Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T18:27:40.012Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Toward a multi-level approach to the study of the intergenerational transmission of trauma: Current findings and future directions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2024

Marjorie Beeghly*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A central goal in the field of developmental psychopathology is to evaluate the complex, dynamic transactions occurring among biological, psychological, and broader social-cultural contexts that predict adaptive and maladaptive outcomes across ontogeny. Here, I briefly review research on the effects of a history of childhood maltreatment on parental, child, and dyadic functioning, along with more recent studies on the intergenerational transmission of trauma. Because the experience and sequelae of child maltreatment and the intergenerational transmission of trauma are embedded in complex biopsychosocial contexts, this research is best conceptualized in a developmental psychopathology framework. Moreover, there is a pressing need for investigators in this area of study to adopt dynamic, multi-level perspectives as well as using developmentally guided, sophisticated research methods. Other directions for research in this field are suggested, including the implementation of collaborative interdisciplinary team science approaches, as well as community-based participatory research, to increase representation, inclusion, and equity of community stakeholders. A greater focus on cultural and global perspectives is also recommended.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Adverse childhood experiences, including child abuse and neglect and other traumatic experiences, affect the lives of millions of children globally each year (Stoltenborgh et al., Reference Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Alink and van IJzendoorn2015; World Health Organization, 2021). Early adversity is a long-standing public health concern because it has well-documented negative biopsychosocial consequences for individuals across the life span (Gonzalez et al., Reference Gonzalez, Bethencourt Mirabal and McCall2023; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Bellis, Hardcastle, Sethi, Autchart, Mikton, Jones and Dunne2017). Here, I briefly review research on the effects of childhood maltreatment on parental, child, and dyadic functioning, along with more recent work on the intergenerational transmission of trauma. Because the experience and sequelae of child maltreatment and the intergenerational transmission of trauma are embedded in complex biopsychosocial contexts, I argue that this research is best conceptualized in a developmental psychopathology framework. Such an approach calls for investigators to adopt dynamic, multi-level perspectives as well as to utilize developmentally guided, sophisticated research methods. Following this discussion, I suggest other directions for research in this field, including the implementation of collaborative interdisciplinary team science approaches, as well as community-based participatory research, to increase representation, inclusion, and equity of community stakeholders. A greater focus on cultural and global perspectives is also recommended.

The study of childhood maltreatment and its sequelae

A burgeoning literature indicates that a history of childhood maltreatment and other traumatic experiences are robustly linked to heightened risk for a variety of physical, behavioral, and developmental maladaptive outcomes. These diverse outcomes include (but are not limited to) poor physical and mental health (Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998; Humphries et al., Reference Humphries, Guyon-Harris, Tibu, Wade, Nelson, Fox and Zeanah2020; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alhamzawi, Alonso, Angermeyer, Benjet, Bromet, Chatterji, de Girolamo, Demyttenaere, Fayyad, Florescu, Gal, Gureje, Haro and Williams2010; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Byambaa, De, Butchart, Scott and Vos2012), insecure and disorganized attachment (Cyr et al., Reference Cyr, Euser, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2010), and lower educational attainment, employment, and life success (Shonkoff et al., Reference Shonkoff, Garner, Dobbins, Earls, Garner, McGuinn, Pascoe and Wood2012).

With the advent of increasingly sophisticated developmental models and methods, however, new findings in this field indicate that the developmental pathways from early adversity to later outcomes are not always direct. Rather, they are often indirect, complex, and nuanced, altered by the dynamic interplay of risk and protective factors from multiple levels of influence across ontogeny (“developmental cascades.” Masten and Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). As a consequence, individuals exposed to early adversity are not uniformly “doomed” but instead exhibit a range of possible outcomes cascading across development periods, including resilient outcomes (vanBronkhorst et al., Reference vanBronkhorst, McCormack, Scorza, Lee, Feng, Hane, Duarte and Monk2023; Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Howell, Dillard, Shockley McCarthy, RaeNapier and Pei2021). These findings are consistent with the concept of “multifinality” in the field of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2010, Reference Hall, Vogel, Huang, Serrano, Rice, Tsakraklides and Fiore2012; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021).

