Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T02:45:10.735Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are dieting-related cognitive impairments a function of iron status?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2012

Michael W. Green*
Affiliation:
Aston Nutrition and Behaviour Laboratory, Psychology Department, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Aston Triangle, Birmingham, B4 7ET, UK Psychology Department, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Aston Triangle, BirminghamB4 7ET, UK
Nicola A. Elliman
Affiliation:
Aston Nutrition and Behaviour Laboratory, Psychology Department, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Aston Triangle, Birmingham, B4 7ET, UK
*
*Corresponding author: M. W. Green, fax +44 121 204 4090, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether the impairments in cognitive function observed in unsupported dieting are related to compromised Fe status. During a non-clinical intervention, overweight participants (age: 18–45 years, BMI: 25–30 kg/m2) either participated in a commercially available weight-loss regimen (n 14), dieted without support (n 17) or acted as a non-dieting control group (n 14) for a period of 8 weeks. Measurements of cognitive function and blood chemistry were taken at a pre-diet baseline, after 1 week and 8 weeks of dieting. After 1 week, unsupported dieters displayed impaired verbal memory, executive function and slower reaction speeds than the other two groups, this difference disappearing by the end of the study. There were no significant group-related changes in blood chemistry over the course of the study, although there were group-related changes in a number of self-reported food-related cognitions. In conclusion, impaired cognition among unsupported dieters is not due to compromised Fe status and is most likely to result from psychological variables.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Deliberate restriction of food intake to control weight is one of the most common food choice-related behaviour in the developed world. Indeed, over one-third of the general US population is classed as obese(1), with an associated impact on health treatment resources(Reference Bray2). Within the context of these rising obesity rates(3), an understanding of the psychological impact of undernutrition and weight loss assumes an increasing importance.

While there are undoubted health benefits of controlling obesity, the attempt to lose weight by dieting has been associated with a number of negative psychological consequences such as increased depression and low self-esteem(Reference Warren and Cooper4). Although the evidence concerning the psychological effects of experimentally induced weight loss is equivocal(Reference Green5), evidence suggests that dieting is reliably associated with a range of impairments in cognitive function. For instance, dieting has been associated with slower reaction speed, impaired verbal memory and an impaired ability to sustain attention(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6, Reference Green and Rogers7). A number of studies have indicated that this pattern of impaired task performance is primarily due to a reduction in available working memory capacity(Reference Green, Elliman and Rogers8Reference Shaw and Tiggemann11), which is primarily due to preoccupying cognitions relating to hunger(Reference Green, Elliman and Rogers8) and body shape-related self-esteem(Reference Green and Rogers12, Reference Vreugdenburg, Bryan and Kemps13). This is important, since working memory is the primary cognitive domain, controlling moment-to-moment rules of action, planning ability and the short-term retention of information. The most commonly accepted model of working memory(Reference Baddeley14) proposes a tripartite system comprising a phonological loop (controlling verbal short-term memory), a visuospatial sketchpad (controlling visual memory) and an overarching supervisory system known as the ‘central executive’.

It has also been found that the way in which weight loss is attempted influences cognitive function, with dieting without support being associated with cognitive impairments and increased stress levels (as measured by salivary cortisol secretion) and dieting within the context of an organised weight-loss group not being associated with impaired cognition(Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15). The present study employed a substantively similar design.

Although the evidence suggests that these negative cognitive consequences of dieting are due to the psychological mechanism noted above (this being supported by the finding of impaired task performance in the absence of any weight loss over the course of the diet(Reference Green and Rogers7)), there are a number of possible subtle physiological mechanisms which may mediate the effect. It is, therefore, important to investigate the role of these mechanisms in mediating the cognitive impairments associated with spontaneous, unsupervised dieting. It has been shown that total energy restriction per se for periods of up to 24 h(Reference Green, Elliman and Rogers16) or partial energy restriction in a laboratory-based weight-loss programme(Reference Kretsch, Green and Fong17) does not significantly impair task performance. It has also been demonstrated that dieting-related cognitive impairments cannot be attributed to compromised glucose metabolism(Reference Green, Elliman and Rogers18). It is also a replicable finding that experimentally controlled food deprivation leads to a reduction in plasma levels of the amino acid tryptophan, which is the precursor for the neurotransmitter serotonin (5-hydroxy tryptophan; 5-HT)(Reference Anderson, Parry-Billings and Newsholme19). There is an argument, therefore, to conclude that the impairments in cognition observed in naturalistic dieting are the result of low dietary intakes of tryptophan and, by implication, lower levels of 5-HT synthesis. However, it has been demonstrated that the impairments in cognitive task performance characteristic of unsupported spontaneous dieting are unrelated to 5-HT function(Reference Green, Jones and Smith20).

