Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T06:16:11.180Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Living the good life? A systematic review of behavioural signs of affective state in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) and factors relating to quality of life. Part I: Fulfilment of species-specific needs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2024

Carol Hall*
Affiliation:
School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Southwell, Nottinghamshire NG25 0QF, UK National Equine Welfare Council, Slad Lane, Princes Risborough, Bucks HP27 0PP, UK
Rachel Kay
Affiliation:
School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Southwell, Nottinghamshire NG25 0QF, UK National Equine Welfare Council, Slad Lane, Princes Risborough, Bucks HP27 0PP, UK
*
Corresponding author: Carol Hall, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The welfare of horses involved in sport and leisure activities has come under increasing scrutiny, both from within the equine sector and from the public. A systematic review of scientific evidence was conducted to derive observable, evidence-based behavioural measures of positive or negative affective state and factors relating to quality of life in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Two separate searches (including the terms ‘emotion/affect’, or ‘stress’) were conducted, with 179 articles being retained. In Part I of this two-part review (companion paper published simultaneously), articles relating to the extent to which the species-specific needs of the horse are fulfilled in the home environment (n = 21), in relation to social grouping (n = 20), and during weaning (n = 14) were reviewed. Experimental tests of affective state in relation to housing and management (n = 8), and studies relating to stereotypical behaviour (n = 7) were also included. Opportunities for behavioural expression were dependent upon the provision of social and physical resources. Withdrawn or agitated behaviours in situ, avoidance behaviour during handling and agonistic intra-species interactions were indicative of negative affective state. Affiliative intra-specific social interactions, and forage ingestion were considered positive signs. For horses to live a good life, their need for space, companionship, and forage must be prioritised from birth and throughout their lives.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Ethical and welfare issues have been widely identified and reported on in relation to many aspects of equestrianism, including training and competition practices, general management, and horse breeding (McLean & McGreevy Reference McLean and McGreevy2010; Campbell Reference Campbell2017; Condon et al. Reference Condon, McGreevy, McLean, Williams and Randle2022; McManus Reference McManus2022). This has led to concerns regarding the inclusion of horses in both sporting (Campbell Reference Campbell2016) and leisure activities (Horseman et al. Reference Horseman, Buller, Mullan and Whay2016). Under increasing public scrutiny, the equestrian community urgently needs to demonstrate that it is accountable for the welfare of horses throughout all stages of their lives (McManus Reference McManus2022; Heleski Reference Heleski2023), and that it is actively working to improve their quality of life (Heleski Reference Heleski2023). Failure to do so may result in equestrian sports’ Social Licence to Operate (SLO), an unwritten social contract between society and participants in equestrian sports, being withdrawn (McManus Reference McManus2022; Waran Reference Waran2023). If this happens, equestrian activities may subsequently become subject to external regulation, or potentially be banned altogether (McManus Reference McManus2022; Waran Reference Waran2023).

Scientific thinking regarding what constitutes good animal welfare has advanced significantly over recent decades. There is now widespread acceptance that it is the animal’s perception of its physical and emotional state that matters (Webster Reference Webster2013), and that for an animal to live a good life it must be provided with opportunities for positive experiences, not just protected from negative ones. The term affective state is used to describe emotions and other feelings associated with brain states induced by sensory input and cognitive processes that are experienced as either pleasant or unpleasant (Kirkwood Reference Kirkwood2007; Fraser Reference Fraser2008). An animal’s quality of life (QOL) is reliant upon the balance between positive (pleasant) and negative (unpleasant) experiences over time, which largely determines how valuable each animal’s life is for that individual animal from the animal’s point of view (Yeates Reference Yeates2016). Throughout this review, welfare and QOL will be considered as synonymous, but to avoid the potential negative connotations commonly associated with the term welfare, and to facilitate a focus on providing positive experiences, QOL is considered the preferable term for future applications (Furtado et al. Reference Furtado, Preshaw, Hockenhull, Wathan, Douglas, Horseman, Smith, Pollard, Pinchbeck, Rogers and Hall2021).

To enable stakeholders and researchers to accurately assess the QOL of a horse throughout its lifetime, evidence-based measures that are relevant and feasible for use across the equine sector, are required. The first stage in developing a framework upon which to base such an assessment involves identifying behaviour indicative of affective state, with the proviso that individual differences in behavioural expression will occur (Manrique et al. Reference Manrique, Bánszegi, Hudson and Szenczi2021). Subsequently, factors found to influence behavioural expression and, by implication, affective state, can be adapted to facilitate improvements in horse QOL which, in turn, can be measured through behavioural change.

To enable the assessment of both physical and mental well-being, two main questions were posed by Dawkins (Reference Dawkins2004): is the animal healthy and does it have what it wants? Although physical health can to a certain extent be measured, assessing whether an animal has what it wants is more of a challenge, particularly in a ‘traditionally’ managed domestic species such as the horse. A comparison with the behaviour of feral horses highlights the extent to which this management generally restricts the natural behavioural repertoire of the horse (Waran Reference Waran1997) and suggests that most do not have what they would ideally want. A study by Schatzmann (Reference Schatzmann1998) demonstrated that sport horses at winter pasture in Switzerland, prioritised being in the company of, or within view of, other horses and that regardless of weather conditions they preferred eating forage outside rather than in individual indoor boxes. These findings support the conclusion that what horses want are friends, forage, and freedom (Fraser Reference Fraser2012). To derive an answer to this second question posed by Dawkins (Reference Dawkins2004), it is necessary to include information about what resources are available to the animal and how the animal behaves in response to this provision.

Multiple factors contribute to an animal’s affective state, and these are encapsulated in the Five Domains model developed by Mellor et al. (Reference Mellor, Beausoleil, Littlewood, McLean, McGreevy, Jones and Wilkins2020). The model was initially modified to facilitate the identification of experiences that an animal may have that relate to positive affect and the potential to enhance welfare (Mellor & Beausoleil Reference Mellor and Beausoleil2015), and subsequently to revise the domain of ‘behaviour’ to refer to behavioural interactions an animal has with other animals, humans, and the environment (Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Beausoleil, Littlewood, McLean, McGreevy, Jones and Wilkins2020). An important adaptation to this model, which aligns with the question of whether the animal has what it wants (Dawkins Reference Dawkins2004), was incorporated in the guiding principles for humane livestock farming in The Netherlands (Council on Animal Affairs 2021). As well as the need to provide good nutrition, housing, and health, these include the need to provide the animal with sufficient opportunities to fulfil its natural behavioural needs. Only if these requirements are met can the animal experience a positive mental state (Council on Animal Affairs 2021). This latter principle is particularly pertinent when considering the QOL of the domestic horse (Equus caballus).

The application of the Five Domains model to assess the quality of life of any animal requires species-specific knowledge of both the behaviour and the behavioural needs of that species. Deriving conclusions about the affective state of the animal based on the other four domains, is undoubtedly challenging, and the association between behaviour and subjective experience unclear. Characterisation of animal emotion in terms of the two components of arousal and valence (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010) provides a basis for assessing affective state. As noted by Hall et al. (Reference Hall, Randle, Pearson, Preshaw and Waran2018), both behavioural and physiological measures yield information relating to arousal, but often conflicting evidence in relation to valence. Comprehensive lists of horse behaviour with descriptors have been compiled by McDonnell (Reference McDonnell2003) and Waring (Reference Waring2003), as well as ethograms identifying specific behaviours associated with negative affective state, including signs of stress (for example, Young et al. Reference Young, Creighton, Smith and Hosie2012) and equine discomfort (Torcivia & McDonnell Reference Torcivia and McDonnell2021). Despite the move towards providing positive experiences and potentially positive affective state (Mellor & Beausoleil Reference Mellor and Beausoleil2015), conclusions regarding behavioural signs of positive affective state in the horse are generally based on the absence of negative signs. Such conclusions may not be warranted.

Identifying behavioural signs of affective state is key to promoting practices that provide the horse with positive experiences, and therefore a good QOL. A systematic review of scientific evidence was conducted to derive observable, evidence-based, behavioural measures of positive or negative affective state and factors that relate to QOL in the domestic horse. Features of management and training that may compromise or improve QOL will be identified. Those relating to the fulfilment of species-specific needs are presented in Part I of this review, those relating to the horse-human relationship in Part II (a companion paper published simultaneously; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024). The findings of Parts I and II will be combined in Part II to identify the changes needed in management and training to achieve a good life for all horses.

Materials and methods

Search strategy

The PRISMA 2020 statement was used as guidance in the collection and presentation of the empirical evidence referred to in this review (Page et al. Reference Page MJ, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald, McGuinness, Stewart, Thomas, Tricco, Welch, Whiting and Moher2021). During February 2023, a systematic literature search was performed to identify evidence-based behavioural indicators of equine affective (emotional) state and associated factors. The electronic databases searched were Science Direct, PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science and PubPsych. To ensure the potential inclusion of evidence of behaviour indicative of both negative and positive affect, two separate search queries were specified, one with the terms emotion/affect, and a second with the term stress:

  • Search 1: ‘(emotion* OR affect* OR cognit*) AND (behav* OR welfare) AND (horse OR pony OR equine)’

  • Search 2: ‘(stress AND behav*) AND (horse OR pony OR equine)’

Where possible, the searches were filtered for research articles only, and the inclusion of the search terms in the title, abstract and keywords of the article. Filters for individual databases varied slightly, with restriction to ‘Title, Abstract, and Keywords’ only available in Science Direct, PubMed and Scopus. In the Web of Science, the restriction to ‘Abstracts’ was used. In Science Direct the use of * (referred to as a wildcard) was not permitted and the search terms used were revised accordingly (in Search 1 to ‘[emotion OR affective OR cognition] AND [behaviour OR welfare] AND [horse OR pony OR equine]’ and in Search 2 to ‘[stress AND behaviour] AND [horse OR pony OR equine]’). In PubPsych, no filters were used, and the search terms were simplified to ‘horse AND emotion’ and ‘horse AND stress AND behaviour.’

Inclusion criteria

A preliminary assessment of the relevance of the articles was carried out for each search. All peer-reviewed experimental and observational studies (primary research papers only) referring to equine emotional responses, equine stress, and emotional state (horses and/or ponies only) were included in the initial pool. No criteria relating to the date of publication were used in the searches. Undergraduate dissertations, post-graduate theses, textbooks, conference proceedings, review papers and publications not in the English language were excluded. Studies published both in abstract and full paper form were only evaluated in their full paper format. For each database, the total number of articles initially identified by each search was recorded. Within search duplicates (from different search engines) were removed at this first screening stage. The number of articles identified per year was calculated for each search separately to inform any change in article focus (Search 1 focused on emotion and Search 2 on stress).

The remaining articles were downloaded to Mendeley desktop. The articles were then assessed and those fulfilling the following criteria were retained. All studies were required to include a subject number greater than four, in any environment, either be observational or include a clear description of methods used, and needed to provide evidence to support the attribution of emotional state. Studies based upon quantitative and qualitative data were included. Articles that did not provide details of the methods used and/or justification for associations between emotional state and behaviour, were excluded.

Data extracted

For each article that fulfilled the inclusion criteria the following information was extracted: the scenario involved (experimental and observational), the behaviour recorded (including the type of ethogram used and cited sources), the subjective experience attributed to the behaviour (positive/negative valence, emotional response, affective state, stress), the nature of the supporting evidence for this conclusion (reference to past studies, physiological measures, pain/pain-inducing procedures, circumstances typically associated with positive or negative experiences, affiliative/agonistic social interactions, approach/avoidance behaviour, evidence of choice/preference) and factors that affected the behaviour expressed (including sex, age, social and environmental features). Features of study design were recorded and reported in relation to each scenario. For each retained article the following details were recorded: the focus of the study, study design and duration, subject numbers, age and sex, the type of behaviour recorded, the type of ethogram used, and the type of supporting evidence presented. These details are available online in the Supplementary material for this study (Part I) and Part II (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024).

Results and Discussion

The total number of articles identified by each search database for Search 1 and Search 2, the number of articles retained following the initial screening of abstracts for relevance (species, behaviour recorded and related to affective state) and duplication (between databases), and the final number of retained articles eligible for inclusion in this review (Search 1 and 2 combined) are shown in Figure 1. A total of 179 articles were retained.

Figure 1. Systematic literature search procedure, including the number of articles identified at each stage of the review, and the total number of articles retained.

Publications identified per four-year period for each search are shown in Figure 2 (the most recent period consists of only three years and two months). No articles pre-1992 were identified. The number of publications per year has increased overall, with numbers identified in Search 1 overtaking those identified in Search 2 from 2016 onwards.