Moreover, concomitant with significant advances in assessment methods in the biological sciences, a growing literature shows that early childhood maltreatment and associated toxic stress can get “embedded in the body”; that is, early adversity is linked to alterations in biological systems, such as brain structure and function (McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, DeBrito and Viding2011; Shonkoff et al., Reference Shonkoff, Garner, Dobbins, Earls, Garner, McGuinn, Pascoe and Wood2012). For example, Broeks et al. (Reference Broeks, Molenaar, Brouwer, van den Akker, van Rossum, Van, van den Berg, Hillegers, Hoogendijk, Burger, Bockting, Kamperman and Lambregtse-van den Berg2023) found links between a history of childhood maltreatment and maternal hair cortisol concentration, a stress biomarker. In a study using functional magnetic resonance imaging, Olsavsky et al. (Reference Olsavsky, Stoddard, Erhart, Tribble and Kim2021) showed that a history of childhood maltreatment was linked to alterations in mothers’ neural responses when exposed to their infant’s distress, compared to other stimuli (white noise). Specifically, mothers with a higher level of childhood maltreatment exhibited higher amygdalar activation when exposed to their own baby’s cries. The amygdala is a key brain structure associated with emotional processing and psychiatric health. Mothers with early adversity also displayed higher differential functional connectivity between the amygdala and amygdala-supplementary motor area in response to infant distress. Other research, guided by ecological systems models, has demonstrated that alterations in brain exist (particularly those in corticolimbic regions linked to emotional and social processing) that are associated with exposure to risk and protective factors from broader ecological contexts, such as neighborhood quality and socioeconomic status (Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Gad, Tomlinson, Suarez and Westerman2022).

Given the multi-level, dynamic complexity of development, there is a pressing need for investigators who study early adversity and its sequelae to adopt multi-level, organizational theoretical models and research methods, if they are to assess these processes accurately and understand them clearly. This call for a multiple levels of analyses approach mirrors those made by researchers in the field of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009), including those focused on the study of resilience in maltreated children (e.g., Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2010; Reference Hall, Vogel, Huang, Serrano, Rice, Tsakraklides and Fiore2013; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010; Masten, Reference Masten2019; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021).

Notably, the idea that development is dynamic and shaped by transacting factors from multiple levels of analysis is not new; in fact, mid-century biologists such as Kuo (Reference Kuo1976) and developmental psychobiologists such as Gilbert Gottlieb (Reference Gottlieb2007) have long described this complexity and pleaded for the implementation of multi-level approaches to the study of development. However, until recently, most researchers studying human development have employed simpler conceptual models and methods.

A notable exception is the field of developmental psychopathology. From its inception, a key tenet has been the multimodal and polyphasic nature of typical and atypical development and the need for multi-level system approaches to study it adequately (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009). A central goal is to evaluate the complex, dynamic transactions occurring among biological, psychological, and broader social-cultural contexts that predict adaptive and maladaptive outcomes across ontogeny (Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti and Curtis2007; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2010). Dante Cicchetti was a founder of this field and the first (and until now, sole) editor of its flagship journal, Development and Psychopathology.

On a personal note, I was fortunate to be mentored as a postdoctoral fellow by Dante Cicchetti as part of the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation’s Research Network on the Transition from Infancy to Early Childhood. During my postdoctoral years, I had many opportunities to ascertain the tenets of the then-emerging developmental psychopathology perspective from Dante, and it is notable that one of our first mentor–mentee discussions focused on Zing-Yang Kuo’s work and his call for the establishment of multidisciplinary research institutes (Kuo, Reference Kuo1976). I also had the opportunity to flesh out and apply this knowledge in a practical way by collaborating with Dante and other members of his research team on two ongoing prospective longitudinal research studies: One evaluated the effects of childhood maltreatment on children’s behavioral and developmental outcomes, and another focused on the development of children with Down syndrome from an organization perspective. Dante’s thinking, guidance, support, and generosity have made a significant impact on me over the years, not only shaping my approach to developmental science but also the way I mentor the trainees under my supervision. I am eternally grateful.

Studies of the intergenerational transmission of trauma

Over the past 30+ years, the field of developmental psychopathology has continued to grow and evolve, shaped by ongoing concomitant advances in multiple other areas of scientific inquiry. One specific example of such an advance in this field is research on the intergenerational transmission of trauma. Although this area of inquiry is particularly complex and challenging to conduct, research findings to date confirm that early adversity can negatively affect not only the individuals who experienced it, but also their offspring. Illustrations from several recent longitudinal studies on the intergenerational transmission of trauma are provided below.