There is, however, a body of evidence suggesting that Fe metabolism may be a factor in the impaired cognitive function associated with dieting. Fe deficiency is commonly associated with impaired cognitive function(Reference Kretsch, Blanton and Green21). In addition, very-low-energy diets have been associated with acute functional Fe deficiency and impairments in cognition(Reference Beard, Borel and Peterson22). Using a substantively similar battery of cognitive performance measures as employed in the present study, Kretsch et al. (Reference Kretsch, Fong and Green23) found that, after 15 weeks of laboratory-controlled energy restriction, there were significant relationships between a number of blood Fe factors and task performance. Specifically, after 15 weeks of weight loss, performance on a task with a high working memory load was correlated with both Hb and transferrin saturation. This indicates that alterations in blood Fe metabolism during controlled weight loss may, at least, partially explain the observed impairments in cognitive function.

The aims of the present study were twofold. First, the study investigated whether the impairments in working memory capacity observed in unsupported v. supported naturalistic dieting are a replicable phenomenon. Second, the study investigated whether any observed impairments could be attributed to alterations in Fe metabolism resulting from attempted weight loss.

Methods

Participants

Participants comprised healthy, pre-menopausal, overweight women (BMI between 25 and 29 kg/m2), aged 20–45 years with normal or corrected to normal visual acuity and English as their first language, recruited via newspaper advertisement asking for individuals to take part in a study examining the relationship between weight status and psychological function from the Birmingham UK area. Potential participants (n 252) completed an initial telephone health screening and, on the basis of this, a postal health screening questionnaire; seventy-five participants attended their baseline session. The screening procedure was designed to exclude participants with physical or psychiatric health problems, smokers, heavy drinkers, those taking dietary supplements, oral contraceptive users, and those currently pregnant or lactating. Written consent was obtained at a pre-baseline screening session after the experimental procedures were explained to all participants. During this pre-baseline session, height and body weight were measured and self-report measures of affective state (the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)(Reference Snaith and Zigmnd24)) were completed to ensure no group baseline differences in affective state (P>0·05). In addition, all participants completed the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ)(Reference Van Strien, Frijters and Bergers25) in order to assess habitual eating behaviour. After the screening procedure, participants were randomised to one of three groups; these being supported dieters, unsupported dieters and a non-dieting control group. The final sample sizes for each group reflect dropout rates, which did not significantly differ between the groups (P>0·05). Only the data for participants who completed the whole 8 weeks of the study are included.

Design and procedure

The present study comprised a randomised, 8-week-long controlled trial in which the effects of supported v. unsupported dieting on cognitive function Fe status were investigated. All participants attended three test sessions; these being a baseline session, a second session 1 week later and a third session 8 weeks post-baseline. In one group, participants attended a commercially available organised weight-loss group for this period. This commercial weight-loss plan comprised an energy-restricted, nutritionally balanced diet in conjunction with weekly weighing and group support sessions. No foods were provided to the participants. In the second group, participants were asked to pursue any diet plan they chose for 8 weeks, provided that the diet chosen did not entail weight loss as part of an organised group. The diets chosen ranged from nutritionally balanced, energy-restricted diets, low-fat diets and low-carbohydrate diets. The final group acted as non-dieting controls. Participants were randomised to one of the three groups on a rolling basis upon initial recruitment, with the intention of a final group of twenty-five. The final group sizes are the result of participant dropout after attendance at the baseline test session and failure to obtain blood samples on all sessions. Final group sizes reflect the number of participants who completed all three test sessions. Ethical approval for the study was given by the Aston University Human Research Ethics Committee.