Figure 2. Number of publications identified under each search term (Search 1 and Search 2) for each four-year period (the period 2020–2023 included three years and two months only).

The change in emphasis on the promotion of positive states to enhance well-being rather than purely the avoidance of negative states (as was previously the focus of many animal welfare studies) was reflected in the comparative number of articles identified using the two search terms (emotion/affect or stress). The Five Domains model, as used to assess animal welfare, was extended to incorporate positive welfare states in the article by Mellor and Beausoleil (Reference Mellor and Beausoleil2015). This changing approach is reflected in the post-2015 relative increase in articles identified in Search 1 (emotion/affect), as illustrated in Figure 2.

The study scenarios relating to the fulfilment of species-specific needs included in the retained articles (n = 70), and the number of articles per scenario are shown in Table 1. The scenarios included in Part I of this review were selected for their association with the extent to which the species-specific needs of the horse were fulfilled by management in the home environment, in relation to social grouping, and during weaning. All the experimental tests of affective state involved an assessment of the effect of housing and management conditions on judgement bias and were consequently included in Part I, as were all studies relating to stereotypical behaviour, the development of which is generally associated with a failure to provide for species-specific needs (Sarrafchi & Blokhuis Reference Sarrafchi and Blokhuis2013). The behaviour recorded in these scenarios was not influenced by interactions with humans, except to the extent that the latter determined the features of management under review. The behaviour of horses in the many and varied situations where more direct interactions with humans occurs, including those where human interpretation of horse behaviour should guide subsequent interactions, is reviewed in Part II (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024).

Table 1. Study scenarios relating to the fulfilment of species-specific needs identified in the literature search, the related table in the Supplementary material and the number of articles retained per scenario. The scenarios are listed in descending order according to the number of articles retained

The scenarios are listed in descending order according to the number of articles retained. Study scenarios relating more specifically to interactions with humans are reported in Part II (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024) of this review. The behaviour attributed to affective state and the factors affecting this behaviour are subsequently reported for each separate scenario. In addition, generic factors found to relate to behavioural expressions of affective state, and behavioural characteristics associated with specific affective states, are evaluated below. Further details of the experimental design used in the articles retained under each scenario are available in the Supplementary material (Tables S1.1–S1.5).

Studies reporting behaviour associated with affective state and related factors for each scenario

The home environment

The greatest number of articles retained in Part I of this review (21) focused on the behaviour of the horse in its home environment. The behaviour recorded varied according to the specific focus of the study and the features of the home environment that were under investigation. Features included stable type/size/design (Burla et al. Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017; Lesimple et al. Reference Lesimple, Gautier, Benhajali, Rochais, Lunel, Bensaïd, Khalloufi, Henry and Hausberger2019; Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019; Marliani et al. Reference Marliani, Sprocatti, Schiavoni, Bellodi and Accorsi2021), paddock vs stabling/turn-out opportunities (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005; Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012; Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016; Suagee-Bedore et al. Reference Suagee-Bedore, Linden and Bennett-Wimbush2021), the social environment (individual, paired or group housing; Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005; Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008; Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012; Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015), bedding area/depth (Burla et al. Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017; Greening et al. Reference Greening, Downing, Amiouny, Lekang and McBride2021), mode of forage provision (Melvin et al. Reference Melvin, Costello and Colpoys2020; Sundman et al. Reference Sundman, Goodwin, Reisinger, Smith, Costello, Walter and Colpoys2022; Baumgartner et al. Reference Baumgartner, Erhard and Zeitler-Feicht2023) and environmental enrichment (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014, Reference Lansade, Lemarchand, Reigner, Arnould and Bertin2022).

The retained articles used one of two main approaches, either assessing the impact of current (long-term) housing or testing the effect of specific conditions set up experimentally for shorter periods. In five of the retained articles the effect of long-term housing type and the association between housing features and behavioural and physiological signs of welfare was assessed (Lesimple et al. Reference Lesimple, Gautier, Benhajali, Rochais, Lunel, Bensaïd, Khalloufi, Henry and Hausberger2019; Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Matzkeit, Nicolau JTde, Castilha, de Oliveira and Bankuti2019; Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019; Sénèque et al. Reference Sénèque, Lesimple, Morisset and Hausberger2019; Sauveroche et al. Reference Sauveroche, Henriksson, Theodorsson, Svensson Holm and Roth2020). Where a timescale for the current housing was provided, this ranged from > 30 days (Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Matzkeit, Nicolau JTde, Castilha, de Oliveira and Bankuti2019) to > 9 months (Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019). The impact of shorter-term exposure to differing housing situations was assessed either by comparing different horses kept under different conditions for a specific length of time (between subjects: 12 weeks; Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008, five weeks; Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014), or by repeated measures where all horses underwent all conditions each for a set time-period which varied from 1 h (Suagee-Bedore et al. Reference Suagee-Bedore, Linden and Bennett-Wimbush2021), 24 h (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005), five days (Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015), six days (Greening et al. Reference Greening, Downing, Amiouny, Lekang and McBride2021), eleven days (Burla et al. Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017), 14 days (Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012; Melvin et al. Reference Melvin, Costello and Colpoys2020), 16 days (Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016), to approximately 34 days non-consecutively in experiment 2 of the study by Lesimple et al. (2019). Periods of adaptation to each condition were generally included, with behavioural and physiological assessments taking place towards the end of the period in each condition (for example, Burla et al. Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017 recorded behaviour over the last three days of eleven-day treatment periods). Subject numbers included in the studies assessing long-term effects of housing ranged from 32 (experiment 1; Lesimple et al. Reference Lesimple, Gautier, Benhajali, Rochais, Lunel, Bensaïd, Khalloufi, Henry and Hausberger2019) to 187 where behaviour was recorded via scan sampling for 50 days over a nine-month period (Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019). Subject numbers included in the shorter-term studies ranged from six (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005) to 38 (Burla et al. Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017). See Table S1.1 in the Supplementary material for details relating to all studies retained in this scenario.

Behaviour recorded

Behaviour in the home environment indicative of affective state (positive or negative) and the type of supporting evidence reported is shown in Table 2(a). The impact of different types of home environment was also assessed by means of additional tests, including learning trials (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014; Greening et al. Reference Greening, Downing, Amiouny, Lekang and McBride2021), handling tests (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005; Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015), reactivity tests (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) and ridden tests (Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012; Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016). Where the home environment was found to affect behaviour in these scenarios this is identified as a factor in the relevant section in Part II (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024) of this review.

Table 2. (a) Behaviour in the home environment indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse / human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach / avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

a Postural attributes: Elevated neck with ears pricked forward, looking intensely at the environment; b Neck horizontal at same level as back, fixed stare, ears, and head static.

c Agonistic behaviours: (chasing, contact biting, and kicking) and low-level threats (pinned ears, tail swishing, bite, and kick threats).

d Positive response to allogrooming: neck moderately raised, eyes open or half-closed, upper lip extended, ears turned backwards almost in line with the nose.

Ethograms were used in the 21 retained articles. One study (Stomp et al. Reference Stomp, Leroux, Cellier, Henry, Lemasson and Hausberger2018a) focused on a very specific behaviour (snorts), and there were only two examples where lists of behaviour were used without further descriptors (Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012 for the tests of ridden behaviours; Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014: response to enrichment). Where the ethogram had been developed with reference to previous studies, a total of 30 different references were cited as sources, with only three being cited by more than one study (McDonnell & Haviland Reference McDonnell and Haviland1995; Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, McDonald and Mills2000; McDonnell Reference McDonnell2003 were each cited by two studies). The behaviours recorded varied according to the focus of the study. Activity patterns and behaviour in situ were used to assess the impact of housing type (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008; Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012; Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015; Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016; Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Matzkeit, Nicolau JTde, Castilha, de Oliveira and Bankuti2019; Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019; Marliani et al. Reference Marliani, Sprocatti, Schiavoni, Bellodi and Accorsi2021), with studies assessing sleep patterns including detail of types of recumbency (Burla et al. Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017; Greening et al. Reference Greening, Downing, Amiouny, Lekang and McBride2021). Only behaviours identified in previous studies as being indicative of stress were recorded by Harewood and McGowan (Reference Harewood and McGowan2005), but no specific references were cited to support the ethogram used. Stereotypical and other abnormal repetitive behaviours were recorded by Lesimple et al. (Reference Lesimple, Gautier, Benhajali, Rochais, Lunel, Bensaïd, Khalloufi, Henry and Hausberger2019), Ribeiro et al. (Reference Ribeiro, Matzkeit, Nicolau JTde, Castilha, de Oliveira and Bankuti2019), Ruet et al. (Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019), Sénèque et al. (Reference Sénèque, Lesimple, Morisset and Hausberger2019) and Sauveroche et al. (Reference Sauveroche, Henriksson, Theodorsson, Svensson Holm and Roth2020). Sénèque et al. (Reference Sénèque, Lesimple, Morisset and Hausberger2019) also recorded the posture of the horse in situ.

When behaviour in horses being kept in groups was recorded, the focus was on the type of social interactions observed and the extent to which individual behaviour was affected by other group members (feeding, drinking, and resting behaviours). Negative social interactions, primarily levels of aggression and avoidance behaviours were recorded in relation to different approaches to forage provision (Melvin et al. Reference Melvin, Costello and Colpoys2020; Sundmann et al. 2022; Baumgartner et al. Reference Baumgartner, Erhard and Zeitler-Feicht2023) and around water resources (Hildebrandt et al. Reference Hildebrandt, Büttner, Krieter and Czycholl2022). Social disturbance of lying bouts was recorded by Burla et al. (Reference Burla, Rufener, Bachmann, Gygax, Patt and Hillmann2017) and by Hildebrandt et al. (Reference Hildebrandt, Büttner, Krieter and Czycholl2022). Both altruistic and different levels of agonistic behaviour were recorded by Suagee-Bedore et al. (Reference Suagee-Bedore, Linden and Bennett-Wimbush2021) in relation to the size of the group turn-out area. The effect of one form of environmental enrichment (auto-brushes) on group-housed horses was assessed in terms of social interactions (both positive and negative) and facial expression when using the brushes by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Lemarchand, Reigner, Arnould and Bertin2022). See Table 2(b) for factors found to affect behaviour in the home environment.

Supporting evidence

The supporting evidence presented in the retained studies was variable. Reference to past studies and comparisons with physiological measures were most frequently used. Where horses were able to demonstrate choice or social interactions were recorded, approach behaviour and affiliative social interactions were deemed positive signs, while avoidance, and social disturbance/agonistic interactions were deemed negative signs (at least for the recipient). This attribution of affective state to different types of social interaction was supported by physiological measures in several studies. Suagee-Bedore et al. (Reference Suagee-Bedore, Linden and Bennett-Wimbush2021) found a reduction in both plasma cortisol levels and agonistic/mild threat behaviours in relation to increased enclosure size. Sauveroche et al. (Reference Sauveroche, Henriksson, Theodorsson, Svensson Holm and Roth2020) found that increased positive social behaviour and resting behaviour both correlated with decreased concentrations of hair cortisol. Although Baumgartner et al. (Reference Baumgartner, Erhard and Zeitler-Feicht2023) found both negative social interactions and salivary cortisol levels increased in the condition when there were fewer feeding stations, the authors noted that the level of physiological response was lower than in studies investigating the effect of other stressors (for example, transport). Similarly, Melvin et al. (Reference Melvin, Costello and Colpoys2020) found that although agonistic threats and avoidance behaviour varied in relation to how forage was presented, no difference in plasma cortisol levels was found.

The effect of individual compared to group housing on behaviour and physiology was assessed in both adult horses and in youngsters stabled for the first time. Visser et al. (Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) recorded the response of young horses to first-time stabling, either individually or in pairs. Individually housed horses displayed more behaviours indicative of stress (neighing, pawing, nibbling, snorting), spent more time standing vigilant and sleeping, and less time eating than those in paired housing. At the end of the 12-week trial period, 67% of the horses housed individually were seen performing one or more types of stereotypical behaviour. The response to a corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) challenge test was reduced in those stabled individually, suggested by the authors to be the result of a desensitisation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in response to the stress associated with isolation (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008). Individually housed horses had higher mean heart rates in the stable than those in paired housing (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008). Harewood and McGowan (Reference Harewood and McGowan2005) recorded more stress-related behaviours when fillies were individually housed for the first time than during the previous 24 h when in groups in an outdoor paddock but found no differences in salivary cortisol levels or heart rate. Foals housed in an enriched environment that included turn-out time in groups showed less vigilant/aberrant behaviour, and increased time lying and with ears forward than foals kept in a non-enriched stable environment with only individual turn-out (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014). After eleven weeks almost 50% of the foals in the control environment did not finish their feed. After six weeks higher levels of salivary cortisol were found in the morning in the enriched group (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014). These three studies highlight the complexity associated with using cortisol concentration as a means of interpreting behaviour. Young horses showed behavioural signs of negative affective state following a short-term housing experience, but no physiological response was recorded (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005). Longer-term exposure to a seemingly negative home environment was found to desensitise the HPA response by Visser et al. (Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) and Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014) found increased cortisol levels associated with an enriched environment.

