unclear. There is some evidence of consistent individual differences in responses to social separation (Pérez Manrique et al. Reference Pérez Manrique, Hudson, Bánszegi and Szenczi2019; Manrique et al. Reference Manrique, Bánszegi, Hudson and Szenczi2021) and the trait of fearfulness (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Bouissou and Erhard2008). Regardless of whether behavioural tendencies are innate or acquired, individual differences in the extent to which observed behaviour reflects underlying affective state must be recognised to reduce the risk of misinterpreting the impact of events and/or procedures on the horse.

The type of work (role) the horse is involved in has been associated with behavioural differences in responses to novel object, startle, isolation, and novel environment tests (Hausberger et al. Reference Hausberger, Muller and Lunel2011). In agreement with Brubaker et al. (Reference Brubaker, Schroeder, Sherwood, Stroud and Udell2021) and Lerch et al. (Reference Lerch, Cirulli, Rochais, Lesimple, Guilbaud, Contalbrigo, Borgi, Grandgeorge and Hausberger2021) (see Table 5[b]), Mendonça et al. (Reference Mendonça, Bienboire-Frosini, Kowalczyk, Leclercq, Arroub and Pageat2019) found that horses involved in Equine Assisted Service work showed behavioural signs of apprehensiveness (and physiological signs including decreases in HRV) in relation to human contact during a novel test. In comparison, dressage horses showed more exploratory behaviour and both dressage and eventing horses were quicker to approach a human during testing. Showjumping horses responded less to novelty than those in other working groups (Mendonça et al. Reference Mendonça, Bienboire-Frosini, Kowalczyk, Leclercq, Arroub and Pageat2019). Breed/type of horse (which is also likely to vary according to the role of the horse) may also be a contributory factor. For example, Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Kim, Lee and Kim2021) found that the physiological response was most strongly correlated with behaviour in thoroughbred mares compared to other breeds.

Consistency of behavioural responses

Individual differences in behavioural responses have, in some cases, been found to be consistent characteristics (temperament, personality). Foal responses to separation from the mare (from birth to six months) were found to be consistent with responses to group separation at nine to twelve months (including frequencies of vocalisation, head tossing, high head carriage and locomotion) (Pérez Manrique et al. Reference Pérez Manrique, Hudson, Bánszegi and Szenczi2019; Manrique et al. Reference Manrique, Bánszegi, Hudson and Szenczi2021). Although consistent, repeatable, fear-related behavioural responses to novel object and startle tests were recorded (from eight months to two and a half years of age) by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Bouissou and Erhard2008), there was no correlation between HR and behaviour. The authors concluded, however, that the consistency in behaviourally measured fear responses indicated that the ‘fearful’ trait varies with individuals and is present from an early age (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Bouissou and Erhard2008). However, in adult horses, Safryghin et al. (Reference Safryghin, Hebesberger and Wascher2019) did not find repeatable behavioural responses to different tests (novel object and pre-feeding test) and, again, physiological responses (HR) were not consistent with observed behaviour. It is likely that both innate and acquired individual differences affect reactivity.

Company

The presence of other horses has been shown to affect behavioural responses in different ways according to context. Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Neveux and Levy2012) found that social isolation for a period of eleven days resulted in reduced reactivity to a novel object in yearling foals and improved learning in a motor task (moving forwards and backwards). A possible explanation for the decreased reactivity is that the absence of a companion resulted in a reduction in the extent to which emotion was expressed and a lack of opportunity for anxiety to be transferred between individuals. Also, a period of isolation would result in an absence of subsequent separation anxiety distracting the foal from learning a task or being an additional factor in response to a novel object. Adult horses with signs of withdrawal/depression were found to be less reactive to auditory stimuli than non-withdrawn horses (Rochais et al. Reference Rochais, Henry, Fureix and Hausberger2016b), which may also be a contributory factor in the reduced reactivity of isolated foals. An example of how reduced emotional responses can also be transferred was reported by Rørvang and Christensen (Reference Rørvang and Christensen2018) who found a reduction in behavioural and physiological signs of fear in response to a startle test when naïve horses were in the presence of a habituated demonstrator. However, Ricci-Bonot et al. (Reference Ricci-Bonot, Romero, Nicol and Mills2021) did not find that the presence of a companion affected the behavioural response of horses in a startle test, but this did result in a quicker physiological recovery (HR return to baseline). The reverse occurred in a novel object test (no effect on HR but a reduction in behavioural reactivity), and the authors suggested that this reflects the increased effect of social buffering in situations where greater contextual processing is facilitated (Ricci-Bonot et al. Reference Ricci-Bonot, Romero, Nicol and Mills2021).

Laterality

Asymmetry of brain function and behaviour in vertebrate animals, including the horse, allows for specialisation and has also been found to be associated with emotional responses. In general, the right hemisphere is involved in rapid, emotional responses, and the left more in decision-making (Rogers Reference Rogers2002). The resultant side-related differences in behaviour, or lateralisation, and the potential association between lateral preferences and affective valence (for example, see Ahern & Schwartz Reference Ahern and Schwartz1979) was investigated but no consistent patterns emerged. De Boyer Des Roches et al. (Reference De Boyer Des Roches, Richard-Yris, Henry, Ezzaouïa and Hausberger2008) found a slight tendency for horses to explore what was deemed a negative stimulus (shirt associated with veterinary surgeon) with their left eye. Breed differences in preferred eye for inspection of a novel object were found by Larose et al. (Reference Larose, Richard-Yris, Hausberger and Rogers2006), with French Saddlebreds using their right eye more and Trotters their left. Baragli et al. (Reference Baragli, Scopa, Felici and Reddon2021) found individual differences in which eye was preferred when inspecting a novel object, but no consistent group-level differences were found. There have been some findings that suggest an association between affective state and lateral preferences (see Marr et al. Reference Marr, Farmer and Krüger2018; Marliani et al. Reference Marliani, Vannucchi, Kiumurgis and Accorsi2022; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024), but the latter is also likely to be influenced by human handling and training (see Table 5[b] and Sankey et al. Reference Sankey, Henry, Clouard, Richard-Yris and Hausberger2011). Although no consistent pattern of lateralisation was found, lateral differences in response to handling, training, and management procedures should be considered at an individual horse level. See Table S2.2 in the Supplementary material for details of the articles retained relating to this scenario.

In summary

Although factors such as age, breed/type and training were associated with the extent to which behavioural reactions occurred in response to various sudden or acute stressors, the potential for such behaviour remains in all horses. However, the results of the retained studies suggest some ways in which both the risk to human safety and negative impact on the horse can be reduced. Gradual habituation to potentially negative experiences, such as separation and isolation, careful introduction to potentially challenging situations, and the company of a more experienced, habituated animal should all lessen the negative impact on the horse. In terms of human safety, the careful pairing of less-reactive horses with inexperienced humans will be mutually beneficial. Also, an increased awareness of what constitutes an acute stressor for the horse and likely behavioural responses would be of value to inexperienced and potentially more experienced humans alike.

Behaviour of the horse during handling and management procedures

There were seventeen articles retained that had a focus on handling and management procedures. The predominant behavioural measures used were the extent to which the horse tried to avoid the procedure or was relaxed/approached the human carrying out the procedure, and/or whether the horse showed behavioural signs indicative of stress. Approach/avoidance behaviours were concluded as signifying positive or negative responses, respectively. Behavioural signs of stress were identified based on past studies (for example, Young et al. Reference Young, Creighton, Smith and Hosie2012) and were believed to indicate negative affective state. Behaviour indicative of positive or negative responses to handling and management procedures are shown in Table 3(a). Included in Table 3 are behaviours during handling tests that were associated with different home environments (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005; Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015) See Part I of this review, The home environment (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024).

Table 3. (a) Behaviour during handling and management procedures indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse/human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach/avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

Handling and management procedures

The handling/management procedures included ear clipping with or without the application of a lip twitch (Ali et al. Reference Ali, Gutwein and Heleski2017), grooming (Hintze et al. Reference Hintze, Smith, Patt, Bachmann and Würbel2016; Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Nowak, Lainé, Leterrier, Bonneau, Parias and Bertin2018, Reference Lansade, Bonneau, Parias and Biau2019), blood and saliva sampling (Lelláková et al. Reference Lelláková, Pavľak, Lešková, Florián, Skurková, Mesarčová, Kottferová, Takáčová and Kottferová2021), hoof trimming, blood sampling and microchip implantation in pre-weaned foals (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jaworski, Suwała, Sobczyńska, Jastrzębska, Ogłuszka, Sankey, Boroń and Jezierski2017), early intensive handling of neonates (Durier et al. Reference Durier, Henry, Sankey, Sizun and Hausberger2012), massage (McBride et al. Reference McBride, Hemmings and Robinson2004) and the application of Flowtrition soft touch therapy (Birt et al. Reference Birt, Guay, Treiber, Ramirez and Snyder2015), participation in practical training sessions for veterinary students (horse handling and mare reproductive/medical rectal examination) (Guinnefollau et al. Reference Guinnefollau, Bolwell, Gee, Norman and Rogers2021), and anthelmintic administration (Whitaker et al. Reference Whitaker, Goupil, Roy, Marciat and McGahie2011).

Behaviours classed as approach/positive included movement of lips, attempting to groom human, leaning towards/against human (McBride et al. Reference McBride, Hemmings and Robinson2004), and attempting to contact, nibble handler (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Nowak, Lainé, Leterrier, Bonneau, Parias and Bertin2018). Avoidance/negative behaviours included moving away, threatening, biting human (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Nowak, Lainé, Leterrier, Bonneau, Parias and Bertin2018), withdrawal, head shaking, rearing (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jaworski, Suwała, Sobczyńska, Jastrzębska, Ogłuszka, Sankey, Boroń and Jezierski2017) and head and whole-body movements away from the handler (Ali et al. Reference Ali, Gutwein and Heleski2017). Behavioural signs of approach/relaxation during massage at preferred sites (McBride et al. Reference McBride, Hemmings and Robinson2004) and the application of soft-touch therapy (Birt et al. Reference Birt, Guay, Treiber, Ramirez and Snyder2015) were associated with reduced heart rate. Increased HR was associated with some potentially aversive procedures that resulted in avoidance behaviour, including hoof trimming in foals (also associated with increases in salivary cortisol: Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jaworski, Suwała, Sobczyńska, Jastrzębska, Ogłuszka, Sankey, Boroń and Jezierski2017), and ear clipping without the use of a lip twitch (Ali et al. Reference Ali, Gutwein and Heleski2017). Ear clipping was also associated with lower HRV when no lip twitch was applied than when carried out with one (Ali et al. Reference Ali, Gutwein and Heleski2017).

Behavioural responses to some intrinsically aversive procedures may be reduced by environmental constraints. For example, an increase in heart rate was recorded in horses restrained in stocks for veterinary student training in rectal examination, but only minimal negative behavioural signs such as backward weight shifts, ears back and reduced time spent eating hay were recorded (Guinnefollau et al. Reference Guinnefollau, Bolwell, Gee, Norman and Rogers2021). The lack of overt behavioural signs may not represent underlying affective state but result from repeated restraint.