Upon arrival at the laboratory for each session, participants were weighed in underwear and without shoes and their percentage of body fat determined using the foot-to-foot bio-impedance/body composition analyser (In-Body 220™; Derwent Healthcare). This was completed behind a screen and participants were not informed of their results until the final debriefing. Height was initially measured using a wall-mounted stadiometer (being used in conjunction with weight to provide data for BMI calculations). Each participant then donated a venous blood sample and then completed a computerised battery of neuro-psychological function tasks. These comprised a measure of vigilance performance (a visual analogue of the Bakan Vigilance Task(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6, Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15)), a measure of immediate verbal recall(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6, Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15), a measure of Simple Reaction Time(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6, Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15), a measure of motor speed(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6, Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15) (Two-Finger Tapping performance), a Mental Rotation Task(Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green and Rogers12, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15, Reference Beard, Borel and Peterson22) (week 1 test session only) and a manual, non-computerised version of the Tower of London Task(Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green and Rogers12, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15, Reference Kretsch, Blanton and Green21). The Tower of London Task was only administered during the second test session due to its widely known susceptibility to practice effects(Reference Beauchamp, Dagher and Aston26) and the previously demonstrated finding that the effects of dieting on cognition are most marked in the first 2 weeks of attempted weight loss(Reference Green and Rogers7). While not comprising a formalised neuro-psychological performance battery validated among clinical populations, these were tasks which have been previously demonstrated to show sensitivity to nutritional manipulations and dieting status, in particular. Given the hypotheses outlined concerning the effects of dieting status on specific components of the working memory system and their previously demonstrated ability to illustrate these effects(Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green, Jones and Smith20), it was judged that they were appropriate measures in order to ensure comparability with past findings.

Cognitive assessment battery

The tasks were presented to subjects via a personal computer running MEL version 2 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.) with a 13″ colour monitor. The tasks were presented in the same order during each session and are described below.

Two-Finger Tapping Task (motor speed)

Subjects were required to alternately tap the ‘1’ and ‘2’ keys of the numeric keypad as quickly as possible, using the first and second fingers of the preferred hand. The dependent variable was the time taken to make 300 key presses.

Verbal Free Recall Task (phonological loop)

Subjects were presented with two lists of twenty words each, one list being presented at 1 s/word and the other at 2 s/word. Immediately after the presentation of each list, subjects were given 4 min to recall as many words as possible from that list.

Bakan Vigilance Task (sustained attention and general working memory)

This was a 6 min long, visual analogue of the procedure developed by Bakan(Reference Bakan27) in which subjects were presented with a continuous stream of single digits(1Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Marshall9) which appeared one at a time, in the centre of the visual display unit. The subjects' task was to press a labelled response key, as quickly as possible, when they believed that they had detected a sequence of either three odd numbers or three even numbers occurring consecutively. The stimulus exposure duration was 600 ms, with no inter-stimulus interval and forty-eight potential correct targets. The dependent variable was the number of correct hits made over the course of the task.

Mental Rotation Task (visuospatial sketchpad)

This task assessed the capacity of the visuospatial sketchpad component of the working memory system. Subjects were presented with a number of pairs of random geometric forms, each comprising the eight- and twelve-point shaped used by Cooper(Reference Cooper28). Each pair of forms was presented simultaneously, positioned in the centre of two circles. The participants' task was to mentally rotate the form on the right-hand side of the screen until they believed it to be in the same orientation as the form on the left-hand side of the screen. They then had to decide whether they believed the form on the right-hand side of the screen to be a duplicate, or a mirrored image of the form on the left. They were instructed to respond by pressing the 1 key on the numeric keypad if they believed the two images to be duplicates and the 2 key if they believed them to be mirror images of each other. The right-hand forms either required a 0, 60, 120, 180, 240 or 300° clockwise mental rotation. In total, there were ninety-six trials, these comprising eight trials each of the two forms, in each of the possible rotation positions.

Simple Reaction Time (reaction speed)

In this task, subjects were presented (via a computer visual display unit), with 100 reaction time trials (divided into five blocks of twenty trials each for the purposes of analysis) with instructions to hit the space bar, as quickly as possible, as soon as they detected a single star appearing in the centre of the monitor. There was a variable stimulus onset of either 1, 3, 7 or 16 s, in order to prevent subjects anticipating the appearance of the stimulus on screen.