The effect of age on cortisol responses and the extent to which this physiological measure can be used to assess the valence of affective state as opposed to arousal is currently unclear. In adult horses, higher levels of faecal corticosterone were found when they were housed individually (as compared with paired or group housing), but the only behavioural differences recorded were an increase in feeding and a decrease in standing behaviour when horses were paddock-housed in a group, which could be accounted for by the availability of grass (albeit relatively bare) and space in this condition (Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015). Considerable individual differences were found when heart-rate variability (HRV) was monitored by Werhahn et al. (Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012), but in general, this was lower in horses with no turn-out time, with these horses spending less time lying down in the stable than those who were allowed turn-out. The latter finding could be explained by an increase in feeding time and decreased resting time during turn-out.

As an additional means of assessing how the home environment related to affective state, additional tests were included in six of the retained studies. Visser et al. (Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) found no difference in response to a novel object test between the housing groups. Harewood and McGowan (Reference Harewood and McGowan2005) found that fillies were less relaxed during saliva sampling after being housed individually for 24 h (compared to when group-housed in a paddock). Yarnell et al. (Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015) found that individually housed horses were less relaxed during temperature sampling than when group or pair housed. Less aggression towards humans was found by Ruet et al. (Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019) when horses were housed in stables with a window to the outside and on straw (as opposed to non-straw) bedding. During ridden work, rider-assessed ‘willingness to perform’ was scored lowest in those horses with no turn-out (Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012). Similarly, when horses stabled in total confinement were assessed by riders after ridden police patrol work, they were deemed to be more alert, nervous, and fearful than when allowed partial turn-out (Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016).

Limiting factors

The retained articles relating to the home environment varied greatly in terms of objectives, experimental design and behaviour recorded. In addition, justification for attributing affective state to specific behaviours relied primarily on past work, with some findings being supported by means of physiological measures and/or responses to handling and ridden activities. However, when recording behaviour in different situations it is important to highlight the fact that certain behaviours can only occur if resources are provided to facilitate them (for example, social behaviour, movement, feeding behaviour) and these limitations must be accounted for. Also, in most studies there was a bias towards recording behavioural signs of negative affective state, with less frequent conclusions relating to behavioural signs of positive affective state.

Identifying behaviour in situ in the home environment that is indicative of positive affective state is limited by and dependent upon features of that environment. Where individual stabling, lack of space and limited access to forage are features of the home environment, the behavioural repertoire of the horse is severely restricted. Whether or not the subjective experience of horses kept in such an environment can ever be positive is debatable and different interpretations of similar behaviours were evident in this review. For example, ‘alert posture (elevated neck with ears pricked forward)’ was deemed a positive sign of interest in the environment by Ruet et al. (Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019) and negative, vigilant behaviour by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014). A similar discrepancy was found in relation to whether increased time spent standing relaxed is a positive sign (Sauveroche et al. Reference Sauveroche, Henriksson, Theodorsson, Svensson Holm and Roth2020), or increased time standing dozing a negative one (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) (see Table 2[a]). Such interpretations can both potentially be correct, as informed by the context, and the degree to which they occur, but this does highlight the challenge when attributing affective state to behavioural signs. An association between certain postural morphometric measurements taken when the horse was standing in the stable and whether the horse was reported as displaying stereotypical behaviour could inform differentiation between positive and negative signs (Sénèque et al. Reference Sénèque, Lesimple, Morisset and Hausberger2019), although potentially confounded by the type and conformation of individual horses. Behavioural signs of negative affective state were reported more consistently, and these included stereotypical behaviours (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008; Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Matzkeit, Nicolau JTde, Castilha, de Oliveira and Bankuti2019) and behavioural events such as ears backwards, sniffing, licking stable walls/bars, repetitive head swinging (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014), pawing, stamping, kicking at walls, and high-pitched whinnies (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008; Lesimple et al. Reference Lesimple, Gautier, Benhajali, Rochais, Lunel, Bensaïd, Khalloufi, Henry and Hausberger2019), as shown in Table 2(a).

In summary

The results of the studies retained all support the conclusion that equine welfare is likely to be compromised by individual, restrictive housing. This was reflected in behaviour expressed in situ and in behavioural responses to handling and ridden tests (see also Part II; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024). The need for company and the negative consequences of lack of social contact were demonstrated in young horses where reduced feeding time and subsequently reduced weight gain were reported when they were housed individually as opposed to in pairs (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008), and in adult horses in the study by Yarnell et al. (Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015). Other features of the home environment found to be necessary for the horse to fulfil its behavioural needs, and by implication to impact on its affective state, included sufficient space and adequate forage provision. Specific factors reported in the retained studies are shown in Table 2(b).

Intra-species social behaviour

A total of twenty articles that focused primarily on intra-species social behaviour were retained, including observational studies of established and/or free-ranging groups, and experimental manipulation of group membership and location, as well as those assessing features of social communication. Social interactions were used as a measure of group welfare and individual affective state in ten studies (Strand et al. Reference Strand, Tiefenbacher, Haskell, Hosmer, McDonnell and Freeman2002; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Søndergaard, Thodberg and Halekoh2011; York & Schulte Reference York and Schulte2014; Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Fureix, Ouvrard, Bourjade and Hausberger2016; Farmer et al. Reference Farmer, Krüger, Byrne and Marr2018; Majecka & Klawe Reference Majecka and Klawe2018; Pierard et al. Reference Pierard, McGreevy and Geers2019; da Cruz et al. Reference da Cruz, Hirata, dos Santos and Mendonça2023; Kieson et al. Reference Kieson, Goma and Radi2023; Stachurska et al. Reference Stachurska, Kędzierski, Kaczmarek, Wiśniewska, Żylińska and Janczarek2023), with affiliative interactions being attributed with a positive valence and agonistic interactions a negative valence (at least in relation to the recipient). When compared with the studies reported in The home environment the sources cited for the development of ethograms by those reporting social behaviour were more consistent. Out of the fourteen sources cited, five studies referred to McDonnell and Haviland (Reference McDonnell and Haviland1995), three to McDonnell (Reference McDonnell2003) and two referred to each of Waring (2008), Christensen et al. (Reference Christensen, Zharkikh, Ladewig and Yasinetskaya2002) and Jørgensen et al. (Reference Jørgensen, Borsheim, Mejdell, Søndergaard and Bøe2009). See Table S1.2 in the Supplementary material for further details of the retained studies. Intra-species social behaviour and communication indicative of affective state (positive or negative) and the supporting evidence is shown in Table 3(a).

Table 3. (a) Intra-species social behaviour and communication indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

* Key to supporting evidence: Past studies (PS), physiological measures (PHYS), assumption of associated pain (PIP), situations deemed positive or negative (+ve/-ve SIT), positive or negative social interactions (horse/human) (+ve/-ve SOC), approach/avoidance (APP/AVO), choice (PREF).

a A neutral/relaxed facial expression is also described as follows: Body and facial muscles are relaxed. Some eye closure. Ears may continue to move or, if very relaxed, will rest in a lateral position. Lower lip may droop (Wathan et al. Reference Wathan, Proops, Grounds and McComb2016).

The physical environment

The impact of physical features of the home environment on social interactions was assessed by Majecka and Klawe (Reference Majecka and Klawe2018) and Pierard et al. (Reference Pierard, McGreevy and Geers2019). An increase in both affiliative and agonistic interactions was associated with a decrease in enclosure size (Majecka & Klawe Reference Majecka and Klawe2018), and aggressive threats increased with increases in stocking density (Pierard et al. Reference Pierard, McGreevy and Geers2019). Affiliative interactions (allogrooming) between preferred partners increased in frequency but were of a decreased duration in confined (n = 85) as opposed to open pasture (n = 115) settings in large established groups of mares (Kieson et al. Reference Kieson, Goma and Radi2023). Other behavioural signs found to be associated with negative valence (agonistic behaviour: Luz et al. Reference Luz, Maia, Pantoja, Neto and Puoli Filho2015; discomfort: Torcivia & McDonnell Reference Torcivia and McDonnell2021), including increased locomotion and vigilant stances were observed in the confined condition (Kieson et al. Reference Kieson, Goma and Radi2023). In addition to spatial differences in the studies by Majecka and Klawe (Reference Majecka and Klawe2018) and Kieson et al. (Reference Kieson, Goma and Radi2023), the larger enclosures also provided more forage/grazing opportunities which are likely to have contributed to behavioural differences.

Group stability

Group instability and change resulted in an increase in agonistic interactions in young (two years of age) domestic mares (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Søndergaard, Thodberg and Halekoh2011) and physiological signs of stress in feral mares that had been treated with porcine zona pellucida (PZP) contraception (Nuñez et al. Reference Nuñez, Adelman, Smith, Gesquiere and Rubenstein2014). In the latter study, the conclusion that these group changes had a negative impact on the mares was informed by an increase in faecal cortisol metabolites associated with these group changes (Nuñez et al. Reference Nuñez, Adelman, Smith, Gesquiere and Rubenstein2014). In domestic horses, an increase in standing alert and decrease in time spent grazing was found when mares were removed from their group and either isolated or kept with a single companion for a short period (6 h). However, no effect on plasma cortisol level or heart rate was found when compared with mares that had been returned to their group rather than put into isolation (Strand et al. Reference Strand, Tiefenbacher, Haskell, Hosmer, McDonnell and Freeman2002). Although increases in whinnying, urination and rolling were recorded, there was no difference between mares kept alone or with a companion, and the authors note that there were three factors that could account for these behaviours: the novelty of the environment, isolation, and separation from the established herd (Strand et al. Reference Strand, Tiefenbacher, Haskell, Hosmer, McDonnell and Freeman2002). Whether this short-term separation from the established group resulted in a change in social interactions when the mares rejoined the group was not reported.

Group structure and composition

In feral horses, affiliative interactions (including grooming) were associated with a right-side preference which the authors (da Cruz et al. Reference da Cruz, Hirata, dos Santos and Mendonça2023) suggested was linked to the left-brain hemisphere dominance for positive affect and affiliative behaviours (Ahern & Schwartz Reference Ahern and Schwartz1979). This finding contrasts with that of Farmer et al. (Reference Farmer, Krüger, Byrne and Marr2018) where domestic horses were shown to have a left-side preference during allogrooming, a consequence perhaps of human handling which tends to occur more frequently from the left side of the horse. Differences between Przewalski (Equus ferus przewalskii) and domestic horses were found by Górecka-Bruzda et al. (Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Fureix, Ouvrard, Bourjade and Hausberger2016) in relation to the association between social interactions and yawning behaviour. There was a positive correlation between the frequency of yawning and aggressive interactions in Przewalski horses and between yawning frequency and affiliative interactions in domestic horses. It was suggested that yawning could relate to the intensity, rather than the valence, of a social interaction (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Fureix, Ouvrard, Bourjade and Hausberger2016).

As well as differences between feral and domestic horses, other characteristics have been shown to affect the type and frequency of social interactions. Groups comprising ponies (Shetland and Welsh) had more friendly interactions than mixed horse groups (Majecka & Klawe Reference Majecka and Klawe2018). Mares in oestrus were recorded as standing for longer and spending less time in locomotion than those in dioestrus, and an increase in the number of agonistic interactions occurred at the start of dioestrus (Stachurska et al. Reference Stachurska, Kędzierski, Kaczmarek, Wiśniewska, Żylińska and Janczarek2023). Less time was spent active, more time feeding, and fewer social interactions were recorded in lactating compared with non-lactating mares (York & Schulte Reference York and Schulte2014). Górecka-Bruzda et al. (Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jastrzębska, Drewka, Nadolna, Becker and Lansade2022) found mares to be more socially dependent than castrated males. In a two-choice test between access to food or a companion, more mares than geldings chose the companion over food. Such choice tests provide valuable insights into factors that are important to the horses and are likely to promote positive affect.