Familiarity and behaviour of human handler

The impact of the behaviour and familiarity of the human handler on the horse’s response to procedures and novel or fear-eliciting situations was the focus of four retained articles. In the study by Watson and McDonnell (Reference Watson and McDonnell2018) the application of handling interventions (eye rubbing, wither scratching and feed presentation) was shown to reduce avoidance behaviour during a simulated healthcare scenario, although no reduction in heart rate compared with the ‘no intervention’ trials was found. Food presentation was the most effective intervention in terms of reducing the frequency of avoidance/stress responses (Watson & McDonnell Reference Watson and McDonnell2018). Although grooming technique (gentle responsive grooming compared with fixed pattern grooming) resulted in behavioural differences, with the gentle technique associated with contact-seeking behaviour, no effect on physiological measures was found (HR, HRV, plasma cortisol) by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Nowak, Lainé, Leterrier, Bonneau, Parias and Bertin2018). However, eleven sessions of gentle grooming reduced oxytocin levels (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Nowak, Lainé, Leterrier, Bonneau, Parias and Bertin2018). In potentially fear-eliciting situations (for example, fear tests involving the negotiation of ground-level novel objects), avoidance behaviours were reduced when the horse was with a familiar (as opposed to unfamiliar) handler, although this had no effect on the associated heart rate (Marsbøll & Christensen Reference Marsbøll and Christensen2015).

Although some association between behaviour and physiological responses was found, this was not consistent across studies. The length of time horses had been with a handler was shown to relate to their behaviour in tests involving novel objects and novel surfaces. The longer the horse-human relationship, the less avoidance behaviour (Liehrmann et al. Reference Liehrmann, Viitanen, Riihonen, Alander, Koski, Lummaa and Lansade2022). However, no physiological measures were included in this study, so whether the familiarity with the handler reduced the potential fear/response to novelty, or just how this was expressed behaviourally is unclear. However, there is the potential for familiar handlers to communicate more effectively with the horse and that familiarity may also facilitate a reduction in anxiety.

Factors affecting behaviour during handling and procedures are shown in Table 3(b).

Horse facial expression in response to handling

Facial expression in response to handling was assessed in three retained articles. Grooming was used as a potentially positive experience for the horse by Dalla Costa et al. (Reference Dalla Costa, Bracci, Dai, Lebelt and Minero2017), to test the application of the horse grimace scale (HGS) to identify affective state. The only facial expression found to be associated with affect was tension in the chewing muscles and changes in ear posture in negative/fear-eliciting situations (Dalla Costa et al. Reference Dalla Costa, Bracci, Dai, Lebelt and Minero2017). Grooming may not be a wholly positive experience for the horse (see also Young et al. Reference Young, Creighton, Smith and Hosie2012, where salivary cortisol levels in response to grooming were not significantly lower than when exposed to the noise of fireworks and were higher than those relating to clipping and social isolation), and it is often associated with preparation for ridden work (which may be a negative experience). Further investigation of the potential for facial expression to identify affective state may be warranted. Hintze et al. (Reference Hintze, Smith, Patt, Bachmann and Würbel2016) found consistent differences in eye wrinkle expression in relation to procedures deemed to be associated with affective state (positive: grooming, food anticipation; negative: food competition, waving a plastic bag). However, variation in relation to breed or type was found by Schanz et al. (Reference Schanz, Krueger and Hintze2019), and again, assumptions were made regarding the valence of the subjective experience of the horse, so further validation is required.

Sources cited for the ethograms used varied according to the focus of the study and type of behaviour recorded. The most prevalent citation for behaviour indicative of stress (n = 3 studies) was Young et al. (Reference Young, Creighton, Smith and Hosie2012), including by Padalino et al. (Reference Padalino, Loy, Hawson and Randle2019) to record stress-related behaviour when horses were tied up in full sun (average temperature of 31.7°C on day 1, 26.0°C on day 2) for two hours. Unsurprisingly, all horses displayed signs of discomfort (pawing, tail swishing, repetitive head movements, licking and chewing, and biting at flies) and increases in heart rate over and above normal values for horses (Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Loy, Hawson and Randle2019). The aim of this study was to test the effect of cotton rugs to reduce the negative impact of hot, sunny weather. A better approach in terms of minimising the distress caused to the horses in this study would have been to provide a shade option and compare the use of this between horses with or without rugs. The authors do conclude that shade should be provided for horses at ambient temperatures of greater than 25°C.

Study details for the articles relating to this scenario are provided in Table S2.3 in the Supplementary material.

In summary

The overt behavioural responses to handling and procedures have been shown to be affected by training and movement restraint. Where there were opportunities to either approach or avoid the procedure and associated human handler, these could be assumed as indicating the horse had a positive or negative expectation of the outcome, respectively. However, when procedures are associated with attempted avoidance, and likely to be aversive in some way, methods of restraint and/or extensive training are often used with the result that behavioural responses are reduced. Inconsistencies with physiological measures have indicated that such procedures remain aversive, although this is not reflected in the behaviour of the horse. An evaluation of how these procedures could be adapted to at least make them less unpleasant for the horse (and ideally more pleasant) is now required.

Behaviour of the horse when ridden

A total of seventeen articles with a focus on ridden behaviour were retained. These included studies assessing the effect of head/neck position on behaviour when ridden (von Borstel et al. Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Beekmans, van Dalum and Van Dierendonck2014), comparative approaches to training (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Van Dierendonck, Ellis, Rijksen and Van Reenen2009; Mendonça et al. Reference Mendonça, Bienboire-Frosini, Sanchez, Kowalczyk, Teruel, Descout and Pageat2020), potential stress associated with therapeutic ridden sessions (Kaiser et al. Reference Kaiser, Heleski, Siegford and Smith2006; McDuffee et al. Reference McDuffee, Carr and Montelpare2022), the effect of different riders (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Munk, Hawson, Palme, Larsen, Egenvall and Rørvang2021) and behavioural indicators of pain (Dyson & Pollard Reference Dyson and Pollard2021) or anticipated pain (Dyson et al. Reference Dyson, Bondi, Routh, Pollard, Preston, McConnell and Kydd2022) associated with ridden work (see also Behavioural signs of pain in the horse). In most studies, the behaviour being assessed was that which relates to how the horse responds to the rider’s signals. The focus was on behaviour that deviated from that desired by the rider, being referred to as indicative of stress, discomfort, fear, frustration, and conflict (Kaiser et al. Reference Kaiser, Heleski, Siegford and Smith2006; Visser et al. Reference Visser, Van Dierendonck, Ellis, Rijksen and Van Reenen2009; von Borstel et al. Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Beekmans, van Dalum and Van Dierendonck2014; Jastrzębska et al. Reference Jastrzębska, Wolska, Minero, Ogłuszka, Earley, Wejer and Górecka-Bruzda2017; Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Biau, Arnould, Galloux, Destrez, Pycik, Boichot and Lansade2020; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Munk, Hawson, Palme, Larsen, Egenvall and Rørvang2021; McDuffee et al. Reference McDuffee, Carr and Montelpare2022). Consequently, the behaviours indicative of negative affect when the horse is ridden as shown in Table 4(a) far outweigh behavioural signs of positive affect (which, if the same criteria were applied as for negative behavioural signs, would presumably be calm acceptance of rider signals and delivery of the requested behavioural response).

Table 4. (a) Behaviour during ridden activities indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse/human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach/avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

The most common justification for assigning valence to the behaviours recorded was reference to previous studies and to commonly accepted interpretations of the responses to rider signals during different ridden scenarios. Of the sources cited as informing the ethograms used, only five were cited by more than one study. McDonnell (Reference McDonnell2003) and Waring (Reference Waring2003) were each cited by three separate studies, and Young et al. (Reference Young, Creighton, Smith and Hosie2012), McGreevy et al. (Reference McGreevy, McLean, Warren-Smith, Waran and Goodwin2005), and Dyson et al. (Reference Dyson, Berger, Ellis and Mullard2018) were each cited by two separate studies (although the latter was only cited to support further studies by the same research group).

Ruet et al. (Reference Ruet, Biau, Arnould, Galloux, Destrez, Pycik, Boichot and Lansade2020) compared ridden performance with behavioural indicators of welfare state in the home environment, using both objective behavioural assessment and the more subjective qualitative behaviour assessment (QBA). They found an association between horses recorded as hypervigilant and/or performing stereotypical behaviours in the stable, and both signs they attributed to negative affective state when ridden (ear and tail positions) and QBA scores (Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Biau, Arnould, Galloux, Destrez, Pycik, Boichot and Lansade2020). Out of the eleven behavioural and postural indicators used to assess affective state when ridden, only three could potentially be considered as positive signs (ears forwards or asymmetric, and snorts). Within the QBA list of thirteen descriptors, six could be described as positive in some scenarios (at ease, curious, friendly, happy, looking for contact, relaxed), although may not be appropriate for assessing ridden behaviour. No association between behaviour in the ridden test and QBA scores was reported (Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Biau, Arnould, Galloux, Destrez, Pycik, Boichot and Lansade2020).

The challenges associated with attributing affective state to specific behaviours were highlighted by von Borstel et al. (Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009) where the inclusion of the vocalisation termed a ‘snort’ as a negative sign was supported by Waring (Reference Waring2003) but deemed a positive sign by Fraser (Reference Fraser1998). Similarly, Visser et al. (Reference Visser, Van Dierendonck, Ellis, Rijksen and Van Reenen2009) interpreted snorts as negative signs in contrast to Stomp et al. (Reference Stomp, Masson, Henry, Hausberger and Lesimple2020). It is possible that subtle differences in acoustic structure relate to valence, but these may be hard (or impossible) for the human ear to distinguish. More conclusive evidence of the subjective experience of the horse when ridden was provided by von Borstel et al. (Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009) in the form of a choice test. Horses showed a preference for an area where they had not been ridden in a coercively obtained head/neck position, with the latter hyperflexion also being found to be associated with increased fear-related responses (von Borstel et al. Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009).

Physiological measures were recorded in ten of the retained studies. Christensen et al. (Reference Christensen, Munk, Hawson, Palme, Larsen, Egenvall and Rørvang2021) found increases in HR and salivary cortisol concentration associated with specific riders. Visser et al. (Reference Visser, Van Dierendonck, Ellis, Rijksen and Van Reenen2009) recorded higher HR in horses that had been trained using conventional as opposed to more sympathetic methods in the final ridden test, although there was no difference in behaviour or performance. When comparing competitive success in showjumping with levels of salivary cortisol and occurrences of conflict behaviour, increased levels of both were associated with poorer performance (Jastrzębska et al. Reference Jastrzębska, Wolska, Minero, Ogłuszka, Earley, Wejer and Górecka-Bruzda2017). Whether such differences relate to physical exertion and/or fitness, or whether they reflect ‘mental stress’, is questionable. It has been noted previously that although physiological measures provide information about arousal levels, the extent to which they can be used to determine the valence of responses has yet to be determined, particularly in scenarios involving physical exertion (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Randle, Pearson, Preshaw and Waran2018).