The Tower of London Task (central executive)

This task is considered to be one of the primary means of assessing the capacity of the Central Executive component of working memory(Reference Shallice29, Reference Krikorian, Bartok and Gay30). In the present study, participants were required to complete a manual version of this task. The task comprised a three-peg board with three moveable wooden discs, one green, one white and one black. The participants' task was to move the discs from a standard start position to a target position in a set minimum number of moves. There were two target positions that could be achieved in a minimum of two moves, two that could be achieved in three moves, four that could be achieved in four moves and four that could be achieved in a minimum of five moves. Administration of the test followed the standardised procedure outlined by Krikorian et al. (Reference Krikorian, Bartok and Gay30). The battery was designed to assess the capacity and function of the three components of working memory and psychomotor speed.

Also, participants recorded a 3 d estimated food record, including one weekend day and two weekdays, during the week before baseline and again before each subsequent test session. Food records were coded and analysed for energy intake (kJ) and using the Weighed Intake Software Program version 2.0 (Tinuviel Software). In addition, participants completed 100 mm visual analogue scales of somatic sensations immediately before cognitive testing on each session, these measuring ‘hunger’, ‘thirst’, ‘desire to eat’ and ‘fullness’(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6Reference Green, Elliman and Rogers8). These were anchored with ‘not at all’ at the zero point and ‘extremely’ at the other end of the scale, participants being instructed to indicate how they felt ‘at the moment’.

Blood collections

Fasting venous blood samples (20 ml) were drawn from the antecubital vein using Vacuette™ (Greiner Bio-One). Blood samples for Zn analysis were drawn into Sarstedt syringes (Sarstedt, Inc.) containing ammonium heparin-coated beads (15 μl heparin/ml blood), placed on ice for no longer than 60 min, and centrifuged in a Heraeus Labofuge 300 centrifuge (Kendro Laboratory Products GmbH) for 10 min at 2400 rpm (1145 g). Plasma was transferred using polypropylene transfer pipettes into new polypropylene tubes. Samples were stored at − 70°C until analysed.

Haematology and iron analyses

Haematological analyses were performed at the City Hospital Pathology Laboratory in Birmingham, West Midlands using a Beckman-Coulter Access 2 Analyzer (chemiluminescence assays) (Beckman Coulter Inc.) and included Hb, haematocrit, erythrocyte count, mean cell volume, mean corpuscular Hb and mean corpuscular Hb concentration. Fe status parameters included serum ferritin, serum Fe, percentage of transferrin saturation and total Fe binding capacity. Serum ferritin was determined using a chemiluminescent system (Dimension Vista 500; Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics). Serum Fe, serum transferrin and unsaturated Fe binding capacity were analysed colorimetrically using the Hitachi 902 Clinical Chemistry Analyzer (Roche Diagnostics).

Vitamin B12 analysis

Vitamin B12 levels were analysed since it is an important cofactor in the synthesis of Hb. Levels were determined using Advia 120's (Bayer Healthcare).

The present study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures were approved by the Aston University Human Research Ethics Committee. Written consent was obtained during the pre-trial screening session.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 16.0 (SPSS, Inc.). Unless otherwise stated, the values in the text and tables are presented as means and standard deviations and the significance criterion used is P < 0·05. Post hoc analysis was completed using Fisher's least significant difference test, with Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons.

Results

Baseline data

The baseline screening data are shown in Table 1. One-way ANOVA, with participant group as the factor, revealed no significant group-related differences in HADS scores or DEBQ subscale scores at the outset of the study (P>0·05).

Table 1 Baseline Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) scores for all three groups (Mean values and standard deviations)

Anthropometric data

The BMI, percentage of body fat and energy intake data for each session are shown in Table 2. All data were analysed via two-way ANOVA (with participant group and test session as factors).

Table 2 BMI, energy (kJ), percentage of body fat and macronutrient (fat, protein and carbohydrate) intake for all participants over each test session (Mean values and standard deviations)

Mean values were significantly different from baseline: * P <0·05, ** P <0·01.

BMI

There were no significant main effects or interactions involving BMI (P>0·05).