Two studies included observations of play behaviour in young (two years of age: Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Søndergaard, Thodberg and Halekoh2011) and adult (Hausberger et al. Reference Hausberger, Fureix, Bourjade, Wessel-Robert and Richard-Yris2012) horses. Play behaviour varied between different groups of young horses and was even more variable in unstable as opposed to stable groups (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Søndergaard, Thodberg and Halekoh2011). In adult horses, those exhibiting play behaviour were found to be more likely to also have potentially painful vertebral issues, and showed more aggression towards humans, than non-players (Hausberger et al. Reference Hausberger, Fureix, Bourjade, Wessel-Robert and Richard-Yris2012), but further evidence is required before wider conclusions can be drawn in relation to underlying affective state.

Communication of affective state

The communication of affective state by means of vocalisation was reported in six retained studies (Pond et al. Reference Pond, Darre, Scheifele and Browning2010; Briefer et al. Reference Briefer, Maigrot, Mandel, Freymond, Bachmann and Hillmann2015, Reference Briefer, Mandel, Maigrot, Briefer Freymond, Bachmann and Hillmann2017; Maigrot et al. Reference Maigrot, Hillmann, Anne and Briefer2017, Reference Maigrot, Hillmann and Briefer2022; Stomp et al. Reference Stomp, Leroux, Cellier, Henry, Hausberger and Lemasson2018b). Differences in vocalisation (whinnies) according to the valence of the situation were reported by Pond et al. (Reference Pond, Darre, Scheifele and Browning2010), and Briefer et al. (Reference Briefer, Maigrot, Mandel, Freymond, Bachmann and Hillmann2015) and a specific vocalisation (snort) was reported by Stomp et al. (Reference Stomp, Leroux, Cellier, Henry, Hausberger and Lemasson2018b) to be associated with positive experiences. Differences in the frequencies, energy spectrum and duration of horse vocalisations have been shown to be associated with the valence and arousal level of the underlying emotion, with physiological responses (HRV, RR) reliably changing with arousal level but not valence (Briefer et al. Reference Briefer, Maigrot, Mandel, Freymond, Bachmann and Hillmann2015). A weak (but inconsistent) association between both arousal and the valence conveyed within the vocalisation and changes in skin temperature (ST) was however found (Briefer et al. Reference Briefer, Maigrot, Mandel, Freymond, Bachmann and Hillmann2015). Species-specific differences may occur in the way that valence is communicated within vocalisations (Maigrot et al. Reference Maigrot, Hillmann, Anne and Briefer2017) but some cross-species recognition of both valence and arousal communicated through vocalisation has been demonstrated in the horse (Maigrot et al. Reference Maigrot, Hillmann and Briefer2022).

Valence has been associated with specific features of horse facial expression (Wathan et al. Reference Wathan, Burrows, Waller and McComb2015), and when presented with 2D images of horse faces portraying positive or relaxed expressions, or agonistic expressions, chose to approach the former and avoid the latter (Wathan et al. Reference Wathan, Proops, Grounds and McComb2016). Horses clearly use both auditory and visual information generated by conspecifics to guide social behaviour, facilitate positive interactions, and avoid aggressive encounters.

In summary

The physical and social environment combine to determine whether the species-specific needs of the horse can be fulfilled. There is a consensus that group membership is a priority for the horse and that the valence of social interactions contributes to and is evidence of individual affective state. Environmental conditions, group structure and composition, and individual characteristics have all been shown to relate to the type and frequency of social interactions occurring within that group, which in turn provide an insight into the affective state of group members. Behavioural signs of group cohesion and the nature of within-group social interactions reflect the quality of life of horses within a social group. Although short-term separation from the group was not found to have a negative impact on some individuals, it is not clear how such removals affect the overall stability of the group. In addition, behaviours associated with separation anxiety can cause safety issues for human handlers as well as being signs of distress in the horse (see Hartmann et al. Reference Hartmann, Christensen and Keeling2011). Regardless, providing the horse with companionship in the form of a stable group of conspecifics is imperative to satisfy social behavioural needs. The potentially negative effect of removal from the group may be lessened by repeated short-term experiences, followed consistently by return to the group. Further evidence is required to determine how the negative impact of separation from the group can be reduced, but keeping horses alone is not an acceptable solution. Factors found to affect behavioural signs of affective state in social situations are shown in Table 3(b).

Weaning

Fourteen articles relating to the behaviour of mares and foals during and after the weaning process were retained. The age at which foals were weaned varied from four and a half months (Henry et al. Reference Henry, Zanella, Sankey, Richard-Yris, Marko and Hausberger2012) to approximately eight months (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Foury, Reigner, Vidament, Guettier, Bouvet, Soulet, Parias, Ruet, Mach, Lévy and Moisan2018), with most weaning occurring at around six months of age. The process of artificial weaning in the domestic horse is acknowledged as being a negative experience for both mare and foal, and the focus of the retained articles was on how this experience could be managed to make it less stressful. The impact of periods of separation pre-weaning, abrupt or gradual weaning, and of different post-weaning groupings, were assessed using both behavioural and physiological measures. Time budgets and activity pre- and post-weaning were compared in both mares and foals, as well as social interactions and behaviour associated with stress (distress). Behaviour of the mare and foal following separation during weaning is shown in Table 4(a). Further details of the retained studies are included in Table S1.3 in the Supplementary material.

Table 4. (a) Mare and foal behaviour following physical separation at weaning, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour. The process of weaning is assumed to be a negative/stressful experience for both mare and foal. Behavioural responses of mare and foal observed immediately post-weaning and factors found to affect this behaviour are shown below. Where physiological measures have been reported this is indicated by PHYS

Pre-weaning mare-foal separation

The effect of short-term (10-min) physical but not auditory separation of mare and foal at two, four, six, eight, ten and twelve weeks of age, on maternal care and subsequent response to weaning was assessed by Moons et al. (Reference Moons, Laughlin and Zanella2005). Post short-term separation both mare and foal looked at each other more frequently and the foal initiated physical contact with the mare more frequently than pre-separation. At weaning there was no difference in HR response between foals that had been separated (n = 5) and the control group (n = 5), but the latter showed more locomotion. The foals that had been exposed to short-term separation had higher concentrations of salivary cortisol on the post-weaning morning (Moons et al. Reference Moons, Laughlin and Zanella2005), possibly a response to the failure to be reunited after this separation. Consistent individual variation in the response of foals to pre-weaning separation from the mare (for 2 min) as tested four times from one week post-natal to six months was found by Pérez Manrique et al. (Reference Pérez Manrique, Hudson, Bánszegi and Szenczi2019) in both behaviour (vocalisation rate, raised tail and locomotion) and physiology (HRV). Due to the small sample size and contradictory behavioural and physiological findings by Moons et al. (Reference Moons, Laughlin and Zanella2005) and the individual variation in foal response to maternal separation, with no subsequent measures taken following weaning (Pérez Manrique et al. Reference Pérez Manrique, Hudson, Bánszegi and Szenczi2019), there is no conclusive evidence that pre-weaning separation reduces the stress of the final physical separation.

A two-stage approach to weaning was trialled by Merkies et al. (Reference Merkies, DuBois, Marshall, Parois, Graham and Haley2016) with nutritional separation (udder cover fitted to the mare to prevent nursing) for four days prior to physical separation. During the period of nutritional separation, foals were found to spend more time lying down than controls, but no behavioural or physiological differences between groups were found after physical separation. The only difference found between the groups was in the mares, where nutritional separation resulted in less vocalisation following physical separation. After physical separation, all foals were recorded as vocalising more, were more active and aggressive, and spent less time lying down. Increases in faecal cortisol metabolites were recorded in all mares and foals following physical separation, but individual variation in both behavioural and physiological responses was again found (Merkies et al. Reference Merkies, DuBois, Marshall, Parois, Graham and Haley2016). A gradual increase in the time spent physically separated (by a fence that allowed mares and foals visual, olfactory, and tactile contact, but prevented suckling) over the month preceding weaning was shown to reduce time spent trotting and vocalising compared with foals weaned without this preparation, as well as resulting in reduced concentrations of salivary cortisol (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Foury, Reigner, Vidament, Guettier, Bouvet, Soulet, Parias, Ruet, Mach, Lévy and Moisan2018). This gradual weaning process also benefited the mares who showed fewer behavioural signs of stress and lower salivary cortisol levels than mares whose foals had been weaned without this progressive separation (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Foury, Reigner, Vidament, Guettier, Bouvet, Soulet, Parias, Ruet, Mach, Lévy and Moisan2018).

Physical and social environment

The physical and social environment has been shown to be one key factor in how foals cope with artificial weaning and in their long-term well-being. For example, the development of stereotypical behaviour has been associated with confinement and isolation of foals during the weaning process (Waters et al. Reference Waters, Nicol and French2002). Regardless of how weaning is managed, changes in foal behaviour include increased vocalisation, locomotion, and reduced feeding, but these can be less pronounced if foals remain in a familiar environment and familiar group with known adult (unrelated) mares, compared with foals kept in peer groups without an adult (Erber et al. Reference Erber, Wulf, Rose-Meierhöfer, Becker-Birck, Möstl, Aurich, Hoffmann and Aurich2012). Foals moved to an unfamiliar environment at weaning, with some unfamiliar group members (foals) were found to spend an increased time lying down initially, followed by resting standing for longer periods than before they were weaned (Delank et al. Reference Delank, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2023). A similar impact of an unfamiliar stable environment on general apathy in foals at weaning was found by Górecka-Bruzda et al. (Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Suwała, Palme, Jaworski, Jastrzebska, Boroń and Jezierski2015), with an increased time spent standing, decreased social activity and reduced feeding recorded in forest-reared compared with stable-reared foals.

In contrast with the findings of Visser et al. (Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) (see The home environment), Hoffman et al. (Reference Hoffman, Kronfel, Holland and Greiwe-Crandell1995) recorded fewer behavioural and physiological signs of stress in foals housed individually at weaning than in those housed in pairs. Foals housed in pairs stood still less than those housed individually (deemed a negative sign by Delank et al. Reference Delank, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2023), with a high frequency of aggressive interactions recorded (Hoffman et al. Reference Hoffman, Kronfel, Holland and Greiwe-Crandell1995). The increased cortisol response in pair-housed foals may have related to the increased arousal associated with social interactions, regardless of valence. Lack of social contact during periods of isolation was found to reduce outward behavioural signs of emotional reactivity in yearlings (Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Neveux and Levy2012, see Part II; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024). The association between having a companion (or not) and the extent to which emotion is communicated behaviourally may explain in part the findings of Hoffman et al. (Reference Hoffman, Kronfel, Holland and Greiwe-Crandell1995). Also, Henry et al. (Reference Henry, Zanella, Sankey, Richard-Yris, Marko and Hausberger2012) found that foals kept in groups that included unrelated (and in this case, unfamiliar) adult horses, rather than in peer-only groups vocalised less and had lower salivary cortisol responses. Only foals kept in groups without an adult horse showed increased aggression towards peers and abnormal behaviour such as wood-chewing (Henry et al. Reference Henry, Zanella, Sankey, Richard-Yris, Marko and Hausberger2012).

Sex-related differences

Although there has been shown to be individual variation in the responses of mares and foals, artificial weaning is always associated with both behavioural and physiological signs of stress. Some differences between fillies and colts in both behaviour and physiological responses were found. Colts spent more time eating than fillies (Hoffman et al. Reference Hoffman, Kronfel, Holland and Greiwe-Crandell1995), and on the day of weaning walked less and defaecated more than fillies (Moons et al. Reference Moons, Laughlin and Zanella2005; Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Suwała, Palme, Jaworski, Jastrzebska, Boroń and Jezierski2015; Wulf et al. Reference Wulf, Beythien, Ille, Aurich and Aurich2018). Subsequently, colts demonstrated more curiosity about the environment, were more socially interactive and lay down less often than fillies (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Suwała, Palme, Jaworski, Jastrzebska, Boroń and Jezierski2015). Although initial weight loss was found to be greater in fillies than colts by Wulf et al. (Reference Wulf, Beythien, Ille, Aurich and Aurich2018), contrary to other studies, they did not find any sex-related differences in feeding or lying behaviour. However, levels of salivary cortisol (Wulf et al. Reference Wulf, Beythien, Ille, Aurich and Aurich2018) and faecal cortisol metabolites (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Suwała, Palme, Jaworski, Jastrzebska, Boroń and Jezierski2015) were both higher in filly than colt foals at weaning. Despite many factors varying between studies, and some findings being based on relatively small sample sizes, there is evidence that there are sex-related differences in how foals respond to artificial weaning.

In summary

The behavioural and physiological response to artificial weaning cannot be avoided but the process can be made less onerous by adopting protocols that have been shown in the retained studies to lessen the negative impact of physical separation. The gradual approach to mare and foal separation outlined by Lansade et al. (Reference Lansade, Foury, Reigner, Vidament, Guettier, Bouvet, Soulet, Parias, Ruet, Mach, Lévy and Moisan2018) had much to recommend it. A priority should be to ensure that the weanling foals are kept in social groups that are familiar to them, including familiar adults, and in an environment that is also familiar to them. Although potentially challenging to many horse breeders, the short- and long-term well-being of these youngsters must be the primary concern. In agreement with Górecka-Bruzda et al. (Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Suwała, Palme, Jaworski, Jastrzebska, Boroń and Jezierski2015), feeding behaviour is an indication of how foals are coping with separation from the mare, and potentially how mares themselves are coping. It is likely that mares will not be as adversely affected by environmental changes, but the provision of a stable social group and the freedom to move and feed are important factors that are likely to reduce the negative impact of separation from their foals. Factors affecting behaviour associated with weaning are shown in Table 4(b).

Experimental tests of affective state

One approach used to objectively assess affective state in animals is to test whether they make a positive or negative decision relating to an ambiguous stimulus, termed a judgement bias test. Such tests have been used to assess the impact of housing and management (and consequently the extent to which species-specific needs were fulfilled) on underlying affective state and were subsequently included in Part I of this review. A total of eight such studies were retained, all of which reported the findings of spatial judgement bias tests (where the animals were trained to associate the location of the stimulus, usually a bucket, with a food reward or no food reward, and then presented with a stimulus in an ambiguous position). The results of these studies were inconsistent.

Two of the studies investigated the impact of current housing and management on whether horses demonstrated positive or negative expectations when presented with a stimulus in an ambiguous position (optimistic or pessimistic behaviour). Henry et al. (Reference Henry, Fureix, Rowberry, Bateson and Hausberger2017) found that those horses kept in a free-ranging social group displayed a positive judgement bias, whereas those kept in a more restrictive system in a riding school displayed a negative judgement bias. Although Marliani et al. (Reference Marliani, Balboni, Tiberi and Malavasi2022a) found some evidence of a more positive judgement bias in horses kept in an ethological stable system (Big Box©) compared to those in traditional stabling or in a more ‘natural’ open system, conflicting results from other measures (faecal and hair cortisol, personality traits, body condition scores) and confounding factors meant that no overall conclusions could be drawn. A similar lack of correlation between physiological measures and the results of judgement bias testing was found by Marliani et al. (Reference Marliani, Vannucchi, Kiumurgis and Accorsi2022b), although they identified individual differences associated with personality traits on the latency to approach certain stimulus positions. However, different personality tests were used in these two studies.

Two studies assessed the impact of management change on affective state as demonstrated by performance in judgement bias tests. Löckener et al. (Reference Löckener, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2016) found a positive judgement bias in horses previously kept in individual housing with no access to pasture (for six months) after they had been moved to pasture and group living (for at least ten days). McGuire et al. (Reference McGuire, Johnson-Ulrich, Robeson, Zeigler-Hill and Vonk2018) found that horses (and one donkey) that had been rescued from abuse/neglect approached ambiguous stimuli more rapidly than those that had not experienced abuse/neglect and consequently had not been rescued.

Limitations of this approach

Although judgement bias testing offers a degree of insight into the affective state of animals, it requires prior training (often a lengthy process), and performance has been found to be affected by previous approaches to training. The effect of training using positive or negative reinforcement on judgement bias was assessed by Briefer-Freymond et al. (Reference Briefer Freymond, Briefer, Zollinger, Gindrat-von Allmen, Wyss and Bachmann2014), with the finding that those previously trained using negative reinforcement made more positive appraisals of ambiguous stimuli than those trained using positive reinforcement. The food reward used in the judgement bias testing may have constituted a change for the better for those that had previously been negatively reinforced (Briefer-Freymond et al. Reference Briefer Freymond, Briefer, Zollinger, Gindrat-von Allmen, Wyss and Bachmann2014).

The search for more immediate objective measures of affective state has included comparisons with performance in judgement bias tests. A study evaluating eye wrinkles in horses during a judgement bias test found no significant association between these and evidence of optimism/pessimism based on performance (Hintze & Schanz Reference Hintze and Schanz2021). However, Marr et al. (Reference Marr, Farmer and Krüger2018) found a significant relationship between motor laterality and performance in judgement bias tests, with a positive judgement bias being displayed by horses that moved off using the right forelimb first. In the study by Marliani et al. (Reference Marliani, Vannucchi, Kiumurgis and Accorsi2022b), horses that had been trained that the rewarded stimulus position was on the right were found to approach the ambiguous stimulus faster than those horses trained to reward from the stimulus position on the left. In terms of more general applicability, such approach behaviour in non-experimental situations could be used as an indication of positive expectations, potentially linked to a positive mental state. However, whether lateral biases or facial expression can provide any further insight is yet to be determined.

In summary

The results of the retained studies involving judgement bias testing provide some further evidence that housing and management that fulfils the need for space and company is associated with a more optimistic (positive) appraisal of ambiguous stimuli by the horse. Although there were confounding factors identified, and a lack of correlation with other measures, studies did demonstrate that change for the better (in terms of addressing the behavioural needs of the horse) was associated with optimism and potentially positive affective state. See Table S1.4 in the Supplementary material for further details of the studies retained.

Stereotypical behaviour

Stereotypical behaviours are defined as being repeated, relatively invariant sequences of movement with no obvious function (Broom & Kennedy Reference Broom and Kennedy1993), and not observed in free-ranging feral horses (Dodman et al. Reference Dodman, Normile, Cottam, Guzman and Shuster2005). Management factors most associated with the development of stereotypies in stabled horses include housing conditions, feeding practices, and weaning method (Sarrafchi & Blokhuis Reference Sarrafchi and Blokhuis2013). In addition to the housing-related development of repetitive/stereotypical behaviour in young horses reported in The home environment (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008; Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, Valenchon, Foury, Neveux, Cole, Layé, Cardinaud, Lévy and Moisan2014), seven articles were retained that reported factors associated with stereotypical and other unwanted stable-based behaviours.

Factors relating to the occurrence of stereotypical behaviour

Arena et al. (2021) compared the current housing and management of horses reported by a veterinarian as displaying behavioural pathologies (oral stereotypies, locomotor stereotypies, social problems, and training behaviours), with those not displaying such behaviour (and deemed healthy). A positive correlation between the occurrence of behavioural pathologies, time spent in the stable and frequency of work, and a low-fibre, high-energy diet was found. Justification for deeming such behavioural pathologies to be signs of chronic stress and potentially negative affective state was based both on the findings of previous studies and on higher plasma cortisol levels compared with healthy horses. No difference in hair cortisol levels between horses displaying behavioural pathologies and their healthy counterparts was found (Arena et al. Reference Arena, Marliani, Sabioni, Gabai, Bucci and Accorsi2021).

It has been suggested that the type of work/role of the horse affects the prevalence and type of behaviour exhibited. Zuluaga et al. (Reference Zuluaga, Mira, Sánchez and Martínez2018) found behavioural signs of chronic stress (including wood chewing, pawing, bed eating, crib-biting, and windsucking) in urban police patrol horses in Columbia, but it was not clear whether this was a direct consequence of the role or management factors. Although differences between horses involved in different sporting disciplines (including dressage, showjumping, eventing, and endurance) have been found (in some cases independently of management differences, for example, Hausberger et al. Reference Hausberger, Gautier, Biquand, Lunel and Jégo2009), it is not clear whether these differences result from initial selection for a role or because of the demands of that role. Hanis et al. (Reference Hanis, Chung, Kamalludin and Idrus2021) found differences between horse working groups (leisure, endurance, patrol horses) in the prevalence of abnormal/stereotypic behaviour in the stable, but also related differences in dietary intake between groups, and concluded that this was a major factor in the development of such behaviour. In general, the effect of work/role on behaviour is confounded by many factors (including selection, training, and management) and findings should consequently be carefully evaluated in terms of all contributory variables. However, as confirmed by the findings of a recent survey by Kádár et al. (Reference Kádár, Maros, Drégelyi, Szedenik, Lukácsi, Pesti, Besenyei and Egri2023), the prime factor is the type of home environment, with an increased occurrence of abnormal repetitive behaviours reported in stabled horses as compared with those kept at pasture.

The association between stereotypies and other features of behaviour

The impact of performing stereotypical behaviours on learning performance has been evaluated. No effect of stereotypical behaviour on learning ability has been found (Briefer Freymond et al. Reference Briefer Freymond, Ruet, Grivaz, Fuentes, Zuberbühler, Bachmann and Briefer2019, Reference Briefer Freymond, Beuret, Ruet, Zuberbühler, Bachmann and Briefer2020), with the proviso that the performance of the stereotypical behaviour is not restricted. Horses that displayed crib-biting during a spatial learning task showed fewer behavioural signs of frustration and had lower levels of salivary cortisol than stereotypical horses that did not display this behaviour (Briefer Freymond et al. Reference Briefer Freymond, Beuret, Ruet, Zuberbühler, Bachmann and Briefer2020). An increased sensitivity to tactile stimuli was found in crib-biting horses, which the authors suggested may be linked to physiological responses to chronic stress, that also potentiated the development of this behaviour (Freymond et al. Reference Freymond, Bardou, Beuret, Bachmann, Zuberbühler and Briefer2019).

In summary

Regardless of the potential positive or negative impact that performing abnormal repetitive behaviours has on an individual’s QOL, research to date provides conclusive evidence that it is the failure to provide for the behavioural needs of the horse that is the causative factor. Although it is not clear whether the performance of stereotypies per se is a sign of current QOL, or a coping response resulting from past sub-optimal conditions (Sarrafchi & Blokhuis Reference Sarrafchi and Blokhuis2013), such behaviour has been shown to develop in young horses exposed to inappropriate housing and/or diet and persists throughout adulthood. As such, this behaviour provides evidence of the importance of providing for the behavioural needs of the horse throughout their life. Despite this, many horses continue to be managed in a way that fails to do so. See Table S1.5 in the Supplementary material for further details of the studies.

Behavioural evidence of affective state

Despite the incorporation of positive welfare states in the extended Five Domains model used to assess animal welfare (Mellor & Beausoleil Reference Mellor and Beausoleil2015), and the relative increase in articles identified by Search 1 (emotion, affect, cognition) compared with Search 2 (stress) post-2015, the focus of most studies was on behavioural signs of negative affective state, with an absence of negatively attributed behaviours being considered as a positive sign. Behavioural expression is inextricably linked to the physical and social environment, with most approaches to horse management providing little opportunity for the horse to display behaviours indicative of positive affective state, at least in situ. Inferences regarding behavioural signs of affective state in the home environment (see Table 2[a]) can be supported with reference to the two components of arousal and valence identified by Mendl et al. (Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010). Behaviours considered to reflect positive affective state included standing calmly, recumbency (low arousal, positive valence: relaxed) and increased time spent feeding, as well as affiliative social interactions with conspecifics and allogrooming (high arousal, positive valence: reward-seeking). Clearly, such positive behavioural signs are dependent upon resource provision and social opportunities. Behaviours considered to reflect negative affective state included pawing, stamping, and kicking at the wall, and other repetitive behaviours including stereotypies, agonistic and avoidance behaviours (high arousal, negative valence), and withdrawn, unresponsive behaviour (low arousal, negative valence).

Reliance on the findings of previous studies and the exclusive recording of behaviours deemed indicative of stress were in some cases how conclusions regarding affective state were arrived at. As noted above, the focus of most studies was on behavioural signs of negative affective state, with an absence of negatively attributed behaviours being considered as a positive sign. This conclusion is not necessarily warranted, and whereas active attempts at escaping from an unsatisfactory situation may be unwelcome for the human carer, they do provide a clear signal that whatever is causing this response is something that the horse is objecting to. A lack of such behaviour may not suggest that the situation is acceptable, rather that the horse has given up trying to escape. General unresponsiveness has been associated with poor condition, poor health, exhaustion, chronic pain, and depression in horses (Burn et al. Reference Burn, Dennison and Whay2010). When exposed to repeated, unavoidable, aversive experiences, where there is no association between behaviour and the outcome for the horse, there is evidence that a condition akin to learned helplessness can ensue (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Goodwin, Heleski, Randle and Waran2008).