It has been demonstrated that it is possible to adopt a responsive approach to the management procedure of grooming, which makes it a positive experience for the horse (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Bonneau, Parias and Biau2019). A similar more responsive approach to other management procedures, making them less aversive and pleasurable where possible, would reduce the cumulative effect of negative experiences for the horse. The same applies to ridden work where the rider was found to be the main factor associated with signs of conflict in the horse (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Munk, Hawson, Palme, Larsen, Egenvall and Rørvang2021). Although the frequency of conflict behaviours has been associated with poor performance in showjumping (Jastrzębska et al. 2017), the occurrence of conflict behaviour was not found to relate to performance evaluation by dressage judges (Hamilton et al. Reference Hamilton, Lancaster and Hall2022). In addition to recommending further training for riders, adapting performance criteria to account for behavioural signs of negative affective state would be positive progress towards improved welfare for ridden horses.

The potential for using behavioural signs to assess affective state in this scenario is limited by the restraint/control exerted by the rider and/or equipment used in ridden work. Also, when horses are kept in constrained home environments (stables) any apparent enthusiasm for exercise when being ridden may be attributed to rebound behaviour. Consequently, behavioural signs of positive or negative anticipation during the preparation for ridden work may provide a more accurate means of assessment. Avoidance behaviours during tacking up and mounting, suggestive of negative anticipation, were found to be common in both leisure and sports horses by Dyson et al. (Reference Dyson, Bondi, Routh, Pollard, Preston, McConnell and Kydd2022). Underlying pain or discomfort during ridden work must be eliminated as the potential cause of this behaviour before considering the need for re-training. Restrictive equipment, often used to mask ‘unwanted’ behaviours (for example, tight nosebands used to prevent mouth movement), undoubtedly causes discomfort, with rebound behaviours occurring once the restriction is removed (Fenner et al. Reference Fenner, Yoon, White, Starling and McGreevy2016). Attempts to avoid the oral pressure of a bit (Eisersiö et al. Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Hartmann and Egenvall2023) and behaviour recorded when the horse’s body is forced and fixed into unnatural positions (Smiet et al. Reference Smiet, Van Dierendonck, Sleutjens, Menheere, van Breda, de Boer, Back, Wijnberg and Van Der Kolk2014) are further examples of how some aspects of ridden work and training can be very negative experiences for the horse. The impact of ridden work on the affective state of the horse can be assessed not only by ridden responses, but also by anticipatory and post-ridden behaviour. Improved welfare in ridden horses can only be achieved if their behavioural signs of confusion, discomfort or pain are responded to, not by restrictive equipment or harsher training methods, but by identifying the underlying cause.

Factors found to affect behaviour of the horse when ridden are shown in Table 4(b). Study details for the articles relating to this scenario are provided in Table S2.4 in the Supplementary material.

In summary

To date, no conclusive evidence has been presented to determine whether horses ever experience ridden activities as positive, or only neutral (responding as trained in response to rider signals) or negative. Where behaviour signifies pain or discomfort this must not be ignored. Developing an approach to training and all ridden activities (including equestrian sports) that has the potential to provide the horse with positive experiences is a challenge that must be addressed with urgency if equestrianism is to maintain its social licence to operate.

Behaviour of the horse during non-procedural horse-human interactions

Horse-human interactions not related to specific management procedures or training were considered separately. The retained articles comprised thirteen in which factors that influence how horses respond to humans in general were investigated. Methods used to assess horse-human interactions included human approach tests, behavioural responses to visual, auditory, and olfactory signs of human emotion, and the effect of watching what were considered by the authors to be positive (grooming) or negative (veterinary intervention) horse-human interactions on behaviour. In the human approach tests the response to the human was assessed according to whether the horse behaved in a way that increased or decreased the distance between horse and human. Tests used included those that allowed the horse to freely choose whether to approach a stationary human (also termed a voluntary human approach test) (Sankey et al. Reference Sankey, Richard-Yris, Henry, Fureix, Nassur and Hausberger2010; Brubaker et al. Reference Brubaker, Schroeder, Sherwood, Stroud and Udell2021; Lerch et al. Reference Lerch, Cirulli, Rochais, Lesimple, Guilbaud, Contalbrigo, Borgi, Grandgeorge and Hausberger2021) or those where the human approached the horse (forced human approach test) (Minero et al. Reference Minero, Dalla Costa, Dai, Canali, Barbieri, Zanella, Pascuzzo and Wemelsfelder2018). Measures used to assess the response of horses (movement of horse in relation to the human and behavioural interactions with the human) in these tests are shown in Table 5(a)(i).

Table 5. (a) (i, ii, iii) Behaviour of the horse during non-procedural horse-human interactions indicative of affective state (positive or negative), (i) during behavioural tests, (ii) in response to human expressions of emotion, and (iii) to watching third party horse-human interactions, supporting evidence* (see footnote below table for an explanation of the abbreviations used), and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse/human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach/avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

The ability of horses to recognise signs of human emotion (visual, auditory, and olfactory) was demonstrated in eight studies. Responses to human facial expressions depicting positive and negative human emotions (including happy, angry, disgust, and neutral expressions) were recorded (from photographs: Smith et al. Reference Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan and McComb2016; live: Baba et al. Reference Baba, Kawai and Takimoto-Inose2019; Merkies et al. Reference Merkies, Sudarenko and Hodder2022). The impact of facial expression, as well as non-verbal auditory stimuli (laughter, growling: Smith et al. Reference Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan, Scott and McComb2018a), human body posture (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Wilson, McComb and Proops2018b) and human body odour (fear, non-fear: Sabiniewicz et al. Reference Sabiniewicz, Tarnowska, Świątek, Sorokowski and Laska2020) on horse behaviour is shown in Table 5(a)(ii). Approach behaviour was more likely to occur in response to positive human facial expressions (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan and McComb2016; Baba et al. Reference Baba, Kawai and Takimoto-Inose2019; Merkies et al. Reference Merkies, Sudarenko and Hodder2022), positive non-verbal communication (laughter: Smith et al. Reference Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan, Scott and McComb2018a), and when the human adopted a submissive rather than a dominant body posture (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Wilson, McComb and Proops2018b). Human body odour associated with fear resulted in increased head raising and touching the human (Sabniewicz et al. 2020). Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan and McComb2016) recorded increased HR in response to the photographs of angry (as opposed to happy) human faces, whereas Merkies et al. (Reference Merkies, Sudarenko and Hodder2022) found when using live actors (with facial expressions of anger, sadness, joy, and neutral) there was no effect on the horse’s HR.

Further evidence of the horse’s ability to recognise the valence associated with human expression was demonstrated in two studies investigating behavioural responses to incongruous cross-modal signals (facial expression and vocalisation: Nakamura et al. Reference Nakamura, Takimoto-Inose and Hasegawa2018; Trösch et al. Reference Trösch, Cuzol, Parias, Calandreau, Nowak and Lansade2019). In both studies, the horses reacted in accordance with the valence of the vocalisation, particularly if this was negative and incongruent with the visual image. Nakamura et al. (Reference Nakamura, Takimoto-Inose and Hasegawa2018) recorded an increase in HR and length of time spent looking at the visual image if the negative vocalisation (scolding voice) was from a familiar human and incongruent with the facial expression on the image. Trösch et al. (Reference Trösch, Cuzol, Parias, Calandreau, Nowak and Lansade2019) also recorded increased HR and attention paid to an incongruent image depicting facial expression when the valence of the human vocalisation was negative (phonetic sound representing anger). See also Part I of this review (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024; Intra-species social behaviour) where the salience of vocalisation as a means of communicating emotion between horses was discussed.

The potential for emotional contagion to occur when horses watched horse-human interactions on a screen was demonstrated by Trösch et al. (Reference Trösch, Pellon, Cuzol, Parias, Nowak, Calandreau and Lansade2020). Responses to watching interactions deemed positive (grooming) differed from those recorded when watching negative (veterinary procedures) horse-handler interactions, with contact-seeking behaviour and lower HR associated with the positive interactions. However, following the viewing, when presented with both handlers, increased contact-seeking behaviour was associated with the negatively interacting handler which the authors tentatively suggested could be linked to attempts at appeasement, but the reason was not clear.

What was evident from the findings of the retained articles was the ability of the horse to respond to human expressions of emotion, with preference generally given for positive signals. It is recognised that human emotion is contagious (Herrando & Constantinides Reference Herrando and Constantinides2021), and the attempted mutual grooming behaviour elicited by watching video footage of positive horse-human interactions during grooming (Trosch et al. 2020), suggests that emotional transfer can occur between human and horse (see Table 5[a [iii]).

Factors found to affect horse behaviour during non-procedural horse-human interactions are shown in Table 5(b).

Study details for the articles relating to this scenario are provided in Table S2.5 in the Supplementary material.

In summary

Given the evidence that horses respond to human expressions of emotion, a positive mood state while interacting with horses in whatever capacity may induce a more positive response from the horse and facilitate an improved horse-human relationship. Unpredictable or negative interactions with humans are likely to result in avoidance behaviour, or at least reduce the inclination for horses to approach humans. This may, in part, explain the lack of voluntary human interaction (Lerch et al. Reference Lerch, Cirulli, Rochais, Lesimple, Guilbaud, Contalbrigo, Borgi, Grandgeorge and Hausberger2021) and reduced affiliative behaviour shown to humans (Brubaker et al. Reference Brubaker, Schroeder, Sherwood, Stroud and Udell2021) by horses with experience of participation in Equine Assisted Services (EAS) compared with those with no such experience.

Behaviour of the horse during transport

The transport of domestic horses is a common occurrence for leisure, sporting, breeding, or veterinary purposes and is acknowledged as potentially having a negative impact on health and performance (Leadon Reference Leadon1994). Twelve articles relating to horse transport were retained, ten of which involved transport by road, with two involving air transport (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Foster and Waas2003; Munsters et al. Reference Munsters, De Gooijer, Van Den Broek and Van Oldruitenborgh-Oosterbaan2013). It was acknowledged that there are physical demands associated with maintaining balance during road transport, and this was demonstrated by the results reported (for example, Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Maggiolino, Boccaccio and Tateo2012; Tateo et al. Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012; Padalino & Raidal Reference Padalino and Raidal2020). Behaviour at different stages of transport was recorded, as well as the impact of factors such as space and position within the transport vehicle (Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Maggiolino, Boccaccio and Tateo2012; Padalino & Raidal Reference Padalino and Raidal2020), and journey duration (Tateo et al. Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012). Behavioural measures taken during transport related to movement associated with maintaining balance (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Maggiolino, Boccaccio and Tateo2012, Reference Padalino, Raidal, Knight, Celi, Jeffcott and Muscatello2018; Tateo et al. Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012; Padalino & Raidal Reference Padalino and Raidal2020), behaviour attributed to signs of mental stress (Kay & Hall Reference Kay and Hall2009; Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Raidal, Knight, Celi, Jeffcott and Muscatello2018; Padalino & Raidal Reference Padalino and Raidal2020) and other behavioural states including social interactions (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Foster and Waas2003; Knowles et al. Reference Knowles, Brown, Pope, Nicol, Warriss and Weeks2010) and feeding (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Foster and Waas2003; Kay & Hall Reference Kay and Hall2009). See Table 6(a) for behaviour associated with positive and negative responses to transport during and post-transportation.