Percentage of body fat

There was a significant interaction between participant group and test session (F (4,82) = 3·415, P =0·012). Post hoc analysis revealed that the supported dieters experienced a decrease in body fat between the first and second sessions, but had regained this by the time of the final session (P <0·05). The unsupported dieters experienced a reduction in body fat between the baseline and final sessions (P <0·05). There were no significant changes in percentage of body fat for the control group over the course of the study (P>0·05).

Energy intake

There was a main effect of test session, indicating that all three participant groups reported a reduction in energy intake (F (2,41) = 6·893, P =0·003). There were no other significant main effects or interactions (P>0·05).

Iron and vitamin B12 status

The results of the blood sample analysis are shown in Table 3. All blood results were within normal parameters.

Table 3 Blood iron and vitamin B12 status for all participants over each test session (Mean values and standard deviations)

RBC, erythrocytes; MCV, mean cell volume; MCH, mean corpuscular Hb; MCHC, mean corpuscular Hb concentration; TIBC, total Fe binding capacity.

* Mean values were significantly different from baseline (P <0·05).

Hb

There was no significant group × session interaction (F (2,82) = 0·395, P>0·05), although there was a significant main effect of session (F (2,41) = 3·571, P =0·032), with levels being higher in the second session than in either the baseline or final session. There were no other significant main effects or interactions (P>0·05).

Mean corpuscular Hb

There was a significant interaction between test session and group (F (2,82) = 2·629, P =0·040). Post hoc analysis revealed that the unsupported dieters exhibited a significant decrease in mean corpuscular Hb over the course of the study, whereas the other two groups did not (P <0·05). There were no significant main effects (P>0·05).

Erythrocyte count

There was no significant group × session interaction (F (2,82) = 1·827, P>0·05), although there was a significant main effect of session (F (2,41) = 4·605, P =0·016), with levels being higher in the second session than in either the baseline or final session. There were no other significant main effects or interactions (P>0·05).

There were no other significant main effects or interactions for any of the other blood Fe, haematological or vitamin variables (P>0·05).

Cognitive function

The data for the Bakan Vigilance, Verbal Free Recall, Two-Finger Tapping, Mental Rotation and SRT tasks are shown in Table 4.

Table 4 Bakan Vigilance Task, Verbal Free Recall, Two-Finger Tapping, Mental Rotation and Simple Reaction Time (SRT) data (Mean values and standard deviations)

Mean values were significantly different from baseline: * P <0·05, ** P <0·01, *** P <0·001.

Bakan Vigilance Task

There was a significant interaction between test session and participant group (F (4,82) = 5·122, P = 0·001). Post hoc analyses indicated that, for the unsupported dieting group, significantly fewer correct hits were made after 1 week of attempted dieting, than either before or after (P <0·01). There was also a significant main effect of time on task (F (5,38) = 28·86, P <0·001), indicating a general decline in performance over the course of each session, consistent with the classically observed vigilance decrement(Reference Cooper28). There was also a significant main effect of session (F (2,41) = 7·245, P =0·002), indicating a general practice effect, with performance overall being better on the final session than at baseline.

Immediate Verbal Recall Task

There was a significant interaction between test session and participant group (F (4,82) = 6·368, P <0·001). Post hoc analysis revealed that, for the unsupported dieting group only, performance was poorer during the second test session than during either the baseline or final test sessions (P <0·01). There was also a significant main effect of presentation speed (F (1,42) = 22·808, P <0·001), with recall being significantly better in the 2 s/word presentation speed than the 1 s/word speed. There were no other significant main effects or interactions (P>0·05).

Two-Finger Tapping Task

There were no significant main effects or interactions for tapping performance (P>0·05).

Mental Rotation Task

There was a significant main effect of test session (F (2,41) = 7·013, P =0·002), with times taken by all participant groups to make the same/mirror image decision improving with each test session, consistent with a practice effect. There was also a significant main effect of angle of rotation (F (5,38) = 9·513, P <0·001), with decision times increasing with angle of mental rotation required to make the same/mirror image decision. There were no other significant main effects or interactions (P>0·05).