In a large covariate-controlled study of 4447 mother–child dyads from 21 longitudinal cohorts evaluated in the NIH’s Environmental Influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) program, Moog et al. (Reference Moog, Cummings, Jackson, Aschner, Barrett, Bastain, Blackwell, Bosquet-Enlow, Breton, Bush, Deoni, Duarte, Ferrara, Grant, Hipwell, Jones, Leve, Lovinsky-Desir, Miller and Buss2023) identified significant associations between maternal childhood maltreatment history and a variety of problems in offspring. Specifically, the authors found that children of mothers with childhood maltreatment were more likely to have clinically significant internalizing problems, autism spectrum disorder, ADHD, and asthma and were also at higher risk for developing multiple problems (multi-morbidity). Moreover, children whose mothers were exposed to multiple forms of childhood maltreatment had the highest risk for negative health outcomes, suggesting a dose–response relationship.

Although these research findings are compelling, the specific processes underlying these associations remain unclear. A large group of studies in this area have focused on parenting quality as a potential explanatory mechanism. Results from scoping and meta-analytic reviews confirm that a history of childhood maltreatment is indeed linked to later parenting quality (Chamberlain et al., Reference Chamberlain, Gee, Harfield, Campbell, Brennan, Clark, Mensah, Arabena, Herrman, Brown and Federici2019; Greene et al., Reference Greene, Haisley, Wallace and Ford2020; Khoury et al., Reference Khoury, Dimitrov, Bosquet-Enlow, Haltigan, Bronfman and Lyons-Ruth2022; Madigan et al., Reference Madigan, Cyr, Eirich, Fearon, By, Rash, Poole and Alink2019; Savage et al., Reference Savage, Tarabulsy, Pearson, Collin-Vezina and Gagne2019). However, consistent with a developmental psychopathology perspective, specific findings in this literature are often complex and nuanced. For example, in one study, the association between maternal childhood maltreatment and disorganized attachment in offspring was mediated by sensitive parenting and maternal depression (Alto et al., Reference Alto, Warmingham, Handley, Rogosch, Cicchetti and Toth2021). In another longitudinal study (Martinez-Torteya et al., Reference Martinez-Torteya, Dayton, Beeghly, Seng, McGinnis, Broderick, Rosenblum and Muzik2014), a history of childhood maltreatment was not directly associated with either parenting quality or infants’ biobehavioral regulatory capacity during a social stressor. Rather, maternal childhood maltreatment was indirectly related to parenting quality, via its links with maternal depression. In turn, parenting quality predicted infants’ biobehavioral regulation (Martinez-Torteya et al., Reference Martinez-Torteya, Dayton, Beeghly, Seng, McGinnis, Broderick, Rosenblum and Muzik2014). In contrast, other studies describe resilient parenting outcomes in the context of early adversity (Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Howell, Dillard, Shockley McCarthy, RaeNapier and Pei2021).

Another possible (but understudied) mechanism that could help explain the intergenerational transmission of childhood maltreatment is the biological embedding of maternal adversity in offspring. In a recent study using magnetic resonance imaging, Moog et al. (Reference Moog, Entringer, Rasmussen, Styner, Gilmore, Kathmann, Heim, Wadhwa and Buss2018) showed that newborns of mothers with a childhood maltreatment history had smaller brains and less gray matter than infants of mothers without early adversity. In a study of infants in the first two years of life, Khoury et al. (Reference Khoury, Ahtam, Sisitsky, Ou, Gagoski, Bosquet Enlow, Teicher, Grant and Lyons-Ruth2021) reported that infants of mothers with a childhood history of maltreatment had lower gray matter volume and lower amygdala volume. Similar (but not identical) findings were reported in a Finnish cohort by Tuulari et al. (Reference Tuulari, Pulli, Kataja, Perasto, Lewis, Karlssson and Karlsson2023) using similar methods. Specifically, maternal childhood maltreatment was associated with infants’ greater left (but not right) amygdala volume. In another study, Hendrix et al. (Reference Hendrix, Dilks, McKenna, Dunlop, Corwin and Brennan2021) evaluated infant functional brain connectivity using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging during natural sleep. Their findings indicated that one-month-old infants of mothers who had experienced a higher level of emotional neglect in childhood exhibited stronger positive amygdala-ventromedial prefrontal cortex connectivity and stronger amygdala-dorsal anterior cingulate cortex connectivity. These associations remained significant after controlling for a composite measure of maternal self-reported prenatal stress. Notably, other forms of maternal childhood maltreatment (e.g., physical or sexual abuse) did not emerge as significant correlates.