Table 6. (a) Behaviour of the horse during and after transportation indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence* (see footnote below table for an explanation of the abbreviations used), and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse/human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach/avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

Pre-transport

It was reported that the loading phase of the journey was found to be the most stressful (Siniscalchi et al. Reference Siniscalchi, Padalino, Lusito and Quaranta2014), with evasive and avoidance behaviour being used as a measure of this (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Hendriksen et al. Reference Hendriksen, Elmgreen and Ladewig2011; Siniscalchi et al. Reference Siniscalchi, Padalino, Lusito and Quaranta2014). Increases in HR were recorded during loading into both road (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Tateo et al. Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012; Siniscalchi et al. Reference Siniscalchi, Padalino, Lusito and Quaranta2014) and air transport vehicles (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Foster and Waas2003; Munsters et al. Reference Munsters, De Gooijer, Van Den Broek and Van Oldruitenborgh-Oosterbaan2013). Although Hendriksen et al. (Reference Hendriksen, Elmgreen and Ladewig2011) found that training to load horses using positive reinforcement resulted in a reduction in behavioural signs of discomfort (compared with using negative reinforcement), this was not associated with any difference in HR. Although the increases in HR may, in part, reflect increased arousal associated with anticipation (positive or negative), it is likely that the primary cause of this was the physical exertion involved in entering the vehicle.

During transport

When the transport vehicle was moving horses spent more time in a braced position and leaning against the partition, and less time feeding than when the vehicle was stationary (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995). Vehicle movement was associated with an increase in HR (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Padalino & Raidal Reference Padalino and Raidal2020), which was also recorded during air transport at times of take-off and landing, but not throughout the rest of the flight (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Foster and Waas2003; Munsters et al. Reference Munsters, De Gooijer, Van Den Broek and Van Oldruitenborgh-Oosterbaan2013). Increases in HR were found to be reduced when the horse was transported with a live companion (as opposed to alone) (Kay & Hall Reference Kay and Hall2009), or when the horse travelled with its head in a lowered position (Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Raidal, Knight, Celi, Jeffcott and Muscatello2018). Although the latter finding may relate to improved balance when in this position, isolation stress during transport was shown to be a contributory factor (Kay & Hall Reference Kay and Hall2009). In addition to a decrease in HR, travelling with a companion was also associated with a decrease in the behavioural signs of stress, including less time spent head tossing and vocalising, and more time spent eating than when travelling alone (Kay & Hall Reference Kay and Hall2009).

Post transport

Post-transport behaviour was recorded by Padalino et al. (Reference Padalino, Maggiolino, Boccaccio and Tateo2012), Tateo et al. (Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012) and Ji et al. (Reference Ji, Yang, Ge, Wang, Cao and Hu2013). Following transportation for the first time (for 5 h, 15 mins), Przewalski horses (Equus ferus prezwalskii) moved more, stood resting less and drank less than pre-transport, and increases in faecal cortisol metabolites were recorded 24 h post-transport (returning to pre-transport levels within three days) (Ji et al. Reference Ji, Yang, Ge, Wang, Cao and Hu2013). After three-hour road journeys an increase in feeding and drinking was recorded by Padalino et al. (Reference Padalino, Maggiolino, Boccaccio and Tateo2012), and by Tateo et al. (Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012). The confinement and frequently unfamiliar environment associated with transport has an impact on behaviour both during and after a journey. Most stress-related behaviours were recorded during the first hour of a journey (Padalino et al. Reference Padalino, Raidal, Knight, Celi, Jeffcott and Muscatello2018) and journeys of over three hours resulted in more rebound behaviour following transport confinement than shorter journeys (Tateo et al. Reference Tateo, Padalino, Boccaccio, Maggiolino and Centoducati2012).

See Table 6(b) for factors affecting horse behaviour during and post-transportation and Table S2.6 (Supplementary material) for details of the studies included in this scenario.

In summary

All stages of this activity have the potential to negatively affect the horse, over and above the physical challenges of remaining upright. Careful pre-planning, training, vehicle design and providing a travel companion, as well as allowing sufficient recovery time should all be travelling requirements. The fact that evasive and avoidance behaviour is so prevalent at the loading stage is evidence that many horses are at best apprehensive about entering confined vehicle spaces, particularly during initial training, and that measures that can be taken to ameliorate this should be applied.

Behaviour of the horse during training other than when ridden (methods and equipment)

Nine articles with a focus on training and groundwork, which did not include riding the horse, were retained. These included behavioural responses to foundation training (Nittynen et al. Reference Nittynen, Riihonen, Moscovice and Koski2022), traditional compared with natural horsemanship training (Fureix et al. Reference Fureix, Pagès, Bon, Lassalle, Kuntz and Gonzalez2009), responses associated with positively or negatively reinforced training (Sankey et al. Reference Sankey, Richard-Yris, Henry, Fureix, Nassur and Hausberger2010; Larssen & Roth Reference Larssen and Roth2022), different head and neck positions during lunging (Smiet et al. Reference Smiet, Van Dierendonck, Sleutjens, Menheere, van Breda, de Boer, Back, Wijnberg and Van Der Kolk2014), a low-intensity, stress-provoking task (walking backwards: Rietmann et al. Reference Rietmann, Stuart, Bernasconi, Stauffacher, Auer and Weishaupt2004b) and the effect of restrictive and/or pressure-inducing equipment (tight nosebands: Fenner et al. Reference Fenner, Yoon, White, Starling and McGreevy2016; rein tension/bit pressure: Eisersiö et al. Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Byström, Baragli and Egenvall2021, Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Hartmann and Egenvall2023). The measures used either focused on behavioural responses towards humans, reactivity in novel object and isolation tests, and signs of discomfort and/or rebound behaviour. Behaviours signifying positive and negative responses to training other than when ridden are shown in Table 7(a).

Table 7. (a) Behaviour of the horse during training other than when ridden indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence* (see footnote below Table for an explanation of the abbreviations used), and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse/human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach/avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

Following positive reinforcement training, increased approach/contact-seeking behaviour towards humans was recorded (Sankey et al. Reference Sankey, Richard-Yris, Henry, Fureix, Nassur and Hausberger2010; Larssen & Roth Reference Larssen and Roth2022), which may in part be the association of the human with food as this was used as the primary positive reinforcer during training. An association between an increase in HR and negative as opposed to positive reinforcement was found by Sankey et al. (Reference Sankey, Richard-Yris, Henry, Fureix, Nassur and Hausberger2010), but reinforcement type was not associated with differences in hair cortisol levels (Larssen & Roth Reference Larssen and Roth2022). However, the type of reinforcement used had no effect on overall affective state as determined by judgement bias testing (Larsson & Roth Reference Larssen and Roth2022).

Fureix et al. (Reference Fureix, Pagès, Bon, Lassalle, Kuntz and Gonzalez2009) found reduced latency to approach a motionless human in horses trained using natural horsemanship methods as opposed to traditional training, but although reduced responses to isolation and novel object tests occurred following training, this did not differ with training type (Fureix et al. Reference Fureix, Pagès, Bon, Lassalle, Kuntz and Gonzalez2009). As well as differences in response associated with the type/approach to training, a variation in how young horses respond to training was found by Nittynen et al. (Reference Nittynen, Riihonen, Moscovice and Koski2022). During foundation training, increases in salivary oxytocin occurred in horses showing affiliative, human-directed behaviours, with decreases in salivary oxytocin occurring in those showing signs of discomfort (Nittynen et al. Reference Nittynen, Riihonen, Moscovice and Koski2022). The authors suggest that salivary oxytocin may be a useful non-invasive indicator of the subjective experience of training on young horses (Nittynen et al. Reference Nittynen, Riihonen, Moscovice and Koski2022). In the same study, decreases in salivary cortisol concentration were recorded as the training progressed, alongside decreased signs of fear and discomfort, but the former increased with longer training sessions, suggestive of a link with physical exertion (Nittynen et al. Reference Nittynen, Riihonen, Moscovice and Koski2022). Horse-human interactions during training can undoubtedly have an impact on how horses perceive humans, but it is unclear the extent to which this contributes to more general affective state.

The exercise of backing up in hand was shown to be challenging for horses by Rietmann et al. (Reference Rietmann, Stuart, Bernasconi, Stauffacher, Auer and Weishaupt2004b), who found correlations between HR and HRV parameters and behaviour during this low intensity but mentally stressful exercise (head high, explosive deviation and stopping). Backing up was also used to investigate the relationship between rein tension and behaviour, and the effect of a bit during in-hand training (Eisersiö et al. Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Byström, Baragli and Egenvall2021, Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Hartmann and Egenvall2023). When a bit was used during the exercise (rather than a bit-less halter), an increase in head and neck movements, and oral behaviours was recorded (Eisersiö et al. Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Byström, Baragli and Egenvall2021). Specific behaviours were found to affect rein tension, with increases of tension occurring with head movement down and forwards, decreased tension when the head was raised, the mouth open, and/or biting on the bit (Eisersiö et al. Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Hartmann and Egenvall2023). Clear behavioural signs linked with attempts to relieve bit pressure on the mouth were recorded. It was also demonstrated that the correct application of negative reinforcement enabled rapid decreases in the rein pressure required to train the task (Eisersiö et al. Reference Eisersiö, Yngvesson, Byström, Baragli and Egenvall2021). Human training in the correct application of learning theory and an awareness of the behavioural signs of discomfort in the horse would both be beneficial in reducing the negative impact of equestrianism on horse welfare.

Restricting movement during training both masks the response of the horse and is likely to cause discomfort. Lunging horses with their head and necks maintained in a specific position (variations in elevation and flexion) resulted in attempted avoidance behaviour when the side reins were being attached and stepping backwards when fitted to maintain an elevated head position with the bridge of the nose around the vertical (Smiet et al. Reference Smiet, Van Dierendonck, Sleutjens, Menheere, van Breda, de Boer, Back, Wijnberg and Van Der Kolk2014). This latter position also resulted in the most conflict behaviour and increases in salivary cortisol concentration. Negative anticipatory behaviour was also recorded when side reins were fixed to maintain the position with the neck lowered and flexed, and the bridge of the nose was pointing towards the carpus (Smiet et al. Reference Smiet, Van Dierendonck, Sleutjens, Menheere, van Breda, de Boer, Back, Wijnberg and Van Der Kolk2014). The impact of noseband tightness on physiology and behaviour while standing in a test area for ten minutes was assessed by Fenner et al. (Reference Fenner, Yoon, White, Starling and McGreevy2016). In the tight noseband condition, an increase in HR, decreased HRV and an increase in eye temperature were recorded. Following the release of the pressure when the noseband was unfastened, yawning, swallowing, and licking (rebound behaviours) occurred (Fenner et al. Reference Fenner, Yoon, White, Starling and McGreevy2016). There is clear evidence that movement restriction is at best unnecessary during training, unhelpful in that it reduces the feedback that the horse can give to the human trainer, and at worst a negative and potentially painful experience for the horse.

Factors affecting behavioural responses during training other than when ridden and in response to the equipment used are shown in Table 7(b). See Table S2.7 in the Supplementary material.

In summary

All approaches to training are likely to be experienced initially as challenging and potentially negative experiences for the horse. Evidence suggests that this lessens as training progresses, and the horse learns to make the association between behaviour and outcome. To ensure that this interaction with humans does not become an increasingly negative experience, the trainer must be consistent in the application of learning theory and both aware of, and responsive to, positive and negative behavioural responses from the horse. The use of equipment that restricts this behavioural expression will also restrict the ability of the trainer to respond appropriately and is likely to substantially increase the aversiveness of this interaction for the horse.

General behavioural signs of affective state during interactions with humans

In the two-dimensional model of core affect proposed by Mendl et al. (Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010), positive valence when the animal is in a relatively high state of arousal is associated with behaviour motivated by reward acquisition. In a comparative state of arousal, negative valence is associated with behaviour motivated by the avoidance of punishment (and potentially pain/discomfort). There were specific examples of avoidance behaviour in the scenarios reported in this review. In ridden work, behaviour associated with restrictive practices (von Borstel et al. Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Beekmans, van Dalum and Van Dierendonck2014) and behavioural signs of negative anticipation during preparation for being ridden (Dyson et al. Reference Dyson, Bondi, Routh, Pollard, Preston, McConnell and Kydd2022), again suggestive of avoidance attempts, should all be regarded as attempts by the horse to avoid negative consequences. During transport, avoidance behaviour was found to be prevalent at the loading stage (Waran & Cuddeford Reference Waran and Cuddeford1995; Hendriksen et al. Reference Hendriksen, Elmgreen and Ladewig2011; Siniscalchi et al. Reference Siniscalchi, Padalino, Lusito and Quaranta2014), potentially associated with the anticipation of entering an unknown environment and being separated from conspecifics. The occurrence of avoidance behaviour, in whatever context-specific form this takes, provides a clear signal that certain experiences are aversive to the horse. In contrast, examples of approach behaviour suggest that the horse anticipates a positive outcome. A tendency to approach a human displaying positive emotional signals as opposed to negative ones (for example, Smith et al. Reference Smith, Proops, Grounds, Wathan, Scott and McComb2018a,b; Merkies et al. Reference Merkies, Sudarenko and Hodder2022), suggests both the ability to recognise human expression and that this is used to predict a positive or negative outcome from subsequent interactions.

As well as providing context-specific information regarding the valence of emotional responses, approach/avoidance behaviour provides some insight into the horse’s overall affective state. Minero et al. (Reference Minero, Dalla Costa, Dai, Canali, Barbieri, Zanella, Pascuzzo and Wemelsfelder2018) found that horses assessed as being more at ease and relaxed in the home environment were most likely to readily approach a human when tested, and this behaviour in the home environment was judged to reflect a positive affective state. A positive association was found between the amount of forage provided and the tendency for horses to approach a human in a voluntary approach test (Lerch et al. Reference Lerch, Cirulli, Rochais, Lesimple, Guilbaud, Contalbrigo, Borgi, Grandgeorge and Hausberger2021).

In states of low arousal, Mendl et al. (Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010) describe the difference between positive and negative affective state as calm and relaxed or sad and depressed, respectively. It was noted in Part I of this review (Hall & Kay; The home environment) that differentiating between positive and negative signs when the horse is stationary may yield inconsistent conclusions, but general demeanour has the potential to provide an insight into the affective state of the horse. Hausberger et al. (Reference Hausberger, Fureix and Lesimple2016) acknowledged the need for robust, objective measures of physical health, and used postural features and stance to differentiate between standing resting, standing observing, and standing withdrawn. Behavioural variability, as observed to increase when horses were recovering from low-level pain (Egan et al. Reference Egan, Kearney, Brama, Parnell and McGrath2021), which includes periods of alertness and relaxation, would appear to be one important positive sign. With the proviso that subjective assessment is likely to be influenced by human experience, training, and role within the equine sector (as found in ridden horses by Hall et al. Reference Hall, Kay and Yarnell2014), a qualitative approach using evidence-based behavioural descriptors (similar to that used in the AWIN protocol: AWIN 2015), could be developed for wider application.

To enable an accurate response to the question posed by Dawkins (Reference Dawkins2004) regarding the health of the horse, behavioural signs of pain must be identified. Such signs should also determine the nature of subsequent interactions with humans, to ensure that these are not associated with increasing discomfort. Behavioural signs of negative anticipation may reflect underlying health issues or an association with past painful/negative experiences. Although it is not always easy to distinguish between behaviour indicative of pain and fear-related behaviour, and individuals vary in how both are expressed, this is an area that requires further investigation. Currently, pain-related behaviour may not always be interpreted as such, resulting in prolonged suffering, and potentially negatively affecting horse-human interactions.

Behaviour during many horse-human interactions is controlled/restricted by the human, and consequently signs indicative of positive or negative experiences may not be expressed by the horse and will also be affected by training. However, better recognition of behavioural signs of how horses experience interactions with humans would facilitate change in those identified as unpleasant for the horse.

The challenges and limitations of this systematic review (Parts I [Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024] and II combined)

The objectives of this systematic review, to identify evidence of how horse behaviour reflects affective state and its determining factors, with the overall aim of enabling the assessment of quality of life, were clear. However, assimilating the findings of the 179 disparate studies that were identified by the search terms and retained has proved to be complex. The variety of the scenarios explored reflects the diversity of activities and procedures that constitute the domestic life of the horse, at least in their role in sporting or leisure pursuits (as covered in this review). Notably, the findings suggest a complex inter-relationship between different aspects of the life of the domestic horse and the impact that they collectively have on their quality of life.

General challenges relating to research in horse welfare include accessibility/availability of subjects, consistency, and comparability of management conditions and ultimately the costs involved in studies involving the horse. Consequently, in many of the retained articles, low subject numbers are involved. A minimum for retention in this review was set at four subjects, although eight subjects per experimental group was suggested by Pongrácz (Reference Pongrácz2023) where statistical analyses were performed. The articles retained in this review included fourteen with fewer than eight subjects per experimental group (seven subjects: Rogers et al. Reference Rogers, Walsh, Gee and Firth2012; Smiet et al. Reference Smiet, Van Dierendonck, Sleutjens, Menheere, van Breda, de Boer, Back, Wijnberg and Van Der Kolk2014; Dalla Costa et al. Reference Dalla Costa, Bracci, Dai, Lebelt and Minero2017; Egan et al. Reference Egan, Kearney, Brama, Parnell and McGrath2021; six subjects: Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005; Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012; Ji et al. Reference Ji, Yang, Ge, Wang, Cao and Hu2013; Destrez et al. Reference Destrez, Grimm, Cézilly and Julliand2015; Reid et al. Reference Reid, Rogers, Gronqvist, Gee and Bolwell2017; Carvalho et al. Reference Carvalho, Trindade, Conde, Antonioli, Funnicelli, Dias, Canola, Chinelatto and Ferraz2022; five subjects: Marliani et al. Reference Marliani, Sprocatti, Schiavoni, Bellodi and Accorsi2021; Robinson & Bye Reference Robinson and Bye2021; and four subjects: Quick & Warren-Smith Reference Quick and Warren-Smith2009; McDuffee et al. Reference McDuffee, Carr and Montelpare2022). Most of the studies with low subject numbers involved repeated measures, an exception being Quick and Warren-Smith (Reference Quick and Warren-Smith2009). In only two articles, both included in Behavioural signs of pain in the horse, were references made to power calculations to establish the minimum required sample size (Dodds et al. Reference Dodds, Knight, Allen and Murrell2017; Ortolani et al. Reference Ortolani, Scilimati, Gialletti, Menchetti and Nannarone2021). Dodds et al. (Reference Dodds, Knight, Allen and Murrell2017) noted that a power calculation was not carried out prior to the start of the study because there was no preliminary data upon which to base such analyses, and data collection was dependent upon the number of eligible horses that presented to the clinic during the study period. Similar challenges were noted by Ortolani et al. (Reference Ortolani, Scilimati, Gialletti, Menchetti and Nannarone2021) who reported calculating the required sample size as 24 but ended up with only 23 subjects.

At least in part, it is the result of challenges relating to availability, controllability, and cost that this review is based on multiple, relatively small studies. It does however provide a broad overview of what is also a large and varied sector. However, one of the downfalls of the number of articles retained within this review has been the time that it has taken to evaluate and assimilate the results of 179 separate studies. The time lapse between the date of the search deadline and manuscript submission has undoubtedly resulted in the omission of the most recent articles, but those retained provide robust evidence of measures that must be taken to improve the quality of life of the domestic horse.

Within the retained articles only three examples of studies where preference tests had been included were identified (see Part I [Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024]; Table 2(b): preferred method of forage presentation in horse groups, Melvin et al. Reference Melvin, Costello and Colpoys2020; Part I (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024); Table 3(b): preference for companion over food in mares, Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jastrzębska, Drewka, Nadolna, Becker and Lansade2022; and Part II; Behaviour of the horse when ridden: preference for a location associated with being ridden in a regular poll position as opposed to coercively obtained hyperflexion of the head and neck: von Borstel et al. Reference von Borstel, Duncan, Shoveller, Merkies, Keeling and Millman2009). Although constrained by human-selected options, further use of preference testing, and the comparison of behaviour in preferred and non-preferred situations could provide valuable insights into behavioural signs of positive affective state (with the proviso that individual differences in the behavioural expression of affective state will occur).

Animal welfare implications

Once the behavioural needs of the horse have been addressed, the impact of other aspects of management and training require consideration. As evidenced by the number of scenarios covered in this review, and factors that have been shown to have a negative impact on behaviour, it is likely that these will have a negative effect on the QOL of the domestic horse. As acknowledged in other species (for example, experimental cattle and pigs; Ryan et al. Reference Ryan, Waters and Wolfensohn2021), QOL is determined by the cumulative impact of past experiences, and the balance of pleasant and unpleasant experiences that horses have in their many and varied interactions with humans will contribute to their QOL. Whenever possible, management procedures should be designed to reduce aversion and adapted to be pleasurable for the horse, as demonstrated during grooming by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Nowak, Lainé, Leterrier, Bonneau, Parias and Bertin2018). Also, there should be careful consideration of whether potentially aversive procedures are necessary and where possible their duration and/or frequency reduced or avoided altogether. Adherence to the LIMA (least invasive, minimally aversive) principles, as included in the Joint Standards of Practice and Code of Ethics adopted by the International Association of Animal Behaviour Consultants (IAABC) in 2018 (https://iaabc.org/code-of-ethics), should be used to guide all aspects of horse management and training. Clearly, some necessary procedures may be unavoidably aversive (for example, veterinary procedures) but any negative impact can be reduced by ensuring that the human handler is skilled in the procedure and that it is completed as quickly as possible (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jaworski, Suwała, Sobczyńska, Jastrzębska, Ogłuszka, Sankey, Boroń and Jezierski2017). Familiarity with the handler (Marsbøll & Christensen Reference Marsbøll and Christensen2015; Liehrmann et al. Reference Liehrmann, Viitanen, Riihonen, Alander, Koski, Lummaa and Lansade2022), physical restraint (Guinnefollau et al. Reference Guinnefollau, Bolwell, Gee, Norman and Rogers2021), the environment in which the procedure is carried out and the level of training/habituation to the procedure, will all affect the experience of the horse. The potentially negative cumulative impact of repeated aversive procedures (as may be required in a veterinary context) must be acknowledged and measures taken to provide positive experiences to counterbalance this.

The extent to which these recommendations are applied will ultimately depend upon the decisions and behaviour of those responsible for caring for the horse. In agreement with the first principle of humane livestock farming compiled by The Council on Animal Affairs in The Netherlands, there must be a recognition of the intrinsic value of the animal as a sentient being that can experience pain and pleasure (Council on Animal Affairs 2021). Acknowledgement of the importance of providing the horse with sufficient opportunities to fulfil its natural behavioural needs, and recognition of the inextricable link between behavioural satisfaction and affective state, is the basis for moving closer to providing a good life for the domestic horse.

To promote positive affective state in the domestic horse there are key issues that need addressing, as shown in Figure 1. The first challenge is for those within the equine sector to acknowledge that change is needed. Changes for the better, however small, in any of the areas identified, have the potential to make a positive impact on the horse. Once horse behaviour is recognised more clearly as an indicator of affective state, and subsequently results in a change in human behaviour, the horse will have a better chance of living a good life. The outcome for the horse is reliant upon these changes in human behaviour.

Figure 1. Flow chart demonstrating how human behaviour can facilitate the progression from satisfying the behavioural needs of the horse to providing a good life for the domestic horse.

Conclusion

Quality of life is determined by the balance between pleasant and unpleasant experiences over time and is reduced by the cumulative effect of repeated negative experiences. The role of the domestic horse as a sporting, leisure or companion animal is associated with many and varied interactions with humans. Behavioural signs indicative of whether these interactions are experienced as pleasant or unpleasant by the horse are confounded by human control and training and are open to misinterpretation. However, conclusions can be drawn in situations where there are opportunities for freedom of movement. Positive experiences of interactions with humans were associated with approach behaviour, negative ones with avoidance behaviour, but training could affect both. The absence of outward behavioural signs of attempted avoidance during interactions may result from training and/or restrictive practices, rather than reflecting the subjective experience of the horse. Many interactions with humans have the potential to be unpleasant for the horse and the equine sector now needs to consider what changes should be made to management and training procedures to make them less aversive and more pleasurable. A good life for horses is only possible if their species-specific needs are met and their lifetime experiences of interactions with humans are predominantly positive. The quality of life of the domestic horse can only improve if stakeholders from across the equine sector acknowledge the need for change and implement the findings of this review.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.41.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Natalie Waran, Hayley Randle, Gemma Pearson, and Mark Farnworth for their input at the planning stage of this review, Liane Preshaw and Jan Rogers for their feedback on an earlier draft, and the Horse Trust for providing funding for the initial part of the study.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: CH, RK; Data curation: CH, RK; Formal analysis: CH; Methodology: CH, RK; Investigation: CH, RK; Supervision: CH; Validation: CH; Visualisation: CH; Funding acquisition: CH; Project administration: CH; Resources: CH, RK; Software: CH; Writing – original draft: CH, RK; Writing – review & editing: CH, RK.