Simple Reaction Time Task

There was a significant three-way interaction between test session, participant group and trial block (F (16,72) = 2·041, P =0·022). Post hoc analysis revealed that, while reaction times generally increased over the course of each test session (consistent with a fatigue effect), this effect was significantly larger for the unsupported dieters in the second test session (P <0·05). There were also significant main effects of test session and trial block (P <0·01).

Tower of London Task

The planning time data for the Tower of London Task are shown in Fig. 1. There was a significant interaction between participant group and problem difficulty (F (6,80) = 2·517, P =0·028). Post hoc analysis revealed that the unsupported dieters had significantly longer planning times for the four- and five-move problems than either the supported dieters or non-dieting control participants (P <0·05). There were also significant main effects of participant group and problem difficulty (P <0·01).

Fig. 1 Mean planning times on the Tower of London Task for all three participant groups. Values are means, with their standard errors represented by vertical bars. *Mean values were significantly different from those of the other two participant groups (P <0·05). , Supervised dieters; , unsupervised dieters; , non-dieting controls.

Somatic Sensations Questionnaire

The results of the Somatic Sensations Questionnaire are shown in Table 5.

Table 5 Somatic Sensations Questionnaire data (mm) (Mean values and standard deviations)

** Mean values were significantly different from baseline (P <0·01).

Hunger

There was a significant interaction between participant group and test session (F (4,82) = 4·678, P =0·002). Post hoc analyses revealed that the unsupported dieters displayed a significantly greater degree of self-reported hunger in the second test session than either at baseline or the final test session (P <0·01). There were also significant main effects of participant group (P <0·05) and test session (P <0·001).

Desire to eat

There was a significant interaction between participant group and test session (F (4,82) = 7·415, P <0·001). Post hoc analysis revealed a significantly higher desire to eat among the unsupported dieters after 1 week of attempted weight loss (P <0·01) than during the other sessions. Desire to eat did not significantly change across the course of the studies for either of the other two test groups (P>0·05). There were also significant main effects of participant group and test session (P <0·001).

Fullness

There was a significant main effect of participant group (F (2,42) = 9·933, P =0·027). Post hoc analyses showed that the unsupervised dieting group displayed lower degrees of self-reported fullness during each test session (P>0·05). There was also a significant main effect of test session (F (2,41) = 6·063, P =0·005). Post hoc analyses revealed that, overall, fullness ratings were lower on the second test session than at baseline or the final session (P <0·05). There was no significant interaction between the two variables analysed (P>0·05).

Thirst

There were no significant main effects or interactions involving self-reported thirst (P>0·05).

Relationships between task performance and other variables

Pearson's product moment correlations (with appropriate Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons) were carried out between the cognitive function variables which were shown to be sensitive to the experimental manipulation and the self-report/blood analysis variables, separately for each participant group. No significant correlations were found between any task performance variables and haematological/vitamin status or anthropomorphic/somatic sensation data (P>0·05).

Discussion

The main finding of the present study was the replication of the impairment in cognitive function among unsupported dieters, relative to supported dieters or non-dieting controls, within the first week of attempted weight loss(Reference Green and Rogers7, Reference Green, Elliman and Kretsch15). Primarily, the impairments appear to be in tasks which assess the capacity of the central executive and phonological loop components of working memory(Reference Green and Rogers12), although simple reaction speed also appeared to be impaired in unsupported dieters during the early stages of weight loss(Reference Green, Rogers and Elliman6). The second main finding of the present study was that, although there were some group-specific changes in blood chemistry over the course of the study, none of these was specifically related to changes in cognitive function. In addition, changes in energy intake, BMI and percentage of body fat were unrelated to changes in cognitive function, as were changes in somatic sensations relating to eating (e.g. self-reported ‘hunger’ or ‘desire to eat’). This latter finding does not support previous findings(Reference Green, Elliman and Rogers8, Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Marshall9, Reference Green and Rogers12, Reference Vreugdenburg, Bryan and Kemps13), which have indicated that the primary cause of dieting-related impairment in cognitive function is the result of preoccupying cognitions relating to food preferentially consuming limited working memory capacity. The present data do not, however, preclude the possibility that the observed effects of dieting on cognitive function are the result of preoccupying cognitions relating to body shape or self-esteem(Reference Shaw and Tiggemann11Reference Vreugdenburg, Bryan and Kemps13). Any future attempt at replication of the present study should include a measure of body shape-related self-esteem such as the Body Attitudes Questionnaire(Reference Krikorian, Bartok and Gay30). The finding of no relationship between task performance and BMI, percentage of body fat and energy intake is consistent with other findings(Reference Green and Rogers7) and is suggestive of the dieting-related cognitive impairments being due to psychological, rather than physiological, factors. This conclusion is further supported by the finding that impaired task performance was only observed among the unsupported dieters and only during the first week of attempted weight loss, with performance returning to levels comparable to those of the other two participant groups by the final test session.