The biological embedding of maternal adversity in offspring may also occur prior to birth. According to the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease model, stress-related alterations in parental biological systems during pregnancy may alter (“program”) fetal brain development and other gestational processes (Buss et al., Reference Buss, Entringer, Moog, Toepfer, Fair, Simhan, Heim and Wadhwa2017; Yehuda & Meaney, Reference Yehuda and Meaney2018). Although human studies evaluating this novel possibility are still sparse, recent methodological advances have supported this line of inquiry by allowing investigators to measure functional connectivity in fetal brain using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (Turk et al., Reference Turk, van den Heuvel, Benders, de Heus, Ranz, Manning, Hect, Hernandez-Andrade, Hassan, Romero, Kahn, Thomason and van den Heuvel2019; van den Heuvel, Reference van den Heuvel2021; van den Heuvel & Thomason, Reference van den Heuvel and Thomason2016). For example, in a recent study by van den Heuvel et al. (Reference van den Heuvel, Monk, Hendrix, Hect, Lee, Feng and Thomason2023), fetuses of mothers with a higher level of childhood maltreatment exhibited greater connectivity of the amygdala network to the left (but not right) frontal areas (prefrontal and premotor), and relatively lower connectivity to right premotor regions and brainstem regions. Notably, these associations remained significant after controlling for a number of maternal and infant covariates, including level of maternal prenatal distress, socioeconomic status, fetal movement, and gestational age (both at the time of scan and at birth). These results mirror the postnatal brain findings reported by Hendrix et al. (Reference Hendrix, Dilks, McKenna, Dunlop, Corwin and Brennan2021) and others for infants of trauma-exposed mothers and are similar to findings regarding altered amygdala networks among trauma-exposed urban youth reported by Thomason et al. (Reference Thomason, Marusak, Tocco, Vila, McGarragle and Rosenberg2015).

The implications of these pre- and postnatal associations for offsprings’ later physical and mental health, behavioral functioning, and developmental trajectories remain unclear. For more than a decade, I have been collaborating on a prospective longitudinal study of urban low-income families, the Brain Development Study (M. Thomason, PI) which is attempting to shed further light on the association between measures of resting-state fetal brain connectivity and postpartum outcomes. This study provided the sample that was evaluated in van den Heuvel et al. (Reference van den Heuvel, Monk, Hendrix, Hect, Lee, Feng and Thomason2023) study, described above. Next steps in this project are to evaluate whether measures of fetal brain functional connectivity (particularly between amygdalar systems and prefrontal cortical circuits) alter the association between maternal childhood maltreatment history and children’s postpartum behavioral and social-emotional outcomes or the quality of the parent–child relationship.

Future directions

The illustrative studies reviewed here highlight that the processes underlying the intergenerational transmission of trauma are dynamic, complex, and biopsychosocial in nature. As such, they require multi-level perspectives, modern developmental conceptual models, and sophisticated, developmentally guided methods utilizing measures from multiple levels of analysis. Additionally, such studies require ongoing collaboration among experts trained in diverse fields (e.g., neuroscience, medicine, psychology, and psychiatry, to name a few) for their successful accomplishment (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009).

For these reasons, I suggest that an important future direction for research in this area of developmental psychopathology is the implementation of collaborative, multi-university team science approaches. In fields outside of developmental psychopathology, there is growing recognition of increasing scientific and societal complexity and the need for collaborative multi-university/organization team-based approaches to address this complexity (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Vogel, Huang, Serrano, Rice, Tsakraklides and Fiore2018; Wuchty et al., Reference Wuchty, Jones and Uzzi2007).

Although team science approaches are promising, collaboration among investigators from multiple, diverse disciplines can be complex and challenging. For example, neuroscientists trained in sophisticated imaging techniques and psychologists or psychiatrists trained in assessing individuals’ trauma history and mental health may face communication barriers because they may not share the same level of understanding regarding the complex issues addressed in the field of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009).