References

Ahern, GL and Schwartz, GE 1979 Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion. Neuropsychologia 17: 693698. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(79)90045-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ali, ABA, Gutwein, KL and Heleski, CR 2017 Assessing the influence of upper lip twitching in naive horses during an aversive husbandry procedure (ear clipping). Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 21: 2025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
AWIN 2015 AWIN assessment protocol for horses. https://doi.org/10.13130/AWIN_horses_2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baba, C, Kawai, M and Takimoto-Inose, A 2019 Are horses (Equus caballus) sensitive to human emotional cues? Animals 9: 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090630CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baragli, P, Banti, L, Vitale, V and Sighieri, C 2014 Effect of aging on behavioural and physiological responses to a stressful stimulus in horses (Equus caballus). Behaviour 151: 15131533. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baragli, P, Scopa, C, Felici, M and Reddon, AR 2021 Horses show individual level lateralisation when inspecting an unfamiliar and unexpected stimulus. PLoS ONE 16: 114. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0255688CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birt, MA, Guay, K, Treiber, K, Ramirez, HR and Snyder, D 2015 The influence of a soft touch therapy flowtrition on heart rate, surface temperature, and behavior in horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 35: 636644. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2015.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brubaker, L, Schroeder, K, Sherwood, D, Stroud, D and Udell, MAR 2021 Horse behavior towards familiar and unfamiliar humans: Implications for equine-assisted services. Animals 11: 8. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11082369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bulens, A, Sterken, H, Van Beirendonck, S, Van Thielen, J and Driessen, B 2015 The use of different objects during a novel object test in stabled horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 10: 5458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burn, CC, Dennison, TL and Whay, HR 2010 Relationships between behaviour and health in working horses, donkeys, and mules in developing countries. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 126: 109118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bussières, G, Jacques, C, Lainay, O, Beauchamp, G, Leblond, A, Cadoré, JL, Desmaizières, LM, Cuvelliez, SG and Troncy, E 2008 Development of a composite orthopaedic pain scale in horses. Research in Veterinary Science 85: 294306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rvsc.2007.10.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carvalho, JRG, Trindade, PHE, Conde, G, Antonioli, ML, Funnicelli, MIG, Dias, PP, Canola, PA, Chinelatto, MA and Ferraz, GC 2022 Facial expressions of horses using weighted multivariate statistics for assessment of subtle local pain induced by polylactide-based polymers implanted subcutaneously. Animals 12: 18. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12182400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, JW, Beekmans, M, van Dalum, M and Van Dierendonck, M 2014 Effects of hyperflexion on acute stress responses in ridden dressage horses. Physiology and Behavior 128: 3945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.01.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, JW, Munk, R, Hawson, L, Palme, R, Larsen, T, Egenvall, A, König von Borstel UU and Rørvang, MV 2021 Rider effects on horses’ conflict behaviour, rein tension, physiological measures and rideability scores. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condon, VM, McGreevy, PD, McLean, AN, Williams, JM and Randle, H 2022 Associations between commonly used apparatus and conflict behaviors reported in the ridden horse in Australia. Journal of Veterinary Behaviour 49: 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.10.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Council on Animal Affairs 2021 Six guiding principles to ensure humane livestock farming. RDA Advisory Report on Humane Livestock Farming: 10. https://english.rda.nl/publications/publications/2022/6/29/humane-livestock-farming (Accessed 25 February 2023).Google Scholar
Dalla Costa, E, Bracci, D, Dai, F, Lebelt, D and Minero, M 2017 Do different emotional states affect the Horse Grimace Scale score? A pilot study. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 54: 114117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2017.03.221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Costa, ED, Pascuzzo, R, Leach, MC, Dai, F, Lebelt, D, Vantini, S and Minero, M 2018 Can grimace scales estimate the pain status in horses and mice? A statistical approach to identify a classifier. PLoS ONE 13: 117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200339CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, MS 2004 Using behaviour to assess animal welfare. Animal Welfare 13: S3S7. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600014317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Boyer Des Roches, A, Richard-Yris, MA, Henry, S, Ezzaouïa, M and Hausberger, M 2008 Laterality and emotions: Visual laterality in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) differs with objects’ emotional value. Physiology and Behavior 94: 487490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Destrez, A, Grimm, P, Cézilly, F and Julliand, V 2015 Changes of the hindgut microbiota due to high-starch diet can be associated with behavioral stress response in horses. Physiology and Behavior 149: 159164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodds, L, Knight, L, Allen, K and Murrell, J 2017 The effect of postsurgical pain on attentional processing in horses. Veterinary Anaesthesia and Analgesia 44: 933942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaa.2016.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durier, V, Henry, S, Sankey, C, Sizun, J and Hausberger, M 2012 Locomotor inhibition in adult horses faced to stressors: A single postpartum experience may be enough! Frontiers in Psychology 3: 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyson, S, Berger, J, Ellis, A and Mullard, J 2018 Development of an ethogram for a pain scoring system in ridden horses and its application to determine the presence of musculoskeletal pain. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 23: 4757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.10.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyson, S, Bondi, A, Routh, J, Pollard, D, Preston, T, McConnell, C and Kydd, JH 2022 An investigation of behaviour during tacking-up and mounting in ridden sports and leisure horses. Equine Veterinary Education 34: e245e257. https://doi.org/10.1111/eve.13432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyson, S and Pollard, D 2021 Application of the ridden horse pain ethogram to horses competing at the Hickstead-Rotterdam grand prix challenge and the British dressage grand prix national championship 2020 and comparison with world cup grand prix competitions. Animals 11: 116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061820CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, S, Kearney, CM, Brama, PAJ, Parnell, AC and McGrath, D 2021 Exploring stable-based behaviour and behaviour switching for the detection of bilateral pain in equines. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 235: 105214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisersiö, M, Yngvesson, J, Byström, A, Baragli, P and Egenvall, A 2021 A rein tension signal can be reduced by half in a single training session. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisersiö, M, Yngvesson, J, Hartmann, E and Egenvall, A 2023 Gaping for relief? Rein tension at onset and end of oral behaviors and head movements in unridden horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 59: 814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.11.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erber, R, Wulf, M, Becker-Birck, M, Kaps, S, Aurich, JE, Möstl, E and Aurich, C 2012 Physiological and behavioural responses of young horses to hot iron branding and microchip implantation. Veterinary Journal 191: 171175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2011.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenner, K, Yoon, S, White, P, Starling, M and McGreevy, P 2016 The effect of noseband tightening on horses’ behavior, eye temperature, and cardiac responses. PLoS ONE 11: 120. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0154179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortin, M, Valenchon, M, Lévy, F, Calandreau, L, Arnould, C and Lansade, L 2018 Emotional state and personality influence cognitive flexibility in horses (Equus caballus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 132: 130140. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, AF 1998 The Behaviour of the Horse pp 75. CABI Publishing: Wallingford, UK.Google Scholar
Fureix, C, Menguy, H and Hausberger, M 2010 Partners with bad temper: Reject or cure? A study of chronic pain and aggression in horses. PLoS ONE 5: 8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012434CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fureix, C, Pagès, M, Bon, R, Lassalle, JM, Kuntz, P and Gonzalez, G 2009 A preliminary study of the effects of handling type on horses’ emotional reactivity and the human-horse relationship. Behavioural Processes 82: 202210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2009.06.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Górecka-Bruzda, A, Jastrzębska, E, Drewka, M, Nadolna, Z, Becker, K and Lansade, L 2022 Female horses are more socially dependent than geldings kept in riding clubs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Górecka-Bruzda, A, Jastrzębska, E, Gajewska, E, Muszyńska, A and Pieniąże, P 2015 Results of behavioural tests of horses are predictive of perceived safety in riders. Animal Science Papers and Reports 33: 373382.Google Scholar
Górecka-Bruzda, A, Jaworski, Z, Suwała, M, Sobczyńska, M, Jastrzębska, E, Ogłuszka, M, Sankey, C, Boroń, M and Jezierski, T 2017 Aversiveness of husbandry procedures for pre-weaned foals: A comparison using behavioural and physiological indices. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 191: 3138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grint, NJ, Beths, T, Yvorchuk-St Jean, K, Whay, HR and Murrell, JC 2017 Analysis of behaviors observed during mechanical nociceptive threshold testing in donkeys and horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 50: 102109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2016.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guinnefollau, L, Bolwell, CF, Gee, EK, Norman, EJ and Rogers, CW 2021 Horses’ physiological and behavioural responses during undergraduate veterinary practical teaching classes. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, C and Kay, R 2024 Living the good life? A systematic review of behavioural signs of affective state in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) and factors relating to quality of life. Part I: Fulfilment of species-specific needs. Animal Welfare 33. https://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.38Google Scholar
Hall, C, Kay, R and Green, J 2020 A retrospective survey of factors affecting the risk of incidents and equine injury during non-commercial transportation by road in the United Kingdom. Animals 10: 288. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, C, Kay, R and Yarnell, K 2014 Assessing ridden horse behavior: Professional judgment and physiological measures. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 9: 2229. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JVEB.2013.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, C, Randle, H, Pearson, G, Preshaw, L and Waran, N 2018 Assessing equine emotional state. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 205: 183195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, KL, Lancaster, BE and Hall, C 2022 Equine conflict behaviors in dressage and their relationship to performance evaluation. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 55–56: 4857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.07.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harewood, EJ and McGowan, CM 2005 Behavioral and physiological responses to stabling in naive horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 25: 164170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2005.03.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausberger, M, Fureix, C and Lesimple, C 2016 Detecting horses’ sickness: In search of visible signs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 175: 4149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausberger, M, Muller, C and Lunel, C 2011 Does work affect personality? A study in horses. PLoS ONE 6(2). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014659CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendriksen, P, Elmgreen, K and Ladewig, J 2011 Trailer-loading of horses: Is there a difference between positive and negative reinforcement concerning effectiveness and stress-related signs? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 6(5): 261266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2011.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrando, C and Constantinides, E 2021 Emotional contagion: A brief overview and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 712606. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintze, S, Smith, S, Patt, A, Bachmann, I and Würbel, H 2016 Are eyes a mirror of the soul? What eye wrinkles reveal about a horse’s emotional state. PLoS ONE 11: 10. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honess, P and Wolfensohn, S 2010 A matrix for the assessment of welfare and cumulative suffering in experimental animals. Alternatives to Laboratory Animals 38: 205212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IAABC (International Association of Animal Behaviour Consultants) 2018 Joint Standards of Practice and Professional Code of Ethics. https://iaabc.org/code-of-ethics (Accessed 1 August 2024).Google Scholar
Jastrzębska, E, Wolska, A, Minero, M, Ogłuszka, M, Earley, B, Wejer, J and Górecka-Bruzda, A 2017 Conflict behavior in show jumping horses: A field study. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 57: 116121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2017.07.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ji, SN, Yang, LL, Ge, XF, Wang, BJ, Cao, J and Hu, DF 2013 Behavioural and physiological stress responses to transportation in a group of Przewalski’s horses (Equus ferus prezwalskii). Journal of Animal and Plant Sciences 23: 10771084.Google Scholar
Kaiser, L, Heleski, CR, Siegford, J and Smith, KA 2006 Stress-related behaviors among horses used in a therapeutic riding program. Journal of the American Veterinary Medicine Association 228: 3945. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.228.1.39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, R and Hall, C 2009 The use of a mirror reduces isolation stress in horses being transported by trailer. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 116: 237243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.08.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelemen, Z, Grimm, H, Long, M, Auer, U and Jenner, F 2021 Recumbency as an equine welfare indicator in geriatric horses and horses with chronic orthopaedic disease. Animals 11: 116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11113189Google ScholarPubMed
Knowles, TG, Brown, SN, Pope, SJ, Nicol, CJ, Warriss, PD and Weeks, CA 2010 The response of untamed (unbroken) ponies to conditions of road transport. Animal Welfare 19: 115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S096272860000110XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansade, L, Bonneau, C, Parias, C and Biau, S 2019 Horse’s emotional state and rider safety during grooming practices, a field study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 217: 4347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.04.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansade, L, Bouissou, MF and Erhard, HW 2008 Fearfulness in horses: A temperament trait stable across time and situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 115: 182200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.06.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansade, L, Neveux, C and Levy, F 2012 A few days of social separation affects yearling horses’ response to emotional reactivity tests and enhances learning performance. Behavioural Processes 91: 94102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2012.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansade, L, Nowak, R, Lainé, AL, Leterrier, C, Bonneau, C, Parias, C and Bertin, A 2018 Facial expression and oxytocin as possible markers of positive emotions in horses. Scientific Reports 8: 14680. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-32993-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larose, C, Richard-Yris, MA, Hausberger, M and Rogers, LJ 2006 Laterality of horses associated with emotionality in novel situations. Laterality 11: 355367. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500600624221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larssen, R and Roth, LSV 2022 Regular positive reinforcement training increases contact-seeking behaviour in horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 252: 105651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laukkanen, T, Karma, L, Virtala, A-M, Mykkänen, A, Pehkonen, J, Rossi, H, Tuomola, K and Raekallio, M 2023 Behavioral signs associated with equine cheek tooth findings. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 121: 104198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2022.104198CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leadon, DP 1994 Studies of the effects of transporting horses: Better to arrive than to travel. Equine Veterinary Journal 26: 346347. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2042-3306.1994.tb04400.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, KE, Kim, JG, Lee, H and Kim, BS 2021 Behavioral and cardiac responses in mature horses exposed to a novel object. Journal of Animal Science and Technology 63: 651661. https://doi.org/10.5187/jast.2021.e51CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lelláková, M, Pavľak, A, Lešková, L, Florián, M, Skurková, L, Mesarčová, L, Kottferová, L, Takáčová, D and Kottferová, J 2021 Monitoring blinks and eyelid twitches in horses to assess stress during the samples collection process. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 26: 530539. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2021.2008249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerch, N, Cirulli, F, Rochais, C, Lesimple, C, Guilbaud, E, Contalbrigo, L, Borgi, M, Grandgeorge, M and Hausberger, M 2021 Interest in humans: Comparisons between riding school lesson equids and assisted-intervention equids. Animals 11: 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11092533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesimple, C, Fureix, C, de Margerie, E, Sénèque, E, Menguy, H and Hausberger, M 2012 Towards a postural indicator of back pain in horses (Equus caballus). PLoS ONE 7: 9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044604CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liehrmann, O, Viitanen, A, Riihonen, V, Alander, E, Koski, SE, Lummaa, V and Lansade, L 2022 Multiple handlers, several owner changes and short relationship lengths affect horses’ responses to novel object tests. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 254: 105709. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malmkvist, J, Poulsen, JM, Luthersson, N, Palme, R, Christensen, JW and Søndergaard, E 2012 Behaviour and stress responses in horses with gastric ulceration. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 142: 160167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manrique, LP, Bánszegi, O, Hudson, R and Szenczi, P 2021 Repeatable individual differences in behaviour and physiology in juvenile horses from an early age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 235: 105227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marliani, G, Sprocatti, I, Schiavoni, G, Bellodi, A and Accorsi, PA 2021 Evaluation of horses’ daytime activity budget in a model of ethological stable: A case study in Italy. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 24: 200213. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2020.1857252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marliani, G, Vannucchi, I, Kiumurgis, I and Accorsi, PA 2022 Limitations of spatial judgment bias test application in horses (Equus ferus caballus). Animals 12: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12213014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, I, Farmer, K and Krüger, K 2018 Evidence for right-sided horses being more optimistic than left-sided horses. Animals 8: 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8120219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsbøll, AF and Christensen, JW 2015 Effects of handling on fear reactions in young Icelandic horses. Equine Veterinary Journal 47: 615619. https://doi.org/10.1111/evj.12338CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride, SD, Hemmings, A and Robinson, K 2004 A Preliminary study on the effect of massage to reduce stress in the horse. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 24: 7681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2004.01.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonnell, S 2003 The equid ethogram: A practical field guide to horse behaviorLexington, US: Eclipse Press.Google Scholar