The main significant finding relating to Fe status was that participants in the unsupervised dieting group experienced a decrease in mean corpuscular Hb over the course of the study which was not found in the other two groups. This was, however, unrelated to cognitive function, weight or energy intake and, during each test session, was still within the normal healthy range. This is in contrast to studies which have demonstrated that very-low-energy, experimentally controlled weight loss induces negative changes in a number of Fe/haematological variables and that these are related to relative impairments in cognitive function(Reference Beard, Borel and Peterson22, Reference Kretsch, Fong and Green23). The present study demonstrates that the cognitive impairments associated with non-laboratory-based, real-world, unsupported dieting are not the result of compromised Fe metabolism. Reasons for the difference in cognitive function between the two groups after 1 week of attempted weight loss are unclear. Potential reasons may include the use of structured diet plans, group support and regular weighing in the presence of others in the supported diet group. These should be the subject of further research. The results also showed that all three participant groups reported progressively smaller energy intakes over the course of the study, but with no corresponding change in BMI. A possible reason for this apparently contradictory finding lies in reliance on the accuracy of self-reported energy intake, which has shown itself to not be completely accurate. For instance, a number of studies have found serious under-reporting biases with such dietary records(Reference Ben-Tovim and Walker31Reference Krall and Dwyer33). Future attempts at verifying the present study should attempt to verify the accuracy of self-reported energy intake using the doubly labelled water technique.

In conclusion, the present data highlight the need to make a clear distinction between the effects of laboratory-induced food restriction and dieting behaviour outside of the laboratory(Reference Green5). While the two forms of energy restriction appear to exert a negative effect on brain function, it is by no means clear that the causal mechanisms of these impairments are the same in both situations.

Acknowledgements

The authors would gratefully like to acknowledge the assistance of Dr Stuart Williams, of the Department of Pathology, City Hospital Birmingham with the analysis of the blood samples obtained during the course of this study. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Both authors contributed equally to the screening data collection and analysis. M. W. G. prepared the manuscript. The authors confirm that there is no conflict of interest.