In addition, multi-site team science approaches are expensive and require complex organization, planning, and communication across institutions. Many administrators question whether team science approaches “add value” over those of unidisciplinary teams, or whether team science is the best, or the most cost-effective way to support science (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Vogel, Huang, Serrano, Rice, Tsakraklides and Fiore2018). These concerns have been sufficiently serious to warrant the provision of additional federal research initiatives to study “the science of team science,” including the evaluation of specific moderators and mediators that contribute to its successful implementation (Stokols et al., Reference Stokols, Misra, Moser, Hall and Taylor2008), and the “best practices” to implement, in order to enhance its effectiveness (National Research Council, 2015). Despite these initiatives and the need for effective, evidence-based interdisciplinary team-based research approaches, the funding available to support team science approaches is still relatively limited (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Vogel, Huang, Serrano, Rice, Tsakraklides and Fiore2018).

Arguably, our knowledge about the complex developmental processes involved in the intergenerational transmission of trauma has grown substantially from collaborative interdisciplinary research approaches. However, the involvement of community stakeholders and participants engaged in this research is still limited, as is the case in studies in the larger field of psychology (Amauchi et al., Reference Amauchi, Gauthier, Ghezeljeh, Giatti, Sholanke, Zachari and Gutberlet2021; Espinosa & Verney, Reference Espinosa and Verney2021; Tebes & Thai, Reference Tebes and Thai2018; Williford et al., Reference Williford, McTate, Hood, Reader, Hildenbrand, Smith-Whitley, Creary, Thompson, Hackworth, Raphael and Crosby2021). Moreover, traditional research methodologies in the field of developmental psychopathology have not adequately represented or addressed the needs or goals of multicultural community members, which may contribute to, or even exacerbate, health disparities (Espinosa & Verney, Reference Espinosa and Verney2021). Thus, efforts to increase the active engagement and participation of community collaborators, research participants, and other stakeholders in all phases of research (“community-based participatory team science”) are needed (Tebes & Thai, Reference Tebes and Thai2018). Such an approach strives to enhance equity and inclusion by promoting social/environmental justice in science (Amauchi et al., Reference Amauchi, Gauthier, Ghezeljeh, Giatti, Sholanke, Zachari and Gutberlet2021; Williford et al., Reference Williford, McTate, Hood, Reader, Hildenbrand, Smith-Whitley, Creary, Thompson, Hackworth, Raphael and Crosby2021).

A potential barrier to implementing this approach is that building relationships with community stakeholders takes time. However, there is growing evidence that such initiatives are successful in enhancing the voice and involvement of underrepresented members of the community in research (Brush et al., Reference Brush, Mentz, Jensen, Jacobs, Saylor, Rowe, Israel and Lachance2020), thereby creating a more equitable and inclusive partnership between researchers and community members (McFarlane et al., Reference McFarlane, Occa, Peng, Awonuga and Morgan2022; Tebes & Thai, Reference Tebes and Thai2018). This approach also has the potential to increase the effectiveness of efforts to translate scientific findings into policy and practice (Amauchi et al., Reference Amauchi, Gauthier, Ghezeljeh, Giatti, Sholanke, Zachari and Gutberlet2021).

Finally, an increased focus on cultural and global perspectives is needed in the field of developmental psychopathology. It is estimated that over 95% of participants in developmental psychology research live in countries that account for only 12% of the global population, that is, Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) societies (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010). Similarly, Mistry et al. (Reference Mistry, Contreras, Dutta, Lerner and Easterbrooks2013) point out that over 90% of developmental research has been conducted in the United States, Canada, or Western Europe, and the remaining 10% comes from regions that account for 90% of the world’s population. These estimates are calculated for the broad field of developmental psychology but likely also apply to the field of developmental psychopathology. Thus, there is a critical need for researchers who study human development, including the intergenerational transmission of trauma, to globalize and expand their models and methods beyond Western borders to include diverse cultural and geopolitical contexts (Jensen, Reference Jensen2012; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Cristia, Karasik, Rajendra and Oakes2023). Such an approach would contribute to the creation of a more inclusive, integrated theory of development. A globalized approach could also help identify universal and context-specific patterns of development and their specific associations with the biopsychosocial risk and protective factors across the life span.