In addition to reference to past studies, and/or ethograms listing behaviours typical of a specific (generally negative) affective state, physiological measures were frequently used as a means of providing objective evidence to support this interpretation. Although consistency between physiological measures of stress (including HR, HRV, rectal temperature and cortisol levels) and behaviour was found in scenarios that were intrinsically negative experiences (for example, weaning), this was not always the case in other situations. Physiological measures have been shown to relate more closely to arousal than to the valence of an emotional response (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Randle, Pearson, Preshaw and Waran2018) and other factors may be influencing the type of behavioural response elicited. For example, Squibb et al. (Reference Squibb, Griffin, Favier and Ijichi2018) attributed the lack of correlation between physiological and behavioural responses in adult horses during a novel task to different levels of training (see Part II: Tests of emotional reactivity; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024). Although the findings of individual studies have varied, and physiological responses have been associated with factors deemed to be negative experiences for the horse (for example, individual housing: Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015), the relationship between physiological response, behaviour, and affective state requires further investigation.

In addition to behaviour in situ, features of the home environment and the provision of forage related to behaviour in other scenarios. For example, ease of handling was facilitated by group/paired housing (Harewood & McGowan Reference Harewood and McGowan2005; Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015) and provision of turn-out resulted in an increase in willingness to perform (Werhahn et al. Reference Werhahn, Hessel and Van den Weghe2012) and a reduction in nervous/fearful behaviour (Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016) when ridden. Behavioural signs of affective state in groups of horses were predominantly assessed by comparing the occurrences of agonistic and affiliative interactions, with the latter considered a positive sign. The QOL of individual group members is likely to be affected by the cohesiveness of the group, amicable sharing of resources (for example, water, see Figure 3), and potentially the social facilitation of activity patterns, for example, group recumbency (K Griffin, personal communication 2023). Further evidence is required to determine the relationship between group behaviour and the QOL of individual group members.

Figure 3. Amicable sharing of resources such as water is a positive sign of group cohesion and well-being in domestic horses.

QOL in breeding stock

Although multiple articles relating to most aspects of the life of the domestic horse were identified by the search terms, there were a few notable omissions. Despite the potential for the management of breeding stock to negatively impact the QOL of both mares and stallions (and subsequently their offspring), no articles specifically relating to this scenario were identified by the searches. This lack of evidence within the scientific literature regarding the welfare of breeding horses was noted by Campbell (Reference Campbell2017), alongside the need to align methods of husbandry more closely with the natural reproductive behaviour of the horse (Waran Reference Waran1997; Campbell & Sandøe Reference Campbell and Sandøe2015). A review of the aspects of stallion management that contribute to their quality of life, or lack thereof, concluded that the way many stallions are currently housed and managed results in a lack of social skills, increased aggression, and stereotypic behaviour (de Oliveira & Aurich Reference de Oliveira and Aurich2021). The home environment and social opportunities afforded to many stallions (Popescu et al. Reference Popescu, Lazar, Borda, Niculae, Sandru and Spinu2019), as well as the welfare problems associated with the way horses are bred (Campbell & Sandøe Reference Campbell and Sandøe2015) require urgent consideration to promote better quality of life for horses within the breeding sector. The findings of this review have highlighted the importance of social stability and access to an appropriate physical and social environment from birth onwards. In addition to the provision of friends, freedom, and forage (Fraser Reference Fraser2012), the importance of family to the horse should also be considered.

Animal welfare implications

The results of Part I of this review provide extensive evidence that the first stage in moving towards a better quality of life for horses is to ensure that throughout their lives the home environment satisfies their need for company, freedom of movement and environmental choices, as well as access to adequate forage. Failure to provide for any of these basic species-specific needs will undermine attempts to achieve a good life for horses. Current findings wholly support those of the early study by Schatzmann (Reference Schatzmann1998) where the highest priority of horses was always to be in the company of, or within view of, other horses and to be eating forage outside.

Although the horse’s need for space, company and forage has long been acknowledged, many are still stabled individually and have restricted access to all three. Current findings support the conclusions of Ruet et al. (Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019) that an optimum environment should allow for free exercise, interactions with conspecifics and ad libitum fibre availability to facilitate positive affective state in horses and ponies. Opportunities for free exercise, as provided by paddock turn-out, were instrumental in increasing signs of relaxation in horses (Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Trigo, Mesquita Neto, Lacreta Junior, Sousa, Muniz and Moura2016), reducing the occurrence of behavioural pathologies (Arena et al. Reference Arena, Marliani, Sabioni, Gabai, Bucci and Accorsi2021) and promoting increased interest in the environment (Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019). The impact of having access to both space and company was demonstrated in the study by Löckener et al. (Reference Löckener, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2016), where an optimistic state of mind was generated in horses tested in a judgement bias task once they had been moved from restrictive individual stabling to being kept in a group on pasture. Housing in company, ideally in a stable group, was shown to be associated with positive behavioural signs from an early age and throughout adulthood. Paired housing (as opposed to individual housing) in young horses stabled for the first time was less likely to result in behavioural signs of stress and the development of stereotypical behaviour patterns (Visser et al. Reference Visser, Ellis and Van Reenen2008) and was associated with increased time spent eating forage in adult horses (Yarnell et al. Reference Yarnell, Hall, Royle and Walker2015).

The social group in which horses live plays a major part in determining their general welfare. Comparisons with feral horse groups are a valuable approach to further understand what constitutes an appropriate group structure (Waran Reference Waran1997), although in most cases domestic constraints will limit the extent to which this can be applied. Group structure has been found to be of particular importance in foals, where the presence of unrelated adult horses was found to reduce the stress associated with weaning (Erber Reference Erber, Wulf, Rose-Meierhöfer, Becker-Birck, Möstl, Aurich, Hoffmann and Aurich2012; Henry et al. Reference Henry, Zanella, Sankey, Richard-Yris, Marko and Hausberger2012). This reflects the social structure in feral horses and the presence of older mares would be likely to exert a calming effect on foals via emotional transfer, as well as contributing to the social learning of foals that will prepare them for future group living. Also, feral group structure may explain why mares (Górecka-Bruzda et al. Reference Górecka-Bruzda, Jastrzębska, Drewka, Nadolna, Becker and Lansade2022) and fillies (Moons et al. Reference Moons, Laughlin and Zanella2005; Wulf et al. Reference Wulf, Beythien, Ille, Aurich and Aurich2018) were found to show a greater level of distress than their male counterparts in response to separation and/or isolation. The survival of feral mares and their offspring is dependent upon the stability of the family group (harem), whereas male groups are much more transitory (Waran Reference Waran1997). Optimum grouping is a necessity for the development of foals and young horses, and the relative impact of change on individual horses should be considered when management decisions are made.

When applying the Five Domains model (Mellor & Beausoleil Reference Mellor and Beausoleil2015) to assess the adverse impacts of husbandry and training on horse welfare, nutrition was considered by a panel of experts to have the least bearing on resultant subjective mental state when compared with other behavioural restrictions (McGreevy et al. Reference McGreevy, Berger, de Brauwere, Doherty, Harrison, Fiedler, Jones, McDonnell, McLean, Nakonechny, Nicol, Preshaw, Thomson, Tzioumis, Webster, Wolfensohn, Yeates and Jones2018). However, the findings of this review suggest otherwise. The provision of adequate forage has been demonstrated as integral to promoting horse health and positive affective state (Ruet et al. Reference Ruet, Lemarchand, Parias, Mach, Moisan, Foury, Briant and Lansade2019). In stabled horses, long gaps between meals and inadequate forage were associated with an increase in the development of unwanted behaviours, including stereotypies (Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Matzkeit, Nicolau JTde, Castilha, de Oliveira and Bankuti2019). Providing forage in a way that allows sufficient space for all individuals to access forage with minimal conflict in a group housing situation reduced the occurrence of agonistic behaviour and increased group cohesion (Sundmann et al. 2022; Baumgartner et al. Reference Baumgartner, Erhard and Zeitler-Feicht2023). Feeding is classed as a behaviour that has critical short-term consequences for an animal, is maintained for as long as possible and is highly resilient to change (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Randle, Pearson, Preshaw and Waran2018). As such, the need to consume sufficient forage and for an adequate time is a prime motivating factor for the horse. Indeed, as reported by Bulens et al. (Reference Bulens, Sterken, Van Beirendonck, Van Thielen and Driessen2015) (see also Part II; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024), when forage was available horses were less inclined to engage in other behaviours with lower resilience, such as exploring the environment and investigating novel objects. The importance of fibre in the diet of foals from one month was evident from the comparative effect of a fat and fibre diet or a starch and sugar diet, with the latter being associated with more behavioural signs of distress post-weaning and increased aversion to human handling (Nicol et al. Reference Nicol, Badnell-Waters, Bice, Kelland, Wilson and Harris2005). A low-forage diet was also associated with health issues and potentially a source of abdominal pain, as discussed in Part II of this review (Destrez et al. Reference Destrez, Grimm, Cézilly and Julliand2015; Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024).

There were a number of general limitations associated with this review which are discussed in Part II (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024), but these do not detract from the unavoidable conclusion that to ensure a good QOL for all horses, the equine sector must urgently review the way horses are managed and implement change to ensure that their species-specific needs are met. Too long have human requirements been prioritised over the needs of the horse, but this must now be addressed to ensure that the horse-human partnership is mutually beneficial.

Conclusion

The potential for horses to live a good life requires them to be provided with opportunities to fulfil their species-specific needs. Current approaches to horse management frequently fail in this respect and consequently, their quality of life is sub-optimal. Behaviours indicative of negative affective state, often resulting from inadequate social and physical environments, in addition to insufficient forage provision, were evident both in the home environment and in other scenarios, including during interactions with humans. Such behavioural signs included withdrawn or agitated behaviour in situ, avoidance behaviour during handling procedures, and agonistic intra-specific social interactions. Although the lack of such behavioural expression may be considered by many in the equine sector as evidence that the horse has an acceptable QOL, the findings of this review suggest otherwise. The focus of many of the studies was to assess the relative inadequacies of approaches to management and the impact of these on the behaviour of the horse. Although fewer negative behavioural signs may be associated with a reduction in the extent to which the horse experiences a situation as unpleasant, positive behavioural signs of pleasure were less frequently identified. The potential for behaviour indicative of pleasure to occur was dependent upon resource provision. Affiliative intra-specific social interactions, approach behaviour indicative of positive anticipation of a reward, and forage ingestion were all considered positive signs. Although a calm demeanour in situ was interpreted as positive, this may just be a sign of acceptance rather than pleasure.

The first stage in moving towards a better life for horses is undoubtedly to provide them with the opportunity to satisfy their behavioural needs from birth onwards and improve their potential for experiencing pleasure. The findings of the first part of this review provide evidence of what horses want and indeed need to live a good life, and how behaviour reflects their QOL. The impact of the many and varied interactions with humans on horse QOL is discussed in Part II (Hall & Kay Reference Hall and Kay2024).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.38.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Natalie Waran, Hayley Randle, Gemma Pearson, and Mark Farnworth for their input at the planning stage of this review, Liane Preshaw and Jan Rogers for their feedback on an earlier draft, John Wyldes for assistance with formatting Figure 1, and the Horse Trust for providing funding for the initial part of the study.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions Conceptualisation: CH, RK; Data curation: CH, RK; Formal analysis: CH; Methodology: CH, RK; Investigation: CH, RK; Supervision: CH; Validation: CH; Visualisation: CH; Funding acquisition: CH; Project administration: CH; Resources: CH, RK; Software: CH; Writing – original draft: CH, RK; Writing – review & editing: CH, RK