McDuffee, L, Carr, L and Montelpare, W 2022 An observational evaluation of stress in horses during therapeutic riding sessions. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 49: 5364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.11.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGreevy, PD, McLean, A, Warren-Smith, AK, Waran, N and Goodwin, D 2005 Defining the terms and processes associated with equitation. Proceedings of the First International Equitation Science Symposium pp. 110143. Melbourne, VIC, Australia.Google Scholar
Mellor, DJ, Beausoleil, NJ, Littlewood, KE, McLean, AN, McGreevy, PD, Jones, B and Wilkins, C 2020 The 2020 Five Domains model: Including human–animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals 10: 1870. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101870CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melvin, MV, Costello, E and Colpoys, JD 2020 Enclosed versus ring feeders: Effects of round-bale feeder type on horse behavior and welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 39: 4146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.07.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP and Paul, ES 2010 An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277: 28952904. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendonça, T, Bienboire-Frosini, C, Kowalczyk, I, Leclercq, J, Arroub, S and Pageat, P 2019 Equine activities influence horses’ responses to different stimuli: Could this have an impact on equine welfare? Animals 9: 114. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendonça, T, Bienboire-Frosini, C, Sanchez, N, Kowalczyk, I, Teruel, E, Descout, E and Pageat, P 2020 de la Guérinière was right: Shoulder-in is beneficial for the physical and mental states of horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 38: 1420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.05.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merkies, K, Sudarenko, Y and Hodder, AJ 2022 Can ponies (Equus caballus) distinguish human facial expressions? Animals 12: 2331. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12182331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minero, M, Dalla Costa, E, Dai, F, Canali, E, Barbieri, S, Zanella, A, Pascuzzo, R and Wemelsfelder, F 2018 Using qualitative behaviour assessment (QBA) to explore the emotional state of horses and its association with human-animal relationship. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 204: 5359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.04.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munsters, CCBM, De Gooijer, JW, Van Den Broek, J and Van Oldruitenborgh-Oosterbaan, MMS 2013 Heart rate, heart rate variability and behaviour of horses during air transport. Veterinary Record 172: 1515. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.100952CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakamura, K, Takimoto-Inose, A and Hasegawa, T 2018 Cross-modal perception of human emotion in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Scientific Reports 8: 19. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26892-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittynen, T, Riihonen, V, Moscovice, LR and Koski, SE 2022 Acute changes in oxytocin predict behavioral responses to foundation training in horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 254: 105707. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105707CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olczak, K, Klocek, C and Christensen, JW 2021 Hucul horses’ learning abilities in different learning tests and the association with behaviour, food motivation and fearfulness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 245: 105498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliveira, T, Santos, A, Silva, J, Trindade, P, Yamada, A, Jaramillo, F, Silva, L and Baccarin, R 2022 Hospitalisation and Disease Severity Alter the Resting Pattern of Horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 110: 103832. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2021.103832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortolani, F, Scilimati, N, Gialletti, R, Menchetti, L and Nannarone, S 2021 Development and preliminary validation of a pain scale for ophthalmic pain in horses: The Equine Ophthalmic Pain Scale (EOPS). Veterinary Journal 278: 105774. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2021.105774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padalino, B, Loy, J, Hawson, L and Randle, H 2019 Effects of a light-colored cotton rug use on horse thermoregulation and behavior indicators of stress. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 29: 134139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2019.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padalino, B, Maggiolino, A, Boccaccio, M and Tateo, A 2012 Effects of different positions during transport on physiological and behavioral changes of horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 7: 135141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2011.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padalino, B and Raidal, SL 2020 Effects of transport conditions on behavioural and physiological responses of horses. Animals 10: 118. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padalino, B, Raidal, SL, Knight, P, Celi, P, Jeffcott, L and Muscatello, G 2018 Behaviour during transportation predicts stress response and lower airway contamination in horses. PLoS ONE 13: 120. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194272CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez Manrique, L, Hudson, R, Bánszegi, O and Szenczi, P 2019 Individual differences in behavior and heart rate variability across the preweaning period in the domestic horse in response to an ecologically relevant stressor. Physiology and Behavior 210: 112652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112652CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perron, B, Ali, ABA, Svagerko, P and Vernon, K 2023 The influence of severity of gastric ulceration on horse behavior and heart rate variability. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 59: 2529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pongrácz, P 2023 What is a too low sample size? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 259: 105852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2023.105852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pritchett, LC, Ulibarri, C, Roberts, MC, Schneider, RK and Sellon, DC 2003 Identification of potential physiological and behavioral indicators of postoperative pain in horses after exploratory celiotomy for colic. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 80: 3143. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00205-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quick, JS and Warren-Smith, AK 2009 Preliminary investigations of horses’ (Equus caballus) responses to different bridles during foundation training. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 4: 169176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2008.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reid, K, Rogers, CW, Gronqvist, G, Gee, EK and Bolwell, CF 2017 Anxiety and pain in horses measured by heart rate variability and behavior. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 22: 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricci-Bonot, C, Romero, T, Nicol, C and Mills, D 2021 Social buffering in horses is influenced by context but not by the familiarity and habituation of a companion. Scientific Reports 11: 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-88319-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rietmann, TR, Stauffacher, M, Bernasconi, P, Auer, JA and Weishaupt, MA 2004a The association between heart rate, heart rate variability, endocrine and behavioural pain measures in horses suffering from laminitis. Journal of Veterinary Medicine Series A: Physiology Pathology Clinical Medicine 51: 218225. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0442.2004.00627.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietmann, TR, Stuart, AEA, Bernasconi, P, Stauffacher, M, Auer, JA and Weishaupt, MA 2004b. Assessment of mental stress in warmblood horses: Heart rate variability in comparison to heart rate and selected behavioural parameters. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 88: 121136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.02.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, N and Bye, TL 2021 Noseband and poll pressures underneath bitted and bitless bridles and the effects on equine locomotion. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 44: 1824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochais, C, Fureix, C, Lesimple, C and Hausberger, M 2016a Lower attention to daily environment: A novel cue for detecting chronic horses’ back pain? Scientific Reports 6: 17. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochais, C, Henry, S, Fureix, C and Hausberger, M 2016b Investigating attentional processes in depressive-like domestic horses (Equus caballus). Behavioural Processes 124: 9396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.12.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, CW, Walsh, V, Gee, EK and Firth, EC 2012 A preliminary investigation of the use of a foal image to reduce mare stress during mare-foal separation. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 7: 4954. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2011.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, LJ 2002 Lateralisation in vertebrates: Its early evolution, general pattern, and development. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 31: 108161. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454%2802%2980007-9Google Scholar
Rørvang, MV and Christensen, JW 2018 Attenuation of fear through social transmission in groups of same and differently aged horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 209: 4146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rørvang, MV, Ničová, K, Sassner, H and Nawroth, C 2021 Horses’ (Equus caballus) ability to solve visible but not invisible displacement tasks is associated with frustration behavior and heart rate. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 15: 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.792035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruet, A, Biau, S, Arnould, C, Galloux, P, Destrez, A, Pycik, E, Boichot, L and Lansade, L 2020 Horses could perceive riding differently depending on the way they express poor welfare in the stable. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 94: 103206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2020.103206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, M, Waters, R and Wolfensohn, S 2021 Assessment of the welfare of experimental cattle and pigs using the animal welfare assessment grid. Animals 11: 999. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11040999CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabiniewicz, A, Tarnowska, K, Świątek, R, Sorokowski, P and Laska, M 2020 Olfactory-based interspecific recognition of human emotions: Horses (Equus ferus caballus) can recognize fear and happiness body odour from humans (Homo sapiens). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 230: 105072. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Safryghin, A, Hebesberger, DV and Wascher, CAF 2019 Testing for behavioral and physiological responses of domestic horses (Equus caballus) across different contexts - consistency over time and effects of context. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sankey, C, Henry, S, Clouard, C, Richard-Yris, MA and Hausberger, M 2011 Asymmetry of behavioral responses to a human approach in young naive vs. trained horses. Physiology and Behavior 104: 464468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sankey, C, Richard-Yris, MA, Henry, S, Fureix, C, Nassur, F and Hausberger, M 2010 Reinforcement as a mediator of the perception of humans by horses (Equus caballus). Animal Cognition 13: 753764. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0326-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schanz, L, Krueger, K and Hintze, S 2019 Sex and age don’t matter, but breed type does-factors influencing eye wrinkle expression in horses. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 6: 154. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2019.00154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siniscalchi, M, Padalino, B, Lusito, R and Quaranta, A 2014 Is the left forelimb preference indicative of a stressful situation in horses? Behavioural Processes 107: 6167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.07.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smiet, E, Van Dierendonck, MC, Sleutjens, J, Menheere, PPCA, van Breda, E, de Boer, D, Back, W, Wijnberg, ID and Van Der Kolk, JH 2014 Effect of different head and neck positions on behaviour, heart rate variability and cortisol levels in lunged Royal Dutch Sport horses. The Veterinary Journal 202: 2632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2014.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, AV, Proops, L, Grounds, K, Wathan, J and McComb, K 2016 Horses give functionally relevant responses to human facial expressions of emotion (Equus caballus). Biology Letters 12(9): 20150907. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0907CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, AV, Proops, L, Grounds, K, Wathan, J, Scott, SK and McComb, K 2018a Domestic horses (Equus caballus) discriminate between negative and positive human nonverbal vocalisations. Scientific Reports 8(1): 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-30777-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, AV, Wilson, C, McComb, K and Proops, L 2018b Domestic horses (Equus caballus) prefer to approach humans displaying a submissive body posture rather than a dominant body posture. Animal Cognition 21(2): 307312. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1140-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squibb, K, Griffin, K, Favier, R and Ijichi, C 2018 Poker Face: Discrepancies in behaviour and affective states in horses during stressful handling procedures. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 202: 3438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, M, Foster, TM and Waas, JR 2003 The effects of air transport on the behaviour and heart rate of horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 80: 143160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00212-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stomp, M, Masson, A, Henry, S, Hausberger, M and Lesimple, C 2020 Could snorts inform us on how horses perceive riding? Behavioural Processes 172: 104041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tateo, A, Padalino, B, Boccaccio, M, Maggiolino, A and Centoducati, P 2012 Transport stress in horses: Effects of two different distances. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 7(1): 3342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2011.04.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorbergson, ZW, Nielsen, SG, Beaulieu, RJ and Doyle, RE 2016 Physiological and behavioral responses of horses to wither scratching and patting the neck when under saddle. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 19(3): 245259. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1130630CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trindade, PHE, Taffarel, MO and Luna, SPL 2021 Spontaneous behaviors of post-orchiectomy pain in horses regardless of the effects of time of day, anesthesia, and analgesia. Animals 11(6): 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trösch, M, Cuzol, F, Parias, C, Calandreau, L, Nowak, R and Lansade, L 2019 Horses categorize human emotions cross-modally based on facial expression and non-verbal vocalizations. Animals 9: 862. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110862CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trösch, M, Pellon, S, Cuzol, F, Parias, C, Nowak, R, Calandreau, L and Lansade, L 2020 Horses feel emotions when they watch positive and negative horse–human interactions in a video and transpose what they saw to real life. Animal Cognition 23: 643653. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-020-01369-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, EK, Van Dierendonck, M, Ellis, AD, Rijksen, C and Van Reenen, CG 2009 A comparison of sympathetic and conventional training methods on responses to initial horse training. The Veterinary Journal 181(1): 4852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2009.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Borstel, UU, Duncan, IJH, Shoveller, AK, Merkies, K, Keeling, LJ and Millman, ST 2009 Impact of riding in a coercively obtained Rollkur posture on welfare and fear of performance horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 116(2–4): 228236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waran, NK and Cuddeford, D 1995 Effects of loading and transport on the heart rate and behaviour of horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 43(2): 7181. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(95)00555-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waring, GH 2003 Horse Behavior, Second Edition. Noyes Publications/William Andrew Publishing: Norwich, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Watson, JC and McDonnell, SM 2018 Effects of three non-confrontational handling techniques on the behavior of horses during a simulated mildly aversive veterinary procedure. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 203: 1923. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werhahn, H, Hessel, EF and Van den Weghe, HFA 2012 Competition horses housed in single stalls (II): Effects of free exercise on the behavior in the stable, the behavior during training, and the degree of stress. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 32(1): 2231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2011.06.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitaker, TC, Goupil, X, Roy, O, Marciat, D and McGahie, D 2011 Evaluation and comparison under field conditions of the stress response induced in horses when administered endoparasiticides in tablet or paste formulations. International Journal of Applied Research in Veterinary Medicine 9: 614.Google Scholar
Yarnell, K, Hall, C and Billet, E 2013 An assessment of the aversive nature of an animal management procedure (clipping) using behavioral and physiological measures. Physiology and Behavior 118: 3239. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarnell, K, Hall, C, Royle, C and Walker, SL 2015 Domesticated horses differ in their behavioural and physiological responses to isolated and group housing. Physiology and Behavior 143: 5157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, J 2016 Quality of life and animal behaviour. Applied Animal Welfare Science 181: 1926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.04.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, T, Creighton, E, Smith, T and Hosie, C 2012 A novel scale of behavioural indicators of stress for use with domestic horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 140(1): 3343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.05.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Study scenarios relating to horse behaviour during horse-human interactions identified in the literature search, the related table in the Supplementary material and the number of articles retained per scenario. The scenarios are listed in descending order according to the number of articles retained

Figure 1

Table 2. Behavioural signs of pain in the horse (in relation to specific health issues and in general)

Figure 2

Table 3. (a) Behaviour during handling and management procedures indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 3

Table 4. (a) Behaviour during ridden activities indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 4

Table 5. (a) (i, ii, iii) Behaviour of the horse during non-procedural horse-human interactions indicative of affective state (positive or negative), (i) during behavioural tests, (ii) in response to human expressions of emotion, and (iii) to watching third party horse-human interactions, supporting evidence* (see footnote below table for an explanation of the abbreviations used), and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 5

Table 6. (a) Behaviour of the horse during and after transportation indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence* (see footnote below table for an explanation of the abbreviations used), and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 6

Table 7. (a) Behaviour of the horse during training other than when ridden indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence* (see footnote below Table for an explanation of the abbreviations used), and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 7

Figure 1. Flow chart demonstrating how human behaviour can facilitate the progression from satisfying the behavioural needs of the horse to providing a good life for the domestic horse.

Supplementary material: File

Hall and Kay supplementary material

Hall and Kay supplementary material
Download Hall and Kay supplementary material(File)
File 497.5 KB