References

1National Center for Health Statistics (2006) Chartbook on Trends in the Health of Americans Health, United States, 2006. Hyattsville, MD: Public Health Service.Google Scholar
2Bray, G (1996) Health hazards of obesity. Endocrin Metab Clin 25, 907919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Department of Health (2008) Healthy Weight, Healthy Lives: A Cross Government Strategy for England (2008). London: UK Government.Google Scholar
4Warren, C & Cooper, PJ (1988) Psychological effects of dieting. Br J Clin Psych 71, 247261.Google Scholar
5Green, MW (2010) Cognitive performance deficits of dieters. In The International Handbook of Behaviour, Diet and Nutrition [, editors]. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
6Green, MW, Rogers, PJ, Elliman, NA, et al. (1994) Impairment of cognitive processing associated with dieting and high levels of dietary restraint. Physiol Behav 55, 447452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Green, MW & Rogers, PJ (1995) Impaired cognitive function during spontaneous dieting. Psychol Med 25, 10031010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Green, MW, Elliman, N & Rogers, PJ (1997) Impaired cognitive processing in dieters: failure of attentional focus or resource capacity limitation? Br J Health Psych 2, 259267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M & Marshall, K (2005) Relationship between dieting to lose weight and the functioning of the central executive. Appetite 45, 287294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Kemps, E & Tiggemann, M (2005) Working memory performance and preoccupying thoughts in female dieters: evidence for a selective central executive impairment. Br J Clin Psych 44, 357366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Shaw, J & Tiggemann, M (2004) Dieting and working memory: preoccupying cognitions and the role of the articulatory control process. Br J Health Psych 9, 179185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Green, MW & Rogers, PJ (1998) Impairments in working memory associated with spontaneous dieting behaviour. Psychol Med 28, 10631070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Vreugdenburg, L, Bryan, J & Kemps, E (2003) The effect of self-initiated weight-loss dieting on working memory: the role of preoccupying cognitions. Appetite 41, 291300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14Baddeley, AD (1986) Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
15Green, MW, Elliman, NA & Kretsch, MJ (2005) Weight loss strategies, stress and cognitive function: supervised vs unsupervised dieting. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 908918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16Green, MW, Elliman, NA & Rogers, PJ (1995) A lack of effect of short-term food deprivation on cognitive function. J Psychiat Res 29, 245253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Kretsch, MJ, Green, MW, Fong, AKH, et al. (1997) Cognitive effects of a long-term weight reducing diet. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 21, 1421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18Green, MW, Elliman, NA & Rogers, PJ (1997) The effects of food deprivation and incentive motivation on blood glucose levels and cognitive function. Psychopharmacology 134, 8894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Anderson, IM, Parry-Billings, M, Newsholme, EA, et al. (1990) Dieting reduces plasma tryptophan and alters 5-HT function in women. Psychol Med 2, 785791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20Green, MW, Jones, AD, Smith, I, et al. (2003) Impairments in working memory associated with naturalistic dieting in women: no relationship with urinary 5-HIAA levels? Appetite 40, 145153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Kretsch, MJ, Blanton, CA & Green, MW (2006) Cognitive impairment in low-iron status premenopausal women. FASEB J 20, A1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22Beard, J, Borel, M & Peterson, J (1997) Changes in iron status during weight loss with very low energy diets. Am J Clin Nutr 66, 104110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Kretsch, MJ, Fong, AKH, Green, MW, et al. (1998) Cognitive function, iron status and haemoglobin concentration in obese dieting women. Eur J Clin Nutr 52, 512518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Snaith, RP & Zigmnd, SS (1994) The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
25Van Strien, T, Frijters, JER, Bergers, GPA, et al. (1986) The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behavior. Int J Eat Disorder 5, 295315.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26Beauchamp, MH, Dagher, A, Aston, JAD, et al. (2003) Dynamic functional changes associated with cognitive skill learning of an adapted version of the Tower of London task. Neuroimage 20, 16491660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Bakan, P (1959) Extroversion–introversion and improvement in an auditory vigilance task. Br J Psychol 50, 323332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28Cooper, LA (1975) Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes. Cog Psych 7, 2043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29Shallice, T (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 298, 199209.Google ScholarPubMed
30Krikorian, R, Bartok, J & Gay, N (1994) Tower of London procedure: a standard method and developmental data. J Clin Exp Neuropsych 16, 840850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31Ben-Tovim, DI & Walker, MK (1991) The development of the Ben-Tovim Walker Body Attitudes Questionnaire (BAQ), a new measure of women's attitudes towards their own bodies. Psychol Med 21, 775784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Livingstone, MBE, Prentice, AM, Strain, JJ, et al. (1990) Accuracy of weighed dietary records in studies of diet and health. Br Med J 300, 708712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Krall, EA & Dwyer, JT (1987) Validity of a food frequency questionnaire and a food diary in a short-term recall situation. J Am Diet Assoc 87, 13741377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Baseline Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) scores for all three groups (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 1

Table 2 BMI, energy (kJ), percentage of body fat and macronutrient (fat, protein and carbohydrate) intake for all participants over each test session (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 2

Table 3 Blood iron and vitamin B12 status for all participants over each test session (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 3

Table 4 Bakan Vigilance Task, Verbal Free Recall, Two-Finger Tapping, Mental Rotation and Simple Reaction Time (SRT) data (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 4

Fig. 1 Mean planning times on the Tower of London Task for all three participant groups. Values are means, with their standard errors represented by vertical bars. *Mean values were significantly different from those of the other two participant groups (P <0·05). , Supervised dieters; , unsupervised dieters; , non-dieting controls.

Figure 5

Table 5 Somatic Sensations Questionnaire data (mm) (Mean values and standard deviations)