References

Ahern, GL and Schwartz, GE 1979 Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion. Neuropsychologia 17: 693698. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(79)90045-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arena, I, Marliani, G, Sabioni, S, Gabai, G, Bucci, D and Accorsi, PA 2021 Assessment of horses’ welfare: Behavioral, hormonal, and husbandry aspects. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 41: 8290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, M, Erhard, MH and Zeitler-Feicht, MH 2023 Which animal-to-feeding-place ratio at time-controlled hay racks is animal appropriate? Preliminary analysis of stress responses of horses. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.1005102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briefer, EF, Maigrot, AL, Mandel, R, Freymond, SB, Bachmann, I and Hillmann, E 2015 Segregation of information about emotional arousal and valence in horse whinnies. Scientific Reports 4: 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep09989Google ScholarPubMed
Briefer, EF, Mandel, R, Maigrot, AL, Briefer Freymond, S, Bachmann, I and Hillmann, E 2017 Perception of emotional valence in horse whinnies. Frontiers in Zoology 14: 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-017-0193-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briefer Freymond, S, Beuret, S, Ruet, A, Zuberbühler, K, Bachmann, I and Briefer, EF 2020 Stereotypic behaviour in horses lowers stress but not spatial learning performance. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 232: 105099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briefer Freymond, S, Briefer, EF, Zollinger, A, Gindrat-von Allmen, Y, Wyss, C and Bachmann, I 2014 Behaviour of horses in a judgment bias test associated with positive or negative reinforcement. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 158: 3445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briefer Freymond, S, Ruet, A, Grivaz, M, Fuentes, C, Zuberbühler, K, Bachmann, I and Briefer, EF 2019 Stereotypic horses (Equus caballus) are not cognitively impaired. Animal Cognition 22: 1733. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1217-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, DM and Kennedy, MJ 1993 Stereotypies in horses: their relevance to welfare and causation. Equine Veterinary Education 5: 151154. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2042-3292.1993.tb01026.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulens, A, Sterken, H, Van Beirendonck, S, Van Thielen, J and Driessen, B 2015 The use of different objects during a novel object test in stabled horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 10: 5458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burla, JB, Rufener, C, Bachmann, I, Gygax, L, Patt, A and Hillmann, E 2017 Space allowance of the littered area affects lying behavior in group-housed horses. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 4: 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2017.00023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burn, CC, Dennison, TL and Whay, HR 2010 Relationships between behaviour and health in working horses, donkeys, and mules in developing countries. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 126: 109118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, MLH 2016 Freedoms and frameworks: How we think about the welfare of competition horses. Equine Veterinary Journal 48: 540542. https://doi.org/10.1111/evj.12598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, MHL 2017 Welfare considerations surrounding the management of breeding horses. Vet Times, March 6. https://www.vettimes.co.uk (accessed 9 July 2023).Google Scholar
Campbell, MLH and Sandøe, P 2015 Welfare in horse breeding. Veterinary Record, April 25. 436440. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.102814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, JW, Søndergaard, E, Thodberg, K and Halekoh, U 2011 Effects of repeated regrouping on horse behaviour and injuries. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 133: 199206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.05.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, JW, Zharkikh, T, Ladewig, J and Yasinetskaya, N 2002 Social behaviour in stallion groups (Equus przewalskii and Equus caballus) kept under natural and domestic conditions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76: 1120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condon, VM, McGreevy, PD, McLean, AN, Williams, JM and Randle, H 2022 Associations between commonly used apparatus and conflict behaviours reported in the ridden horse in Australia. Journal of Veterinary Behaviour 49: 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.10.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, JJ, McDonald, L and Mills, DS 2000 The effect of increasing visual horizons on stereotypic weaving: implications for the social housing of stabled horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 69: 6783. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00115-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Council on Animal Affairs 2021 Six guiding principles to ensure humane livestock farming. RDA Advisory Report on Humane Livestock Farming. https://english.rda.nl/publications/publications/2022/6/29/humane-livestock-farming (accessed 25 February 2023).Google Scholar
da Cruz, AB, Hirata, S, dos Santos, ME and Mendonça, RS 2023 Show me your best side: Lateralization of social and resting behaviors in feral horses. Behavioural Processes 206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2023.104839CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, MS 2004 Using behaviour to assess animal welfare. Animal Welfare 13: S3S7. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600014317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Oliveira, RA and Aurich, C 2021 Aspects of breeding stallion management with specific focus on animal welfare. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 107: 103733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2021.103773CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delank, K, Reese, S, Erhard, M and Wöhr, AC 2023 Behavioral and hormonal assessment of stress in foals (Equus caballus) throughout the weaning process. PLoS ONE 18: 116. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Destrez, A, Grimm, P, Cézilly, F and Julliand, V 2015 Changes of the hindgut microbiota due to high-starch diet can be associated with behavioral stress response in horses. Physiology and Behavior 149: 159164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodman, NH, Normile, JA, Cottam, N, Guzman, M and Shuster, L 2005 Prevalence of compulsive behaviors in formerly feral horses. International Journal of Applied Research in Veterinary Medicine 3: 2024.Google Scholar
Erber, R, Wulf, M, Rose-Meierhöfer, S, Becker-Birck, M, Möstl, E, Aurich, J, Hoffmann, G and Aurich, C 2012 Behavioral and physiological responses of young horses to different weaning protocols: A pilot study. Stress 15: 184194. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2011.606855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falomo, ME, Gabai, G, Franchini, G, Poltronieri, C, Rossi, M and Normando, S 2020 Behavioral and hormonal effects of 2 weaning methods in trotter mares. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 35: 4753. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2019.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farmer, K, Krüger, K, Byrne, RW and Marr, I 2018 Sensory laterality in affiliative interactions in domestic horses and ponies (Equus caballus). Animal Cognition 21: 631637. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1196-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, D 2008 Understanding animal welfare. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica 50: S1. https://doi:10.1186/1751-0147-50-S1-S1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, L 2012 The Horse’s Manifesto: What do we want?? Friends, Forage and Freedom! International Association of Animal Behavior Consultants. https://iaabc.org/horse/the-horses-manifesto-what-do-we-want-friends-forage-and-freedom-part-1-of-3 (accessed 29 July 2022).Google Scholar
Freymond, SB, Bardou, D, Beuret, S, Bachmann, I, Zuberbühler, K and Briefer, EF 2019 Elevated sensitivity to tactile stimuli in stereotypic horses. Frontiers in Veterinary Science. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2019.00162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furtado, T, Preshaw, L, Hockenhull, J, Wathan, J, Douglas, J, Horseman, S, Smith, R, Pollard, D, Pinchbeck, G, Rogers, J and Hall, C 2021 How happy are equine athletes? Stakeholder perceptions of equine welfare issues associated with equestrian sport. Animals 11: 3228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11113228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Górecka-Bruzda, A, Fureix, C, Ouvrard, A, Bourjade, M and Hausberger, M 2016 Investigating determinants of yawning in the domestic (Equus caballus) and Przewalski (Equus ferus przewalskii) horses. Science of Nature 103: 910. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-016-1395-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Górecka-Bruzda, A, Jastrzębska, E, Drewka, M, Nadolna, Z, Becker, K and Lansade, L 2022 Female horses are more socially dependent than geldings kept in riding clubs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Górecka-Bruzda, A, Suwała, M, Palme, R, Jaworski, Z, Jastrzebska, E, Boroń, M and Jezierski, T 2015 Events around weaning in semi-feral and stable-reared Konik polski foals: Evaluation of short-term physiological and behavioural responses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 163: 122134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greening, L, Downing, J, Amiouny, D, Lekang, L and McBride, S 2021 The effect of altering routine husbandry factors on sleep duration and memory consolidation in the horse. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 236: 105229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, CA, Goodwin, D, Heleski, C, Randle, H and Waran, N 2008 Is there evidence of learned helplessness in horses? Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 11: 249266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, C and Kay, R 2024 Living the good life? A systematic review of behavioural signs of affective state in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) and factors relating to quality of life. Part II: Horse-human interactions. Animal Welfare 33. https://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, C, Randle, H, Pearson, G, Preshaw, L and Waran, N 2018 Assessing equine emotional state. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 205: 183195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanis, F, Chung, ELT, Kamalludin, MH and Idrus, Z 2021 Do nutrient composition of feedstuffs affect the proportion of oral stereotypies and redirected behaviors among horse working groups? Journal of Veterinary Behavior 46: 714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.07.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harewood, EJ and McGowan, CM 2005 Behavioral and physiological responses to stabling in naive horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 25: 164170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2005.03.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartmann, E, Christensen, JW and Keeling, LJ 2011 Training young horses to social separation: Effect of a companion horse on training efficiency. Equine Veterinary Journal 43: 580584. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2042-3306.2010.00326.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hausberger, M, Fureix, C, Bourjade, M, Wessel-Robert, S and Richard-Yris, MA 2012 On the significance of adult play: What does social play tell us about adult horse welfare? Naturwissenschaften 99: 291302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-012-0902-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hausberger, M, Gautier, E, Biquand, V, Lunel, C and Jégo, P 2009 Could work be a source of behavioural disorders? A study in horses. PLoS ONE 4: 28. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007625CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heleski, CR 2023 Social license to operate – why public perception matters for horse sport – some personal reflections. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 124: 104266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2023.104266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, S, Fureix, C, Rowberry, R, Bateson, M and Hausberger, M 2017 Do horses with poor welfare show ‘pessimistic’ cognitive biases? Science of Nature 104: 12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-016-1429-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, S, Zanella, AJ, Sankey, C, Richard-Yris, MA, Marko, A and Hausberger, M 2012 Adults may be used to alleviate weaning stress in domestic foals (Equus caballus). Physiology and Behavior 106(4): 428438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.02.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hildebrandt, F, Büttner, K, Krieter, J and Czycholl, I 2022 The behavior of horses stabled in a large group at essential resources (watering point and lying halls). Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 118: 104106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2022.104106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintze, S and Schanz, L 2021 Using the judgment bias task to identify behavioral indicators of affective state: Do eye wrinkles in horses reflect mood? Frontiers in Veterinary Science 8: 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.676888CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, RM, Kronfel, DS, Holland, JL and Greiwe-Crandell, KM 1995 Preweaning diet and stall weaning method influences on stress response in foals. Journal of Animal Science 73(10): 29222930. https://doi.org/10.2527/1995.73102922xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horseman, SV, Buller, H, Mullan, S and Whay, HR 2016 Current welfare problems facing horses in Great Britain as identified by equine stakeholders. PLoS One 11(8): e0160269. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160269CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jørgensen, GHM, Borsheim, L, Mejdell, CM, Søndergaard, E and Bøe, KE 2009 Grouping horses according to gender — Effects on aggression, spacing and injuries. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 120: 9499. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.05.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kádár, R, Maros, K, Drégelyi, Z, Szedenik, Á, Lukácsi, A, Pesti, A, Besenyei, M and Egri, B 2023 Incidence of compulsive behavior (stereotypies/abnormal repetitive behaviors) in populations of sport and racehorses in Hungary. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 61: 3749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2023.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kieson, E, Goma, AA and Radi, M 2023 Tend and befriend in horses: Partner preferences, lateralization, and contextualization of allogrooming in two socially stable herds of Quarter Horse mares. Animals 13: 225. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13020225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkwood, JK 2007 Quality of life: The heart of the matter. Animal Welfare 16: 37. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600031663CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansade, L, Foury, A, Reigner, F, Vidament, M, Guettier, E, Bouvet, G, Soulet, D, Parias, C, Ruet, A, Mach, N, Lévy, F and Moisan, MP 2018 Progressive habituation to separation alleviates the negative effects of weaning in the mother and foal. Psychoneuroendocrinology 97: 5968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansade, L, Lemarchand, J, Reigner, F, Arnould, C and Bertin, A 2022 Automatic brushes induce positive emotions and foster positive social interactions in group-housed horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 246: 105538 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansade, L, Neveux, C and Levy, F 2012 A few days of social separation affects yearling horses’ response to emotional reactivity tests and enhances learning performance. Behavioural Processes 91: 94102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2012.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansade, L, Valenchon, M, Foury, A, Neveux, C, Cole, SW, Layé, S, Cardinaud, B, Lévy, F and Moisan, MP 2014 Behavioral and transcriptomic fingerprints of an enriched environment in horses (Equus caballus). PLoS One 9: 119. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114384CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesimple, C, Gautier, E, Benhajali, H, Rochais, C, Lunel, C, Bensaïd, S, Khalloufi, A, Henry, S and Hausberger, M 2019 Stall architecture influences horses’ behaviour and the prevalence and type of stereotypies. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 219: 104833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104833CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löckener, S, Reese, S, Erhard, M and Wöhr, AC 2016 Pasturing in herds after housing in horseboxes induces a positive cognitive bias in horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 11: 5055. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.11.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luz, MPF, Maia, CM, Pantoja, JCF, Neto, MC and Puoli Filho, JNP 2015 Feeding time and agonistic behavior in horses: Influence of distance, proportion, and height of troughs. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 35: 843848. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2015.08.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maglieri, V, Bigozzi, F, Riccobono, MG and Palagi, E 2020 Levelling playing field: synchronization and rapid facial mimicry in dog-horse play. Behavioural Processes 174: 104104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maigrot, AL, Hillmann, E, Anne, C and Briefer, EF 2017 Vocal expression of emotional valence in Przewalski’s horses (Equus przewalskii). Scientific Reports 7: 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09437-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maigrot, AL, Hillmann, E and Briefer, EF 2022 Cross-species discrimination of vocal expression of emotional valence by Equidae and Suidae. BMC Biology 20: 114. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12915-022-01311-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majecka, K and Klawe, A 2018 Influence of paddock size on social relationships in domestic horses. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 21: 816. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2017.1360773CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manrique, LP, Bánszegi, O, Hudson, R and Szenczi, P 2021 Repeatable individual differences in behaviour and physiology in juvenile horses from an early age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 235: 105227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marliani, G, Balboni, A, Tiberi, C, Malavasi, R, Gardini A and Accorsi PA 2022a Is the judgment bias test a good tool to assess the quality of horse management? Journal of Veterinary Behavior 58: 6269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marliani, G, Sprocatti, I, Schiavoni, G, Bellodi, A and Accorsi, PA 2021 Evaluation of horses’ daytime activity budget in a model of ethological stable: A case study in Italy. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 24: 200213. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2020.1857252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marliani, G, Vannucchi, I, Kiumurgis, I and Accorsi, PA 2022b Limitations of spatial judgment bias test application in horses (Equus ferus caballus). Animals 12: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12213014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, I, Farmer, K and Krüger, K 2018 Evidence for right-sided horses being more optimistic than left-sided horses. Animals 8: 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8120219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonnell, S 2003 The equid ethogram: A practical field guide to horse behavior. Lexington, US: Eclipse PressGoogle Scholar
McDonnell, SM and Haviland, JCS 1995 Agonistic ethogram of the equid bachelor band. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 43(3): 147188. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(94)00550-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGreevy, P, Berger, J, de Brauwere, N, Doherty, O, Harrison, A, Fiedler, J, Jones, C, McDonnell, S, McLean, A, Nakonechny, L, Nicol, C, Preshaw, L, Thomson, P, Tzioumis, V, Webster, J, Wolfensohn, S, Yeates, J and Jones, B 2018 Using the Five Domains model to assess the adverse impacts of husbandry, veterinary, and equitation interventions on horse welfare. Animals 8: 41. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8030041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuire, MC, Johnson-Ulrich, Z, Robeson, A, Zeigler-Hill, V and Vonk, J 2018 I say thee “neigh”: Rescued equids are optimistic in a judgment bias test. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 25: 8591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2018.03.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLean, AN and McGreevy, PD 2010 Ethical equitation: Capping the price horses pay for human glory. Journal of Veterinary Behaviour 5: 203209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2010.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, P 2022 Animal-based entertainment industries, animal death and Social Licence to Operate (SLO): an analysis of ‘The Final Race’ and the 2019 Melbourne Cup. Social & Cultural Geography 24: 12421261. https://doi.org/10.1080/14649365.2022.2053194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ and Beausoleil, NJ 2015 Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Animal Welfare 24: 241253. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.24.3.241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ, Beausoleil, NJ, Littlewood, KE, McLean, AN, McGreevy, PD, Jones, B and Wilkins, C 2020 The 2020 Five Domains model: Including human–animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals 10: 1870. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101870CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melvin, MV, Costello, E and Colpoys, JD 2020 Enclosed versus ring feeders: Effects of round-bale feeder type on horse behavior and welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 39: 4146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.07.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP and Paul, ES 2010 An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277: 28952904. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merkies, K, DuBois, C, Marshall, K, Parois, S, Graham, L and Haley, D 2016 A two-stage method to approach weaning stress in horses using a physical barrier to prevent nursing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 183: 6876. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.07.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moons, CPH, Laughlin, K and Zanella, AJ 2005 Effects of short-term maternal separations on weaning stress in foals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 91: 321335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.10.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicol, CJ, Badnell-Waters, AJ, Bice, R, Kelland, A, Wilson, AD and Harris, PA 2005 The effects of diet and weaning method on the behaviour of young horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 95: 205221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuñez, CMV, Adelman, JS, Smith, J, Gesquiere, LR and Rubenstein, DI 2014 Linking social environment and stress physiology in feral mares (Equus caballus): Group transfers elevate fecal cortisol levels. General and Comparative Endocrinology 196: 2633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2013.11.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, Shamseer, L, Tetzlaff, JM, Akl, EA, Brennan, SE, Chou, R, Glanville, J, Grimshaw, JM, Hróbjartsson, A, Lalu, MM, Li, T, Loder, EW, Mayo-Wilson, E, McDonald, S, McGuinness, LA, Stewart, LA, Thomas, J, Tricco, AC, Welch, VA, Whiting, P and Moher, D 2021 The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. British Medical Journal 372: n71. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71Google Scholar
Pérez Manrique, L, Hudson, R, Bánszegi, O and Szenczi, P 2019 Individual differences in behavior and heart rate variability across the preweaning period in the domestic horse in response to an ecologically relevant stressor. Physiology and Behavior 210: 112652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112652CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pessoa, GO, Trigo, P, Mesquita Neto, FD, Lacreta Junior, ACC, Sousa, TM, Muniz, JA and Moura, RS 2016 Comparative well-being of horses kept under total or partial confinement prior to employment for mounted patrols. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 184: 5158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.08.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierard, M, McGreevy, P and Geers, R 2019 Effect of density and relative aggressiveness on agonistic and affiliative interactions in a newly formed group of horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 29: 6169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2018.03.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pond, RL, Darre, MJ, Scheifele, PM and Browning, DG 2010 Characterization of equine vocalization. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 5: 712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2009.08.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popescu, S, Lazar, EA, Borda, C, Niculae, M, Sandru, CD and Spinu, M 2019 Welfare quality of breeding horses under different housing conditions. Animals 9: 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9030081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ribeiro, LB, Matzkeit, TV, Nicolau JTde, S, Castilha, LD, de Oliveira, FCL and Bankuti, FI 2019 Determinants of undesirable behaviors in American Quarter Horses housed in box stalls. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 80: 6975. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2019.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, CW, Walsh, V, Gee, EK and Firth, EC 2012 A preliminary investigation of the use of a foal image to reduce mare stress during mare-foal separation. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 7: 4954. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2011.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rørvang, MV and Christensen, JW 2018 Attenuation of fear through social transmission in groups of same and differently aged horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 209: 4146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruet, A, Biau, S, Arnould, C, Galloux, P, Destrez, A, Pycik, E, Boichot, L and Lansade, L 2020 Horses could perceive riding differently depending on the way they express poor welfare in the stable. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 94: 103206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2020.103206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruet, A, Lemarchand, J, Parias, C, Mach, N, Moisan, MP, Foury, A, Briant, C and Lansade, L 2019 Housing horses in individual boxes is a challenge with regard to welfare. Animals 9: 119. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarrafchi, A and Blokhuis, HJ 2013 Equine stereotypic behaviors: Causation, occurrence, and prevention. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 8: 386394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2013.04.068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sauveroche, M, Henriksson, J, Theodorsson, E, Svensson Holm, AC and Roth, LSV 2020 Hair cortisol in horses (Equus caballus) in relation to management regimes, personality, and breed. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 37: 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2019.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatzmann, U 1998 Winter pasturing of sport horses in Switzerland. Equine Clinical Behaviour. Equine Veterinary Journal Supplement 27(3): 5354.Google Scholar
Sénèque, E, Lesimple, C, Morisset, S and Hausberger, M 2019 Could posture reflect welfare state? A study using geometric morphometrics in riding school horses. PLoS One 14: 120. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squibb, K, Griffin, K, Favier, R and Ijichi, C 2018 Poker Face: Discrepancies in behaviour and affective states in horses during stressful handling procedures. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 202: 3438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stachurska, A, Kędzierski, W, Kaczmarek, B, Wiśniewska, A, Żylińska, B and Janczarek, I 2023 Variation of physiological and behavioural parameters during the oestrous cycle in mares. Animals 13(2): 211. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13020211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stomp, M, Leroux, M, Cellier, M, Henry, S, Hausberger, M and Lemasson, A 2018b Snort acoustic structure codes for positive emotions in horses. Science of Nature 105: 910. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-018-1582-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stomp, M, Leroux, M, Cellier, M, Henry, S, Lemasson, A and Hausberger, M 2018a An unexpected acoustic indicator of positive emotions in horses. PLoS One 13(7): 123. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197898CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand, SC, Tiefenbacher, S, Haskell, M, Hosmer, T, McDonnell, SM and Freeman, DA 2002 Behavior and physiologic responses of mares to short-term isolation. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 78: 145157. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00106-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suagee-Bedore, JK, Linden, DR and Bennett-Wimbush, K 2021 Effect of pen size on stress responses of stall-housed horses receiving one hour of daily turnout. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 98: 103366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2020.103366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundman, ER, Goodwin, JD, Reisinger, CN, Smith, MR, Costello, E, Walter, K and Colpoys, JD 2022 Short communication: Round bale hay net effects on agonistic behaviors of group fed horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 55–56: 5862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.07.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torcivia, C and McDonnell, S 2021 Equine discomfort ethogram. Animals 11: 580. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visser, EK, Ellis, AD and Van Reenen, CG 2008 The effect of two different housing conditions on the welfare of young horses stabled for the first time. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 114(3–4): 521533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.03.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waran, N 2023 Equine Ethics and Wellbeing Commission Interim Report to the FEI Sports Forum April 2023. https://equinewellbeing.fei.org/key-documents.html (Accessed 12 July 2023).Google Scholar
Waran, NK 1997 Can studies of feral horse behaviour be used for assessing domestic horse welfare? Equine Veterinary Journal 29(4): 249251. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2042-3306.1997.tb03117.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waring, GH 2003 Horse Behavior, Second Edition. Noyes Publications/William Andrew Publishing: Norwich, NY, USAGoogle Scholar
Waters, AJ, Nicol, CJ and French, NP 2002 Factors influencing the development of stereotypic and redirected behaviours in young horses: Findings of a four-year prospective epidemiological study. Equine Veterinary Journal 34(6): 572579. https://doi.org/10.2746/042516402776180241CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wathan, J, Burrows, AM, Waller, BM and McComb, K 2015 EquiFACS: The Equine Facial Action Coding System. PLoS One 10(9): e0137818https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wathan, J, Proops, L, Grounds, K and McComb, K 2016 Horses discriminate between facial expressions of conspecifics. Scientific Reports 6: 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep38322CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, J 2013 International standards for farm animal welfare: science and values. The Veterinary Journal 198: 34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2013.08.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werhahn, H, Hessel, EF and Van den Weghe, HFA 2012 Competition horses housed in single stalls (II): Effects of free exercise on the behavior in the stable, the behavior during training, and the degree of stress. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 32(1): 2231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jevs.2011.06.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wulf, M, Beythien, E, Ille, N, Aurich, J and Aurich, C 2018 The stress response of 6-month-old horses to abrupt weaning is influenced by their sex. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 23: 1924. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.10.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yarnell, K, Hall, C, Royle, C and Walker, SL 2015 Domesticated horses differ in their behavioural and physiological responses to isolated and group housing. Physiology and Behavior 143: 5157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, J 2016 Quality of life and animal behaviour. Applied Animal Welfare Science 181: 1926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.04.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
York, CA and Schulte, BA 2014 The relationship of dominance, reproductive state and stress in female horses (Equus caballus). Behavioural Processes 107: 1521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, T, Creighton, E, Smith, T and Hosie, C 2012 A novel scale of behavioural indicators of stress for use with domestic horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 140(1): 3343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.05.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuluaga, AM, Mira, A, Sánchez, JL and Martínez, AJR 2018 Frequency of abnormal and stereotypic behaviors in urban police patrolling horses: A continuous 48-hour study. Revista Colombiana de Ciencias Pecuarias 31(1): 1725. https://doi.org/10.17533/udea.rccp.v31n1a03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Systematic literature search procedure, including the number of articles identified at each stage of the review, and the total number of articles retained.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Number of publications identified under each search term (Search 1 and Search 2) for each four-year period (the period 2020–2023 included three years and two months only).

Figure 2

Table 1. Study scenarios relating to the fulfilment of species-specific needs identified in the literature search, the related table in the Supplementary material and the number of articles retained per scenario. The scenarios are listed in descending order according to the number of articles retained

Figure 3

Table 2. (a) Behaviour in the home environment indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 4

Table 3. (a) Intra-species social behaviour and communication indicative of affective state (positive or negative), supporting evidence*, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour

Figure 5

Table 4. (a) Mare and foal behaviour following physical separation at weaning, and (b) factors affecting this behaviour. The process of weaning is assumed to be a negative/stressful experience for both mare and foal. Behavioural responses of mare and foal observed immediately post-weaning and factors found to affect this behaviour are shown below. Where physiological measures have been reported this is indicated by PHYS

Figure 6

Figure 3. Amicable sharing of resources such as water is a positive sign of group cohesion and well-being in domestic horses.

Supplementary material: File

Hall and Kay supplementary material

Hall and Kay supplementary material
Download Hall and Kay supplementary material(File)
File 63.1 KB