Funding statement

I am grateful to Dante Cicchetti for his valuable and supportive mentorship. The writing of this article was supported, in part, by NIH grants RO1ES032294 and RO1MH12247.

Competing interests

None.

References

Alto, M. E., Warmingham, J. M., Handley, E. D., Rogosch, F., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2021). Developmental pathways from maternal history of childhood maltreatment and maternal depression to toddler attachment and early childhood behavioral outcomes. Attachment and Human Development, 23(3), 328349. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amauchi, J. F. F., Gauthier, M., Ghezeljeh, A., Giatti, L. L., Sholanke, D., Zachari, D., & Gutberlet, J. (2021). The power of community-based participatory research: Ethical and effective ways of researching. Community Development, 53(1), 320. https://doi.org/10.1080/15575330.2021.1936102 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broeks, C. W., Molenaar, N., Brouwer, M., van den Akker, E. L. T., van Rossum, E. F. C., Van, R., van den Berg, S. A. A., Hillegers, M., Hoogendijk, W. J. G., Burger, H., Bockting, C., Kamperman, A. M., & Lambregtse-van den Berg, M. P. (2023). Intergenerational impact of childhood trauma on hair cortisol concentrations in mothers and their young infants. Comprehensive Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14, 100167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.epnec.2023.100167 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brush, B. L., Mentz, G., Jensen, M., Jacobs, B., Saylor, K. M., Rowe, Z., Israel, B. A., & Lachance, L. (2020). Success in long-standing community-based participatory research (CBPR) partnerships: A scoping literature review. Health, Education, and Behavior, 47(4), 556568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, C., Entringer, S., Moog, N. K., Toepfer, P., Fair, D. A., Simhan, H. N., Heim, C. M., & Wadhwa, P. D. (2017). Intergenerational transmission of maternal childhood maltreatment exposure: Implications for fetal brain development. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(5), 373382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, C., Gee, G., Harfield, S., Campbell, S., Brennan, S., Clark, Y., Mensah, F., Arabena, K., Herrman, H., Brown, S., Federici, S., & for the ‘Healing the Past by Nurturing the Future’ group (2019). For the healing the past by nurturing the future group, 2019, parenting after a history of childhood maltreatment: A scoping review and map of evidence in the perinatal period. PLoS One, 14(3), e0213460. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213460 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2010). Resilience under conditions of extreme stress: A multilevel perspective. World Psychiatry, 9(3), 145154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2013). Annual research review: Resilient functioning in maltreated children—past, present, and future perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(4), 402422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2007). Multilevel perspectives on pathways to resilient functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 19(3), 627629. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095459407000314 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of discipline. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, W. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: Theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributions to resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 773810. https://doi.org/doi:10.1017.S0954579403000373 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cyr, C., Euser, E. M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2010). Attachment security and disorganization in maltreating and high-risk families: A series of meta-analyses. Development and Psychopathology, 22(1), 87108. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409990289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espinosa, P. R., & Verney, S. P. (2021). The underutilization of community-based participatory research in psychology: A systematic review. American Journal of Community Psychology, 67(3-4), 312326. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajcp.12469 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez, D., Bethencourt Mirabal, A., & McCall, J. D. (2023). Child abuse and neglect. In StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, C. A., Haisley, L., Wallace, C., & Ford, J. D. (2020). Intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review of the parenting practices of adult survivors of childhood abuse, neglect, and violence. Clinical Psychology Review, 80, 101891. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101891 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, K. L., Vogel, A. L., Huang, G. C., Serrano, K. J., Rice, E. L., Tsakraklides, S. P., & Fiore, S. M. (2018). The science of team science: A review of the empirical evidence and research gaps on collaboration in science. American Psychologist, 73(4), 532548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendrix, C. L., Dilks, D. D., McKenna, B. G., Dunlop, A. L., Corwin, E. J., & Brennan, P. A. (2021). Maternal childhood adversity associates with frontoamygdala connectivity in neonates. Biological Psychiatry, 6(4), 470478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.11.003 Google ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 2929. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, K., Bellis, M. A., Hardcastle, K. A., Sethi, D., Autchart, A., Mikton, C., Jones, L., & Dunne, M. P. (2017). The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 2(8), e356e366. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphries, K. L., Guyon-Harris, K. L., Tibu, F., Wade, M., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2020). Psychiatric outcomes following severe deprivation in early childhood: Follow-up of a randomized controlled trial at age 16. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 88(12), 10791090. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000613 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, L. W., Gad, A. M., Tomlinson, R. C., Suarez, G. L., & Westerman, H. B. (2022). Parents, neighborhoods, and the developing brain. Child Development Perspectives, 16(3), 148156. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12453 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (2012). Bridging universal and cultural perspectives: A vision for developmental psychology in a global world. Child Development Perspectives, 6(1), 98104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00213.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alhamzawi, A. O., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M., Benjet, C., Bromet, E., Chatterji, S., de Girolamo, G., Demyttenaere, K., Fayyad, J., Florescu, S., Gal, G., Gureje, O., Haro, J. M., … Williams, D. R. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO world mental health surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(5), 378385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoury, J. E., Ahtam, B., Sisitsky, M., Ou, Y., Gagoski, B., Bosquet Enlow, M., Teicher, M. H., Grant, P. E., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2021). Maternal childhood maltreatment is associated with lower infant gray matter volume and amygdala volume during the first two years of life. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 2(4), 440449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.09.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoury, J. E., Dimitrov, L., Bosquet-Enlow, M., Haltigan, J. D., Bronfman, E., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2022). Patterns of maternal childhood maltreatment and disrupted interaction between mothers and their 4-month-old infants. Child Maltreatment, 27(3), 366377. https://doi.org/10.1177/10775595211007567 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, Z.-Y. (1976). The dynamics of behavior development: An epigenetic view. New York: Plenum Publishing.Google Scholar
Madigan, S., Cyr, C., Eirich, R., Fearon, R. P., By, A., Rash, C., Poole, J. C., & Alink, L. R. (2019). Testing the cycle of maltreatment hypothesis: Meta-analytic evidence of the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 2351. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001700 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez-Torteya, C., Dayton, C. J., Beeghly, M., Seng, J. S., McGinnis, E., Broderick, A., Rosenblum, K., & Muzik, M. (2014). Maternal parenting predicts infant biobehavioral regulation among women with a history of childhood maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 379392. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2019). Resilience from a developmental systems perspective. World Psychiatry, 18(1), 101102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Lucke, C. M., Nelson, K. M., & Stallworthy, I. C. (2021). Resilience in development and psychopathology: Multisystem perspectives. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17, 521549. https://doi.org/10.1146/annrev-clinpsy-081219-120307 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E., DeBrito, S., & Viding, E. (2011). The impact of childhood maltreatment: A review of neurobiological and genetic factors. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00048 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFarlane, S. J., Occa, A., Peng, W., Awonuga, O., & Morgan, S. E. (2022). Community-based participatory research (CBRR) to enhance participation of racial/ethnic minorities in clinical trials: A 10-year systematic review. Health Communication, 37(9), 10751092. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2021.1943978 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mistry, J., Contreras, M., & Dutta, R. (2013). Culture and child development. In Lerner, R. M., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (Ed.), Developmental psychology. (vol. 6, 2nd ed. pp. 265285). John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Moog, N. K., Cummings, P. D., Jackson, K. L., Aschner, J. L., Barrett, E. S., Bastain, T. M., Blackwell, C. K., Bosquet-Enlow, M., Breton, C. V., Bush, N. R., Deoni, S. C. I., Duarte, C. S., Ferrara, A., Grant, T. L., Hipwell, A. E., Jones, K., Leve, L. D., Lovinsky-Desir, S., Miller, R. K.Buss, C. (2023). Intergenerational transmission of the effects of maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment in the USA: A retrospective cohort study. The Lancet Public Health, 8(3), e226e237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moog, N. K., Entringer, S., Rasmussen, J. M., Styner, M., Gilmore, J. H., Kathmann, N., Heim, C. M., Wadhwa, P. D., & Buss, C. (2018). Intergenerational effect of maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment on newborn brain anatomy, Biological Psychiatry, 83(2), 120127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.07.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Research Council (2015). Enhancing the effectiveness of team science. The National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Norman, R. E., Byambaa, M., De, R., Butchart, A., Scott, J., & Vos, T. (2012). The long-term health consequences of child physical abuse, emotional abuse, and neglect: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Medicine, 9(11), e1001349. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001349 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsavsky, A. K., Stoddard, J., Erhart, A., Tribble, R., & Kim, P. (2021). Reported maternal childhood maltreatment experiences, amygdala activation and functional connectivity to infant cry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(4), 418427. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, L. E., Tarabulsy, G. M., Pearson, J., Collin-Vezina, D., & Gagne, L. M. (2019). Maternal history of childhood maltreatment and later parenting behavior: A meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 921. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001542 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., Dobbins, M. I., Earls, M. F., Garner, A. S., McGuinn, L., Pascoe, J., Wood, D. L., & Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care, and Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129(1), e232e246. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2663 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Cristia, A., Karasik, L., Rajendra, S. J., & Oakes, L. M. (2023). Diversity and representation in infant research: Barriers and bridges toward a globalized science of infant development. Infancy, 28(4), 130. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12545 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokols, D., Misra, S., Moser, R. P., Hall, K. L., & Taylor, B. K. (2008). The ecology of team science: Understanding contextual influences on transdisciplinary collaboration. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35(2S), S96S115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2007.05.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoltenborgh, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Alink, L. R. A., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: Review of a series of meta-analyses. Child Abuse Review, 24(1), 3750. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.2353 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tebes, J. K., & Thai, N. D. (2018). Interdisciplinary team science and the public: Steps toward a participatory team science. American Psychologist, 73(4), 549562. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000281 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomason, M. E., Marusak, H. A., Tocco, M. A., Vila, A. M., McGarragle, O., & Rosenberg, D. R. (2015). Altered amygdala connectivity in urban youth exposed to trauma. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(11), 14601468. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turk, E., van den Heuvel, M. I., Benders, M. J., de Heus, R., Ranz, A., Manning, J. H., Hect, J. L., Hernandez-Andrade, E., Hassan, S. S., Romero, R., Kahn, R. S., Thomason, M. E., & van den Heuvel, M. P. (2019). Functional connectome of the fetal brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 39(49), 97169724. https://doi.org/10.1521/JNEUROSCI.2891-18.2019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuulari, J. J., Pulli, E. P., Kataja, E.-L., Perasto, L., Lewis, J. D., Karlssson, L., & Karlsson, H. (2023). Parental childhood maltreatment associates with offspring left amygdala volume at early infancy. bioRxiv, 2023-02, preprint. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.02.23.529799 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. I. (2021). Intergenerational transmission of childhood adversity-related risk: Fetal brain programming as a potential mechanism. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(4), 385386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.01.008 Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. I., Monk, C., Hendrix, C. L., Hect, J., Lee, S., Feng, T., & Thomason, M. E. (2023). Intergenerational transmission of maternal childhood maltreatment prior to birth: Effects on human fetal amygdala functional connectivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 62(10), 11341146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2023.03.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, M. I., & Thomason, M. E. (2016). Functional connectivity of the human brain in utero. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(12), 931939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.001 Google ScholarPubMed
vanBronkhorst, S. B., McCormack, C. A., Scorza, P., Lee, S., Feng, T., Hane, A., Duarte, C. S., & Monk, C. (2023). Maternal childhood trauma and observed maternal care behaviors with 4-month-old infants. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, and Policy, Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0001554 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williford, D. N., McTate, E. A., Hood, A. M., Reader, S. K., Hildenbrand, A. K., Smith-Whitley, K., Creary, S. E., Thompson, A. A., Hackworth, R., Raphael, J. L., & Crosby, L. E. (2021). Psychologists as leaders in equitable science: Applications of antiracism and community participatory strategies in a pediatric behavioral medicine clinical trial. American Psychologist, 78(2), 107118. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001086 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (2021). Child maltreatment. Available at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/child-maltreatment. Accessed January 15, 2024.Google Scholar
Wuchty, S., Jones, B. F., & Uzzi, B. (2007). The increasing dominance of teams in the production of knowledge. Science, 316(5827), 10361038. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1136099 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., & Meaney, M. J. (2018). Relevance of psychological symptoms in pregnancy to intergenerational effects of preconception trauma. Biological Psychiatry, 83(2), 9496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoon, S., Howell, K., Dillard, R., Shockley McCarthy, K., RaeNapier, T., & Pei, F. (2021). Resilience following child maltreatment: Definitional considerations and developmental variations. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 22(3), 541559. https://doi.org/10.117/1524838019869094 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed